Skip to main content
. 2016 Oct 6;70(11):2595–2610. doi: 10.1111/evo.13071

Table 1.

Summary of the hypotheses and evolutionary concepts relating to how the relative survival and reproductive benefits of dispersal and philopatry may drive prospecting and floating behavior and, ultimately, cooperative behavior or sociality in vertebrates

Prediction met in Seychelles warblers
Theory Principles Concepts Predictions relative to philopatry in Seychelles warblers Prospectors Floaters Residents
Benefits of prospecting and floating Survival benefits: access to food 1. Better access to food outside resident territory 1. Prospectors/floaters are in better condition No No
2. Prospectors/floaters have better access to food No No
Reproductive benefits: improved—current or future—reproduction 2A. Higher likelihood of obtaining a territory 1. Prospectors/floaters are more likely to obtain a territory Yes Yes
2. Prospectors/floaters get a territory further away Yes Yes
2B. Obtaining a better quality breeding position 1. Prospectors/floaters obtain a territory with higher food abundance No No
2. Prospectors/floaters get a less related partner Yes No
2C. Access to extrapair mating 1. Prospecting/floating males fertilize extragroup females No1 No1
2. Prospecting/floating females are fertilized by an extragroup male Maybe1 No1
3. Females (mainly) prospect in fertile period No N/A
Ecological constraint Habitat saturation 3. No suitable breeding habitat for independent breeding available 1. Individuals that do not find a vacancy return to resident territory Yes No
2. Vacancies are filled very rapidly Yes/No2 Yes/No2
3. Breeding positions are obtained by inheritance only/mainly No3 No3
Ecological constraints and Survival benefits: a resident territory 4A. Reduced predation 1. Prospectors/floaters suffer more predation No No
survival benefits‐of‐philopatry provides a “safe‐haven” 4B. Access to food in resident territory 1. Individuals are tolerated in resident territory Yes No
2. Individuals in good quality resident territories are less likely to leave No No
3. Related individuals prospect/float less (nepotism) No Yes
4. Individuals in smaller groups prospect/float less No Yes
4C. Prospecting is energetically costly 1. Prospectors/floaters are in worse condition Yes No4
2. Prospectors/floaters have lower survival No4 Yes
3. Prospectors/floaters are attacked in intruded territory Yes Yes
4. Prospecting/floating is condition dependent Maybe4 N/A
Reproductive benefits‐of‐philopatry Reproductive benefits: individuals stay in a resident territory for—future, current, or indirect—reproductive benefits 5A. Territory inheritance 1. Resident individuals inherit the territory No5 N/A Only 8%5
5B. Access to nearby vacancies ( = shifting) 1. Individuals obtain a breeding position nearby resident territory Yes5 N/A Yes5
2. Individuals with more neighboring territories prospect/float less No No
3. Competitive individuals prospect/float less No/Yes6 No6
5C. Access to own reproduction 1. Individuals obtain parentage in their resident territory Yes7 N/A Yes7
2. Individuals prospect/float less if opposite‐sex breeder is unrelated No N/A
3. Females prospect/float less than males (more parentage7) No No
5D. Individuals can obtain (indirect) benefits from helping 1. Nonhelpers prospect more than helpers No N/A
2. Individuals related to the breeders prospect/float less (kin benefits) No Yes

To test whether these concepts may explain subordinate behavior in Seychelles warblers, a set of specific predictions was developed and tested in this study. Prospectors were individuals with a resident territory who made temporary prospecting trips, whereas floaters left a territory permanently to float through the population with no resident territory (note therefore that some predictions only apply to prospectors as specific data are not available for floaters (N/A).

1Subordinate females sometimes lay eggs sired by an extragroup male (Richardson et al. 2001). It is unclear, however, whether these fertilizations are a result of male pursuits, or females actively prospecting to obtain copulations with extragroup males (although most females prospected outside their fertile period [see Results] suggesting that the primary reason for females to prospect is not extragroup matings). Moreover, extragroup offspring are always sired by dominant males, and egg‐dumping (e.g., by floaters) is absent (Richardson et al. 2001).

2Eikenaar et al. (2009) showed that the median time to occupation of a breeding vacancy was 3.5 days, with a range of 1–20 days. Two vacancies remained unoccupied for more than 3 months.

3No direct evidence, but tendency for individuals in better quality territories being more likely to prospect (P = 0.06–0.08; Table 4) and prospecting seems to occur mainly in periods with high food availability (Supporting Information).

4Nonsignificant tendency (see Fig. 1).

5Of the 170 individuals that obtained a breeding position, 81 (48%) obtained a position further than two territories away from their last resident territory, 75 (44%) obtained a territory nearby (one or two territories from their last resident territory), and 14 (8%) inherited the resident territory.

6Males prospect less than females, but prospecting is independent of age (Table 4); both sexes, however, may be considered “competitive” as both defend a territory. Floating was independent of age and sex.

7Forty‐four percent of subordinate females lay an egg, but only one of 55 young was sired by a subordinate male (Richardson et al. 2001, 2002). Subordinate cobreeding females are, however, almost always older (>1 year) subordinates (Kingma et al., unpubl. ms.).