Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2016 Jul 15;29(3):875–885. doi: 10.1017/S0954579416000547

A cascade from disregard for rules of conduct at preschool age to parental power assertion at early school age to antisocial behavior in early preadolescence: Interplay with the child’s skin conductance level

Grazyna Kochanska a, Rebecca L Brock b, Lea J Boldt a
PMCID: PMC5237621  NIHMSID: NIHMS806327  PMID: 27417305

Abstract

Young children’s disregard for conduct rules (failing to experience discomfort following transgressions and violating adults’ prohibitions) often foreshadows future antisocial trajectories, perhaps in part because it elicits more power-assertive parental discipline, which in turn promotes children’s antisocial behavior. This process may be particularly likely for children with low skin conductance level (SCL). In 102 two-parent community families, we tested a model in which children’s SCL, assessed at 8 years, was posed as a moderator of the cascade from children’s disregard for conduct rules at 4.5 years to parents’ power assertion at 5.5 and 6.5 years to antisocial behavior at 10 and 12 years. Children’s disregard for conduct rules was observed in scripted laboratory paradigms, parents’ power assertion was observed in discipline contexts, and children’s antisocial behavior was rated by parents. Conditional process analyses revealed that the developmental cascade from early disregard for rules to future parental power assertion to antisocial outcomes occurred only for the children with low SCL (below median), but not their high-SCL (above median) peers. By elucidating the specific interplay among children’s disregard for rules, the parenting they receive, and their psychophysiology, this study represents a developmentally informed, multilevel approach to early etiology of antisocial behavior.


Because antisocial and disruptive behavioral trajectories tend to emerge early, to remain relatively stable, and to be linked to a variety of costs for individuals, families, and societies, interest in their early origins has grown exponentially (e.g., Dishion & Patterson, 2006; Hyde et al., 2013; Shaw, 2013; Wakschlag, Tolan, & Leventhal, 2010). There is a consensus that to substantially inform our understanding of risks for antisocial trajectories, research needs to integrate constructs assessed at the level of the child’s biological individuality, behavior, and the parent–child relationship, especially in longitudinal designs (Burnette & Cicchetti, 2012). We propose such a multilevel developmental approach, bridging several literatures: research on children’s early internalization of rules of conduct, their psychophysiology, and the parenting they receive. We test our model in a study of community families (mothers, fathers, and children) followed longitudinally from preschool age to early preadolescence. These families represented a normative community sample, and children were not screened for the presence of elevated behavior problems; we believe, however, that understanding processes unfolding in typically developing children can potentially inform clinical research, consistent with tenets of developmental psychopathology.

Research has increasingly considered emotional and behavioral aspects of young children’s emerging regard for rules of conduct (guilt and concern about transgressions and fledgling adherence to adults’ prohibitions, particularly without surveillance) to be markers of adaptive early development (Kochanska, Koenig, Barry, Kim, & Yoon, 2010; Thompson, 2012, 2014). In developmental and socialization literatures, children’s lack of concern about their transgressions, poor adherence to standards for behavior, and rule breaking have long been considered the most common triggers for parental deployment of broadly ranging discipline techniques, beginning in the second and third years of life, including occasional harsh discipline and power assertion (Gralinski & Kopp, 1993; Grusec & Kuczynski, 1980; Hoffman, 1975; Smetana, 1989).

In developmental psychopathology, the child’s disregard for rules, for example, failing to feel discomfort after misbehavior or engaging in rule breaking, is a key component of oppositional, disruptive conduct problems. High levels of disregard for rules in childhood are often seen as early signs of an emerging trajectory toward disruptive conduct problems (Bufferd, Dyson, Hernandez, & Wakschlag, 2016; Callender, Olson, Kerr, & Sameroff, 2010; Frick & Viding, 2009; Frick & White, 2008; Petitclerc, Boivin, Dionne, Perusse, & Tremblay, 2011; Petitclerc, Boivin, Dionne, Zoccolillo, & Tremblay, 2009; Wakschlag et al., 2007). Such trajectory is now believed to begin early and be relatively stable (Shaw, 2013).

Bringing those converging literatures together may be productive. It is quite likely that the often-observed link between young children’s disregard for conduct rules and future antisocial behavior is mediated through harsh or power-assertive discipline that parents deploy toward children who display high level of disregard for rules. Links between parental power assertion and antisocial behavior are well established (e.g., Bender et al., 2007; Dodge, Coie, & Lynam, 2006; Gershoff, 2002; Kimonis & Frick, 2010; McCord, 1997; Patterson, Dishion, & Bank, 1984). Consequently, studying the cascade from the child’s disregard for rules to future antisocial behavior problems as mediated by parental power-assertive discipline may provide insights into one mechanism explaining complex etiology of conduct problems and the stability of disruptive problems from childhood to adolescence and beyond.

In this article, we propose a more complex conditional process model, in which the developmental cascade from early disregard for rules to parental power assertion to future antisocial behavior is expected to occur for children who have low skin conductance level (SCL). Such children presumably fail to respond to subtle, gentle levels of parental discipline, in contrast to children with high SCL. Few, if any, studies have examined children’s SCL as a potential moderator of the process leading from early disregard for rules of behavior, to parental power-assertive discipline, to future antisocial behavior problems. We believe that such a model, consistent with recent biopsychosocial perspectives on parenting (Calkins, Propper, & Mills-Koonce, 2013), could substantially enhance our understanding of etiology of conduct problems, improve our ability to make prognostic predictions from preschoolers’ disregard for rules to future antisocial behavior, and inform translational hypotheses about effective parenting interventions.

Conceptually, children’s biologically founded temperament characteristics, including low SCL, have been broadly implicated as a general risk factor for antisocial trajectories (Blair, Peschardt, Budhani, Mitchell, & Pine, 2006; Fowles & Kochanska, 2000; Frick & Morris, 2004; Nigg, 2006; Raine, 2002; Raine, Venables, & Williams, 1990). SCL, a classic measure of the functioning of the autonomic nervous system, reflects variations in the ease with which electrical current passes across the skin due to fluctuations in sweat gland activity. Low SCL represents a psychophysiological diathesis for and a correlate of antisocial externalizing problems (Crowell et al., 2006; Fowles, 1993; Lorber, 2004; Lykken, 1995; Posthumus, Bocker, Raaijmakers, Van Engeland, & Matthys, 2009; Raine, 2002).

One reason, perhaps the main reason, why low SCL is a risk factor for antisocial trajectories is its potential link to children’s responsiveness to socialization (Dadds & Salmon, 2003). Children with low SCL, due to their relative insensitivity to punishment compared to children with high SCL, likely fail to respond to subtle parenting pressure in the context of transgressions (Raine, 2002). Consequently, over time, they likely elicit gradually increasing parental pressure and more forceful, power-assertive discipline. Thus, physiologically based punishment insensitivity may lead to the gradual unfolding of mutually adversarial, power-assertive parent–child cycles that undermine an adaptive, normative course of socialization and launch the dyad on a maladaptive course, leading to antisocial developmental trajectories. In contrast, children with high SCL respond positively to subtle parental socialization, before the pressure reaches a high level.

This developmental account converges with the somatic marker hypothesis, proposed by Damasio, Tranel, and Damasio (1991), Sobhani and Bechara (2011), and Tranel (1994). That hypothesis holds that most people’s choices of actions, including morally relevant decisions, are regulated by an automatic guiding system that activates “somatic markers.” Those markers are associated with one’s past affective experience, and they involve an uncomfortable “gut feeling” that occurs while one ponders an act that has been punished or disapproved of in the past. Such negative somatic state, activated by the marker, consequently aids in inhibiting the relevant behavior. Note that SCL reflects the behavioral inhibition system, which governs sensitivity to or avoidance of aversive circumstances (Erath, El-Sheikh, & Cummings, 2009; Fowles, 1980). Higher SCL is involved in amplifying the process of “somatic marking” (Carter & Pasqualini, 2004; Damasio, 1994). Together, those convergent bodies of work suggest that for children with temperamentally based propensity to experience higher autonomic arousal, even subtle parenting discipline and the resulting discomfort lead to rule-breaking behavior becoming strongly somatically marked. Thus, those children become particularly attuned to the possible negative consequences of rule violations. The process is much less effective for children with lower SCL.

Multiple works in the literature have conceptualized and supported the child’s evocative role in coercive parenting and future maladaptive developmental outcomes (Bates, Schermerhorn, & Petersen, 2012; Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, & Mead, 2007; Bell, 1968; Blair et al., 2006; Briggs-Gowan et al., 2014; Dadds & Salmon, 2003; Dishion & Patterson, 2006; Erath et al., 2009; Kimonis & Frick, 2010; Pardini, 2008; Raine, 2002; Shaw, Gilliom, Ingoldsby, & Nagin, 2003). However, very little of that research has relied on physiological measures of children’s autonomic responses and behavioral observations of parenting in longitudinal designs, and very few studies have examined different sequences, unfolding over time, in children with varying biological profiles.

There is some empirical support for components of the model. Children’s SCL has been found to moderate the link between parenting and antisocial behavior, with negative effects of adverse experiences typically found for children with low SCL but not for those with high SCL. Erath et al. (2009) found concurrent (at age 8) associations between parent- and child-reported harsh parenting and children’s externalizing problems for children with low, but not high, SCL. Erath, El-Sheikh, Hinnant, and Cummings (2011) found that harsh parenting (reported by parents and children) in conjunction with low SCL was associated with boys’ high and stable level of externalizing behavior from age 8 to 10. In those studies, the difference between the child’s SCL during a challenging or stressful task (e.g., an audiotaped argument or star tracing) and at baseline was typically used.

In the present sample, we found significant relations between variation in the history of parental care and children’s externalizing behavior problems across the first 10 years, but only for low-SCL children (Kochanska, Brock, Chen, Aksan, & Anderson, 2015). In another sample, we focused on the absence of children’s externalizing problems (i.e., mature conscience) at age 4 as the outcome, and we found effects of parenting for children with low SCL, but not for those with high SCL (Fowles & Kochanska, 2000). In our studies, we have used a tonic measure of SCL that was averaged across the child’s responses to a variety of scripted stimuli, episodes, and tasks (e.g., rest, deep breaths, anticipation, and bursts of noises). Such an approach has also been supported in the literature (Crider et al., 2004; Dawson, Schell, & Filion, 2007; see Fowles & Missel, 1994, for review), including studies that have linked children’s SCL to their observed temperament. For example, Scarpa, Raine, Venables, and Mednick (1997) used an average SCL across auditory stimuli (six recorded standard orienting tones).

To our knowledge, however, the entire conditional process model has not yet been tested using behavioral methods and in a longitudinal design. To do so was the goal of this study. Toward that goal, we examined the cascade from children’s observed disregard for conduct rules at preschool age, to parents’ (mothers’ and fathers’) observed power assertion at early school age, to children’s antisocial behavior problems, rated by parents in early preadolescence. We anticipated that the cascade would be present for children with low SCL, assessed at age 8, but that the cascade would be absent for children with high SCL.

Method

Participants and overview

Two-parent families of typically developing infants (N = 102) responded to ads distributed in several counties in Iowa, advertising a longitudinal study. To be accepted, the biological parents had to be living together, both willing to participate, able to speak English during sessions, and not planning to move in the next 5 years. The parents ranged in education (25% of mothers and 30% of fathers had no more than high school education, and 21% of mothers and 20% of fathers had postgraduate education) and annual income (25% made less than $40,000, and 49% made over $60,000). Ninety percent of mothers and 84% of fathers were White, 3%and 8% Hispanic, 2% and 3% African American, 1% and 3% Asian, 1% of mothers Pacific Islanders, and 3% and 2% “other” non-White. In 20% of families, one or both parents were non-White. The study was approved by the University of Iowa Institutional Review Board; parents completed informed consent, and children (at age 8) completed assent.

This article draws from the following assessments. At 4.5 years (N = 99), children’s disregard for rules of conduct was observed in multiple paradigms in lengthy sessions in a psychology laboratory, one with each parent; at 5.5 and 6.5 years (Ns = 92 and 90), parental power-assertive discipline was observed, again in separate sessions with each parent at each age (power-assertive discipline was also observed at 15 months, N = 101, and treated as a covariate); at age 8 (N = 81), SCL was assessed in a psychophysiology laboratory; and at ages 10 and 12 (Ns 82 and 79), children’s antisocial behavior was rated by mothers and fathers. Female experimenters (FEs) conducted all sessions.

All behavioral data were coded from videotapes. Reliability was typically established on approximately 15%–20% of cases, followed by frequent realignments, and depending on the type of code, relied on alphas, intraclass correlations, or kappas. Data were aggregated at multiple levels to produce robust final constructs (Rushton, Brainerd, & Pressley, 1983).

Measures of children’s disregard for rules of conduct at age 4.5 years

Children’s lack of concern or remorse following rule violations

Paradigms

Children were observed during two highly scripted, contrived “mishaps” at each session (Kim, Kochanska, Boldt, Koenig Nordling, & O’Bleness, 2014). FE asked the child to be “very careful” while handling her “special object” (a doll and alarm clock). As soon as the child began to handle the object, it fell apart in a fairly dramatic way. At that point, FE expressed mild regret (e.g., “Oh, my clock.”), sat silently for 60 s, and then asked the child scripted questions (e.g., “What happened?” and “Who did this?”). FE then left the room with the object for 30 s “to fix it,” and returned with an exact undamaged replica, reassuring the child that he or she was not at fault for the damage (e.g., referring to a preexisting damage that was easily fixed).

Coding

Behavioral codes applied to 5-s segments included avoiding gaze, presence of facial tension, and bodily tension. Avoiding gaze was coded when the child looked away, downward, or askance (if gaze avoidance lasted throughout the entire segment, it was multiplied by 2). Facial tension was coded as present when the child displayed the following: twisting or biting lips, raising/furrowing eyebrows, squinting eyes, or crinkling nose. Bodily tension was coded when the child showed squirming, backing away, hanging head down, hunching shoulders, hugging self, or covering face with hands (0 = no codeable signs of tension, 1 = one sign of tension, and 2 = multiple and/or strong signs of tension).

Several codes were applied to the entire longer epochs (60 s after the mishap, 60 s during FE’s queries, 30 s during FE’s absence to “fix” the toy, and 60 s after FE’s return with the “fixed” object): the child’s overall distress response (for the first three epochs) and affect (for all four epochs). Overall distress response was rated as 1 = child oblivious to, not distressed or affected by the mishap in any way, 2 = child notices mishap, briefly, mildly distressed or affected, 3 = child distressed or affected by mishap, stilling, uneasy, concerned, and 4 = child strongly distressed or, affected, freezes, cries, very uncomfortable or uneasy. Affect was rated as strong negative (multiplied by 2), negative, strong positive (multiplied by 2), positive, or neutral (not used in this report). The reliability ks were 0.61–0.73, and the as were all >0.94.

Data aggregation

For each mishap, instances of each 5-s code were totaled and divided by the number of coded segments. The scores for overall response were summed across the three epochs. The strong negative and negative affect scores, and strong positive and positive affect scores were summed, respectively. The codes cohered within each mishap (αs = 0.64 and 0.67). Therefore, a composite guilt score for each mishap was produced by aggregating across those (standardized) scores: avoiding gaze, facial tension, bodily tension, overall distress response, negative affect, and (reversed) positive affect. Those composite guilt scores correlated across the mishaps, r (99) = .52, p < .001, and thus were aggregated. To denote the child’s overall lack of remorse, that final score was reversed (M = 0.00, SD = 0.87, range = −4.58 to 1.53).

Children’s disregard for parents’ rules

Paradigms

Children’s (dis)regard for parents’ standards of behavior was observed when children were alone (for 8 min) with objects designated as off-limit by the parents (both maternal and paternal prohibition was observed), at the end of the session with each parent. Those objects, very attractive to young children, were located on a low shelf in the laboratory; the parent had been asked to prohibit the child from touching those objects at the outset and to enforce the rule throughout the session. Procedural details have been published in multiple studies (e.g., Kochanska, Coy, & Murray, 2001; Kochanska & Kim, 2014).

Coding

The child’s behavior was coded for each 5-s segment. The reliability κ was 0.95. For the current purpose, we used the code of rule violations, the instances when the child played with the objects, divided by the number of segments (data with mothers first, with fathers second; M = 0.09, SD = 0.22, range = 0.00–0.91, M = 0.08, SD = 0.19, range = 0.00–0.82). Those scores were standardized.

Children’s disregard for another adult’s rules

Paradigms

Children’s behavior was observed when they were alone playing two games, having been given rules by the FE that made the games essentially impossible to win (e.g., Kochanska & Kim, 2014). Before each game, the FE described the rules and promised rewards for winning, and asked the child to follow the rules and not to “cheat,” in a friendly but serious manner. The child was then left alone for 3 min. Then, FE returned, apologized for having given “the wrong rules,” and invited the child to play again (every child won a prize). One game involved throwing darts into a small ring, without leaving the designated space, moving the ring, or retrieving darts once they were thrown. The other game involved finding a small plastic fish of a certain shape in glass jars that were covered with cloths, only by haptic exploration, without lifting the cloth, looking, or returning fish to a jar once picked.

Coding and data aggregation

Child behaviors (multiple types of rule violations, for example, leaving the designated space, lifting cloth, retrieving darts or fish, or moving the ring) were coded for every 3-s segment. Latencies to violate rules were also coded. The reliability as ranged from 0.76 to 1.00. Those codes were standardized, and first aggregated within each game (the dart game, M = 0.00, SD = 0.59, range = −0.86 to 2.25, α = 0.73; the fish game, M = −0.02, SD = 0.57, range = −0.83 to 1.35, α = 0.83), then across the two games, r (98) = .19, p < .06, into an overall score of disregard of FE’s rules (M = 0.00, SD = 0.77, range = −1.43 to 1.67).

Overall measure of disregard for conduct rules

Children’s lack of concern following rule violations, disregard for the mother’s, father’s, and the FE’s rules were aggregated (all the scores were standardized, see above; M = 0.01, SD = 0.66, range = −1.72 to 2.38). The Cronbach α value for the four scores was satisfactory (0.66), and the average interitem correlation was 0.33.

Measures of mothers’ and fathers’ power assertive discipline at ages 5.5 and 6.5 years

Observed contexts

Parents and children were observed during several naturalistic but scripted control contexts encompassing a request, “Do” (toy cleanup, 10 min), and a prohibition, “Don’t” (not touching the very attractive, off-limits objects displayed on a low shelf in the laboratory, 60 min). Thus, the total coded times were 70 min with each parent at each age (total of 280 min for each child).

Coding and data aggregation

Parental style of control was coded for every 30-s segment (throughout the entire toy cleanup and whenever the parent and/or child were involved with the prohibited objects). The codes used to produce the power assertion measure included, for each segment, global ratings (“no interaction,” “social exchange,” “gentle guidance,” “control,” and “forceful, negative control”) and records of all physical techniques (“assertive interventions,” such as holding the child’s hand firmly, physically preventing the child from leaving the chore, blocking access to toys, and “forceful interventions,” such as handling the child roughly, wresting a toy angrily away from the child). The reliability ks ranged from 0.66 to 0.83 across several teams.

For each context (“Do” and “Don’t”), each code was tallied and divided by the number of segments. Then weights were applied to reflect the amount of power used: −2 = no interaction, −1 = social exchange, 1 = gentle guidance, 2 = control, 3 = forceful control, 4 = physical assertive, and 5 = physical forceful. Those figures were summed, creating weighted power assertion composites for “Do” and for “Don’t.” Those two composites were then standardized and averaged across “Do” and “Don’t,” into one overall power assertion score for each parent at each age. Then, the four scores were combined into the overall amount of parental power-assertive discipline the child received (M = 0.01, SD = 0.60, range = −1.21 to 3.54, Cronbach α = 0.77).

An analogous measure was observed and computed when children were 15 months. This measure served as a covariate (M = 0.00, SD = 0.62, range = −1.11 to 1.89).

Measures of children’s SCL at age 8

Procedure

The detailed description is in Kochanska et al. (2015). Data on children’s SCL were obtained in five tasks, presented in fixed order: rest 1 (3 min), deep breathing (2 min), startle (3 min, acoustic startle probes, 90 db white noise, 500 ms, with the intervals between the probes lasting 15–25 s), rest 2 (3 min), and gift anticipation (child was asked to wait for a gift while a timer on the computer screen showed the countdown, 2 min).

Data acquisition

SCL data were acquired using BIOPAC MP100 system at the sampling rate of 1000 Hz. Two Ag/AgCl electrodes were placed on the thenar and hypothenar eminences of the child’s left hand. Data from 74 out of 81 children were of good quality. SCL data were first downsampled to 100 Hz (centisecond); artifacts were identified manually by a trained research assistant blind to hypotheses, and corrected using Ledalab software (Benedek & Kaernbach, 2010). Centisecond by centisecond SCL data were averaged for each task and then were log-transformed. Data from Tasks 1–5 were aggregated to represent the child’s SCL (Fowles, 2008; Fowles & Kochanska, 2000). The Cronbach α was 0.95. That composite was standardized (M = 0.00, SD = 1.00, range = −1.97 to 2.17).

Note that in the extant studies of interplay between SCL and parenting, SCL has been approached as either a dichotomous (low vs. high) construct (Fowles & Kochanska, 2000; Kochanska et al., 2015), or a continuous construct (e.g., Erath et al., 2009). In our past work with this and another sample, we have adopted the former approach. Of note, recent studies using sophisticated analytical approaches to children’s SCL data have produced evidence supporting the feasibility of two distinct groups. For example, Gao, Raine, Venables, Dawson, and Mednick (2010), using latent class growth analyses of children followed from age 3 to 8, identified “good conditioners” and “poor conditioners.” Buodo, Moscardino, Scrimin, Altoe, and Palomba (2013) examined 9- to 12-year-old children, using cluster analyses, and identified “lower SCRs” and “higher SCRs.”

Consequently, we again examine SCL as low versus high (using the median split of the overall standardized composite) as our primary approach in the main conditional process analyses; however, we complement those analyses with secondary analyses, in which SCL is treated as a continuous construct (the average across the five tasks, see above). This approach provides consistency with our past work while also allowing us to pinpoint more precisely the levels of SCL where the conditional effects are present.

Measures of children’s antisocial behavior at ages 10 and 12

Parents rated children’s behavior problems in the well-established clinical instruments: at age 10, the Child Symptom Inventory (CSI-4; Gadow & Sprafkin, 2002) and the Inventory of Callous–Unemotional Traits (ICU; Frick, 2003; Frick, Bodin, & Barry, 2000; Frick & White, 2008), and at age 12, the Adolescent Symptom Inventory (ASI-4R; Gadow & Sprafkin, 2008) and the ICU.

In the CSI-4 and ASI-4R, we used the symptom severity scoring, with each item rated from 0 (never) to 3 (very often). We selected oppositional defiant disorder (e.g., defies, refuses, or deliberately annoys; 8 items; at age 10 for mothers and fathers, αs = 0.85 and 0.89, respectively; at age 12, αs = 0.90 and 0.88, respectively) and conduct disorder (e.g., bullies others or lies; 15 items; at age 10, αs = 0.77 and 0.59, respectively; at age 12, αs = 0.80 and 0.64, respectively). At age 12, we also included antisocial personality disorder (five items, αs = 0.87 and 0.76, respectively). At each age, the scores were added into one antisocial behavior rating for each parent (age 10, mothers and fathers, M = 6.73, SD = 4.31, range = 0 – 21, respectively, and M = 6.17, SD = 4.24, range = 0–19, respectively; age 12, mothers and fathers, M = 6.60, SD = 6.46, range = 0–47, respectively, and M = 5.76, SD = 5.15, range = 0–23, respectively).

The ICU has 24 items, rated from 0 (not at all true) to 3 (definitely true), targeting the absence of guilt and empathy and disregard for rules and standards of behavior (e.g., “does not care if s/he is in trouble,” “does not like to put time into doing things well,” and “feelings of others are unimportant”). We computed the mean of all items for each parent (age 10, mothers and fathers, M = 0.69, SD = 0.32, range = 0.13–1.83, and M = 0.74, SD = 0.35, range = 0.13–1.83, respectively; age 12, mothers and fathers, M = 0.73, SD = 0.36, range = 0.00–2.50, and M = 0.73, SD = 0.31, range = 0.21–1.88, respectively). The Cronbach αs were 0.84 and 0.87 at age 10 and 0.86 and 0.83 at age 12 for mothers and fathers, respectively.

To create the final antisocial score, we standardized and averaged the eight scores (four for each parent: age 10 CSI-4 and ICU, and age 12 ASI-4R and ICU; M = 0.03, SD = 0.79, range = −1.14–3.62). Even though this score integrated items from two different instruments that differ somewhat in emphasis, it was highly coherent (α = 0.87). There was no item whose dropping would have increased coherence.

Data analyses

Data were analyzed using Mplus software (Muthén & Muthén, 2010) to address missing data using full information maximum likelihood, considered superior to other methods of handling missing data (Enders & Bandalos, 2001). Thus, the complete sample of 102 families was retained for the analyses. Preliminary regression analyses suggested that the first path in the cascade (child disregard for conduct rules at 4.5 years to parental power assertion at 5.5–6.5 years) was moderated by SCL (B = −0.64, SE = 0.22, p = .004), but that the second path (parental power assertion at 5.5–6.5 years to child antisocial behavior at 10–12) was not moderated by SCL (B = −0.37, SE = 0.34, p = .269). Consequently, in the analyses that follow, a first-stage conditional process model (Preacher, Rucker, & Hayes, 2007) was tested such that only the first path in the cascade (from child disregard for rules to power assertion) varied as a function of SCL. Parental power assertion at 15 months and child gender were included as the covariates.

The comparative fit index (CFI; Bentler, 1990) was selected to assess incremental fit of the tested models given it performs especially well in relatively small sample sizes. We also examined the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; Browne & Cudeck, 1992) and the standardized root mean residual (SRMR; Hu & Bentler, 1995) to further assess global fit.

Conditional indirect effects of child disregard for rules (4.5 years) on child antisocial behavior (10–12 years) via parental power assertion (5.5–6.5 years) were estimated for high- versus low-SCL children. A bootstrap approach (Shrout & Bolger, 2002) was implemented, which provides an empirical approximation of sampling distributions of indirect effects to provide confidence intervals (CIs) of estimates. The presence of an indirect effect is detected if zero does not fall within the CI (i.e., the indirect effect is different than zero). A bootstrapping sampling method is the preferred method for testing indirect effects because no assumptions need to be made about the shape of the sampling distribution, and power is maximized while minimizing Type I error rate (MacKinnon, Lockwood, & Williams, 2004; Preacher et al., 2007; Shrout & Bolger, 2002). We performed a nonparametric resampling method (bias-corrected bootstrap) with 5,000 resamples drawn to derive the 95% CIs for the indirect effects at various levels of SCL.

Results

Preliminary analyses

We examined the correlations among the studied constructs (using the continuous measure of SCL). Those data are in Table 1. As anticipated, children’s stronger disregard for rules of conduct at 4.5 years was associated with more parental power-assertive discipline at ages 5.5–6.5 years (but only marginally with earlier parental power-assertive discipline at 15 months) and with more parent-rated antisocial behavior at ages 10–12 years. Parents’ more power-assertive discipline (only at 5.5–6.5 years) was also associated with their ratings of their children as more antisocial at 10–12. SCL was not associated with any other variable.

Table 1.

Intercorrelations among the constructs

15 Months
M/F
Power-Assertive
Disciplinea
4.5 Years
Child Disregard
for Conduct
Rulesa
5.5–6.5 Years
M/F
Power-Assertive
Disciplinea
10–12 Years
M/F
Child Antisocial
Behaviorb
8 Years
Child SCLc
M/F power-assertive discipline, 15 months .19 .13 .13 .08
Child disregard for conduct rules, 4.5 years .56**** .33*** −.09
M/F power-assertive discipline, 5.5–6.5 years .40**** −.11
Child antisocial behavior, 10–12 years −.02

Note: M/F, Mother/father; SCL, skin conductance level.

a

Composite of observed measures.

b

Composite of parent-rated measures.

c

Physiological assessment, composite across all episodes (continuous score).

p < .10.

***

p < .01.

****

p < .001.

Variations in the cascade from child disregard for conduct rules at 4.5 years to parental power assertion at 5.5–6.5 years to child antisocial behavior at 10–12 as a function of SCL

We tested the proposed, first-stage conditional process model for the measure of SCL (coded 1 = high, above median, 0 = low, below median). Global model fit was deemed adequate (CFI = 0.937, SRMR = 0.029, RMSEA = 0.202), although it was not excellent. Results of this model are reported in Figure 1. The overall conditional indirect effect of child disregard for conduct rules at 4.5 years on child antisocial behavior at 10–12, as mediated by parental power assertion at 5.5–6.5 years, was present for the low-SCL children, but not for the high-SCL children. Conditional indirect effects are reported with bias-corrected bootstrap 95% CI, and significant indirect effects are bolded in Figure 1. Approximately 21% of the variance in child antisocial behavior and 46% of the variance in parental power was accounted for by the model.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Results of first-stage conditional process model. Significant conditional indirect effects are in bold. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .005. Child gender and parental power assertion at 15 months were covaried but are not depicted. Unstandardized coefficients and standard errors are presented for the direct paths. Solid lines indicate significant paths. Dashed line indicates a nonsignificant path. The indirect effect of child disregard for conduct rules (4.5 years) on child antisocial behavior (10–12 years) via parental power assertion (5.5–6.5 years) was present for the low skin conductance level children (bolded) but not high skin conductance level children, as indicated by the 95% confidence intervals.

Next, we conducted complementary analyses of the first-stage conditional process model, using a continuous measure of SCL. Global model fit was excellent (CFI=1.00,SRMR= 0.004, RMSEA = 0.000). The findings essentially replicated those for SCL conceptualized as low versus high, in that a conditional indirect effect from disregard for conduct rules to parental power to antisocial behavior were present at very low SCL (1.5 SD below the mean), 95% CI = (0.039, 0.753), low SCL (1 SD below the mean), 95% CI = (0.031, 0.644), and average SCL (at the mean), 95%CI= (0.013, 0.463). There was no indirect effect at high SCL (1 SD above the mean), 95% CI = (−0.013, 0.414), or very high SCL (1.5 SD above the mean), 95% CI = (−0.052, 0.422). Of note, the parameter representing the interaction between child disregard for conduct rules and the continuous measure of SCL did not reach statistical significance (B = −0.15, SE = 0.11, p = .176) within the conditional process model; however, as stated by Hayes (2013), “statistically significant moderation of a path in a mediation model is not a necessary condition for an indirect effect to be moderated” (p. 396). Approximately 21% of the variance in child antisocial behavior and 35% of the variance in parental power were accounted for by the model.

Discussion

Within a developmental psychopathology framework, researchers have been urged to adopt a multilevel approach to the study of antisocial behavior (Burnette & Cicchetti, 2012). There is a growing consensus, fueled by the rapidly growing research on differential susceptibility (Belsky & Pluess, 2009), that socialization processes may operate differently depending on the child’s physiology. As well, developmental psychopathology researchers have been urged to examine cascades that unfold in the origins of antisocial behavior, including the specific causal processes and conditional factors that influence their occurrence (Masten & Cicchetti, 2010). Modern statistical tools aid in the pursuit of such questions.

Embracing those tenets, this multiple-method, multiple-trait, multiple-informant, and multiple-wave study examines a socialization mechanism that accounts for developmental links between children’s early disregard for rules of conduct and future antisocial behavior problems, and the child’s biological characteristics that moderate conditions under which that mechanism operates. An integration of research on children’s internalization of rules, on parents’ discipline style, and on children’s biological individuality can enhance a developmentally informed approach to understanding a long-term unfolding trajectory to antisocial behavior problems.

The findings elucidate diverging developmental pathways from disregard for conduct rules at preschool age to antisocial outcomes in preadolescence for children with distinct physiological profiles, illustrating the principle of multifinality (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996). Researchers typically assume that children’s early compromised guilt, callousness, and disregard for parents’ and other adults’ rules and standards for behavior, if sufficiently intense or frequent, uniformly foreshadow future antisocial outcomes (Wakschlag et al., 2007). However, disregard for rules is quite common at toddler and preschool age, and the prediction is vastly imperfect because of the absence of objective norms for early behaviors reflecting regard for rules. Behavioral research on young children’s responses to transgression and rule breaking is challenging, and observational studies are sparse, although promising (Callender et al., 2010). Moreover, observational research allows only for comparing children’s relative rankings within the studied samples. Consequently, it is important to understand under what conditions and through what processes early disregard for rules may foreshadow future behavior problems.

Bufferd et al. (2016) recently emphasized that internalization of rules, reflected in guilt and rule-compatible behavior, is a key developmental task in the preschool period, and its substantial, pervasive, and extreme disruptions indicate early risk for disruptive and antisocial psychopathology. They also, however, point out that the prediction is a dynamic and complex process, impacted by child temperament. This is consistent with our findings, based on a biological temperament characteristic: although early disregard for conduct rules was not associated with the child’s SCL, the sequence toward future antisocial problems that it triggered was different for children with different levels of skin conductance. The indirect effect of early disregard for rules on children’s antisocial behavior in preadolescence was present only for children with relatively lower SCL scores. Moreover, we elucidated the mechanism mediating the links between early disregard for rules and future antisocial problems for those children. Early disregard for rules elicited increased parental power assertion, presumably due to those low-SCL children’s history of failing to respond to subtle, gentle punishment (Bates et al., 2012; Bell, 1968; Blair et al., 2006; Dadds & Salmon, 2003; Dishion & Patterson, 2006; Matthys, van Goozen, Snoek, & van Engeland, 2004; Pardini, 2008; Raine, 2002), and the increased power assertion, in turn, led to future antisocial behavior.

In contrast, as hypothesized, such cascade was absent for children who had relatively higher SCL scores. For those children, there was no indirect effect from disregard for rules to future antisocial behavior. In particular, early disregard for rules by those children did not appear to elicit parental power assertion. Children with high SCL presumably respond positively to subtle parental socialization before the pressure reaches a high level (Blair et al., 2006; Briggs-Gowan et al., 2014; Dadds & Salmon, 2003; Kimonis & Frick, 2010; Raine, 2002).

This study has several limitations. The families were demographically diverse, but relatively homogeneous ethnically (although 20% had at least one non-White parent). Given the evidence of differential effects of power assertion on externalizing problems in different ethnic groups (e.g., Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997; Deater-Deckard, Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1996; Lansford, Deater-Deckard, Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 2004), a replication in a more ethnically diverse sample will be crucial.

The children in this low-risk, two-parent community sample were generally functioning well. At early age, most showed what appeared to be typical tension and discomfort following the mishaps, and they mostly adhered to the rules set by parents and others adults (although again, observational studies of young children’s disregard for rules are rare, and no objective norms exist). In addition, parental ratings of antisocial behaviors were generally low and consistent with normative samples. The aggregation across informants, assessments, and instruments produced a reasonably distributed and highly coherent measure. Of course, a replication in a clinical sample, perhaps enriched for inclusion of children with elevated psychopathology, may produce additional important insights. However, we note that the anticipated effects were found even for those typically developing children and their low-risk families.

By and large, parents deployed adaptive discipline techniques, and power assertion was rare. This is a well-known problem and a challenge in observational research, often handled by various aggregation techniques (see, e.g., Joosena, Mesman, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2012). Our data aggregation approach, based on multilevel aggregation across contexts, assessments, and parents, and on a weighing system that assigns lower weights to parental behaviors that use no or little power, and higher weights to more power-assertive behaviors, has produced reasonably distributed scores that reflected differences in the overall amount of parenting pressure the child received from age 5.5 to 6.5 years. Again, the expected effects were present, despite the generally low level of forceful discipline. A replication in populations in which power assertion is more common (e.g., in families referred for child maltreatment), however, would be very useful.

We should note that, although we found the expected indirect effect from child disregard for rules to parental power assertion to antisocial behavior only for the low-SCL children, the conditional nature of the indirect effect was due to SCL moderating the link between disregard for rules and power assertion. In other words, for low-SCL children, disregard for rules triggered parental increased power assertion, thus initiating the cascade from disregard for rules to power assertion to antisocial behavior. The link from power assertion to antisocial behavior, however, was not moderated by SCL: that direct effect was present for all children regardless of SCL, such that more power assertion predicted more antisocial behavior (consistent with the large literature on detrimental effects of power-assertive discipline).

However, our past work found a moderating effect of SCL on the link between parental power assertion and children’s antisocial behavior, with variation in power assertion associated with antisocial behavior for low-SCL children, but not high-SCL children (Kochanska et al., 2015). Most likely, the absence of the interaction effect in the current analyses was due to the power assertion measure being substantially less robust, compared with the past work. In that past work, power assertion scores had been based on data from six assessments (from 15 months to 6.5 years), whereas in the current work they were based on two assessments (at 5.5 and 6.5 years only, covarying 15 months). This may have reduced our statistical power to detect the interaction. Of course, the present longitudinal analyses required the mediator (power assertion) to be assessed after the independent variable (disregard of conduct rules at 4.5 years) and before the outcome measure (antisocial behavior at age 10 and 12). In addition, in the previous work, externalizing behaviors were assessed closer in time, in part concurrent to SCL (at ages 8 and 10), but in the current analyses, they were assessed at ages 10 and 12. As well, although similar, the measures of those behaviors were not the same in the past and present analyses. Perhaps even more important, previous analyses did not include children’s disregard for rules in the model.

Several limitations were associated with our measure of SCL. One was its timing. Because of the logistical constraints of the overall study, we were only able to conduct one psychophysiological session. Note that El-Sheikh (2007) reported that, at least between ages 6 and 13, SCL appears to reflect stable individual differences. However, SCL may be influenced by the child’s experiences (Raine et al., 2001), and thus may be open to change. The parent–child relationship and the child’s physiology represent systems that dynamically interact over time and influence each other (Calkins et al., 2013). In this study, we did not find significant correlation between earlier power assertion and the continuous measure of SCL (and only a modest relation, r = −.29 with the dichotomous measure); nevertheless, collecting multiple and parallel measures of child behavior, psychophysiology, and parenting repeatedly over time would be highly desirable in future longitudinal research. Such designs could address key questions of the interplay between parental socialization and child biology, unfolding over time.

Another issue was our specific psychophysiological paradigm and data aggregation. Although we believe that our tonic SCL measures, based on past work, were appropriate and robust, alternative approaches are certainly possible and may yield promising results. As discussed earlier, an alternative approach, adopted by other laboratories investigating links among electrodermal activity, parenting, and developmental outcomes (Erath et al., 2009, 2011), involves analyses of change in SCL response from the baseline to an emotionally challenging task. In particular, assessments that would deploy an emotional challenge due to experience of a transgression and the following discomfort (thus parallel to our mishap paradigms) may be especially revealing in future research. Those assessments, however, may pose difficulty associated with artifacts due to children’s movement.

The findings have translational implications. Although parents and clinicians do not have routine access to children’s measures of autonomic functioning (and in any event, no norms for “low” or “high” SCL exist; D. C. Fowles, personal communication, April 2015), they can be made aware of observed characteristics of temperament that are typically associated with autonomic hyporeactivity, such as fearlessness, and tendency toward uninhibited, impulsive, and risk-taking behavior (Scarpa et al., 1997). The gist of our findings is that emotions and behaviors indicating low regard for conduct rules shown by young children with such temperaments may be more of a concern than similar behaviors in their peers without such temperament characteristics. That concern may be particularly heightened if disregard for rules triggers parental forceful response. Over time, forceful discipline may contribute to an adversarial, mutually coercive parent–child cascade, leading ultimately to antisocial behavior problems. Parents of children with fearless, hyporeactive temperaments would be well advised to avoid being drawn into coercive exchanges and to rely instead on alternative socialization strategies, such as warmth, use of positive affect and rewards, responsiveness, and other techniques that create an affectively positive relationship. Growing literature has shown that such alternative strategies may be especially effective for behaviorally fearless and physiologically hyporeactive children, and may result in lowering their risk for antisocial problems (Fowles & Kochanska, 2000; Kochanska, 1997; Kochanska, Aksan, & Joy, 2007; Shaw, 2003).

Future research will likely reveal further complexities. A continuing integration of research on children’s biological individuality, their behavior, the parenting they receive, and the unfolding adaptive and maladaptive developmental trajectories remains a promising enterprise.

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by NIMH (R01 MH63096, K02 MH01446) and NICHD (R01 HD069171) and by the Stuit Professorship (to G.K.). We thank Katherine Jonas for methodological contributions; Nazan Aksan, Steven W. Anderson, and Kuan-Hua Chen for collecting and processing psycho-physiological data; Don Fowles for very helpful guidance on those measures; Lauren Wakschlag for input regarding disruptive behavior; many colleagues, students, and staff for contributions to the Family Study, especially Jarilyn Akabogu, Sanghag Kim, Jeung Eun Yoon, and Jessica O’Bleness; and all of the parents and children in the Family Study for their long-term commitment to this research.

Footnotes

The authors had no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Bates JE, Schermerhorn AC, Petersen IT. Temperament and parenting in developmental perspective. In: Zentner M, Shiner R, editors. Handbook of temperament. New York: Guilford Press; 2012. pp. 425–441. [Google Scholar]
  2. Beauchaine TP, Gatzke-Kopp L, Mead HK. Polyvagal theory and developmental psychopathology: Emotion dysregulation and conduct problems from preschool to adolescence. Biological Psychology. 2007;74:174–184. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bell RQ. A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review. 1968;75:81–95. doi: 10.1037/h0025583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135:885–908. doi: 10.1037/a0017376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bender HL, Allen JP, Boykin Mcelhaney KB, Antonishak J, Moore CM, O’Beirne Kelly H, et al. Use of harsh physical discipline and developmental outcomes in adolescence. Development and Psycho-pathology. 2007;19:227–242. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benedek M, Kaernbach C. A continuous measure of phasic electrodermal activity. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2010;190:80–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.04.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;107:238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blair RJR, Peschardt KS, Budhani S, Mitchell DGV, Pine DS. The development of psychopathy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:262–275. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01596.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Briggs-Gowan MJ, Nichols SR, Voss J, Zobel E, Carter AS, McCarthy KJ, et al. Punishment insensitivity and impaired reinforcement learning in preschoolers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2014;55:154–161. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1993. pp. 136–162. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bufferd SJ, Dyson MW, Hernandez IG, Wakschlag LS. Explicating the “developmental” in preschool psychopathology. In: Cicchetti D, editor. Developmental psychopathology. 3rd. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2016. pp. 152–186. [Google Scholar]
  12. Buodo G, Moscardino U, Scrimin S, Altoe G, Palomba D. Parenting stress and externalizing behavior symptoms in children: The impact of emotional reactivity. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 2013;44:786–797. doi: 10.1007/s10578-013-0371-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Burnette ML, Cicchetti D. Multilevel approaches toward understanding antisocial behavior: Current research and future directions. Development and Psychopathology. 2012;24:703–704. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Calkins SD, Propper C, Mills-Koonce WR. Biopsychosocial perspective on parenting and developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25:1399–1414. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Callender KA, Olson SL, Kerr DCR, Sameroff AJ. Assessment of cheating behavior in young school-age children: Distinguishing normative behaviors from risk markers of externalizing psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2010;39:776–788. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.517165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Carter S, Pasqualini MS. Stronger autonomic response accompanies better learning: A test of Damasio’s somatic marker hypothesis. Cognition and Emotion. 2004;18:901–911. [Google Scholar]
  17. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:597–600. [Google Scholar]
  18. Crider A, Kremen WS, Xian H, Jacobson KC, Waterman B, Eisen SA, et al. Stability, consistency, and heritability of electrodermal response lability in middle-aged male twins. Psychophysiology. 2004;41:501–509. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00189.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Crowell SE, Beauchaine TP, Gatzke-Kopp L, Sylvers P, Mead H, Chipman-Chacon J. Autonomic correlates of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and oppositional defiant disorder in preschool children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2006;115:174–178. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dadds MR, Salmon K. Punishment insensitivity and parenting: Temperament and learning as interacting risks for antisocial behavior. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2003;6:69–86. doi: 10.1023/a:1023762009877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Damasio AR. Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Avon Books; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  22. Damasio AR, Tranel D, Damasio H. Somatic markers and the guidance of behavior: Theory and preliminary testing. In: Levin HS, Eisenberg HM, Benton AL, editors. Frontal lobe junction and dysfunction. New York: Oxford University Press; 1991. pp. 217–229. [Google Scholar]
  23. Dawson ME, Schell AM, Filion DL. The electrodermal system. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson G, editors. Handbook of psychophysiology. 3rd. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2007. pp. 159–181. [Google Scholar]
  24. Deater-Deckard K, Dodge KA. Externalizing behavior problems and discipline revisited: Nonlinear effects and variation by culture, context, and gender. Psychological Inquiry. 1997;8:161–175. [Google Scholar]
  25. Deater-Deckard K, Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS. Physical discipline among African American and European American mothers: Links to children’s externalizing behaviors. Developmental Psychology. 1996;32:1065–1072. [Google Scholar]
  26. Dishion TJ, Patterson GR. The development and ecology of antisocial behavior in children and adolescents. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathology. 2nd. Vol. 3. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2006. pp. 503–541. [Google Scholar]
  27. Dodge KA, Coie JD, Lynam D. Aggression and antisocial behavior in youth. In: Damon W, editor; Eisenberg N, editor. Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development. 6th. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2006. pp. 719–788. (Series Ed.) (Vol. Ed.) [Google Scholar]
  28. El-Sheikh M. Children’s skin conductance level and reactivity: Are these measures stable over time and across tasks? Developmental Psychobiology. 2007;49:180–186. doi: 10.1002/dev.20171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Enders CK, Bandalos DL. The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling. 2001;8:430–457. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Erath SA, El-Sheikh M, Cummings EM. Harsh parenting and child externalizing behavior: Skin conductance level reactivity as a moderator. Child Development. 2009;80:578–592. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01280.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Erath SA, El-Sheikh M, Hinnant JB, Cummings EM. Skin conductance level reactivity moderates the association between harsh parenting and growth in child externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychology. 2011;47:693–706. doi: 10.1037/a0021909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fowles DC. The three arousal model: Implications of Gray’s two-factor learning theory for heart rate, electrodermal activity, and psychopathy. Psychophysiology. 1980;17:87–104. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1980.tb00117.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fowles DC. Electrodermal activity and antisocial behavior: Empirical findings and theoretical issues. In: Roy JC, Boucsein W, Fowles DC, Gruzelier JH, editors. Progress in electrodermal research. New York: Springer; 1993. pp. 223–237. [Google Scholar]
  34. Fowles DC. The measurement of electrodermal activity in children. In: Schmidt LA, Segalowitz SJ, editors. Developmental psychophysiology: Theory, systems, and methods. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2008. pp. 286–316. [Google Scholar]
  35. Fowles DC, Kochanska G. Temperament as a moderator of pathways to conscience in children: The contribution of electrodermal activity. Psychophysiology. 2000;37:788–795. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Fowles D, Missel K. Electrodermal hyporeactivity, motivation, and psychopathy: Theoretical issues. In: Fowles D, Sutker P, Goodman S, editors. Progress in experimental personality and psychopathology research 1994: Special focus on psychopathy and antisocial behavior: A developmental perspective. New York: Springer; 1994. pp. 263–283. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Frick PJ. The inventory of callous–unemotional traits. Unpublished manuscript, University of New Orleans, Department of Psychology. 2003 [Google Scholar]
  38. Frick PJ, Bodin S, Barry CT. Psychopathic traits and conduct problems in community and clinic-referred samples of children: Further development of the psychopathy screening device. Psychological Assessment. 2000;12:382–393. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Frick PJ, Morris AS. Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2004;33:55–68. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Frick PJ, Viding E. Antisocial behavior from a developmental psychopathology perspective. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:1111–1131. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409990071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Frick PJ, White S. Research Review: The importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 2008;49:359–375. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01862.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gadow KD, Sprafkin J. Child Symptom Inventory: Screening and norms manual. 4th. Stony Brook, NY: Checkmate Plus; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  43. Gadow KD, Sprafkin J. Child and Adolescent Symptom Inventory—4R. Stony Brook, NY: Checkmate Plus; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  44. Gao Y, Raine A, Venables PH, Dawson ME, Mednick SA. Reduced electrodermal fear conditioning from ages 3 to 8 years is associated with aggressive behavior at age 8 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51:550–558. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02176.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Gershoff ET. Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviors and experiences: A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128:539–579. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Gralinski JH, Kopp CB. Everyday rules for behavior: Mothers’ requests to young children. Developmental Psychology. 1993;29:573–584. [Google Scholar]
  47. Grusec JE, Kuczynski L. Direction of effect in socialization: A comparison of the parent’s versus the child’s behavior as determinants of disciplinary techniques. Developmental Psychology. 1980;16:1–9. [Google Scholar]
  48. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  49. Hoffman ML. Moral internalization, parental power, and the nature of parent-child interaction. Developmental Psychology. 1975;11:228–239. [Google Scholar]
  50. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6:1–55. [Google Scholar]
  51. Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Gardner F, Cheong J, Dishion TJ, Wilson M. Dimensions of callousness in early childhood: Links to problem behavior and family intervention effectiveness. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25:347–363. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412001101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Joosena KJ, Mesman J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. Maternal sensitivity to infants in various settings predicts harsh discipline in toddlerhood. Attachment and Human Development. 2012;14:101–117. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2012.661217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kim S, Kochanska G, Boldt LJ, Koenig Nordling J, O’Bleness JJ. Developmental trajectory from early responses to transgressions to future antisocial behavior: Evidence for the role of the parent-child relationship from two longitudinal studies. Development and Psychopathology. 2014;26:93–109. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kimonis ER, Frick PJ. Etiology of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: Biological, familial and environmental factors identified in the development of disruptive behavior disorders. In: Murrihy RC, Kidman AD, Ollendick TH, editors. Handbook of clinical assessment and treatment of conduct problems in youth. New York: Springer; 2010. pp. 49–76. [Google Scholar]
  55. Kochanska G. Multiple pathways to conscience for children with different temperaments: From toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:228–240. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.2.228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kochanska G, Aksan N, Joy ME. Children’s fearfulness as a moderator of parenting in early socialization: Two longitudinal studies. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43:222–237. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kochanska G, Brock RL, Chen KH, Aksan N, Anderson SW. Paths from early mother-child and father-child relationships to externalizing behavior problems in children differing in electrodermal reactivity: A longitudinal study from infancy to age 10. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2015;43:721–734. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9938-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kochanska G, Coy KC, Murray KT. The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Development. 2001;72:1091–1111. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kochanska G, Kim S. A complex interplay among the parent-child relationship, effortful control, and internalized, rule-compatible conduct in young children: Evidence from two studies. Developmental Psychology. 2014;50:8–21. doi: 10.1037/a0032330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Kochanska G, Koenig JL, Barry RA, Kim S, Yoon JE. Children’s conscience during toddler and preschool years, moral self, and a competent, adaptive developmental trajectory. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46:1320–1332. doi: 10.1037/a0020381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lansford JE, Deater-Deckard K, Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS. Ethnic differences in the link between physical discipline and later adolescent externalizing behaviors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45:801–812. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00273.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lorber MF. Psychophysiology of aggression, psychopathy, and conduct problems: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2004;130:531–552. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Lykken DT. The antisocial personalities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  64. Mackinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Williams J. Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2004;39:99–128. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Masten AS, Cicchetti D. Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:491–495. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Matthys W, van Goozen SHM, Snoek H, van Engeland H. Response preservation and sensitivity to reward and punishment in boys with oppositional defiant disorder. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2004;13:362–364. doi: 10.1007/s00787-004-0395-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. McCord J. On discipline. Psychological Inquiry. 1997;8:215–217. [Google Scholar]
  68. Muthén L, Muthén B. Mplus user’s guide. 6th. Los Angeles: Author; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  69. Nigg JT. Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:395–422. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01612.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Pardini DA. Novel insights into longstanding theories of bidirectional parent-child influences: Introduction to the special section. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36:627–631. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9231-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Patterson GR, Dishion TJ, Bank L. Family interaction: A process model of deviancy training. Aggressive Behavior. 1984;10:253–267. [Google Scholar]
  72. Petitclerc A, Boivin M, Dionne G, Perusse D, Tremblay RE. Genetic and environmental etiology of disregard for rules. Behavior Genetics. 2011;41:192–200. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9393-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Petitclerc A, Boivin M, Dionne G, Zoccolillo M, Tremblay RE. Disregard for rules: The early development and predictors of a specific dimension of disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50:1477–1484. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02118.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Posthumus JA, Bocker KBE, Raaijmakers MAJ, Van Engeland H, Matthys W. Heart rate and skin conductance in four-year-old children with aggressive behavior. Biological Psychology. 2009;82:164–168. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Preacher KJ, Rucker DD, Hayes AF. Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2007;42:37–41. doi: 10.1080/00273170701341316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Raine A. Biosocial studies of antisocial and violent behavior in children and adults: A review. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2002;30:311–326. doi: 10.1023/a:1015754122318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Raine A, Venables PH, Dalais C, Mellingen K, Reynolds C, Mednick SA. Early educational and health enrichment at age 3–5 years is associated with increased autonomic and central nervous system arousal and orienting at age 11 years: Evidence from the Mauritius Child Health Project. Psychophysiology. 2001;38:254–266. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Raine A, Venables PH, Williams M. Relationships between central and autonomic measures of arousal at age 15 years and criminality at age 24 years. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1990;47:1003–1007. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810230019003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Rushton JP, Brainerd CJ, Pressley M. Behavioral development and construct validity: The principle of aggregation. Psychological Bulletin. 1983;94:18–38. [Google Scholar]
  80. Scarpa A, Raine A, Venables PH, Mednick SA. Heart rate and skin conductance in behaviorally inhibited Mauritian children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997;106:182–190. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.2.182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Shaw DS. Innovative approaches and methods to the study of children’s conduct problems. Social Development. 2003;12:309–313. [Google Scholar]
  82. Shaw DS. Future directions for research on the development and prevention of early conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2013;42:418–428. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.777918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Shaw D, Gilliom M, Ingoldsby E, Nagin D. Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39:189–200. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.39.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Shrout PE, Bolger N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:422–445. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Smetana JG. Toddlers’ social interactions in the context of moral and conventional transgressions in the home. Developmental Psychology. 1989;25:499–508. [Google Scholar]
  86. Sobhani M, Bechara A. A somatic marker perspective of immoral and corrupt behavior. Social Neuroscience. 2011;6:640–652. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2011.605592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Thompson RA. Whither the preconventional child: Toward a life-span moral development theory. Child Development Perspectives. 2012;6:423–429. [Google Scholar]
  88. Thompson RA. Conscience development in early childhood. In: Killen M, Smetana J, editors. Handbook of moral development. 2nd. New York: Taylor & Francis; 2014. pp. 73–92. [Google Scholar]
  89. Tranel D. “Acquired sociopathy”: The development of sociopathic behavior following focal brain damage. In: Fowles DC, Sutker P, Goodman SH, editors. Experimental personality and psychopathology research. New York: Springer; 1994. pp. 286–311. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Wakschlag LS, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS, Hill C, Danis B, Keenan K, et al. A developmental framework for distinguishing disruptive behavior from normative misbehavior in preschool children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:976–987. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01786.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Wakschlag LS, Tolan PH, Leventhal BL. Research review: “Ain’t misbehaving”: Towards a developmentally-specified nosology for preschool disruptive behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51:3–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02184.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES