Skip to main content
Data in Brief logoLink to Data in Brief
. 2017 Jan 11;11:54–60. doi: 10.1016/j.dib.2017.01.004

Functional magnetic resonance imaging data of incremental increases in visuo-spatial difficulty in an adult lifespan sample

Kristen M Kennedy 1,, Jenny R Rieck 1, Maria A Boylan 1, Karen M Rodrigue 1
PMCID: PMC5256670  PMID: 28138504

Abstract

These data provide coordinates generated from a large healthy adult lifespan sample undergoing functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) while completing a spatial judgment task with varying levels of difficulty, as well as a control categorical condition. The data presented here include the average blood-oxygen-dependent (BOLD) response to the spatial judgment vs. the control task, as well as the BOLD response to incremental increasing difficulty; see also “Age-related Reduction of BOLD Modulation to Cognitive Difficulty Predicts Poorer Task Accuracy and Poorer Fluid Reasoning Ability” (Rieck et al., 2017) [1].

Keywords: fMRI, Spatial judgment, Difficulty, Lifespan, Aging, BOLD


Specifications Table

Subject area Cognitive Neuroscience
More specific subject area Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of spatial judgment
Type of data Coordinate tables, figures
How data was acquired Philips Achieva 3 T whole body scanner
Data format Analyzed using Statistical Parametric Mapping 8
Experimental factors
Experimental features Participants performed a spatial judgment task in which they conducted two types of judgments. A categorical (LEFT/RIGHT) judgment was used as a control condition and a coordinate (NEAR/FAR) judgment was used with three levels of difficulty.
Data source location Dallas, Texas, United States of America
Data accessibility Data provided in article

Value of the data

  • This dataset provides a sizable sample of healthy adults who performed a spatial judgment task.

  • These data show differential BOLD responses for varying levels of visuo-spatial difficulty across the sample.

  • The data provide specific MNI coordinates of brain regions evoked by the task.

  • These data are potentially useful to investigators studying differences in fMRI activation to non-verbal, spatial stimuli across the adult lifespan.

1. Data

While undergoing fMRI, healthy adult participants completed a blocked-design spatial judgment task with three levels of difficulty (Easy, Medium, and Hard). These data have previously been analyzed with regard to age [1]. The data shown here represent the group level analyses examining the effect of the distance judgment task (Easy, Medium, Hard vs. Control – Table 1 and Fig. 1) as well as the effect of incremental increasing difficulty (Medium vs. Easy – Table 2 and Hard vs. Medium – Table 3, both shown in Fig. 2).

Table 1.

Cluster peaks for the whole sample effect of distance judgment task [Easy, Medium, Hard vs. Control].

A. Positive effect cluster-level
Cluster Label BA k X Y Z t-value pfwe
L/R superior occipital gyrus 18 7013 −6 −102 6 17.3 <.001
and R precuneus 7 18 −66 45 14.46
7 27 −72 36 13.94
L/R middle frontal gyrus 8 745 6 21 42 12.82 <.001
6 −24 0 51 9.62
R middle and inferior frontal 46 1409 45 36 18 12.54 <.001
gyrus 13 33 21 −3 11.55
44 48 6 24 10.99
L inferior frontal gyrus and 47 1305 −30 21 −3 11.6 <.001
insula 6 −45 6 27 10.64
9 −45 27 27 8.38
R middle frontal gyrus 6 323 30 6 54 10.05 <.001
R thalamus/caudate 50 88 18 −27 15 6.5 0.004
48 18 −15 18 6.1
48 18 −3 18 5.76
L thalamus/caudate 48 90 −18 −27 15 6.27 0.004
48 −18 0 18 6.1
50 −15 −6 6 4.59



B. Negative effect cluster-level
Cluster Label BA k X Y Z t-value pfwe



 

 

 

 

 

 

 


R lingual gyrus 18 332 21 −90 −3 17.86 <.001
L inferior occipital gyrus 18 238 −21 −96 −9 16.21 <.001
R superior and middle 22 2342 60 −45 12 14.8 <.001
temporal gyrus 39 60 −60 21 12.59
21 57 −9 −15 10.48
L/R medial orbital and 10 3230 0 57 −3 12.61 <.001
middle frontal gyrus; 8 −21 33 42 11.57
anterior cingulate 10 9 54 12 11.16
L/R posterior and middle 23 1959 −3 −45 33 12.46 <.001
cingulate gyrus 24 3 −21 39 10.6
23 −6 −27 39 10.5
L middle occipital and 39 1429 −45 −75 39 11.8 <.001
posterior parietal gyrus 39 −57 −63 27 10.44
19 −57 −69 9 10.4
L middle temporal gyrus 21 278 −54 −9 −15 8.55 <.001
L orbital frontal gyrus 47 95 −33 33 −15 7.75 0.003
R orbital frontal gyrus 47 93 36 36 −12 7.06 0.003
45 54 33 0 5.93
R hippocampus 54 112 27 −12 −18 6.61 0.001
L cerebellum crus 2 39 −24 −81 −36 5.74 0.031
L hippocampus and fusiform 54 65 −27 −15 −18 5.61 0.008
gyrus 37 −30 −33 −15 5.44
L inferior temporal gyrus 20 41 −45 3 −36 5.1 0.022
38 −33 3 −39 4.51
R cerebellum crus 2 46 21 −87 −39 5.06 0.031
30 −81 −36 4.48

Note. p<.0001 uncorrected, cluster-level FWE p<.05 correction. BA=Brodmann׳s area.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Effect of Easy, Medium, and Hard Tasks vs. Control. Hot blobs indicate regions in which there was greater activity during all levels (Easy, Medium, Hard) of the coordinate distance judgment task versus the coordinate control task. Cool blobs indicate regions in which there was greater activity during the left-right coordinate control condition. Color scale indicates t-values; Abbreviations: LH – Left Hemisphere; RH – Right Hemisphere.

Table 2.

Cluster peaks for the whole sample effect of increasing difficulty from Easy to Medium.

A. Increased activation from easy to medium cluster-level
Cluster Label BA k X Y Z t-value pfwe
L superior and middle 18 344 −9 −102 9 9.58 <.001
occipital gyrus 18 −24 −93 15 7.68
18 −36 −78 3 4.66
R cuneus and middle occipital 18 536 12 −96 15 9.51 <.001
gyrus 18 27 −87 18 8.18
18 3 −81 −3 6.21
L/R anterior cingulate gyrus 8 71 6 21 42 5.48 0.008
32 −6 21 39 4.26



B. Deceased activation from easy to medium cluster-level
Cluster Label BA k X Y Z t-value pfwe



 

 

 

 

 

 

 


R inferior occipital gyrus 18 200 24 −93 −3 9.99 <.001
L inferior occipital gyrus and 18 379 −18 −93 −9 8.68 <.001
cerebellum crus 1 & 2 −21 −78 −39 6.22
−33 −84 −30 6.15
R middle temporal and gyrus 39 747 54 −60 21 7.63 <.001
angular gyrus 37 66 −48 −3 5.59
39 48 −66 39 4.93
L/R posterior cingulate gyrus 23 814 6 −45 30 6.32 <.001
and precuneus 7 0 −57 45 6.31
7 −6 −57 66 6.25
L middle and superior 8 215 −30 27 48 6.12 <.001
frontal gyrus 8 −15 39 45 4.49
9 −36 36 36 4.11
R cerebellum crus 1 358 54 −66 −33 6.02 <.001
45 −72 −33 5.14
27 −81 −30 5.08
L posterior parietal and 39 573 −39 −72 42 5.82 <.001
middle temporal gyrus 39 −54 −45 30 5.57
39 −51 −63 18 5.13
R middle frontal gyrus 8 74 27 30 45 5.35 0.007
R middle temporal gyrus 21 94 60 −9 −18 5.33 0.003
L inferior temporal gyrus 37 99 −57 −51 −6 5.2 0.003
L/R superior medial frontal 10 37 12 63 15 4.99 0.039

Note. p<.0001 uncorrected, cluster-level FWE p<.05. BA=Brodmann׳s area.

Table 3.

Cluster peaks for the whole sample effect of increasing difficulty from Medium to Hard.

A. Increased activation from medium to hard cluster-level
Cluster Label BA k X Y Z t-value pfwe
R inferior and superior 7 1315 42 −51 54 8.6 <.001
parietal lobule 7 33 −60 54 8.43
40 42 −42 42 8.3
R inferior frontal and 9 2220 48 33 21 8.38 <.001
insula 44 48 9 24 8.32
13 30 24 −3 7.88
L cerebellum crus 1 & 2 476 −9 −81 −33 8.29 <.001
−33 −72 −48 7.77
−30 −66 −30 7.2
R superior medial frontal gyrus 8 432 9 27 45 7.21 <.001
8 3 33 39 6.98
R lingual gyrus 18 37 18 −87 −6 6.75 0.04
L insular cortex 13 99 −33 21 −3 6.42 0.003
L inferior frontal gyrus 44 53 −57 21 30 5.45 0.019
L middle occipital gyrus 18 35 −36 −93 9 5.09 0.044
L orbitofrontal gyrus 47 41 −45 45 −6 5.06 0.033
L inferior and superior 40 141 −39 −48 48 4.81 0.001
parietal lobule 39 −33 −57 48 4.79
7 −21 −66 51 4.6



B. Deceased activation from medium to hard cluster-level
Cluster Label BA k X Y Z t-value pfwe



 

 

 

 

 

 

 


L/R posterior and anterior 18 10811 0 −72 24 10.96 <.001
medial wall and precuneus 10 −3 57 −3 9.75
23 9 −57 27 9.29
L middle temporal and 39 1846 −42 −57 21 8 <.001
angular gyrus 39 −42 −72 36 7.19
21 −54 −6 −18 6.93
L superior frontal gyrus 8 222 −18 33 42 7.06 <.001
L orbital frontal gyrus 47 69 −27 36 −15 6.31 .014

Note. p<.0001 uncorrected, cluster-level FWE p<.05. BA=Brodmann׳s area.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Effect of incremental increasing difficulty across the entire sample. Panel A shows the contrast of activation for Medium>Easy trials. Panel B shows the contrast of activation to Hard>Medium trials. Color scale indicates t-values. Abbreviations: LH – Left Hemisphere; RH – Right Hemisphere.

2. Experimental design, materials and methods

2.1. Participants

Participants included 161 healthy adults, ages 20–94 (mean age=51.93±18.9 years; 95 women; 66 men) who volunteered from the Dallas-Fort Worth area. Inclusion criteria for the study required that all participants be right-handed, fluent English speakers, and have normal or corrected-to-normal vision (at least 20/40). Participants were also screened for dementia using the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE; [2]), with a cutoff of 26; volunteers were also required to have no history of neurological or psychiatric conditions, head trauma, drug or alcohol problems, or significant cardiovascular disease (however, n=32 with a self-reported diagnosis of hypertension). Participants were compensated for their time and informed consent was obtained in accordance with protocol approved by the University of Texas at Dallas and the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center.

2.2. Experimental design

The data shared here are from a large lifespan dataset in which 161 healthy adults completed a blocked-design distance judgment task while undergoing fMRI. The spatial judgment task involved two types of judgments (modeled after [3] and [4]). The first type of judgment, which served as the control condition, required participants to make a categorical (LEFT/RIGHT) judgment. Participants saw a dot on the left or right side of a horizontal bar and had to indicate using a button press on which side of the bar the dot was present.

Participants also made a coordinate (NEAR/FAR) distance judgment which had three levels of difficulty: Easy, Medium, and Hard. First, participants saw a vertical reference line, next they were shown a horizontal line with a dot either above or below the line; the judgment required participants to determine whether the dot was “nearer to” or “farther from” the horizontal bar, given the previously seen vertical line. As difficulty increased, the distance between the dot and the horizontal line became harder to determine the “nearness” or “farness” compared to the reference line. A schematic of the task can be found in Fig. 1 of Rieck and colleagues [1]. Prior to the scanning session, participants completed a practice session to ensure that the participants were comfortable with the instructions. Each participant completed three runs of the task, resulting in ~15 min of scan time. The task was presented using PsychoPy v1.77.02 [5,6].

2.3. Image acquisition

Data were acquired on a single Philips Achieva 3 T whole body scanner using a 32-channel head coil. BOLD fMRI data were collected using a T2*-weighted echo planar imaging sequence in 29 interleaved axial slices parallel to AC-PC line, 64×64×29 matrix, 3.4×3.4×5 mm3, Field of View (FOV)=220 mm, Echo Time (TE)=30 ms, Repetition Time (TR)=1500 ms. High-resolution anatomical images were also acquired with a T1-weighted MP-RAGE sequence with the following parameters: 160 sagittal slices, 1×1×1 mm3 voxels; 256×204×160 matrix, FOV=256 mm, TE=3.8 ms, TR=8.3 ms, Flip angle=12°.

2.4. Image processing

Data from each individual were preprocessed using SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK). Preprocessing included the following steps: slice time acquisition correction, motion correction, normalization, and smoothing (using an isotropic 8 mm3 full-width-half-maximum Gaussian kernel). In order to identify runs with motion outliers, ArtRepair [7] was used to determine potential outlier volumes for each participant. We examined all three runs for each participant, and runs that had more than 15% outlier volumes (~30 volumes) with greater than 3% deviation from the mean in global intensity spikes or greater than 2 mm of motion displacement were flagged. Five participants had one run with more than 15% percent outlier volumes, so that run was excluded.

At the individual subject level, BOLD response to each condition (Control, Easy, Medium, Hard) was modeled in SPM as a block convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function; six directions of motion-estimates for each volume generated from ArtRepair were also included as nuisance covariates. Several contrasts of interest were computed at the individual level for subsequent analysis at the group level: Easy+Medium+Hard vs. Control (Table 1, Fig. 1), which represents the effect of the distance judgment task; and Medium vs. Easy (Table 2, Fig. 2), Hard vs. Medium (Table 3, Fig. 2) to examine the brain regions responsive to increment increases in difficulty for visuo-spatial judgments.

Conflict of Interest

The authors (KMK, JRR, MAB, KMR) of this manuscript (Functional magnetic resonance imaging data of incremental increases in visuo-spatial difficulty in an adult lifespan sample) have no conflicts of interest to report.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported in part by the National Institutes of Health (AG-036818 and AG-036848). We thank Andy Hebrank for assistance with the functional task programming and Asha Unni for help with behavioral piloting and data collection.

References

  • 1.Rieck J.R., Rodrigue K.M., Boylan M.A., Kennedy K.M. Age-related reduction of BOLD modulation to cognitive difficulty predicts poorer task accuracy and poorer fluid reasoning ability. NeuroImage. 2017;147:262–271. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Folstein M.F., Folstein S.E., McHugh P.R. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Baciu M., Koenig O., Vernier M.P., Bedoin N., Rubin C., Segebarth C. Categorical and coordinate spatial relations: fMRI evidence for hemispheric specialization. Neuroreport. 1999;10:1373–1378. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199904260-00040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Park D.C., Polk T.A., Hebrank A.C., Jenkins L.J. Age differences in default mode activity on easy and difficult spatial judgment tasks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2010;3:75. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.075.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Peirce J.W. PsychoPy--Psychophysics software in Python. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2007;162:8–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Peirce J.W. Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Front. Neuroinform. 2009;2:10. doi: 10.3389/neuro.11.010.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.P. Mazaika, S. Whitfield-Gabrieli, A. Reiss, Artifact repair for fMRI data from high motion clinical subjects. Poster session presented at Human Brain Mapping, Chicago, IL, 2007.

Articles from Data in Brief are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES