Skip to main content
. 2017 Mar 24;18:48. doi: 10.1186/s12875-017-0621-5

Table 4.

Predictors of frequent attenders (0 = Non-frequent attenders; 1 = Frequent attenders; cut-off at the highest 5%). Results of multiple logistic regressionsa

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)
Independent variables Frequent attenders Frequent attenders Frequent attenders Frequent attenders Frequent attenders Frequent attenders Frequent attenders Frequent attenders
Potential confounders
Life satisfaction 0.79***
(0.68 – 0.91)
Positive affect 0.83+
(0.68 – 1.02)
Negative affect 1.34**
(1.10 – 1.63)
Optimism 0.74**
(0.62 – 0.89)
Self-efficacy 0.71**
(0.56 – 0.89)
Self-esteem 0.81+
(0.63 – 1.03)
Self-regulation 1.05
(0.86 – 1.28)
Perceived stress 1.37***
(1.18 – 1.61)
Constant 0.14* 0.21 0.05** 0.26 0.33 0.23 0.13* 0.04**
(0.02 – 0.94) (0.03 – 1.58) (0.01 – 0.37) (0.04 – 1.79) (0.05 – 2.37) (0.03 – 1.72) (0.02 – 1.00) (0.00 – 0.29)
Observations 6,730 6,725 6,724 6,764 6,760 6,778 6,662 6,692
Pseudo R2 0.124 0.121 0.123 0.123 0.123 0.120 0.118 0.125

aAll estimations include age, (log) equivalence income, number of chronic diseases, Body-Mass-Index, as well as dummy-variables for sex, marital status, employment status, region, sports, alcohol consumption and smoking status as potential confounders. Odds ratios were reported; 95% confidence intervals in parentheses; ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, + p < 0.10. Life satisfaction (SWLS, Pavot & Diener, 1993); Positive and negative affect (PANAS, Watson et al., 1988); Optimism (Brandtstädter & Wentura, 1994); Self-efficacy (Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1999); Self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965); Self-regulation (Freund & Baltes, 2002); Perceived stress (Cohen et al., 1983). The Wald test was used to test the significance of each parameter