Dahl (1992) |
25 |
10.3 ± 1.6 years |
CRH challenge: 1 μg/kg i.v. in the late afternoon |
9×, 3 before, 6 after |
Blood |
Greater peak in boys |
Dorn (1996) |
20 control subjects |
15.1 ± 1.0 years |
CRH challenge: 1 μg/kg i.v. in the evening |
12×, 6 before, 6 after |
Blood |
No sex differences; groups matched for pubertal status, effect not analyzed |
Forest (1978) |
20 infants, 35 prepubertal children |
Infants: 5–365 days; children: 1–12.6 years |
ACTH test: 500 μg/m2 i.m. at 8:00 and 20:00 on 3 days |
2×, 1 before, 1 after |
Blood |
No sex differences; pubertal status not assessed |
Lashansky (1991) |
102 |
2 months–17 years |
ACTH test: 0.25 mg i.v. in the morning |
2×, 1 before, 1 after |
Blood |
No sex differences; decrease in stimulated cortisol levels with puberty, more pronounced in boys |
Ross (1986) |
21 |
6–15 years |
CRH challenge: 1 μg/kg i.v. in the evening |
7×, 2 before, 5 after |
Blood |
No sex differences; pubertal status not associated with reactivity |
Stroud (2011) |
68 |
11.6 ± 1.9 years |
CRH challenge: 1 μg/kg i.v. in the late afternoon |
9–10×, 3 before, 6–7 after |
Blood |
Sex by Tanner differences: girls increase and boys decrease in cortisol with pubertal maturation, girls decrease and boys are stable in reactivity. Boys have larger peak change |
Tsvetkova (1977) |
31 |
4–14 years |
ACTH test: 0.5 mg i.m. in the morning |
2×, 1 before, 1 after |
Blood |
No sex differences; pubertal status not assessed |