Skip to main content
. 2017 Aug 3;14(8):873. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14080873

Table 4.

Path Interventions (Type B).

Authors Intervention & Study N, Response Rate & Method Exposure Levels Change in Levels and Distribution of Change across Participants Outcome Measure(s) before and after Outcomes Did Outcome Change with Change in Exposure?
Yes/No
(Significance Tested?)
Before/after Outcome Change Compared to That Estimated from an ERF Comments Confounders Adjusted for in Analyses
Nature Design Before After
Amundsen, Klaeboe & Aasvang (2011) [27] Norway
Façade insulation
Two survey rounds: B&A
Target; Control & Supplement groups.
B & A surveys approx 6 mos. either side of intervention
Target:
B: 168
Response rates 57%
A: 161 (65%)
Control: B: 469 (57%)
A: 254 (65%)
Supplement: 112
Mail survey
LAeq,24 h
61–78 outside. Calculated
Mean 71 (Av inside level before change is 43)
Façade insulation reduces inside levels by 7 dB –7 dB for indoor noise levels for all Ps in target group Standard ISO annoyance scale (5 point verbal). %HA calculated using top two points of scale
B: 42%HA
A: 16%HA
Control:
B: 24%HA
A: 29%HA
Yes
Intervention resulted in substantial and significant (p < 0.001) reductions in individual annoyance scores
Authors chose not to compare their results with Miedema & Oudshoorn ERF [13].
Fitted a model of individual annoyance responses to outdoor levels for all Ps (target, control and supplementary: n = 738) with receiving the intervention as a dummy variable. Estimate of effect size −0.820 (p < 0.000) and 95% CI −1.170 to −0.470.
Authors claim size of annoyance reductions with intervention is in line with ERF modelled from individual indoor levels. However this appears to be contradicted by the large reduction in the Target Group’s %HA (42% before intervention to 16% after).
Summarised as ‘unclear’
Authors note no explanation why %HA sig. lower in control than target before intervention; and second round higher than first in control Gender, age, education level, marital status, access to a bedroom on the quiet side of the building, and sensitivity
Amundsen, Klaeboe & Aasvang (2013) [28] See Amundsen, Klaeboe & Aasvang (2011) [27] above.
Same study details but this was a repeat survey 2 year after first post-intervention study. Mailed to all Ps who had completed first post-intervention study. Number of participants now 104 (Response rate 58%) in target; 139 in Control; 63 in supplement
2nd after-study:
A: 16%HA
Result the same two years after initial After survey (p < 0.01)
Additionally, repeated ANOVA was conducted on panel who had answered all three survey rounds (N = 212). Change in annoyance as a result of intervention significant in first (p < 0.0005) but not second (p = 0.33) after survey
In this repeated ANOVA, multivariate partial eta square = 0.44
Bendtsen, Michelsen & Christensen (2011) [29] Denmark
Enlargement of motorway lanes but with dwelling insulation, barriers, & quiet pavement
B/A study 1 year before constr & 1 year after B/A gap 6 year Q sent to 1200 dwel. In 6 areas out to800 m from motorway
Response rates B:71% A: 65%
38% B&A
Mail survey
Lden 45–65
Calculated.
Unclear as to whether calculated levels included traffic sources other than motorway
Lden 45–60
Calculated.
Not reported is whether some Ps may have experience increased after-levels
Reductions in extent of exposure 60–65 & 55–60 bands but increase in lower two bands.
Reported only at population level. No indication of the change experienced by individual Ps
ISO scale (5 point verbal)
% top three annoyance categories dropped, other two categories increased
Top two categories (%HA—but authors did not use this term) dropped from 37% to 16%
Yes
but no data presented of change in exposure of those reporting change in annoyance
No statistical tests
n.a. Classed as path intervention, even though includes quiet pavement as intervention
Multiple sources of road traffic exposure—not just motorway
Gidlöf-Gunnarsson, Öhrström & Kihlman (2010) [30] Sweden
Full-scale filling-in building gaps; barriers & housing improvement
B/A study
5 year apart
B: 160 Response rate: 56%
A: 153 (47%)
Mail survey
LAeq,24 h at façade
48–71
Calculated
–5 to −10 on exposed facades; −4 to −10 courtyards ISO scale (5 point verbal)
%Annoyed cut-off includes top 3 points.(Note: NOT %HA)
For Ps highly exposed and with large change:
B: 84% Annoyed
A: 28% Annoyed
For P’s with less change:
B: 45–55% Annoyed
A: 21–22% Annoyed
Yes
Large and consistent reductions in %Annoyed associated with reduction in noise exposure (but no statistical tests)
Authors refer to Öhrström [7] who cites ERF of Miedema & Vos [12]
For Ps in most exposed part of study, B/A 84/28%Annoyed outcomes both higher than estimated by this ERF, but also show much larger decrease in response than estimated by ERF. Response to change in same direction as estimated by ERF, but steeper, indicating excess response. (But no statistical tests)
This was a reconstruction project that included many other environmental changes—not just change in noise exposure (Ps reported 36% increase in overall satisfaction with area)
Kastka, Buchta, Ritterstaedt, Paulsen & Mau (1995) [31] Germany
Noise barriers at 12 sites + 2 control sites
B/A study 1–2 years B, 8–10 years A barriers were built 283 B
Response rate 59%
212 A
(72%)
97 Ps both B&A
Leq,D
B 50–70
Measured
Leq,D
A:51–66
Measured
–13 dB close to barriers to 0 dB at 200 m
Av. Change −4.1 dB
(1) 5 point verbally labelled disturbance scale
(2) %HA calc. as top two responses on scale in (1)
(3) factor K1: sensory-perceptional and emotional experience of traffic noise (0–10)
(4) factor K2: noise interferences
Yes
All response variables show significant reductions, e.g., %HA B: 64%; A:35% (chi2 = 39.69 p < 0.005)
Control sites response variables same B&A
Authors calculated an ERF using the steady-state before-responses. For this, mean disturbance scores (and, separately, other outcome variables including %HA) were regressed against before Leq,D
Mean (After) disturbance scores were compared to those estimated by this ERF. At 11 of the 12 sites, estimated mean disturbance score was greater than observed. Difference was statistically significant (matched pair t-test, df = 11, p < 0.05). Response to change in same direction as estimated by ERF, but steeper, indicating excess response following barrier construction
Authors reported extensive additional analyses
They suggest no simple causal relation between noise level reduction and annoyance reduction
Nilsson & Berglund (2006) [32] Sweden
Noise barrier
B/A study + control
9 mos. B; 15 mos. A
Repeated measures on 59%, 46% only
Before 304
Response rate 77%
(241 control
Response rate 66%)
(After Response rates: 72%, 69%)
Self-administered
Lden 70 to <45
Calculated
Lden 62.5 to <45
Calculated
–7.5 dB; with reducing change out to 100 m from barrier. Distribtn of change was:
–7.5 dB 52 Ps
−5 dB 47 Ps
−2.5 dB 31 Ps
Visual analogue scale 7-point annoyance scale. Transformed to 0–100 scale. Reports %HA as above cut-off 72 Yes
Reductions in %HA were significant (p < 0.05, sign-test) for three groups of Ps within 100 m of roadway
Control: no diff in B&A %HA
ERF cited was Miedema & Oudshoorn (2001) [13]
Reports both B&A %HA agree with prediction by ERF (no statistical test)
Response to change same direction and magnitude as estimated by ERF
Outdoor annoyance did not conform to ERF
Vincent & Champelovier (1993) [33] France
Noise barriers and low noise road surface
B/A study at 2(?) sites. 75
Response rate not reported
Leq,12 h 65.1
Location of measurement site relative to Ps not reported
Leq,12 h
56.3
Location not reported
Change in levels was variable with distance from road: −10 to −3 dB between 10 and 100 m. % highly annoyed (scale and definition of HA not reported).
B: 22%HA
A: 8%HA
Yes
(but no statistical test)
No comparison of change to any ERF Author notes that response to ‘Often disturbs sleep’ dropped from 13% to 6%