Summary
Objective
Manipulating plate sizes could possibly introduce perceptual biases for judging food satiation and intake, which is thought to be related to the Delbeouf illusion – a visual illusion based on the perceived size of one object related to another. This study was to investigate whether an association exists between an individual's susceptibility to the plate‐size‐effect and their weight status (i.e. normal‐weight versus overweight).
Methods
The study assessed the effect of plate size amongst normal‐weight (N = 124) and overweight (N = 79) New Zealand Europeans. All participants were asked to rate estimated satiation (ES) and intake (EI) on Visual Analogue Scales for 20 food images, which comprised photographs of ten different dishes placed on large versus small plates. These responses were analysed by mixed‐model ANCOVA.
Results
The results showed that the plate size had significant effects on ES (F (1, 1986) = 19.14, p < 0.001) and EI (F (1,1986) = 5.25; p = 0.048), with the small plate associated with higher ES and lower EI than the large plate. Significant differences in ES and EI were also evident across the weight groups (ES: F (1,1986) = 4.26, p = 0.039; EI: F (1,1986) = 42.22, p < 0.001), with the normal‐weight group reported higher ES and lower EI than the overweight group. Furthermore, the weight group and the plate‐size‐effect were found to be involved in a significant interaction for EI. Post‐hoc tests showed that the plate size only had a significant effect for the normal‐weight group (p < 0.05), but not for the overweight group.
Conclusions
Overall, the study demonstrated that the normal‐weight and overweight group differed in their susceptibilities to the plate‐size‐effect (reflected by EI). This study revealed some potential moderators for the plate‐size‐effect, such as the type of dish, and its associated appeal and familiarity, and provided useful indications about the effectiveness of small plates for food reduction.
Keywords: Eating behaviour, expected satiation, plate size, weight status
Introduction
Overconsumption of food has become a common phenomenon in many modern societies, where foods are increasingly accessible, diverse and palatable 1. An increasing amount of research has suggested that perceptual factors are important for regulating food consumption 2, 3, 4. Identifying these perceptual factors represents an important research question in the current rapid‐changing food environment.
Decisions on portion size largely depend on perception of food volume 5, 6, which can be measured by the estimated satiation (ES) or estimated intake (EI) of the food. Defined by Brunstrom and Rogers 5, ES is the relative feeling of fullness to be expected after consuming different foods when compared on a calorie‐for‐calorie basis, which is acquired by a learned association between sensory properties of food and their abilities to promote satiation. EI is referred to as estimated consumption volume of food 7. Conceivably, an inverted relationship is expected between ES and EI. While it has been previously suggested that people are extremely adept at estimating these entities 2, individual's estimation can be susceptible to environmental factors. For instance, the context of eating, such as restaurant versus laboratory setting, could alter people's intake 8, 9.
In particular, factors like the size and weight of utensils or containers could have impacts on individual's judgements of food portions 10. People tend to underestimate the quantity of food when it is presented on a large plate, or conversely overestimate the quantity when it is presented on a small plate 11, 12, 13. This bias has been explained by Van Ittersum and Wansink 14 by the Delboeuf illusion – when the perceptual of object size changes based on the surrounding context. In the context of food consumption, a small difference between the plate area and the food area increases the perception of food volume, and correspondingly leads to enhanced ES and diminished EI. This effect is herein referred to as the plate‐size‐effect.
The plate‐size‐effect was not shown to have direct effects on reduction of food intake. Many studies therefore disputed the effectiveness of using small plates for food reduction 15, 16, 17. Robinson and others 18 in their systematic review reported that three out of the nine studies found significant relationships between plate size and intake volume, five showed no difference and one found mixed results. Similarly, Libotte and others 19 in a separate review also concluded that the available data on the relationship between plate size and intake were contradictory. As a result, potential benefits of using small dishware to reduce food intake have yet to receive widespread support 18, 20. These contradictory evidence also suggested further factors to be identified for understanding the plate‐size‐effect.
Aside from environmental factors, characteristics of individuals, such as gender, age and body mass index, have been shown to be influential on behaviours relating to food portion judgements 2, 21, 22, 23, 24. Conceivably, these individual characteristics could also be important in determining the individual's susceptibility to the plate‐size‐effect. Of many possible individual characteristics, body weight is of particular interest and relevance. However, only a few studies have specifically discussed the plate‐size‐effect with respect to weight status, which yielded some unexpected findings. Shah and others 25 found that plate size had no effect on overweight group or normal‐weight group, contradictory to some other studies on lean participants. In addition, Yip and others 26 assessed effects of plate size on overweight, unrestraint female eaters, and also concluded no effect due to plate size. These authors suspected that the ad libitum design contributed to these unexpected results.
In light of the relatively few studies conducted and the inconsistent findings, the present study was to test individual's susceptibility to the Delbeouf illusion with food images, by asking participants to rate ES and EI on food placed on large and small plates. Specifically, the study was to test for the hypothesis that a potential association exists between an individual's susceptibility to the plate‐size‐effect and their weight status (i.e. normal‐weight versus overweight).
Methods
Participants
All participants in this study were New Zealand Europeans between the age of 18 and 35 years, healthy and mostly university students. The recruitment for participants was done through university websites, student associations, social media and participant database from previous studies. Potential participants were provided participant information sheets and asked to give written consents prior to the study. People in any BMI range were invited to participate in this study, although the subsequent data analyses were only performed on the participants whose BMI was within the specified intervals (normal weight: 18.5–24.9 kg m−2; overweight: 25.0–29.9 kg m−2). Originally, 456 individuals opened the questionnaire link, of which responses from 203 were considered acceptable according to the pre‐determined data checking protocol, yielding an overall completion rate was 44.5%.
Of the participants, 79 were grouped into the overweight group (22 males; BMI: 27.25 ± 1.4 kg m−2; Age: 24.1 ± 3.4 years), and 124 were grouped into the normal‐weight group mean (28 males; mean BMI: 22.21 ± 1.9 kg m−2; age: 23.8 ± 2.8 years). Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the University of Otago Human Ethics Committee (16/01B).
Questionnaire design
In order to obtain food images to be used in the questionnaire, ten different dishes were made. These particular ten dishes were selected for based on a preliminary questionnaire study. Six postgraduate students were shown recipes of 20 dishes and asked to rate their levels of liking and familiarity for each dish on a scale. Based on their responses, these ten dishes were selected to represent a wide range of hedonic and familiarity values. All of these dishes contained energy levels within a range of 375–440 kcal, with balanced sources of protein, fibre and carbohydrates. Each dish was photographed twice on a 23‐cm‐diameter plate and a 27‐cm‐diameter plate. Care was taken when transferring the food from one plate to another, in order to minimize variation in the presentation. The food was ensured to be mounted to a certain height. These photographs were taken under a constant lighting condition (Canon SX50 HS, 180 dpi). All photos included cutlery (a knife and fork set or a pair of chopsticks depending on the ethnic origin of the dish), and a standard 355‐ml drink can serve as visual references of the size of the dish (see examples in Figure 1). For each photograph, the food area on the plate was measured three times by image measurement software (Klonk, Image Measurement Corporation, USA), the average of which was recorded for computing the Food/Plate ratio. Consistently across all dishes, the small plates are associated with higher Food/Plate ratios than the large plates. Details about the dishes and photographs are in Table 1.
Table 1.
Name | Code | Energy (kcal) | Weight (g) | Food/Plate ratio | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
27‐cm plate | 23‐cm plate | ||||
Chicken Caesar Salad | 0 | 398.4 | 346.0 | 0.53 | 0.74 |
Dumplings | 1 | 378.8 | 168.4 | 0.31 | 0.43 |
Beef Stir fry | 2 | 406.2 | 321.2 | 0.35 | 0.49 |
Fish Linguine | 3 | 407.6 | 257.1 | 0.32 | 0.45 |
Chicken Fried Rice | 4 | 374.6 | 262.3 | 0.46 | 0.64 |
Korean Beef Plate | 5 | 379.0 | 337.2 | 0.48 | 0.66 |
Lamb and Potato | 6 | 377.8 | 301.2 | 0.41 | 0.56 |
Meatballs and Potato | 7 | 436.3 | 337.6 | 0.32 | 0.45 |
Teriyaki Chicken | 8 | 374.4 | 265.9 | 0.32 | 0.45 |
Mushroom Risotto | 9 | 421.8 | 148.2 | 0.32 | 0.44 |
The questionnaire was delivered in a form of questionnaire, which was developed and distributed using Qualtrics© (2016). The questionnaire consisted of questions presented to each participant in a randomized order. With each image presentation, the participant was asked to respond, on a 100‐point Visual Analogue Scale (VAS), to the question ‘How full will you be after consuming the entire dish?’ with the end points anchored with ‘not full at all’ and ‘extremely full’; and the question ‘As your main meal of the day, what portion of the plate would you usually consume?’ with the end points anchored with ‘none’ and ‘all of the food’. For the present study, the response to the former question is referred to as ES; the response to the latter question is referred to as EI. The participants were also required to rate Liking and Familiarity of each dish on similar line scales, with the right‐hand‐end anchored with ‘not at all’ and the left‐hand‐end anchored with ‘extremely like (or familiar)’. At the end of the questionnaire, the participants were asked to report their weight (in kg) and height (in cm). In order to minimize false reporting, an option of ‘prefer not to say’ was available.
Data analysis
First, a univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed on the dataset of liking and familiarity, respectively, to assess differences across the dishes and the BMI groups. Subsequently, a mixed‐model univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was applied to data of ES and EI to assess effects of plate size on people of separate weight groups, controlling for liking and familiarity of the food presented. With this model, Plate Size was defined as the within‐subjects factor; BMI Group and Gender were the between‐subjects factors; ratings for liking and familiarity of each dish were the covariates. Any dataset that violated the assumption of sphericity was corrected using Greenhouse–Geisser corrections. Simple tests with Bonferroni corrections were employed as post‐hoc tests where appropriate to disentangle higher‐order effects. All data analyses were performed using SPSS (23.0; IBM®).
Results
The ANOVA on the liking and familiarity ratings suggested significant differences across the dishes (liking: F (9,1998) = 16.66, p < 0.001; familiarity: F (9,1998) = 14.36, p < 0.001), but not between the BMI groups.
The mixed‐model ANCOVA on ES revealed a significant main effect of the Plate Size (F (1, 1986) = 19.14, p < 0.001). On average, the ES rating associated with the large plate (M = 80.13; SE = 0.355) was significantly lower than that associated with small plate (M = 86.83; SE = 0.320). Furthermore, the BMI group was also found to have a significant effect on the ES rating. Specifically, the normal‐weight group gave a slightly but significantly higher ES rating (M = 84.08; SE = 0.368) compared to the overweight group (M = 82.88; SE = 0.454; F (1,1986) = 4.26, p = 0.039). The results also showed that these ten dishes were given significantly different ES ratings (F (9,1986) = 51.14, p < 0.001), despite having similar caloric values. Figure 2 (Panel A) displays the averaged ES rating for each of the ten dishes. In comparison, Dish 0 and 7 were associated with lower ratings.
Interestingly, no significant interaction was evident between the Plate Size and BMI Group (p > .05; see Table 2), implying little difference in the ES between the large and small plate across the normal‐weight and overweight group. A significant interaction effect on the ES was observed for the Plate Size and Dish (F (9,1986) = 6.85, p < 0.001), suggesting the difference in ES across the plate size was dependent of the type of dish. This result was further explained by the significant interaction effect between the Plate Size and the covariates – liking and familiarity ratings (liking: F (1,1986) = 8.51, p = 0.004; familiarity: F (1,1986) = 15.52, p < 0.001).
Table 2.
Weight group | Estimated satiation | Estimated intake | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Large plate | Small plate | Large plate | Small plate | |||||
Mean | SE | Mean | SE | Mean | SE | Mean | SE | |
Normal weight | 81.67 | 1.28 | 85.89 | 1.25 | 85.70 | 0.82 | 80.37 | 1.13 |
Overweight | 79.92 | 1.44 | 84.74 | 1.79 | 93.64 | 1.53 | 92.24 | 1.60 |
The same statistical model – the mixed‐model ANCOVA – was also applied to the dataset of EI. The results suggested significant main effects due to the Plate Size (F (1,1986) = 5.25; p = 0.048), BMI Group (F (1,1986) = 42.22, p < 0.001) and Dish (F (9,1986) = 9.84, p < 0.001). Of particular interests, the results showed that the EI rating of the small plate (M = 89.11, SE = 0.284) was significantly lower than that of the large plate (M = 94.09; SE = 0.276), and the EI rating of the normal‐weight group (M = 80.06, SE = 0.299) was significantly lower than that of the overweight group (M = 93.14, SE = 0.367). In addition, slight yet significant differences were observed across the ten dishes, as shown in Figure 2 (Panel B).
The results also showed that the Plate Size was significantly interacted with the BMI Group for influencing the EI ratings (F (1,1986) = 27.26, p < 0.001). Averaged EI ratings from the separate BMI group for each of the plate sizes are presented in Table 2. Post‐hoc test with Bonferroni corrections showed that the plate size only had a significant effect on EI ratings for the normal‐weight group (p < 0.05), but not for the overweight group. No other significant interaction was observed in the result of this model.
In order to further disentangle interactions between the tested factors, a series of paired‐sample t‐tests were performed, separately for the normal‐weight and overweight group, on ES and EI ratings. The results (Table 3) indicated similar differences between weight groups for the ES ratings – eight dishes being rated significantly differently when they were placed on different plates. Notably, results from Dish 0 and 7 did not present significant difference between the large and small plate. For the EI ratings, the normal‐weight group reported significant differences between the large and small plates for six dishes, whereas the overweight group reported no difference. These results further consolidated the findings obtained from the mixed‐design ANCOVA.
Table 3.
Dish | Estimated satiation (ES) | Estimated intake (EI) | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Normal weight | Overweight | Normal weight | Overweight | |||||||||
L‐S | SE | p‐Value | L‐S | SE | p‐Value | L‐S | SE | p‐Value | L‐S | SE | p‐Value | |
0 | 2.686 | 2.763 | 0.572 | 2.884 | 2.382 | 0.321 | 2.208 | 1.620 | 0.179 | 2.302 | 1.151 | 0.052 |
1 | −5.060 | 2.455 | 0.045 | −4.805 | 1.874 | 0.001 | 1.941 | 1.812 | 0.289 | 3.881 | 2.267 | 0.087 |
2 | −2.942 | 1.474 | 0.050 | −3.970 | 1.932 | 0.041 | 3.510 | 1.730 | 0.047 | −2.643 | 2.321 | 0.261 |
3 | −9.706 | 2.141 | 0.000 | −7.659 | 2.275 | 0.002 | 4.735 | 1.761 | 0.010 | 2.000 | 2.381 | 0.406 |
4 | −7.596 | 1.494 | 0.000 | −8.286 | 1.842 | 0.000 | 3.938 | 1.466 | 0.010 | 2.558 | 1.709 | 0.142 |
5 | −5.353 | 1.349 | 0.038 | −4.535 | 1.252 | 0.003 | 3.082 | 1.466 | 0.036 | −1.326 | 1.428 | 0.359 |
6 | −5.804 | 1.420 | 0.000 | −5.488 | 1.823 | 0.004 | 0.940 | 1.239 | 0.452 | 1.419 | 1.356 | 0.301 |
7 | 1.577 | 1.830 | 0.393 | 4.146 | 2.133 | 0.059 | −3.146 | 1.824 | 0.091 | −0.310 | 1.630 | 0.850 |
8 | −10.529 | 2.348 | 0.000 | −15.429 | 3.302 | 0.000 | 4.178 | 1.879 | 0.002 | 3.025 | 1.848 | 0.110 |
9 | −7.118 | 1.681 | 0.000 | −6.537 | 2.080 | 0.003 | 3.327 | 1.606 | 0.039 | −1.953 | 2.202 | 0.380 |
Note: For the t‐test, data of the large plate were the first variable, and data of the small plate were the second variable. Thus, negative t‐statistics indicates that larger plates are associated with lower estimates. Degree of freedom is 122 for the normal‐weight group, and 77 for the overweight group.
Discussion
Findings from the present study pointed to a potential role of body weight in moderating the plate‐size‐effect. Despite the fact that both groups rated foods on the small plates to be more satiating, only the normal‐weight group reported their EI ratings in accordance with this perceptual bias. On the contrary, individuals in the overweight group gave similar EI ratings for foods on both the large and small plates, which were significantly higher than the EI ratings from the normal‐weight group. In line with some previous data 27, 28, these results implied that the overweight group were more inclined to clean the plate, and therefore less susceptible to the plate‐size‐effect. This finding may be relevant to another recent study, in which the susceptibility to the plate‐size‐effect was found to be correlated with the individual's level of intuitive eating 24. Tentatively, a potential relationship may be present between an individual's body weight and level of intuitive eating, both of which in turn influence the plate‐size‐effect.
With regards to the relationship between the weight status and the plate‐size‐effect, findings from the present study presented contradictory evidence to previous studies. The available evidence in the literature suggested little effect of plate size on food or energy intake for either normal‐weight or overweight group 17, 25, 26. Discrepancy between the previous and present studies may be attributed to differences in the methodologies. Previous studies typically used an ad libitum design in a buffet setting, which gave a more realistic account of the eating situation, but could also introduce bias to the participant's eating behaviours 26, 29. Findings from the present study tentatively support the use of VAS on food images for capturing different responses to plate sizes.
In addition to BMI, the type of dish was shown to have a significant effect on the plate‐size‐effect. As the results indicated, the plate size did not affect the ES rating for Dish 0 and 7, differing from the observations from the other eight dishes. In addition, review of the main effect of ES suggested that Dish 0 and 7 received significantly lower ES rating than other dishes. This suggested that the overall ES rating of a dish might have provided a baseline for the plate‐size‐effect. In other words, a dish with a low ES rating is associated with a small likelihood of the presence of the plate‐size‐effect.
Relating to the effect of dishes, liking and familiarity, as covariates in the statistical model, were shown to be influential on the plate‐size‐effect by ES. However, these measures were comparable across the BMI groups. The role of liking or familiarity was never assessed previously in the context of plate‐size‐effect, but it was learnt from studies of ‘portion estimation’ that both liking and familiarity were correlated to estimated food portions, although directions of these correlations were not consistent across foods 30, 31. Future studies are required to confirm whether liking or familiarity can directly moderate the plate‐size‐effect, but evidence from the present study eliminated the role of liking or familiarity in mediating the interaction between BMI and the plate‐size‐effect.
A possible limitation of this study was that the participants were not asked to report their state of hunger at the time of testing. Another factor that might have introduced potential bias was that participant's BMI was based on self‐reported data, instead of empirical measures. Finally, the choice of the VAS method should be considered when interpreting the current findings. Future studies should further verify these findings using other common method, such as the method of adjustment 5.
Overall, the present study revealed that BMI could have a moderate effect on individual's susceptibility to the plate‐size‐effect. This finding was possibly explained by different tendencies between the normal‐weight and overweight group to ‘clean the plate’. This study also suggested some potential moderators for the plate‐size‐effect, such as the type of dish, and its associated appeal and familiarity. Altogether, these findings provided additional knowledge about the plate‐size‐effect and gave further suggestions on the effectiveness of using small plates for prevention of overeating.
Conflict of Interest Statement
Dr. Peng has nothing to disclose.
Funding
This research work was financially supported by the departmental research fund from the Department of Food Science, University of Otago.
Acknowledgements
Professor F. B. Waters is gratefully acknowledged for providing insights on this research topic, and for proofreading early drafts of the manuscript. The author thanks Sarah Adam for her assistance with a part of the data collection.
Peng, M. (2017) How does plate size affect estimated satiation and intake for individuals in normal‐weight and overweight groups?. Obesity Science & Practice, 3: 282–288. doi: 10.1002/osp4.119.
References
- 1. Cohen DA. Obesity and the built environment: changes in environmental cues cause energy imbalances. International Journal of Obesity 2008; 32: S137–S142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Brunstrom J. Mind over platter: pre‐meal planning and the control of meal size in humans. International Journal of Obesity 2014; 38: S9–S12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Brunstrom J. The control of meal size in human subjects: a role for expected satiety, expected satiation and premeal planning. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 2011; 70: 155–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Sobal J, Wansink B. Kitchenscapes, tablescapes, platescapes, and foodscapes influences of microscale built environments on food intake. Environment and Behavior 2007; 39: 124–142. [Google Scholar]
- 5. Brunstrom J, Rogers PJ. How many calories are on our plate? Expected fullness, not liking, determines meal‐size selection. Obesity 2009; 17: 1884–1890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Brunstrom JM, Shakeshaft NG. Measuring affective (liking) and non‐affective (expected satiety) determinants of portion size and food reward. Appetite 2009; 52: 108–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Wansink B, Painter JE, North J. Bottomless bowls: why visual cues of portion size may influence intake. Obesity Research 2005; 13: 93–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Diliberti N, Bordi PL, Conklin MT, Roe LS, Rolls BJ. Increased portion size leads to increased energy intake in a restaurant meal. Obesity Research 2004; 12: 562–568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Ayala GX, Rogers M, Arredondo EM, et al. Away‐from‐home food intake and risk for obesity: examining the influence of context. Obesity 2008; 16: 1002–1008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Marchiori D, Corneille O, Klein O. Container size influences snack food intake independently of portion size. Appetite 2012; 58: 814–817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Wansink B, Cheney MM. Super bowls: serving bowl size and food consumption. JAMA 2005; 293: 1723–1728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Wansink B, van Ittersum K, Painter JE. Ice cream illusions: bowls, spoons, and self‐served portion sizes. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 2006; 31: 240–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Wansink B. Environmental factors that increase the food intake and consumption volume of unknowing consumers. Annual Review of Nutrition 2004; 24: 455–479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Van Ittersum K, Wansink B. Plate size and color suggestibility: the Delboeuf illusion's bias on serving and eating behavior. Journal of Consumer Research 2012; 39: 215–228. [Google Scholar]
- 15. Koh J, Pliner P. The effects of degree of acquaintance, plate size, and sharing on food intake. Appetite 2009; 52: 595–602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Penaforte FRO, Japur CC, Diez‐Garcia RW, Hernandez JC, Palmma‐Linares I, Chiarello PG. Plate size does not affect perception of food portion size. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics 2014; 27: 214–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Halverson KH, Meengs JS. Using a smaller plate did not reduce energy intake at meals. Appetite 2007; 49: 652–660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Robinson E, Nolan S, Tudur‐Smith C, et al. Will smaller plates lead to smaller waists? A systematic review and meta‐analysis of the effect that experimental manipulation of dishware size has on energy consumption. Obesity Reviews 2014; 15: 812–821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Libotte E, Siegrist M, Bucher T. The influence of plate size on meal composition. Literature review and experiment. Appetite 2014; 82: 91–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. English L, Lasschuijt M, Keller KL. Mechanisms of the portion size effect. What is known and where do we go from here? Appetite 2015; 88: 39–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Ello‐Martin JA, Ledikwe JH, Rolls BJ. The influence of food portion size and energy density on energy intake: implications for weight management. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 2005; 82: 236S–241S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Wardle J. Conditioning processes and cue exposure in the modification of excessive eating. Addictive Behaviors 1990; 15: 387–393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Weingarten HP. Meal initiation controlled by learned cues: basic behavioral properties. Appetite 1984; 5: 147–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Anderson DA, Schaumberg K, Anderson LM, Reilly EE. Is level of intuitive eating associated with plate size effects? Eating Behaviors 2015; 18: 125–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Shah M, Schroeder R, Winn W, Adams‐Huet B. A pilot study to investigate the effect of plate size on meal energy intake in normal weight and overweight/obese women. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics 2011; 24: 612–615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Yip W, Wiessing KR, Budgett S, Poppitt SD. Using a smaller dining plate does not suppress food intake from a buffet lunch meal in overweight, unrestrained women. Appetite 2013; 69: 102–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Krassner HA, Brownell KD, Stunkard AJ. Cleaning the plate: food left over by overweight and normal weight persons. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1979; 17: 155–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. LeBow MD, Chipperfield J, Magnusson J. Leftovers, body weight and sex of eater. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1985; 23: 217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Holden SS, Zlatevska N, Dubelaar C. Whether smaller plates reduce consumption depends on who's serving and who's looking: a meta‐analysis. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research 2016; 1: 134–146. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Brogden N, Almiron‐Roig E. Food liking, familiarity and expected satiation selectively influence portion size estimation of snacks and caloric beverages in men. Appetite 2010; 55: 551–555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Almiron‐Roig E, Solis‐Trapala I, Dodd J, Jebb SA. Estimating food portions. Influence of unit number, meal type and energy density. Appetite 2013; 71: 95–103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]