Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Psychol. 2017 Mar 10;15:182–188. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.024

Alternatives for Abandoned Children: Insights from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project

Charles H Zeanah 1, Kathryn L Humphreys 2, Nathan A Fox 3, Charles A Nelson 4,5
PMCID: PMC5607636  NIHMSID: NIHMS869444  PMID: 28813259

Abstract

The Bucharest Early Intervention Project is the first and only randomized controlled trial of foster care as an alternative to institutional care for orphaned and abandoned children. Across various domains of brain and behavioral development we demonstrated that children in families developed more favorably than children in institutions, that foster care remediates some but not all compromises associated with institutional placement and that earlier placement in foster care leads to more developmental gains in some but not all domains. In addition to early placement, higher quality of care provided and more stable placements for children all enhanced outcomes. These results have important implications for science, practice and policy.

Keywords: deprivation, institutional rearing, early experience, foster care, parenting


Millions of orphaned, abandoned, and maltreated children worldwide require care that cannot be provided by their parents or families. For hundreds of years, these children have posed challenges that have led to two major societal approaches: caring for children in institutional settings (orphanages), or placing children in families, through foster care or adoption.

Prior to the study this paper describes, only a small number of descriptive studies, mostly conducted in the mid-20th century, had compared children in foster care to children in institutional care (4–13). Results demonstrated that children in foster care developed more favorably than children in institutions. On the other hand, interpreting these results is challenging, given that it is plausible that more handicapped children might be more likely to be placed in institutions rather than foster care. More recent studies of children adopted from institutions similarly show substantial gains after family placement, but they are limited by the potential for selection bias inherent in adoption. In addition, most did not assess the children within the institutions prior to adoption.

Origins and Design of the Bucharest Early Intervention Project

Following the infamous economic policies of Nicolae Ceausescu, Romania faced an unprecedented child protection crisis at the turn of the 21st century, with an estimated 170,000 children living in large, socially impoverished institutions [1, 14]. Our group was invited to conduct an intervention study by the Romanian Secretary of State for Child Protection, because of a policy debate about the best approach to caring for large numbers of abandoned children [1].

To address the challenges and limitations of previous research, we decided to conduct a randomized controlled trial (RCT) of foster care as an alternative to institutional care among children who had been abandoned in infancy and were being raised in institutions. The unique and crucial advantage of randomizing infants already living in institutions is that both groups share pre-existing risk factors, and any differences in outcomes can be confidently attributed to the intervention. Thus, this was the only design that could provide definitive evidence of which form of care was preferable [1,2].

We also wanted to assess the children’s development comprehensively, and we determined that measures of brain and behavioral functioning could be used to address the questions of concern. We recruited 136 children 6 to 31 months of age who were living in Bucharest in institutions, following abandonment and who had neither obvious genetic syndromes or severe neurological conditions. All the children had lived at least half of their lives in institutions. We also recruited 72 typically developing children from pediatric clinics in Bucharest to serve as a comparison group, since the measures we planned to use had not been used in Romania before (see design in Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Participants in BEIP

Following baseline assessments, we randomly allocated half of the sample to care as usual and half of the sample to placement with foster parents whom we recruited, managed and supported. As illustrated in Figure 1, the original RCT was conducted from baseline through age 54 months. At that point, we turned support of the foster care network over to local governmental authorities in Bucharest. We then conducted follow-ups of the children at 8 years and 12 years.

Ethical issues

Studying a vulnerable population and conducting an RCT raises a number of concerns. We [1, 15, 16] and others [17, 18, 19] have discussed these issues in detail. Briefly, we conducted a study with a vulnerable population only because it addressed a question relevant to them. Second, we did not interfere in any decisions about changes in placement of the children since legally those decisions had to be made by Romania’s child protection officials. Third, we used only measures that posed no more than minimal risk to participants. Fourth, there were multiple levels of oversight of the study within Romania and at each principal investigator’s university.

The BEIP Intervention

We endeavored to create a foster care program in Bucharest that was culturally appropriate, feasible, replicable and informed by developmental science. We have previously described these efforts in detail [1, 20]. The essence of the foster care intervention was to recruit, train and support foster parents willing to make a full and long-term commitment to the children in their care.

Drawing upon our experiences of intervening with young children in the U.S. [21], we recruited and trained foster parents who could understand and respond effectively to meet the needs of young children in their care. We knew from informal observations and pilot assessments [22] that these young children had serious developmental delays and deviances, and we expected that they would present challenging behaviors to parents caring for them. We also knew that resources for special needs children were not widely available at that time in Romania. Therefore, we knew that the care that foster parents provided would be the major means through which children’s developmental delays and deviant behavior would be addressed.

A particular challenge that we faced was that not only was the fledging child protection system in Bucharest overwhelmed and underfunded at the time, the entire profession of social work in Romania was inexperienced due to Ceausescu’s elimination of social work as a profession (deeming it unnecessary). Therefore, we provided expert mentoring to BEIP social workers from the United States. Mental health clinicians from Tulane University who had had many years of experience working with young maltreated children in foster care provided weekly consultation to BEIP social workers about how to support foster parents in caring for formerly institutionalized children. Social workers had frequent in person and phone contact and tried to be responsive to needs that foster parents expressed.

Scientific questions

BEIP addressed three major questions across multiple domains of development (Table 1). First, we documented developmental differences between young children living in institutions and those with no history of institutionalization living in families. Second, we determined if and how much recovery foster care might produce for young children who had been abandoned and placed in institutions, using a stringent intent to treat analysis, in which data were analyzed by original group assignment rather than subsequent placement status. This provided a conservative estimate of intervention effects. Third, we examined whether the timing of placement affected outcomes and how this varied across domains of development.

Table 1.

Domains of assessment

Baseline 6–31 months 30 months 42 months 54 months 8 years 12 years

Cognition
 IQ x x x x x x
 executive functions x x x x x x

Language
 expressive x x x
 receptive x x x

Emotional expression x x x

Psychopathology
 symptoms x x x
 disorders x x x
 impairment x x x

Attachment
 formation x
 security x x
 organization x x
 disorders x x x x x x

Social behavior
 competence x x x
 peer relations x x
 social skills x x

Stress response X

Brain functioning
 EEG x x x x x
 ERP x x x x x

Brain structure
 MRI x

Outcomes

Baseline results

At baseline, there were no differences between the children later randomized to foster care or care as usual, but these children living in institutions were different from their never institutionalized counterparts in virtually every developmental domain [1,23].

Intervention effects: Foster care and care as usual

Results from the RCT were consistent across most developmental domains -- children in foster care developed more favorably than children who received care as usual. Selected intervention outcomes are shown in 2a, with effect sizes and confidence intervals to allow comparisons across domains. Overall, the magnitude of the effects illustrated in Table 2a are consistently in the small to medium range (all in the expected direction, FCG have better outcomes than CAUG). Because of the randomized design, these results are definitive proof that foster care placement led to enhanced functioning.

Table 2a.

Selected Intervention Effects (CAUG > FCG)

Domain 54 months 8 year follow-up 12 year follow-up
Full scale IQ −0.48 [−3.48, 2.53] −0.36 [−3.16, 2.43] −0.42 [−3.64, 2.80]
Height1 -- −0.40 [−1.61, 0.81] −0.40 [−2.01, 1.22]
Weight1 -- −0.68 [−1.90, 0.54] −0.46 [−2.25, 1.33]
Externalizing signs (girls/boys) -- -- 0.19 [−0.55, 0.92]/0.63 [−0.08, 1.35]
Internalizing signs (girls/boys) 0.62 [0.06, 1.19/−0.14 [−0.86, 0.57] -- --
RAD signs 0.66 [0.35, 0.97] 0.53 [0.21, 0.86] 0.58 [−0.41, 1.56]
DSED signs 0.37 [0.03, 0.72] 0.57 [0.32, 0.81] 0.43 [0.001, 0.85]
EEG alpha power1 -- −0.43 [−0.53, −0.33] −0.56 [−0.58, −0.54]

Note. d Effect Size using Maximum Likelihood Estimator [95% Confidence Interval].

-- indicates no intent-to-treat finding or not assessed.

1

Height and weight presented after statistically controlling for age and sex.

2

EEG alpha power at 8 years presented from central regions; at 12 years overall relative alpha power is presented.

Perhaps more interesting, is when these same domains are examined comparing the FCG and NIG, demonstrating “catch-up” of the FCG to a (quasi-matched) comparison group of children with no history of institutionalization (Table 2b). Here, effect sizes vary dramatically, with some domains showing almost complete catch-up (e.g., height, weight, EEG alpha power) and others demonstrating a lack of complete catch-up despite notable difference between those children who did and did not get assigned to the intervention. For example, the magnitude of IQ differences between the FCG and NIG were large across all time points considered. Most often the FCG children, as a group, occupy a level of functioning that is intermediate to the CAUG and NIG.

Table 2b.

Foster Care Group Catch-up (FCG > NIG)

Domain 54 months 8 year follow-up 12 year follow-up
Full scale IQ −1.56 [−5.20, 2.30] −2.07 [−4.59, 0.45] −1.38 [−4.55, 1.79]
Height1 -- −0.13 [−1.02, 0.76] −0.21 [−1.75, 1.34]
Weight1 -- 0.03 [−0.97, 1.03] −0.15 [−1.98, 1.68]
Externalizing signs (girls/boys) -- -- 0.75 [0.17, 1.33]/0.46 [−0.09, 1.00]
Internalizing signs (girls/boys) 0.51 [0.07, 0.95]/0.86 [0.08, 1.64] -- --
RAD signs −0.08 [−0.24, 0.08] 0.48 [0.32, 0.63] 0.64 [0.27, 1.00]
DSED signs 0.74 [0.47, 1.01] 0.50 [0.35, 0.65] 0.60 [0.35, 0.84]
EEG alpha power2 -- −0.24 [−0.35, −0.13] 0.00 [−0.02, 0.02]

Note. d Effect Size using Maximum Likelihood Estimator [95% Confidence Interval].

-- indicates no intent-to-treat finding or not assessed.

1

Height and weight presented after statistically controlling for age and sex.

2

EEG alpha power at 8 years presented from central regions; at 12 years overall relative alpha power is presented.

Although IQs of FCG children were significantly higher than those CAUG children over time [26, 27], there were few intervention effects on executive functioning skills [28, 29], which were not assessed in early childhood. FCG children also demonstrated an attention bias to positive facial expressions of emotion at both 8 and 12 years [30, 31]. Social behaviors, including peer relations and social skills were enhanced [32, 33]. Externalizing signs, CU traits and attachment disorder behaviors at 12 years all were reduced for children living in foster care [3437]. Brain structure and functioning at 8 and 12 years also were enhanced by the intervention [3841]. Further, cortical white matter volume partially mediated the effect of institutional rearing on EEG alpha power, suggesting that reductions in circuitry were associated with reductions in brain electrical activity. Also, white matter tracts in the external capsule and corpus callosum partially explained links between institutional rearing status and internalizing symptoms in middle childhood and early adolescence [39]. Children randomized to foster care demonstrated healthier stress responses, as indexed both by cortisol levels and autonomic nervous system indices [42].

Timing of enhanced care

Because of the variability of children’s ages at the time they were randomized to foster care (7–33 months), we were able to examine the effects of age at which their placements occurred in relation to outcomes (see Table 3). For some, but clearly not all domains, children placed into foster care at earlier ages fared better than those placed at older ages. The ages at which differences became significant ranged from 12–24 months, but most were between 20 and 24 months of age. We have considered these results as compatible with sensitive periods in brain development [1, 43, 44].

Table 3.

Timing effects: Age of placements at which differences between earlier and later placed children became significant.

Assessed at 42 months Assessed at 54 months Assessed at 8 years Assessed at 12 years
Stereotypies 12 months 12 months n/a1 n/a1
Expressive language 15 months n/a1 n/a1 n/a1
Receptive language 15 months n/a1 n/a1 n/a1
IQ 24 months 24 months negative2 negative2
Organization of attachment 24 months n/a1 n/a1 n/a1
Security of attachment 24 months n/a1 n/a1 n/a1
Indiscriminate behavior 24 months 24 months negative2 negative2
Error related negativity n/a1 n/a1 20 months negative2
Alpha and theta negative2 n/a1 24 months negative2
Social skills n/a1 n/a1 20 months n/a1
Cortisol reactivity n/a1 n/a1 n/a1 24 months
Parasympathetic nervous system reactivity n/a1 n/a1 n/a1 18 months
Competence n/a1 n/a1 n/a1 20 months
1

n/a means that the variable was not assessed

2

negative means no timing effects were detected

Keys to the success of the intervention

Quality of caregiving, which was objectively coded from videotaped observations, was higher in the children in BEIP foster care than children who received care as usual [1]. During naturalistic interactions at 30 months, quality of caregiving was an important predictor of early psychopathology [45]. Secure attachments at 42 months, which were significantly more likely in children in foster care, mediated the effect of caregiving quality on psychopathology at 54 months [46]. In fact, secure attachment in early childhood also predicted other subsequent outcomes, including IQ and peer relations at 8 years [26, 47].

At age 12 years, children who had remained in a stable foster placement since original randomization were less likely to display signs of internalizing and externalizing disorders [34], as well as more likely to demonstrate an attention bias to positive emotional displays, while instability of foster care placement was related to threat bias. The magnitude of the positive bias was associated with fewer internalizing problems and better coping mechanisms [31]. Of course, children with higher levels of serious behavior problems might precipitate more disruptions, but we found no differences on multiple measures at earlier ages in children whose placements subsequently disrupted and those that did not, so it is unlikely that challenging behaviors led to the disruptions. Instead, the disruptions often resulted from decisions to reunify children with their biological families, sometimes with subsequent later returns to institutional care.

Conclusions

Implementation of a quality foster care intervention for young children who had experienced severe deprivation proved feasible and led to improved developmental outcomes across numerous domains compared to care as usual. Earlier placement into foster care, higher quality of care provided, and more stable placements all enhanced outcomes. Though early institutional rearing was associated with large deleterious effects across physical, cognitive, socioemotional and behavioral domains, the effects on cognitive development and psychopathology showed the least response to quality foster care intervention. The urgency of early family placements is underscored by others’ findings that adoption of institutionalized Romanian infants before 6 months of age in led to near complete recovery across domains [48].

From a practice and policy perspective, the lack of sustained and meaningful support to the foster care program at the trial’s conclusion was accompanied by an increase in disrupted placements and loss of some developmental and clinical gains. Given that developmental delays and deviance from deprivation seem not to be transient, sustained investments in high quality care are worth pursuing and sustaining.

Table 4.

Timing effects: Age of placements at which differences between earlier and later placed children became significant.

Assessed at 42 months Assessed at 54 months Assessed at 8 years Assessed at 12 years
Stereotypies 12 months 12 months n/a1 n/a1
Expressive language 15 months n/a1 n/a1 n/a1
Receptive language 15 months n/a1 n/a1 n/a1
IQ 24 months 24 months negative2 negative2
Organization of attachment 24 months n/a1 n/a1 n/a1
Security of attachment 24 months n/a1 n/a1 n/a1
Indiscriminate behavior 24 months 24 months negative2 negative2
Error related negativity n/a1 n/a1 20 months negative2
Alpha and theta negative2 n/a1 24 months negative2
Social skills n/a1 n/a1 20 months n/a1
Cortisol reactivity n/a1 n/a1 n/a1 24 months
Parasympathetic nervous system reactivity n/a1 n/a1 n/a1 18 months
Competence n/a1 n/a1 n/a1 20 months
1

n/a means that the variable was not assessed

2

negative means no timing effects were detected

Highlights.

  • BEIP is the first ever RCT comparing foster care to institutional care for young children.

  • Institutional rearing led to substantial developmental compromises across almost all domains.

  • FCG children made significant gains compared to CAUG children.

  • Timing of placement into families was critical for selected domains of development.

  • Recovery from early deprivation was incomplete for most domains of development.

  • Key recovery factors were earlier placement, and quality of care and stability of placements.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Nelson CA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH. Romania’s abandoned children: Deprivation, brain development and the struggle for recovery. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Zeanah CH, Nelson CA, Fox NA, Smyke AT, Marshall P, Parker SW, Koga S. Designing research to study the effects of institutionalization on brain and behavioral development: The Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:885–907. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Zeanah CH, Smyke AT, Settles L. Children in orphanages. In: McCartney K, Phillips D, Malden MA, editors. Blackwell Handbook of Early Childhood Development. Blackwell Publishing; 2006. pp. 224–254. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Goldfarb W. Infant rearing and problem behavior. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 1943;13:249–265. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Goldfarb W. Infant rearing as a factor in foster home replacement. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 1944;14:162–173. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Goldfarb W. Effects of psychological deprivation in infancy and subsequent stimulation. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1945;102:18–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Goldfarb W. Psychological privation in infancy and subsequent adjustment. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 1945;15:247–255. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Levy RJ. Effects of institutional vs. boarding home care on a group of infants. Journal of Personality. 1947;15:233–241. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Dennis W, Najarian P. Infant development under environmental handicap. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied. 1957;71:1–13. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Provence S, Lipton RC. Infants in institutions. New York: International Universities Press; 1962. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Roy P, Rutter M, Pickles A. Institutional care: Risk from family background or pattern of rearing? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2000;41:139–141. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Harden BJ. Congregate care for infants and toddlers: Shedding new light on an old question. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2002;23:476–495. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ahmad A, Qahar J, Siddiq A, Majeed A, Rasheed J, Jabar F, von Knorring A-l. A 2-year follow-up of orphans’ competence, socioemotional problems and post-traumatic stress symptoms in traditional foster care and orphanages in Iraqi Kurdistan. Child Care, Health and Development. 2005;31:203–215. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2004.00477.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rosapepe JC. Half way home: Romania’s abandoned children ten years after the revolution. Report of the US Ambassador to Romania. 2005 [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Nelson CA. The Bucharest Early Intervention Project: Case study in the ethics of mental health research. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2012;200:243–247. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e318247d275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Zeanah CH, Koga SF, Simion B, Stanescu A, Tabacaru CL, Fox NA, Nelson CA the BEIP Core Group. Ethical considerations in international research collaboration: The Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2006;27:559–576. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Millum J, Emanuel EJ. The ethics of international research with abandoned children. Science. 2007;318:1874–1875. doi: 10.1126/science.1153822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Miller FG. The randomized controlled trial as a demonstration project: An ethical perspective. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166:743–745. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09040538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Rid A. When is research socially valuable? Lessons from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2012;200:248–249. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e318247d124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Smyke AT, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Nelson CA. A new model of foster care for young children: The Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2009;18:721–734. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2009.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Larrieu J, Zeanah CH. An intensive intervention for infants and toddlers in foster care. In: Pruett K, Pruett M, editors. Custody Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. Philadelphia: Williams and Wilkins; 1998. pp. 357–371. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Smyke AT, Dumitrescu A, Zeanah CH. Attachment disturbances in young children: I The continuum of caretaking casualty. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41:972–982. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200208000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Smyke AT, Koga SF, Johnson DE, Fox NA, Marshall PJ, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH the BEIP Core Group. The caregiving context in institution-reared and family-reared infants and toddlers in Romania. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:210–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01694.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Marshall PJ, Smyke AT, Guthrie D. Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: The Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Science. 2007;318:1937–1940. doi: 10.1126/science.1143921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Smyke AT, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Nelson CA, Guthrie D. Placement in foster care enhances quality of attachment among young institutionalized children. Child Development. 2010;81:212–223. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01390.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Fox NA, Almas AN, Degnan KA, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH. The effects of severe psychosocial deprivation and foster care intervention on cognitive development at 8 years of age: Findings from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52:919–928. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02355.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *27.Almas AN, Degnan KA, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA. IQ at age 12 following a history of institutional care: Findings from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Developmental Psychology. 2016;52:1858–1866. doi: 10.1037/dev0000167. This study demonstrates an intervention effect on IQ 8 years after completion of the RCT. 12-year-old children originally randomized to foster care had higher full-scale and verbal IQs than children originally randomized to care as usual. Attachment security at 42 months predicted a profile of stable, typical IQ scores over time. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.McDermott JM, Westerlund A, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA, Fox NA. Early adversity and neural correlates of executive function: Implications for academic adjustment. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2012;2:S59–S66. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bick J, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Nelson CA. Memory and executive functioning in 12-year-old children with histories of institutional rearing. Child Development. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12952. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Troller-Renfree S, McDermott JM, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA. The effects of early foster care intervention on attention biases in previously institutionalized children in Romania. Developmental Science. 2015;18:713–722. doi: 10.1111/desc.12261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Troller-Renfree S, McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA. The beneficial effects of a positive attention bias amongst children with a history of psychosocial deprivation. Biological Psychology. 2017;122:110–120. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Almas AN, Degnan KA, Radulescu A, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA. Effects of early intervention and the moderating effects of brain activity on institutionalized children’s social skills at age 8. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109:17228–17231. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121256109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Almas AN, Degnan KA, Walker OL, Radulescu A, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA. The effects of early institutionalization and foster care intervention on children’s social behaviors at age 8. Social Development. 2015;24:225–239. doi: 10.1111/sode.12089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • **34.Humphreys KL, Gleason MM, Drury SS, Miron DM, Nelson CA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH. Effects of institutional rearing and foster care on psychopathology at age 12 years in Romania: follow-up of an open, randomised controlled trial. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2015;2:625–634. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00095-4. Ten years after the study was initiated and eight years after the RCT was completed, boys randomized to foster care rater than care as usual showed significant reductions in signs of externalizing disorders. Children who had remained in their original foster care placements showed significantly fewer signs of internalizing and externalizing disorders. This emphasizes the importance of stability in addition to quality of foster care as an intervention. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *35.Humphreys KL, McGoron L, Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA, Fox NA, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH. High quality foster care mitigates callous-unemotional traits following severe early deprivation. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2015;54:977–983. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.09.010. At 12 years, boys randomized to foster care showed sigificantly fewer callous unemotional traits than boys randomized to care as usual. This is the first study to demonstrate that altering early experience can lead to prevention of callous unemotional traits in late childhood. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Smyke AT, Zeanah CH, Gleason MM, Drury SS, Fox NA, Nelson CA, Guthrie DG. A randomized controlled trial comparing foster care and institutional care for children with signs of reactive attachment disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;169:508–514. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11050748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *37.Humphreys KL, Nelson CA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH. Signs of reactive attachment disorder and disinhibited social engagement disorder at age 12 years: Effects of institutional care history and high-quality foster care. Development and Psychopathology. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000256. in press. At age 12 years, children randomized to foster care showed fewer signs of reactive attachment disorder and disinhibited social engagement disorder. This study demonstrates that an early alteration in the caregiving environment leads to longterm reductions in signs of attachment disorders. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sheridan MA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, McLaughlin KA, Nelson CA. Variation in neural development as a result of exposure to institutionalization early in childhood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109:12927–12932. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1200041109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bick J, Zhu T, Stamoulis C, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Effect of early institutionalization and foster care on long-term white matter development: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatrics. 2015;169:211–219. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.3212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Vanderwert RE, Marshall PJ, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA. Timing of intervention affects brain electrical activity in children exposed to severe psychosocial neglect. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:1–5. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *41.Vanderwert R, Fox NA, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH. Normalization of EEG activity among previously institutionalized children placed into foster care: A 12-year follow-up of the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2016;17:68–75. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.12.004. At age 12 years, children originally randomized to foster care showed significantly greater EEG power in the alpha frequency and reduced power in the theta frequency than children randomized to care as usual. Children in stable BEIP placements also showed greater power in the beta frequency. These results indicate that altering early experience enhances brain functioning, especially if quality caregiving is sustained. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • **42.McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Tibu F, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Causal effects of the early caregiving environment on development of stress response systems in children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015;112:5637–5642. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1423363112. This study demonstrated healthier parasympathetic nervous system and hypothalamic-pituitary axis responses to laboratory stressors at 12 years of age in children randomized to foster care rather than care as usual. The results were explained by children placed prior to 18–24 months of age, underscoring the urgency of early placement of children into adequate caregiving environments. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. A matter of timing: Enhancing positive change for the developing brain. Journal of Zero to Three: National Center for Infants, Toddlers and Families. 2014;34:4–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Zeanah CH, Gunnar MR, McCall RB, Kreppner JM, Fox NA. Sensitive periods. Children without permanent parents: Research, practice, and policy. In: McCall RB, van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F, Groark CJ, Groza VK, editors. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. Vol. 76. 2011. pp. 147–162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.McGoron L, Gleason MM, Smyke AT, Drury SD, Nelson CA, Gregas MC, Fox NA, Zeanah CH. Recovering from early deprivation: Attachment mediates effects of caregiving on psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2012;51:683–693. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.McLaughlin KA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Nelson CA. Attachment security as a mechanism linking foster care placement to improved mental health outcomes in previously institutionalized children. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry. 2012;53:46–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02437.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Almas AN, Degnan KA, Walker OL, Radulescu A, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA. The effects of early institutionalization and foster care intervention on children’s social behaviors at age 8. Social Development. 2015;24:225–239. doi: 10.1111/sode.12089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Rutter M, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Beckett C, Castle J, Kreppner J, Kumsta R, Stevens S, Bell C, Gunnar M. Deprivation-specific psychological patterns: Effects of institutional deprivation. Monographs of Research in Child Development. 2010;75:1–252. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2010.00550.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES