Table 1.
Task | Conditions included | Findings and effect sizes in Ophir et al. (2009) | P(rep) Exp. 1 |
P(rep) Exp. 2 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Change detection | Memory set of 2, with 0, 2, 4, or 6 distractors | Interaction of group (LMM vs. HMM) and number of distractors for Memory Set Size 2 condition (f = .34; d = .68): HMMs showed a decline in performance with increasing numbers of distractors; LMMs did not | .95 | .97 |
Memory set of 4, with 0, 2, or 4 distractors | ||||
Memory set of 6, with 0 or 2 distractors | No analyses reported for conditions with 4 and 6 targets | |||
Memory set of 8, with 0 distractors | No significant difference in memory capacity of HMMs and LMMs in comparison of memory sets of 2, 4, 6, and 8 items, without distractors | |||
AX-CPT | With vs. without distractors | Significant interaction of group (LMM vs. HMM) and distractors (present vs. absent) for response times: HMMs slower to respond to target (d = 1.19) and nontarget (d = 1.19) probes only in the condition with distractors | .86 | .76 |
.86 | .76 | |||
N-back task | 2-back vs. 3-back | Interaction of Group (LMM vs. HMM) × Condition (2-back vs. 3-back) for false-alarm rate, with HMMs showing a stronger increase in false alarms as memory load increased from 2-back to 3-back (f = .42; d = .84) | .95 | .92 |
Task switching: number-letter | Task-repeat and task-switch trials | HMMs showed significantly slower response times for both switch (d = .97) and repeat (d = .83) trials and a larger switch cost (d = .96) | .72 | .80 |
.60 | .69 | |||
.71 | .79 | |||
Stop-signal task | Not specified | No analyses reported, but Ophir et al. did mention there was no significant difference between LMMs and HMMs | ||
Stroop task | Not specified | No analyses reported | ||
Task switching | Not specified | No analyses reported |
Note. LMM = light media multitaskers; HMM = heavy media multitaskers; d = effect size in Cohen’s d for the effects reported by Ophir et al.; P(rep) = acquired replication power for our replication tests with α = .05