Recall |
Experimental result |
Mechanism |
|
Low frequency words and high frequency words are equally likely to be recalled. |
Links from episodic context to lexical or semantic representations of words are independent of frequency |
|
Low frequency phrases and high frequency phrases are under some conditions, equally likely to be recalled at least in part |
Links from episodic context to lexical or semantic representations of words (and therefore phrases) are independent of frequency |
|
High frequency phrases are more likely than low frequency phrases to be completed once one word has been recalled |
Associations between the words within the lexical-semantic system are stronger in high frequency phrases |
|
Concrete phrases are easier to recall than abstract phrases |
Concrete phrases have more active features, so the associations between a new episode and a concrete phrase is stronger |
|
Recognition (Jacobs et al., 2016) |
Experimental result |
Mechanism |
|
Low frequency words are better discriminated than high frequency words |
Studied high frequency words suffer from more interference from prior episodes |
|
High frequency phrases get more “yes” responses regardless of whether they were studied or not (a bias) |
Associations between the words within the lexical-semantic system are stronger in high frequency phrases, contributing to greater familiarity |
|
High and low frequency phrases are equally well discriminated |
There are many more episodes sharing a word in a phrase than the whole phrase. Thus, interference from other phrase episodes is minimal. |
|
Low frequency words facilitate phrase discrimination |
Compositional phrases access word episodes, so high frequency words within phrases generate more interference just as they do in recognition for single words |
|
Concrete phrases are better discriminated than abstract phrases |
Concrete phrases have more active features, so the associations between a new episode and a concrete phrase are stronger |