Abstract
Most of our knowledge of wild chimpanzee behaviour stems from fewer than 10 long-term field sites. This bias limits studies to a potentially unrepresentative set of communities known to show great behavioural diversity on small geographic scales. Here, we introduce a new genetic approach to bridge the gap between behavioural material evidence in unhabituated chimpanzees and genetic advances in the field of primatology. The use of DNA analyses has revolutionised archaeological and primatological fields, whereby extraction of DNA from non-invasively collected samples allows researchers to reconstruct behaviour without ever directly observing individuals. We used commercially available forensic DNA kits to show that termite-fishing by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) leaves behind detectable chimpanzee DNA evidence on tools. We then quantified the recovered DNA, compared the yield to that from faecal samples, and performed an initial assessment of mitochondrial and microsatellite markers to identify individuals. From 49 termite-fishing tools from the Issa Valley research site in western Tanzania, we recovered an average of 52 pg/μl chimpanzee DNA, compared to 376.2 pg/μl in faecal DNA extracts. Mitochondrial DNA haplotypes could be assigned to 41 of 49 tools (84%). Twenty-six tool DNA extracts yielded >25 pg/μl DNA and were selected for microsatellite analyses; genotypes were determined with confidence for 18 tools. These tools were used by a minimum of 11 individuals across the study period and termite mounds. These results demonstrate the utility of bio-molecular techniques and a primate archaeology approach in non-invasive monitoring and behavioural reconstruction of unhabituated primate populations.
Introduction
Genetic studies that target the evolutionary history of hominin individuals, groups and species have revolutionised human archaeology over the past decade. These studies have revealed the existence of previously unknown taxa [1] and demonstrated both genetic diversity [2] and migration patterns [3] that would otherwise remain unknown. As this work has progressed, data from genomic and more recently proteomic approaches have been tied back to the tools that accompany the skeletal archaeological record [4]. These new approaches provide a richer context for understanding the suite of behavioural changes that took place in the Late Pleistocene hominin lineage.
The general absence of skeletal remains from past populations of African great apes [5] currently precludes the use of the same molecular approach to reconstruct ancient ape evolution. However, a modern-day analogy presents itself in the numerous great ape populations that remain unhabituated to human presence, and therefore whose behaviour goes undescribed and more broadly, undetected. For wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), for example, of over about 150,000 remaining individuals, our cumulative dataset is comprised from fewer than ten medium or long-term sites [6]. This bias has limited studies of wild chimpanzees to an unrepresentative subset of communities that are known to show great behavioural diversity on small geographic scales.
Three different approaches have been used to increase information on unhabituated wild chimpanzee populations through non-invasive monitoring. First, genetic data from living apes have allowed reconstruction of past population sizes and interbreeding events leading to the current species [7–10]. Non-invasive genetic sampling methods also have been developed to provide information on chimpanzee populations where direct observations of behaviour are impossible or very rare. These methods have led to a better understanding of ranging patterns, population estimates [11,12], and kin relationships [13–15].
A second approach involves the recording of material evidence, such as abandoned tools or information on nests, from chimpanzee home ranges. This technique has proven useful for assessing the behavioural repertoire of wild chimpanzee populations [16–23]. However, in both of these approaches the behaviour of individual group members cannot typically be reconstructed, and potential diversity of behaviours between individuals is therefore lost. Where such behaviours involve tool use, we cannot, for example, assess tool selection or modification practices at an individual level, leaving questions of social influence and traditions unanswerable [24].
A third approach of camera trapping at potential tool use sites does allow for learning about tool use behaviours at the individual level in unhabituated communities [25]. However, the field of view of the camera can miss individuals that are present, and individuals are not always identifiable depending on the images captured [26].
Here, we introduce a new method that bridges the gap between behaviour and material evidence in unhabituated chimpanzees and the application of genetic advances to the field of primatology. Our analysis focuses on genetic evidence recovered from wild Eastern chimpanzee (P. t. schweinfurthii) termite-fishing tools. As chimpanzees mouth these plant tools to remove termites, epithelial cells are transferred to the tools in a manner that resembles mouth swabs used in human genetic sampling. The chimpanzees in this study live in the mosaic woodlands of the Issa Valley in western Tanzania (Fig 1), and direct observation of their behaviour is less reliable than at longer-term sites where chimpanzees are well habituated to the presence of human observers and typically occupy smaller, forest dominated home ranges [27]. In this study, we aimed to (i) identify whether termite fishing leaves behind detectable chimpanzee DNA evidence, (ii) quantify the DNA recovered and compare it to other recovery techniques (e.g., faecal sampling), and (iii) perform an initial assessment of the feasibility of using mitochondrial and microsatellite markers to determine the minimum number of tool users in our sample. The results help establish a new approach combining molecular and archaeological methods to reconstructions of primate behaviour.
Methods
Study site
Samples were collected at the Issa Valley research site in western Tanzania (S 5.50°, E 30.56°) between October 2014 and January 2015. Permission to conduct research at Issa and export samples was granted by the Tanzanian Wildlife Research Institute (TAWIRI) and Commission for Science and Technology (COSTECH). Samples were imported to the UK under (DEFRA) permit TARP/2014/236. Issa lies approximately 90 km east of Lake Tanganyika, within the Greater Mahale Ecosystem (GME). The GME is characterized by broad valleys separated by steep mountains and flat plateaus ranging from 900 to 2100 m elevation. The vegetation is dominated by miombo woodland (Brachystegia and Julbernardia, Fabaceae), although it also includes swamp and grassland, as well as evergreen gallery and thicket riverine forests. There are two distinct seasons: wet (October–April) and dry (May–September), with dry months defined as having <100 mm of rainfall. Termite fishing (Macrotermes spp.) occurs predominantly during the wet season [27]. Rainfall averages 1250 mm per annum [28], and temperatures range from 11 to 35°C [27]. Chimpanzees in Issa are partially habituated, and research focuses on an 85 km2 study area. Preliminary genetic analyses identified at least 67 individuals, including 31 females and 27 males (and 9 individuals that could not be sexed definitively), within the Issa community [29]. As of December 2016, eleven chimpanzees were individually recognizable to researchers.
Sample collection
Thirty termite mounds were monitored three to four times weekly for chimpanzee activity. All new termite-fishing probes that were abandoned in situ where they had been inserted into a mound were collected for DNA analyses. Each tool was collected using a new pair of gloves. A 5 cm segment of the end of the tool that was inserted for fishing was cut using scissors sterilized with bleach and ethanol between each use and preserved in 5 ml of RNA later (Life Technologies). Faecal samples were collected from within the Issa chimpanzee study area between 2009 and 2012. They were collected from trails when tracking chimpanzee parties, or from beneath chimpanzee night nests, and preserved in an equal volume of RNAlater.
All samples were kept frozen at -18°C on site in a solar powered DC freezer (Model number ARB, 47L), before shipment at room temperature. Termite tools were shipped to the University of Cambridge in January 2015 and stored at -20°C, whilst chimpanzee faeces were shipped several times yearly to the University of Pennsylvania and stored at -80°C.
DNA extraction and quantification
DNA was extracted from the chimpanzee tools using two commercially available forensic DNA kits: the QIAamp® DNA Investigator Kit for use with the Qiacube system and the DNA IQ™ Casework Pro Kit for Maxwell 16®. Extractions took place inside a UV sterilised laminar flow hood and gloves were changed between samples to prevent human contamination. All consumable plastics used were single use only and scissors/ forceps were sterilised in an auto-clave in order to prevent cross-contamination between chimpanzee samples. In each case, DNA was extracted from a 1 cm section of each tool following the manufacturer’s protocol for trace DNA extraction from a solid substrate, and approximately 60 μl and 50 μl, respectively, of DNA was eluted. We alternated the extraction method that was used for the first (tip) or second 1cm section of tool. DNA extraction from faeces followed previously described methods [30,31]. Approximately 0.7 ml RNAlater preserved stool was used in each extraction.
Real-time quantitative PCR (qPCR) followed methods described by Morin et al. [32] to amplify an 81bp portion of the chimpanzee c-myc gene of all samples. qPCR amplification of chimpanzee DNA from termite tools and faeces was performed in a 7500 RT PCR system (Applied biosystems) and triplicate sets of size standards of known DNA concentration and negative controls were included with each set of samples prepared with a single qPCR reaction mix. Analyses were conducted in DataAssist™ Software and were checked using standard curves and calculations in Microsoft Excel. Chimpanzee DNA quantification was performed for each DNA extract (QIAamp and DNA IQ) in quadruplicate to calculate an average quantity of chimpanzee DNA per sample.
Mitochondrial genotyping
A 498 bp fragment of the mitochondrial (mt) D-loop region was amplified from termite tool DNA extracts using primers L15996-M13RpUC[33] and H16498-M13F[34]. PCR amplification was performed in a total volume of 25 μl consisting of 3 μl template, 1x PCR buffer, 3mM MgCl2, 200 μM of each dNTP, 1μg BSA, 1 μM each primer, and 1.25 U Bioline Taq polymerase. Amplification conditions were: initial denaturation at 94°C for 1 min, 40 cycles of 20s at 94°C, 30s at 55°C, 1 min at 72°C, and a final extension at 72°C for 10 min. Where amplification was unsuccessful a nested protocol was used by first amplifying the whole D-loop using outside primers L15926 [35] and CEH5 [36] followed by inside amplification as described above, excluding BSA. Products were separated on a 1% agarose gel (100 V, 15 min) and visualized using Et Br and UV light. PCR products were sent for sequencing at Macrogen (Korea). Independent PCR products of each tool extraction were sequenced in both directions and sequences were aligned using the Clustal W function in MEGA 5.1. Termite tool derived sequences were subjected to phylogenetic analyses to identify distinct mtDNA haplotypes, all of which matched previously identified haplotypes from this same region [29].
Microsatellite genotyping
Only samples with more than 25 pg/μl were selected for microsatellite genotyping, as previous research has shown that reactions containing <100pg/rxn require seven replicates to determine genotypes with high confidence, whilst 101-200pg/rxn requires four and >201pg/rxn requires only two replicates for similar confidence [32]. Two microsatellite loci known to be variable in this population (d5s1457 and d2s1326) [12] and one locus known to be variable in chimpanzees in general (d1s550) [37] were amplified from termite-fishing tool DNA extracts in a single-step multiplex PCR reaction.
PCR amplification was carried out in a total volume of 9 μl, consisting of up to 4 μl of template, 4.5 μl of 2X Qiagen multiplex mastermix, 200nM of each primer, and 60ng BSA. Amplification conditions were: initial denaturation at 95°C for 15 min, 40 cycles of 95°C for 30s, 60°C for 90s, 72°C for 60s, and a final extension of 30 min at 72°C. The 5’ end of the forward primer was fluorescently labeled, and products were separated using capillary electrophoresis (ABI 3730xl DNA analyser). Alleles were then sized relative to a size standard (HD400 labeled ROX) using Geneious 7.1.9 with microsatellite plugin. A modified multiple-tubes approach was adopted based on the quantity of DNA in each extract. Following Morin et al. [32], two replicates of two alleles were required to call a heterozygous locus, four replicates were required to call a homozygous locus if there was 100-200pg/rxn and two replicates if there was >200pg/rxn.
Analyses
We estimated allelic dropout following Gushanski et al. [38], by dividing the total number of allelic dropouts observed by the total number of successful heterozygous reactions, across all loci. Ideally, a larger number of microsatellite loci would be used to determine individual identity with more certainty; however, three loci are sufficient for testing the feasibility of microsatellite genotyping of termite-fishing tool DNA extracts. In order to determine a minimum number of individuals represented from the tool samples and any potential matching genotypes we used CERVUS 3.0 [39] to perform an identity analysis and assess the probability of full siblings or unrelated individuals having an identical genotype (pIDsib and pID).
Results
Sample collection
Twelve of the thirty monitored termite mounds were actively fished during the monitoring period, and tools were collected for DNA analysis from six mounds. We collected 49 tools on 11 occasions between November 2014 and January 2015 (Table 1; Fig 1). Between one and six tools were collected from each fishing episode. Five out of the six mounds were fished on more than one occasion. All tools were made of stripped bark and averaged 410±230 mm in length and 5±2.6 mm in width, conforming to initial descriptions of termite tool characteristics at Issa [27]. Of 450 faecal samples collected from Issa between 2009 and 2012, 313 were subjected to DNA extraction and quantification.
Table 1. Genetic identity (ID), genotype and mtDNA haplotype for each tool.
Date | Tool Number | Termite Mound | mtDNA haplotype* | Individual genetic ID | d5s1457 | d1s550 | d2s1326 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
17/10/14 | 1 | 4503 | GM7 | ||||
17/10/14 | 2 | 4503 | GM7 | ||||
01/11/14 | 1 | 4862 | GM7 | 1 | 110/114 | 154/166 | 210/218 |
01/11/14 | 2 | 4862 | UG59 | 2 | 114/114 | 150/154 | 206/218 |
01/11/14 | 1 | 5298 | GM7 | 3 | 110/114 | 154/154 | 206/210 |
01/11/14 | 2 | 5298 | GM7 | ||||
01/11/14 | 3 | 5298 | GM7 | ||||
07/11/14 | 1 | 5298 | GM7 | 4 | 102/114 | 150/150 | 218/218 |
07/11/14 | 2 | 5298 | GM7 | 5 | 110/114 | 166/166 | 206/206 |
07/11/14 | 3 | 5298 | GM7 | 6 | 102/114 | 150/150 | 210/218 |
07/11/14 | 4 | 5298 | GM7 | ||||
09/11/14 | 2 | 5298 | GM7 | 6 | 102/114 | 150/150 | 210/218 |
09/11/14 | 3 | 5298 | MH37 | 7? | 114/122 | 150/150 | 210/218 |
09/11/14 | 4 | 5298 | MH37 | 7 | 114/122 | 150/154 | 210/218 |
09/11/14 | 5 | 5298 | MH37 | 8 | 106/114 | 150/154 | 214/218 |
09/11/14 | 6 | 5298 | UG59 | 9 | 114/114 | 150/170 | 210/210 |
11/11/14 | 1 | 5298 | UG59 | 10 | 106/114 | 150/154 | 206/218 |
11/11/14 | 2 | 5298 | UG59 | 11 | 106/126 | 150/154 | 206/218 |
11/11/14 | 3 | 5298 | UG59 | 2 | 114/114 | 150/154 | 206/218 |
19/11/14 | 1 | 4502 | GM7 | ||||
19/11/14 | 1 | 5298 | GM7 | 12 | 102/114 | 170/170 | 206/218 |
21/11/14 | 1 | 4030 | GM7 | 6 | 102/114 | 150/150 | 210/218 |
21/11/14 | 2 | 4030 | GM7 | 6 | 102/114 | 150/150 | 210/218 |
30/11/14 | 1 | 4030 | UG59 | 13 | 106/126 | 154/154 | 206/218 |
30/11/14 | 3 | 4030 | UG59 | ||||
30/11/14 | 1 | 4502 | UG59 | ||||
17/12/14 | 1 | 4769 | UG59 | ||||
17/12/14 | 2 | 4769 | UG59 | 2 | 114/114 | 150/154 | 206/218 |
17/12/14 | 4 | 4769 | UG59 | ||||
17/12/14 | 1 | 5298 | GM7 | ||||
19/12/14 | 1 | 5298 | UG59 | 14? | 102/114 | 154/166 | 186/218 |
08/01/15 | 1 | 4502 | UG59 | ||||
08/01/15 | 3 | 4502 | GM7 | ||||
08/01/15 | 5 | 4502 | UG59 | 2? | 114/114 | 150/154 | 206/218 |
08/01/15 | 6 | 4502 | UG59 | ||||
08/01/15 | 1 | 4769 | UG59 | 15 | 102/114 | 150/150 | 210/218 |
08/01/15 | 2 | 4769 | GM7 | 6 | 102/114 | 150/150 | 210/218 |
08/01/15 | 3 | 4769 | GM7 | 6 | 102/114 | 150/150 | 210/218 |
08/01/15 | 1 | 5298 | UG59 | ||||
08/01/15 | 2 | 5298 | GM7 | 16 | 102/114 | 154/154 | 186/218 |
08/01/15 | 3 | 5298 | UG59 | 14 | 102/114 | 154/166 | 186/218 |
Shaded cells contain unconfirmed genotypes (with fewer than necessary replicates per DNA quantity, see methods) and blank cells represent no data.
? These IDs are possible matches, but genotypes remain unconfirmed.
Chimpanzee DNA quantification
The average chimpanzee DNA concentration found in tool DNA extracts was 52 pg/μl (range 0–2080; Fig 2). There was no difference in chimpanzee DNA quantity between DNA extracted using the QIAamp® DNA Investigator kit or the DNA IQ™ Casework Pro Kit (Wilcoxon’s matched pairs V = 704, p = 0.14). There was no difference in chimpanzee DNA quantity between first and second extracts of each tool (Wilcoxon’s rank sum test; W = 793.5, p = 0.24). Extracts with <5 pg/μl chimpanzee DNA were considered unusable for microsatellite analyses [32] and 37% of all extracts had <5 pg/μl chimpanzee DNA (15 or 31% of QIAamp and 21 or 43% of IQ™ Casework extractions).
The extractions with <5pg/μl chimpanzee DNA did not result from the same tools. For example, nine samples yielding unusable extracts with the QIAamp® Kit (mean 2.2 pg/μl), yielded usable extracts with the DNA IQ™ Kit (mean 31 pg/μl) and three samples yielding unusable extracts with the DNA IQ™ Kit (mean 2.9 pg/μl), yielded usable extracts with the QIAamp® Kit (mean 11.8 pg/μl). In comparison, the average chimpanzee DNA concentration found in faeces extracts was 376.2 pg/μl (range 0–4867; Fig 1) and only 16 samples, or 5% of extractions, yielded <5 pg/μl of chimpanzee DNA.
Mitochondrial DNA
PCR amplification success rate for HV1 mitochondrial DNA from tool samples was 86% (197 of 228 PCRs). There was no difference in amplification success rate between DNA extracted using the QIAamp® DNA Investigator kit (88% or 88 of 104 PCRs) or the DNA IQ™ Casework Pro Kit (85% or 109 of 124 PCRs). Extracts that yielded mtDNA HV1 sequences had significantly more DNA (mean 66.7 pg/μL, range 0–2080 pg/μl) than those that did not (mean 2.8, 0–20.6 pg/μl; Wilcoxon’s rank sum test, W = 209, p<0.0001). mtDNA haplotypes could be assigned to 41 of 49 tools (Table 1). All tools had one of three haplotypes known for this community of chimpanzees [29] and the wider GME region [40].
Microsatellite genotyping
Twenty-six tool DNA extracts yielded >25 pg/μl chimpanzee DNA and were selected for microsatellite analyses. All loci amplified in at least one replicate per extract. Results were attained for between two and seven replicates for each tool, which allowed genotypes to be determined with confidence for 18 of 26 tools (Table 1). The extent of dropout across all heterozygous loci was 8%, and only one case (0.06%) of irreproducible sporadic alleles was observed. Dropout was not observed in samples with more than 235pg/rxn (Fig 3).
Identity analysis in CERVUS was used to find matching genotypes within the 18 confirmed genotypes. These tools represent a minimum of 11 different individuals; two genotypes occurred more than once, representing three and six tool samples used by two different individuals (Table 1; IDs 2 and 6 respectively). If unconfirmed genotypes are included in the analysis, the 26 tools were used by a minimum of 16 individuals, and four tools could potentially have been used by a further two different individuals (Table 1; IDs 7 and 14). However, the theoretical probability of two full siblings or two unrelated individuals sharing the same genotype at three loci was high (pIDsib = 0.15, pID = 0.021) given the limited number of loci genotyped.
Tools re-used by the two confirmed individuals were similar in length, but may differ in width; ID 2 tools averaged 400±260 mm in length and 6.3±3.2 mm in width, whilst ID 6 tools averaged 430±150 mm in length and 3.8±1.2 mm in width.
Discussion
This study presents the first successful extraction, mitochondrial DNA amplification, and microsatellite genotyping of DNA from material artefacts left behind by wild chimpanzees. These results demonstrate the utility of molecular techniques and a primate archaeology approach [41] in non-invasive monitoring and behavioural reconstruction of unhabituated primate populations. Our findings are the first step in linking non-invasive genetic techniques, traditionally used with extant non-human primates, to the study of material culture remains left from hominin landscape use. Our results validate a genetic approach to the recovery of otherwise unobtainable behavioural information directly related to tool use by wild animals.
Our monitoring program identified and recovered chimpanzee termite-fishing tools from multiple mounds, and on multiple occasions from single mounds. Genetic identification of tool users opens up opportunities to record unobserved chimpanzee behaviour, including inter-site ranging and regularity of site visits. They also allow us to begin cataloguing individual chimpanzee preferences for tool size, shape and material, and to do so over any time period during the life of the animal. For example, the tools of two individuals identified in this study show a difference in the width of tools manufactured. A longitudinal study with a larger sample size would determine whether this difference is significant.
The mtDNA analysis placed all recovered genetic material into known haplotypes of the study community [26] and region [40]. This result is unsurprising; however we anticipate that if future tool collection expands into less well-studied parts of the GME, we will begin to see additional haplotypes. Ultimately, we expect that we will be able to assess whether there is a potential long-lasting maternal influence on tool forms, by comparing data on tool material selection and manufacture between the different mitochondrial lineages, akin to what has recently been reported for hand-clasping in Ugandan chimpanzees [42]. Our initial hypothesis would be that the patrilocal nature of chimpanzee society, coupled to the sustained cultural variation seen between neighbouring groups [43], would act to diffuse any matriline-specific technological attributes, but at this point that remains an open question.
Tool transfers do occur between wild chimpanzees; Mothers have been observed to share tools with their offspring [25], adult tool sharing has been observed in a similarly dry site [44], and chimpanzees have been seen to sniff and inspect tools found on mounds. Despite this, we found no evidence of cross-contamination in our study. Such tool transfers might not occur at Issa, or might occur at a rate too low to have been detected here in a small sample of tools. Cross-contamination by multiple chimpanzees using the same tool could therefore be an issue to be addressed in future research using this method.
Our microsatellite analysis is preliminary, but demonstrates feasibility of identifying tool users directly from the tool. If we assume that genotypes matching at three loci represent the same individual, we found that one individual (ID 6 in Table 1) may have used up to six of the tools recovered during this study. These tools were collected from three different termite mounds, across a period of two months. Archaeologically, this ability to track an individual’s use of specific tools over that time is the equivalent of attempting to track the products of an individual stone knapper at a Palaeolithic human site [e.g., 45], but with the added detail and linkages provided by the genetic data. We anticipate that the routine and long-term application of our methods at a single site would reveal the links between social and genetic influences on tool selection and modification at a level that is currently unobtainable. We expect that the extension of this work to habituated wild chimpanzee sites would yield a similar increase in knowledge, as even these sites cannot directly observe and collect all evidence of tool use all the time [46]. Genetic analysis of tools would complement well studies of tool use in habituated and unhabituated apes using camera traps (e.g. [25]), as observation and video of tool use provides information about age classes not obtainable from genetics.
The chimpanzee DNA quantity in our tool extracts (52 pg/μl) was lower than that found in commonly used DNA extracts from faeces, but was significantly higher than that of shed hair samples (4.4pg/μl; Morin et al., 2001). Likewise, although a large proportion of tool DNA extractions (37%) contained <5pg/μl chimpanzee DNA, this is lower than 79% of extractions of shed hair found by Morin et al [32] to contain 0pg/μl chimpanzee DNA. Hair extractions typically use only a single hair follicle, in order to be certain that only one individual is sampled. Differences in chimpanzee DNA quantity may be influenced by the amount of starting material, or due to greater sensitivity of forensic techniques, or tools may actually be a better source of chimpanzee DNA than shed hairs. RNAlater might also improve the yield of DNA extractions, as we found a much higher concentration of DNA in extractions from faeces than Morin et al. [32] (192 pg/μl), who preserved faeces in silica gel and used 100mg dried faeces for DNA extraction. The Issa environment is also much drier than that of the Tai forest, where Morin et al. [32] collected samples. Such a difference in environment, or possible differences in the time since deposition to collection of samples, could influence DNA yield. Tools are also beneficial as a DNA source as they provide more than one opportunity to extract DNA to potentially improve yield, similar to faeces, whereas hair samples allow only a single extraction. The two forensic kits that we employed were found to be equally reliable, with no difference in the DNA quantity extracted nor in ease of implementation. Both kits also have a very similar cost per microliter of extracted DNA of around 3.5 μl/£1.
A logical next step for this work would be to identify non-chimpanzee activities that similarly may result in individual DNA preservation on tools. Human ancestors, for example, used a wide variety of plant and stone tools in the past, and while the perishable nature of much material culture necessarily places restrictions on that search, DNA recovery from human stone tools is possible [47]. Similarly, stone tools have been recovered that were used by past groups of capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) and long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea) to open encased foods [48,49], and populations of orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) are habitual tool users that extensively orally manipulate plant tools [50]. We recognise that the technique introduced here is likely to be more effective for some tool types and tasks than others, but we also believe that the ultimate benefits of successfully linking genetic and technological records are sufficient to warrant the continued exploration of its potential.
Acknowledgments
We thank the Tanzanian Wildlife Research Institute (TAWIRI) and Commission for Science and Technology (COSTECH) for permission to carry out research in Tanzania, field assistants from the Ugalla Primate Project (UPP) for support with data collection, Noemie Bónnin for research assistance, and Lilian Pintea (JGI) for providing vegetation classification. Finally, we thank Mimi Arandjelovic and an anonymous reviewer for their valuable feedback on the manuscript.
Data Availability
All data files are available from the Dryad Digital Repository at DOI: 10.5061/dryad.h39n1.
Funding Statement
Long-term support for the Ugalla Primate Project is provided to AKP and FAS by the University of California, San Diego/Salk Institute Center for Academic Research and Training in Anthropogeny (CARTA; https://carta.anthropogeny.org). This research was funded by an European Research Council Starting Investigator Grant #283959 (Primate Archaeology; https://erc.europa.eu) awarded to MH and National Institutes of Health grants (R01 AI 120810 and R37 AI050529; https://www.nih.gov) awarded to BH. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Reich D, Green RE, Kircher M, Krause J, Patterson N, Durand E, et al. Genetic history of an archaic hominin group from Denisova Cave in Siberia. Nature. 2010;468: 1053–1060. doi: 10.1038/nature09710 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Sánchez-Quinto F, Lalueza-Fox C. Almost 20 years of Neanderthal palaeogenetics: adaptation, admixture, diversity, demography and extinction. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2015;370: 20130374 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0374 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Malaspinas A, Westaway MC, Muller C, Sousa VC, Lao O, Alves I, et al. A genomic history of Aboriginal Australia. Nature. Nature Publishing Group; 2016; 1–20. doi: 10.1038/nature18299 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Welker F, Hajdinjak M, Talamo S, Jaouen K, Dannemann M, David F. Palaeoproteomic evidence identifies archaic hominins associated with the Châtelperronian at the Grotte du Renne. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016; doi: 10.1073/pnas.1605834113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.McBrearty S, Jablonski N. First fossil chimpanzee. Nature. 2005;437: 105–108. doi: 10.1038/nature04008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.McGrew WC. Field studies of Pan troglodytes reviewed and comprehensively mapped, focussing on Japan’s contribution to cultural primatology Primates. Springer; Japan; 2016; 1–22. doi: 10.1007/s10329-016-0554-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hvilsom C, Rasmus Heller FC, Jaffré N, Siegismund HR. Contrasting demographic histories of the neighboring bonobo and chimpanzee. Primates. 2014;55: 101–112. doi: 10.1007/s10329-013-0373-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Prufer K, Munch K, Hellman I, Akagi K, Miller J, Walenz B, et al. The bonobo genome compared with the chimpanzee and human genomes. Nature. 2012;486: 527–531. doi: 10.1038/nature11128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Scally A, Dutheil JY, Hillier LW, Jordan GE, Goodhead I, Herrero J, et al. Insights into hominid evolution from the gorilla genome sequence. Nature. Nature Publishing Group; 2012;483: 169–175. doi: 10.1038/nature10842 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.de Manuel M, Kuhlwilm M, Frandsen P, Sousa VC, Desai T, Prado-Martinez J, et al. Chimpanzee genomic diversity reveals ancient admixture with bonobos. Science. 2016;354: 477 LP-481. doi: 10.1126/science.aag2602 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Arandjelovic M, Head J, Rabanal LI, Schubert G, Mettke E, Boesch C, et al. Non-invasive genetic monitoring of wild central chimpanzees. PLoS One. 2011;6: e14761 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014761 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Moore DL, Vigilant L. A population estimate of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Ugalla region using standard and spatially explicit genetic capture-recapture methods. Am J Primatol. 2013;76: 335–346. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Moore DL, Langergraber KE, Vigilant L. Genetic Analyses Suggest Male Philopatry and Territoriality in Savanna-Woodland Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of Ugalla, Tanzania. Int J Primatol. 2015; doi: 10.1007/s10764-015-9830-8 [Google Scholar]
- 14.Constable JL, Ashley M V, Goodall J, Pusey a E. Noninvasive paternity assignment in Gombe chimpanzees. Mol Ecol. 2001;10: 1279–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Gagneux P, Wills C, Gerloff U, Tautz D, Morin P a, Boesch C, et al. Mitochondrial sequences show diverse evolutionary histories of African hominoids. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1999;96: 5077–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hicks TC, Fouts RS, Fouts DH. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) tool use in the Ngotto Forest, Central African Republic. Am J Primatol. 2005;65: 221–237. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sanz C, Call J, Morgan D. Design complexity in termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Biol Lett. 2009;5: 293–296. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0786 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Koops K, Schöning C, McGrew WC, Matsuzawa T. Chimpanzees prey on army ants at Seringbara, Nimba Mountains, Guinea: Predation patterns and tool use characteristics. Am J Primatol. 2015;77: 319–329. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22347 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Stewart FA, Piel AK, McGrew WC. Living archaeology: artefacts of specific nest site fidelity in wild chimpanzees. J Hum Evol. Elsevier Ltd; 2011;61: 388–95. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2011.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Wondra EM, Casteren A Van, Pascual-Garrido A, Stewart F, Piel AK. A new report of chimpanzee ant-fishing from the Issa valley, Tanzania. African Primates. 2016;11: 1–18. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Pascual-Garrido A, Umaru B, Allon O, Sommer V. Apes finding ants: Predator–prey dynamics in a chimpanzee habitat in Nigeria. Am J Primatol. 2013;14: 1–14. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Park CN, Carvalho JS, Marques TA, Vicente L. Population Status of Pan troglodytes verus in Lagoas de. PLoS One. 2013;8: e71527 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071527 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Carvalho S, Sousa C, Matsuzawa T. New Nut-Cracking Sites in Diecké Forest, Guinea: An Overview of the Surveys. Pan Africa News. 2007;14: 11–13. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Luncz L V., Boesch C. Tradition over trend: Neighboring chimpanzee communities maintain differences in cultural behavior despite frequent immigration of adult females. Am J Primatol. 2014;76: 649–657. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Musgrave S, Morgan D, Lonsdorf E, Mundry R, Sanz C. Tool transfers are a form of teaching among chimpanzees. Sci Rep. Nature Publishing Group; 2016;6: 34783 doi: 10.1038/srep34783 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Boesch C, Kalan AK, Agbor A, Arandjelovic M, Dieguez P, Lapeyre V, et al. Chimpanzees routinely fish for algae with tools during the dry season in Bakoun, Guinea. Am J Primatol. 2017;79: 1–7. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22613 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Stewart FA, Piel AK. Termite fishing by wild chimpanzees: new data from Ugalla, western Tanzania. Primates. 2014;55: 35–40. doi: 10.1007/s10329-013-0362-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Piel AK, Lenoel A, Johnson C, Stewart FA. Deterring poaching in western Tanzania: The presence of wildlife researchers. Glob Ecol Conserv. 2015;3: 188–199. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Rudicell RS, Piel AK, Stewart F, Moore DL, Learn GH, Li Y, et al. High Prevalence of Simian Immunodeficiency Virus Infection in a Community of Savanna Chimpanzees. J Virol. 2011;85: 9918–9928. doi: 10.1128/JVI.05475-11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Santiago ML, Lukasik M, Kamenya S, Li Y, Bibollet-Ruche F, Bailes E, et al. Foci of Endemic Simian Immunodeficiency Virus Infection in Wild-Living Eastern Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). J Virol. 2003;77: 7545–7562. doi: 10.1128/JVI.77.13.7545-7562.2003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Keele BF, Van Heuverswyn F, Li Y, Bailes E, Takehisa J, Santiago ML, et al. Chimpanzee reservoirs of pandemic and nonpandemic HIV-1. Science. 2006;313: 523–6. doi: 10.1126/science.1126531 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Morin P a, Chambers KE, Boesch C, Vigilant L. Quantitative polymerase chain reaction analysis of DNA from noninvasive samples for accurate microsatellite genotyping of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus). Mol Ecol. 2001;10: 1835–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Vigilant L, Pennington R, Harpending H, Kocher TD, Wilson AC. Mitochondrial DNA sequences in single hairs from a southern African population. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1989;86: 9350–9354. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.23.9350 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kocher TD, Wilson AC. Sequence evolution of mitochondrial DNA in humans and chimpanzees: control region and a protein-coding region In: Osawa SHT, editor. Evolution of Life: Fossils, Molecules and Culture. Tokyo: Springer Verlag; 1991. pp. 391–413. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kocher TD, Thomas WK, Meyer A, Edwards S V., Paabo S, Villablanca FX, et al. Dynamics of mitochondrial DNA evolution in animals: amplification and sequencing with conserved primers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1989;86: 6196–6200. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.16.6196 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Koops K, McGrew WC, Matsuzawa T, Knapp LA. Terrestrial nest-building by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Implications for the tree-to-ground sleep transition in early hominins. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2012;148: 351–361. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.22056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bradley BJ, Boesch C, Vigilant L. Identification and redesign of human microsatellite markers for genotyping wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) and gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) DNA from faeces. Conserv Genet. 2000;1: 289–292. doi: 10.1023/A:1011514007741 [Google Scholar]
- 38.Guschanski K, Vigilant L, McNeilage A, Gray M, Kagoda E, Robbins MM. Counting elusive animals: Comparing field and genetic census of the entire mountain gorilla population of Bwindi Impenetrable National Park, Uganda. Biol Conserv. Elsevier Ltd; 2009;142: 290–300. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2008.10.024 [Google Scholar]
- 39.Kalinowski ST, Taper ML, Marshall TC. Revising how the computer program CERVUS accommodates genotyping error increases success in paternity assignment. Mol Ecol. 2007;16: 1099–1106. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2007.03089.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bonnin N, Piel AK, Ramirez M, Li Y, Loy E, Crystal P, et al. Gene flow and genetic diversity of Tanzania’s Greater Mahale Ecosystem chimpanzees European Federation of Primatology; Rome; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Haslam M. Towards a prehistory of primates. Antiquity. 2012;86: 299–315. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Wrangham RW, Koops K, Machanda ZP, Worthington S, Bernard AB, Brazeau NF, et al. Distribution of a Chimpanzee Social Custom Is Explained by Matrilineal Relationship Rather Than Conformity. Curr Biol. Elsevier Ltd.; 2016;0: 108–114. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Luncz L V, Mundry R, Boesch C. Report Evidence for Cultural Differences between Neighboring Chimpanzee Communities. Curr Biol. Elsevier Ltd; 2012;22: 922–926. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Pruetz JD, Lindshield S. Plant-food and tool transfer among savanna chimpanzees at Fongoli, Senegal. Primates. 2012;53: 133–145. doi: 10.1007/s10329-011-0287-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Eren MI, Bradley BA, Sampson CG. Middle Paleolithic skill level and the individual knapper: An experiemnt. Am Antiq. 2011;76: 229–251. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Carvalho S, Biro D, McGrew WC, Matsuzawa T. Tool-composite reuse in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): archaeologically invisible steps in the technological evolution of early hominins? Anim Cogn. 2009;12 Suppl 1: S103–14. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0271-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kimura B, Brandt S a., Hardy BL, Hauswirth WW. Analysis of DNA from Ethnoarchaeological Stone Scrapers. J Archaeol Sci. 2001;28: 45–53. doi: 10.1006/jasc.1999.0551 [Google Scholar]
- 48.Haslam M, Pascual-Garrido A, Malaivijitnond S, Gumert M. Stone tool transport by wild Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea). J Archaeol Sci Reports. Elsevier Ltd; 2016;7: 408–413. doi: 10.1016/j.jasrep.2016.05.040 [Google Scholar]
- 49.Haslam M, Luncz L, Pascual-Garrido A, Falotico T, Malaivijitnond S, Gumert M. Archaeological excavation of wild macaque stone tools. J Hum Evol. 2016;96: 134–138. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2016.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.van Schaik CP, Fox EA, Sitompol AF. Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans. Naturwissenschaften. 1996;83: 186–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All data files are available from the Dryad Digital Repository at DOI: 10.5061/dryad.h39n1.