Abstract
The manuscript proposes and evaluates a real-time algorithm for estimating eye gaze angle based solely on single-channel electrooculography (EOG), which can be obtained directly from the ear canal using conductive ear moulds. In contrast to conventional high-pass filtering, we used an algorithm that calculates absolute eye gaze angle via statistical analysis of detected saccades. The estimated eye positions of the new algorithm were still noisy. However, the performance in terms of Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients was significantly better than the conventional approach in some instances. The results suggest that in-ear EOG signals captured with conductive ear moulds could serve as a basis for light-weight and portable horizontal eye gaze angle estimation suitable for a broad range of applications. For instance, for hearing aids to steer the directivity of microphones in the direction of the user’s eye gaze.
Introduction
Following a conversation in noisy environments can be challenging for hearing aid users because hearing aids amplify noise together with the target signal. Thus, hearing aids are often equipped with directional microphones, which attenuate background noise and amplify only the signals originating in front of the listener. In a typical conversation, however, the conversational partners can be outside the amplification pattern and the hearing impaired people adopt a strategy to follow a talker with the eyes [1–3]. The hearing devices do not take into account the eye movements; and therefore, it would be desirable that hearing prostheses were able to adapt according to the direction of eye gaze. Some authors have suggested that using eye gaze angle to steer hearing aid directional microphones could be of benefit to a listener [4,5]. How eye gaze is measured, however, remains an open question. The most reliable methods for mobile eye tracking involve cameras mounted on glass frames, but the cameras obstruct the field of view [6], and not every hearing aid user is willing to wear glasses.
A viable candidate for measuring eye gaze angle is electrooculography (EOG) which measures an electrical signal that arises from the rotation of electrically charged eyeballs. Consequently, electrodes placed in the vicinity of the eyes can measure these potentials, and the magnitude of these potentials depend on the eye gaze angle. EOG has many practical applications including wheelchair control [7,8], activity recognition [9,10], retinal function testing [11,12], sleep stage classification [13], or as a general gaze control interface [14–16]. It is also known as an artifact of electroencephalography (EEG) [17]. However, its full potential for hearing aids (or indeed any mobile applications) has not been fully recognized, mainly because the EOG is typically measured by using large obtrusive electrodes that are attached to the sides of the head. Due to the electrical properties of the body, however, EOG can be measured anywhere on the head, although its magnitude varies with the electrode placement. When measured in peri-orbital positions it usually has values 8–33 μV/ 1° of visual angle, and around 3 μV /1° can be measured inside the ear canals [11,18]. This finding suggests that eye movements can be analysed by hearing aids with nothing more than conductive ear-moulds.
Eye movements are seen in the EOG signal as a change of the potential across two electrodes placed either horizontally or vertically around the eyeballs. The analysis of EOG is usually based on detection of saccades, fixations, and blinks [10,14–16,19–21]. Saccades are the most common type of eye movement, and they are characterized by a rapid change of the eye position between two relatively stable fixation points. They produce very distinct patterns in the EOG voltage, which are relatively easy to detect because the deflections have magnitudes which are above the usual high-frequency noise level, and they are short in duration. Microsaccades are tremor-like movements during fixation periods but they produce relatively small EOG signals that are difficult to detect. Other types of eye movements such as smooth pursuit, vestibulo-ocular reflex, vergence movement, nystagmus, or optokinetic reflex could be analysed by EOG, but they are not in the focus of this study.
Saccade detection algorithms often claim near perfect detection rates. However, the performance of these algorithms vary with the quality of the EOG recording, which is influenced by electrode types, the electrode placements, lighting conditions [11], and the degree of physical activity. Most methods are based on the analysis of the derivative of the EOG signal and subsequent classification. The derivative function can be understood as a high-pass (HP) filter with a cut-off frequency proportional to the sampling rate. The output of the derivative is usually very noisy, and therefore various approaches proposed ways to increase the signal to noise ratio. The method [14] used a rule-based algorithm to classify the derivative output as a saccade if the derivative changed the sign. The methods [15,20] employed probabilistic feature-based classification using Gaussian mixture models on the derivative output. The methods [10] and [16], instead of the derivative, analysed parameters of continuous wavelet transformation. The transformation parameters are then used as an input into a neural network or nearest neighbour classifier. Yet another method [19] analysed the second derivate (acceleration) of the EOG signal. The saccades were then detected by thresholding the acceleration values, and the threshold was adapted based on the previous measurements. Although these methods perform well, they have not considered the saccade magnitude as a predictor and using this predictor can possibly improve the performance of the detection algorithm.
Obtaining the saccade magnitude from EOG will, however, require a calibration of the EOG signal to the eye gaze angle [16]. Under ideal conditions, the relationship is straightforward: EOG = constant * sin(eye angle) for all eccentricities, and this linear relationship holds for small and intermediate eccentricities. However, the actual relationship depends on the placement of electrodes, properties of body tissue, the shape of the head and other factors.
Various techniques detect saccades, blinks, and fixations, but only a few estimate the actual eye gaze angle from EOG. For instance, the method [22] estimated eye gaze using a comparison of EOG signals from multiple electrodes in different locations around the eyes. The method took into account the non-linear relationship of the signals from different measurements sites, which enabled it to cancel out the errors. The method reported the accuracy of about 4°. Such an approach, however, is inapplicable to a setting with only one EOG channel. In another work [23], an external video was used to calibrate the system. The method was based on a comparison of the saliency maps [24] and the EOG signal. This technique achieved an estimation error of about 15°. However, this method is also not suitable for applications without an external video source. In summary, the state-of-the-art technology [6] does not provide a solution to estimate the actual gaze position from a single-channel recording. The reason is that EOG has previously been considered for the detection of only relative—not absolute—changes of eye position. In this paper, we aim to challenge this view by assuming that eye position can be restored by integrating past saccades. Such an approach will lead to noisy predictions, but we argue that the level of noise will be acceptable for some applications.
The EOG signal is often polluted by various sources of noise [12], which are usually difficult to eliminate by simple filtering. The most dominant noise component in the EOG is the direct current (DC) drift. The drift can be characterized as a low-frequency noise (less than 1 Hz) with unstable spectral structure and with magnitudes up to several hundred mV. DC drift is inherent to any process involving electrodes attached to the skin [25], and it arises from the imbalance of the half-cell potentials of the two electrodes. When skin conductance changes (e.g., when sweat is released), the salt concentrations of the electrode gels change, and any differences between the two electrodes result in a slowly changing DC potential. The voltage changes that correspond to the actual EOG are small, and they ride on top of this large DC component (Fig 1). Although it is theoretically possible to decrease the drift in laboratory environments [12], this is not expected in real settings. The second largest source of noise is muscle activity [10]. These artifacts are stronger if the electrodes are placed closer to the muscles generating the electrical activity such as eye muscles, facial muscles, jaw muscles, neck muscles, tongue, or limbs. For example, vertical EOG electrodes pick up eye blinks whereas horizontal electrodes are less affected or not affected at all [21]. These artifacts are present in any type of muscle activity, and they can be seen as a broadband noise with magnitudes similar or greater than EOG. Electrical activity of the brain itself has virtually no impact on the EOG because of its very low amplitudes in comparison to the EOG signal. The synchronized activity of specific brain regions could potentially influence EOG, but again only at very low amplitudes and not at the locations where EOG is recorded. Finally, noise may arise from external sources including environmental and powerline noise which are picked up by the wires of the measurement device. These are typically limited to a narrow spectral region (e.g., 50 Hz or 60 Hz) and can easily be overcome by notch filters. Although the noise negatively impacts the quality of the EOG signal, transient events, such as saccades, are less error-prone to external noise due to their transitory nature. Saccade detection is therefore relatively reliable, even if the signal is corrupted.
The primary aim of this manuscript is to determine whether eye gaze direction can be estimated from the in-ear EOG recordings. Specifically, we aim to test a saccade integration algorithm and compare it to the output of the high-pass filtering approach. The saccade integration algorithm is a novel approach to estimate the actual eye gaze angle. It relies on the assumption that the variance of the eye gaze direction can be explained mainly by the saccadic movement, and only to a small extent by other types of eye movements. In this work, the saccade integration scheme has the following assumptions: 1) every eye movement can be characterized as an instantaneous step-like change of location, and all other types of eye movements can be ignored, 2) the eye is perfectly still during the fixation period, 3) there exists an approximately linear relationship on a short time scale between the change of the EOG signal and the change of the eye position [16], 4) noise related to the estimation of saccade magnitude has a normal distribution, 5) eye gaze is constrained by physical limitations, and 6) the head remains still. In future, the sixth assumption could be omitted, and the information about head movements could be used to enhance the estimation of eye gaze angle [26]. However, in this initial work, we decided to keep the head fixed. The model prediction is that the performance of the integration scheme will be better than working directly on the HP-filtered EOG.
For the remainder of the paper, we define these two approaches as:
EOGHP, where the HP-filtered EOG is directly used for eye gaze angle estimation;
SACCINT, where the HP-filtered EOG is fed into a saccade detector, and then the result is integrated.
This manuscript describes the eye gaze estimation scheme using in-ear EOG recordings and compares the output to the actual eye position monitored by a video-based eye tracker.
Methods
Participants
Seven normal- or corrected-to-normal-sighted human participants participated in the experiment. One participant could not perform the task with the eye tracker, and one participant was equipped with different type of electrodes. The data of the five remaining participants were used in the subsequent analysis. This study was approved by the West of Scotland Research Ethics Service. The participants were members of the Institute of Hearing Research, and they provided written informed consent.
Setup and procedures
The experiment was conducted in a testing booth (4.6 m x 4.1 m x 2.5 m—l x w x h) with lights turned off during the experiment. The acoustically treated room is one of the booths which are commonly used for auditory experiments. The participants were seated directly in the front of a 40” LCD screen (Samsung, UE40ES5500) at distance of 77 cm from the screen to the eyes (Fig 2A). The participants’ heads were not restrained or supported, but the participants were instructed to remain still and fixate on a small white dot (1° of visual angle) on a grey background at the height of their eyes. The position of the dot was drawn from a pseudorandom de Bruijn sequence [27] of 11 possible target locations that spanned from the left to right margins of the screen, covering approximately ± 30.5° of visual field. The dots changed their position after a pseudorandom interval of 0.8–1.2 s. The sequence was constructed so that all positions were equally represented and the transition between each possible pair of the positions occurred exactly 11 times, which led to 113 (1331) total target presentations. The actual measurement period lasted about 22 minutes (excluding preparation). In the offline analysis, the measurement was split into a training period, in which the parameters of the model were estimated, and a testing period, in which the performance of the system was assessed.
A DC-coupled differential bio-amplifier (Attys, Glasgow Neuro LTD, UK) and a video-based eye tracker (Pupil Labs, Berlin, Germany) [28] that served as ground truth were used for the recording (Fig 2B). The bio-amplifier was equipped with a low-noise 24-bit sigma-delta AD converter and a Bluetooth transmitter which transmitted the measurements to the experimental computer at a sampling frequency (fs) of 83.34 Hz. When a measurement was unavailable, the previous value was used instead. Two disposable conductive ear moulds (Fig 2C) were made of ER1-14A ear tips (Etymotic Research, Elk Grove Village, IL, USA) and conductive thread (Electro Fashion, Kitronic, Nottingham, UK). The electrodes were attached to the bio-amplifier by 20 cm long non-shielded cables, and a small portion of electrode gel was put on the tip of the electrodes before insertion into the ear canal. The ground electrode was connected to the forehead with a regular medical grade Ag-Cl electrode. The bio-amplifier was held near the head using an adjustable plastic headband sourced from the inside of a construction hard hat. The ground truth eye tracker was connected to a dedicated Linux computer running eye tracker software (Pupil Capture, v0.7.5). The data from the ground truth eye tracker were collected for both eyes at 60 Hz with a resolution of 800 x 600 pixels. The eye tracker software directly outputted the eye gaze angle using a 3D eye model. The eye tracker computer transmitted the measurements via the local network to the experimental computer, where all recordings were kept for offline analysis. The experimental computer executed custom Matlab (v8.6.0, Natick, USA), Psychtoolbox [29–31] and Python (v3.5.1) scripts, which controlled the pace of the experiment.
Ground truth
The eye data outputted by the eye tracker software were calibrated to the actual positions using histograms of all measurements. Subsequently, a linear transformation was applied to match the measurements with the positions of the targets. The resulting eye gaze angle was computed as a mean of the angles from the left and right eyes. The eye tracker and the bio-amplifier data were then temporally aligned using timestamps recorded by the eye tracker and the timestamps obtained by the experimental computer during the recording of the bio-amplifier data. For the purpose of the analysis, saccades from the ground truth were detected using the EyeMMV toolbox [32] with the following settings: minimum saccade duration was set to 50 ms, spatial parameters were set to x = 0.06 and y = 0.05 of the tracker units (normalized to 0–1) using the option ‘3s’ (i.e., the fixation cluster is defined as three standard deviations from the centre). In order to evaluate the system, the saccades obtained from the eye tracker were matched to the saccades estimated from the EOG data. Two saccades were matched if they were temporally closest to each other (sooner or later) and if
(1) |
where |saccEOG| is the magnitude of the EOG saccade and |saccGT| is the magnitude of the saccade obtained from the ground truth. That means that the computed magnitude of the saccade had to be less than 1.5 times the magnitude of the ground truth saccade plus 10° (i.e., only the saccades of approximately equal magnitudes could be matched). These corrections were used to ensure correct matching when the EOG and the ground truth signals were not perfectly aligned in time, which was a side effect of the wireless transmission. In order to minimize the problems related to the delays and to make sure that the matching procedure worked as expected, matching was visually checked. We concluded that the delays between the EOG and the eye tracker had only a small impact and the delay could not influence the difference between the methods which are compared in this manuscript.
Saccade integration scheme
The proposed EOG to eye gaze algorithm, SACCINT (Fig 3), used a single-channel EOG signal as an input. Ground truth measurements served to calibrate the system before the actual testing. The algorithm outputted eye gaze angle with a theoretical delay of up to 200 ms, which corresponded to half of the temporal window (see below) and the delay due to the HP filter. The analysis was run offline, though the algorithm can be run in real-time. In the first step of the scheme, the HP-filtered signal of the length of the temporal window was used to estimate three parameters of a non-linear model of a saccade, which was an s-shaped function: magnitude Sx, gain Sg, and temporal offset So. These parameters were evaluated to confirm the saccade detection, and they were then used in subsequent integration. The saccades were identified when the time offset parameter So crossed the midline of the temporal window and when the magnitude of the signal deflection Sx was in a range defined by a minimum (Nx,min) and a maximum of 70°. In the integration step, the saccades were represented as noisy measurements using a Gaussian probability density function (PDF). Subsequently, they were integrated over time. The integration had two steps. First, the mean of a new PDF was obtained as a sum of the previous mean and the magnitude of the new saccade. The variance was increased by a constant Nm which represented noise related to the measurement. In the second step, the PDF was clipped at the fixed boundaries ±EAnax. The algorithm used one more parameter Ci [°/mV], which defined the linear relationship between the change of EOG and the change of the horizontal eye gaze angle. Ci was estimated for each participant (index i). The parameters Ci, Nm, and Nx,min were calibrated for the whole dataset during the training phase using the ground truth measurements.
High-pass filtering
The bio-amplifier readings were HP filtered with a second-order Butterworth filter with the cut-off frequency fHP. The purpose of the filtering was to remove as much of the low-frequency noise as possible but at the same time preserve information regarding eye position.
Fig 4 shows the step response of HP filters with cut-off frequencies between 0.01 and 0.06 Hz for up to 6 seconds (6 seconds is a reasonable time for a very long off-axis fixation). The HP filter with cut-off frequency of 0.01 Hz reduces the step signal only by ~9% after 1 second (which was the typical duration of the visual target in this experiment) and it does not cross 0 even after 6 seconds. On the other hand, the HP filter with 0.06 Hz cut-off reduces the signal by ~47% after 1 second and crosses zero at less than 3 seconds. While the first example would affect a typical eye movement to a small extent, the latter example would change the slope—particularly for long fixations.
At this HP filtering stage of the algorithm, the traditional EOGHP algorithm took this signal and calculated the eye angle directly. The SACCINT algorithm, however, further processed the HP-filtered signal as described in the following section.
Non-linear fit
The next step of the SACCINT algorithm was to fit the non-linear function to the HP-filtered signal. The signal was used to estimate the parameters of the simple saccadic model, which consisted of the s-shaped function:
(2) |
The fit was obtained using a standard non-linear fitting procedure with constraints [33] with a limit of 20 iterations, and the objective function defined as the least square error. The parameters were constrained with the following values: So = <-136 ms, 136 ms>, Sg = <-150,150>, Sx = (0 mV, 1,05 x (max(xEOG)-min(xEOG))) /2 mV> where xEOG is the EOG signal in the temporal window. The initial estimates for the three parameters were obtained as a weighted average of the previously estimated value and a pseudo randomly selected value from the above intervals with weights 0.95 and 0.05, respectively. The fit was repeated until the root mean square (RMS) error of the fit was below 4/Ci with a maximum of three repetitions but with completely random initial starting points within the accepted range of the constraints. The length of the temporal window was set 273 ms (23 measurements), which covers the duration of a typical saccade.
Saccade detection
Fig 5A shows 12 seconds of EOG recording together with derived parameters (Fig 5B) Sx x Ci (Fig 5C) So (Fig 5D) Sg on the left y-axis. The most important parameter for saccade detection was So. As a deflection in the EOG appeared in the temporal window, So progressively increased from negative values and changed its sign to positive when the deflection was in the middle of the window [14]. The second identifier of a saccade was its magnitude Sx due to physiological limits of eye. Thus the largest accepted saccade was Nx,max, which was set to 70°. The smallest accepted saccade (Nx,min) was a parameter likely to affect the number of detected saccades. Therefore, the influence of this parameter was investigated as a part of the study. The input for the detection algorithm were the parameters estimated at the time t and t-1.
Following a set of rules was used for saccade detection in each time t.
(3) |
(4) |
(5) |
(6) |
The first and second conditions [(3) and (4)] define the boundaries for saccade magnitude in the current estimate (at time t), and the estimate from the previous step (at time t-1). The third and fourth conditions [(5) and (6)] define the zero-crossing time of parameter So between t and t-1, and limit that time to be no farther than 4/fs from zero. Although it would be possible to use more conditions and achieve better detection performance, or use a statistically based model; here we aimed to demonstrate that a simple rule-based model is capable of saccade detection, and restoring actual eye gaze angle.
Saccade integration
Saccade integration is a novel method of estimating eye gaze angle. It is based on an assumption that in many real situations eye gaze behaviour can be characterized solely in terms of saccades and fixations. It relies on the fact that eye positions resemble a normal distribution with the mean in the midline of the visual field [34], and that eye positions are naturally limited. The scheme is based on the step-like saccades (i.e. step changes) and stable fixations (i.e., no movement). A simple summation of the saccades would be unstable because (a) the estimation of saccade magnitude is noisy, (b) very small saccades cannot be detected, (c) the eye is not stable during the fixation period, and (d) any detection algorithm on a noisy signal will always have false alarms and misses. As a result, a simple summation of noisy estimates of saccades would lead to integration errors (e.g., the estimates could depart from the natural boundaries).
One way of mitigating these problems is to represent the eye position as a Gaussian PDF with the mean Xt,i and variance σ2t,i. If a saccade is detected, the magnitude of the new saccade in degrees is added to the mean of the previous estimate.
(7) |
where Xt-1,i is the previous estimate. The magnitude of saccade at time t is defined as
(8) |
and the variance of the PDF increases by the measurement noise Nm:
(9) |
The new estimates of position (Xt,i) and variance (σ2t,i) are computed by simulation of the newly obtained PDF, which is a truncated Gaussian distribution with the mean X’t,i and variance σ’2t,i clipped at the constraints EAmax = ±35°.
In order to characterize the saccade integration scheme, let us assume a perfectly performing saccade detector. In our case, this was obtained from the video-based eye tracker. Hence, there are two parameters Nm and Nx,min which influence the performance of the saccade integration.
Fig 6 shows the effect of the Nm and Nx,min parameters on the performance of the saccade integration scheme, with Nm on the x-axis and different line styles showing different values of the Nx,min parameter. The performance (y-axis) was measured in the square of Person product-moment correlation coefficients (r2) of the prediction versus the actual eye position. This figure illustrates that in our dataset the maximum possible performance is r2 = 0.87 (Nx,min = 0°, Nm = 0°). This value decreases with increasing values of the investigated parameters. Further, there is only a small difference between the lines Nx,min = 0°, and Nx,min = 4°. The line representing Nx,min = 8° shows r2 values 0.71–0.76. The line representing Nx,min = 12° drops even further to r2 values of approximately 0.64 with further decrease for small values of Nm.
The analysis determines the expected level of noise of the saccade integration scheme for the perfect detector of saccades (without false alarms and misses). It also shows that if the detector is capable of reliably estimating magnitudes and directions of saccades between 4–8°, then it can estimate the actual eye gaze angle with the r2 more than 0.8.
Training
The algorithm required three input parameters that had to be calibrated using the ground truth. The training period was defined as the first five minutes of the experiment after stabilization of the HP filter with 0.01 Hz cut-off frequency; the training period lasted approximately 94 seconds. The current detection implementations had significant problems with rejecting false alarms for steeply decreasing/increasing signals. Therefore the initial part of the signal had to be discarded (see Sec. Results). The inputs for training were the EOG signal and the ground truth. In the first step, the saccades were estimated from both the EOG and video-based eye tracker. The EOG saccades were estimated with the current algorithm using a default set of the parameters (fHP = 0.03 Hz, Nx,min = 6°, Nm = 10°, Ci = 700°/mV). In the second step, the ground truth saccades were matched to the EOG based saccades and the parameter Ci was estimated using a simple linear regression with one linear parameter. In the third step, the algorithm was run again (fHP = 0.03 Hz and Ci set to a new value) for different values of the Nm, and Nx,min parameters in order to find the optimal combination in terms of across-subject mean r2 values. The values Nm = 9° and Nx.min = 8° represent the global maximum for the current dataset. Subsequently, the estimated parameters Ci, Nm, and Nx,min were used to run the algorithm on the data in the testing period.
Results
The experiment was designed in order to characterize in-ear EOG measurements and to test whether the saccade integration algorithm using only single-channel recordings could reconstruct horizontal eye gaze angles.
Raw data
Fig 1A and 1C show two samples of raw in-ear EOG recordings over 22 minutes. Fig 1B and 1D show the details of the EOG waveforms together with the positions of the visual targets. These data illustrate the magnitude of the DC drift in comparison to the magnitude of the EOG signal. Fig 1B demonstrates that the EOG signal correlates with the eye gaze angle and that this relationship can be influenced by DC drift (Fig 1D). Fig 1D shows the case of high DC drift, where it is more difficult to see the relationship between the EOG signal and the ground truth. Transient events that relate to the saccades, however, are still visible in the raw data.
In-ear EOG
In order to characterize in-ear EOG and its relationship to eye movements, Fig 7 shows the magnitude of change in the EOG signal as a function of the change of position in the visual target. Each thin grey line shows data from one participant, and the dashed line shows across-subject means. For each transition of the target location, a 300 ms pre-transition to 600 ms post-transition window was used, and the change in the EOG signal was defined as the median value of the first 120 ms minus the median of the last 120 ms of the 900 ms period. The data were pooled across initial target angles and repetitions, and medians were computed for each magnitude of change. This method was preferred over using the linear regression, as in the case of Ci computation, in order to avoid the assumptions of the current technique.
Fig 7 illustrates the linear relationship between the change of the target angle and the change of EOG in the range of investigated eccentricities, confirming previous results [16] when EOG was measured in the vicinity of eyes. The data also show that a change of 1° of visual angle corresponded to 2.2 ± 0.5 μV (mean ± 95% CI). The across-subject variance could be a consequence of various factors including the electrode contact, the shape of the head, and the individual differences in the corneo-retinal potential (CRP). The CRP is the source of EOG and it is known to vary with the luminance of the visual scene [11], but it can also be influenced by individual differences in eye physiology.
Training performance
The performance of the whole algorithm was evaluated in terms of the parameters of a linear model (standard deviation and gain) and the proportion of variance explained by the linear model (r2). The saccade detection part of the algorithm was evaluated in terms of F-scores, a measure which takes into account hits, misses, and false alarms [10,15]. The F-score is a measure based on true positive rates (TPR), the percentage of true positives with respect to all true events, and positive predictive values (PPV), the percentage of true positive events with respect to all detected events by this method. The events were saccades obtained from the ground truth measurements greater than Nx,min. Greater F indicates better detection; an F equal to 1 indicates perfect performance.
(10) |
The F-score does not take into account the magnitude and direction of the saccade. In terms of our algorithm, the magnitudes of the saccades were important, because we aimed to reconstruct the actual eye gaze angle by integrating saccades. Therefore, this measure was introduced only to allow comparison with the previous work.
During the training period, the performance was evaluated at different values of the Nx,min and Nm parameters. Fig 8 shows a subset of the training dataset for (Fig 8A–8D) Nm = 9° and (Fig 8E–8G) Nx,min = 8° while varying the other parameter (fHP was set to 0.03 Hz). Nx,min is the parameter of the detection step, thus the panel D shows how the F ratio was influenced by this parameter. Nm is the parameter of the integration step; the detection was not influenced by this parameter.
The upper row of Fig 8 shows that r2 peaks for the intermediate values of Nx,min, and it has a maximum of 0.64. The standard deviation of error shows a similar but opposite pattern as r2; the smallest value was 10°. The gain of the linear model is constant around a value of 1 for Nx,min up to 10° and then decreases. On the other hand, the F-score increases monotonically from ~0.49 at 0° to ~0.93 at 9° and then slowly increases further up to ~0.95 at 14°. The bottom row shows similar patterns for the r2 (Fig 8E) and the error statistics (Fig 8F); however, the gain statistic increases monotonically with Nm.
The upper row illustrates that it is beneficial to restrict the detection algorithm to saccades of certain magnitude by setting the Nx,min parameter. The analysis also shows that the Nx,min parameter mostly affects the error of the linear model. The bottom row shows that Nm influences the slope of the estimated values. For example, the monotonic increase of the gain (Fig 8G) illustrates how the estimated eye positions become more ‘compressed’ with greater Nm. Gain < 1 indicates that the output values systematically undershoot the true values; gain > 1 indicates overshooting.
Testing performance
In this section, we compare the SACCINT algorithm against the traditional EOGHP approach during the testing period for various cut-off frequencies. In order to obtain the output values of eye positions, the HP filtered data were multiplied by Ci/2 and clipped at ±EAmax (±35°).
Fig 9 shows the performance of SACCINT for each participant (‘x’ symbol) versus the performance of EOGHP as a function of fHP. The figure shows that ‘x’ symbols lie above the dashed lines in the most cases. One participant is below the dashed lines for certain fHP, and one other participant is below the dashed line for fHP = 0.03 Hz. This suggests that our novel algorithm SACCINT performs better than a simple HP filter (EOGHP) in most cases. The performance of SACCINT improves with increasing performance of the HP filtering approach.
The results also demonstrate that the SACCINT approach is almost independent of the value of fHP. The performance of some participants nearly approached the theoretical limit of saccade integration. The difference between ideal performance (dotted horizontal line) and the performance of the participants (‘x’ symbols) can be attributed only to the quality of the detection step. Fig 9B also shows that the overall performance is limited by the measurements of one particular participant. It is possible that the electrodes had poor contact during this measurement which decreased the quality of the signal (see Sec. Errors in saccade detection).
The above observations can be summarized in the across-subject analysis of r2 (Fig 10A) values and the analysis of RMS errors (Fig 10B). These data show that the saccade integration scheme achieved maximum performance of r2 = 0.54 ± 0.14 (95% CI) for fHP = 0.01 Hz, and the performance changed slightly with varying HP cut-off frequency. The across-subject performance of the EOGHP was poor (r2 = 0) when no HP filter was applied. The performance improved with increasing cut-off frequency and peaked at r2 = 0.46 for fHP = 0.03 Hz. It then decreased for greater cut-off frequencies. The SACCINT performed better than the EOGHP for some fHP but not all. SACCINT performed significantly better than EOGHP without the HP filter (two-tailed paired t-test, t4 = 3.6244; p < 0.05, controlled for false discovery rate [35]) and with fHP = 0.01 Hz (t4 = 2.8886; p < 0.05).
The RMS error of the SACCINT had minimum 12° ± 2° (95% CI) for fHP = 0.01 Hz. The performance was approximately constant for the set of investigated frequencies. The RMS error of the EOGHP was 15° for fHP = 0.03 Hz and above, and this value was elevated for decreasing the cut-off frequency. The only statistically significant difference between SACCINCT and EOGHP was without HP filtering (t4 = 8.8034; p < 0.05).
These data illustrate that even EOGHP could predict eye gaze angle from in-ear EOG measurements. The SACCINT algorithm is a more robust way of estimating the eye gaze angle than the standard HP filtering. The performance of the EOGHP approach is strongly influenced by the fHP value. The fact that EOGHP was not significantly different from SACCINT for most fHP values partly relates to the design of the experiment. For example, fixation periods > 1 s would deteriorate the EOGHP approach, but would not affect the new saccade integration scheme SACCINT.
Errors in saccade detection
In order to analyse the detection algorithm of SACCINT, Fig 11 shows the patterns of errors of a representative participant during the testing phase (fHP = 0.03 Hz). Panel Fig 11A shows that computed saccades only slightly underestimated the magnitude of the actual saccades. When the slope was fitted to the data of all participants, it had a value of 0.95 ± 0.05 (mean ± 95% CI). This deviation from 1 relates to the selection of Nm = 9° in the training phase and the cut-off frequency of the HP filter. The standard deviation of the error between the fit and the matched saccades was 4.67°± 0.9 (across-subject mean ± 95% CI), which characterizes high-frequency noise inherently present in the EOG signal and the precision of the saccade magnitude estimation. Panel Fig 11B shows the relatively small number of misses and relatively large number of undetectable saccades, which suggests that the improvements in the saccade detection algorithm may improve the performance. Fig 11C shows that the majority of the correct rejections were of small magnitudes and they were filtered out at the detection step. Notably, the algorithm still reports a substantial number of false alarms (276) which is 22% of all real saccades.
Fig 12 provides further insight into the patterns of errors of the saccade detection algorithm. The figure shows the temporal distributions of the misses and false alarms of the two example participants whose raw EOG traces are shown in Fig 1. When Fig 12 is compared with Fig 1 (Fig 12A corresponds to Fig 1A and 1B and Fig 12B corresponds to Fig 1C and 1D), three characteristics of the algorithm’s errors are shown. First, the number of false alarms dramatically increased if the signal had a very strong DC component (Fig 1C) and contained more high-frequency noise (Fig 1D). That is, the areas of densely distributed false alarms being in one direction indicate that they had a common origin from the DC component; the false alarms in both directions were present due to high-frequency noise. Second, the number of misses was approximately constant. Third, many false alarms were due to the sloping signal before the DC filter stabilised.
Taken all together, this indicates that the high false alarm rate was the most prominent constraint of the current implementation. A proper statistical model of the eye movements (e.g., [36]) should eliminate the majority of the observed errors.
Discussion
The experiment and analysis demonstrated that it is possible to estimate horizontal eye gaze angle using a single-channel EOG measurement with a pair of ear moulds positioned inside the ear canal. However, the estimates are still noisy. SACCINT achieved the across-subject performance of r2 = 0.54, which was better than a simple EOGHP approach for some values of fHP. The performance of the EOGHP depends on the fixation period and the fHP, while SACCINT is independent of the fixation period and much less dependent on fHP. Therefore, it is likely that SACCINT would outperform the EOGHP in scenarios involving movements and real visual targets once the saccade detection step is improved. However, only five participants were tested in this study in a very controlled environment (e.g., with heads fixed), and with custom-made electrodes. Therefore, the current results have to be understood in the context of these and other limitations.
Firstly, the quality of the EOG measurements is critical for any EOG-based technology, and it was observed that the quality varied across participants. This can be related to the quality of our custom made electrodes, and it is very likely that better electrodes can substantially improve this system.
Secondly, the analysis of errors showed that most of the errors were introduced by the detection algorithm. Many of the errors could be eliminated by more advanced methods of detection; for example, by probabilistic classification [15]. If the detection was ideal, the performance could achieve an r2 of 0.8. One of the problems was that the current model could not properly describe the drifting EOG signal (e.g., when the EOG was dominated by the DC drift, or when the HP filter was introduced), because this type of signal was always incorrectly interpreted as a series of saccades in one direction. This resulted in a large number of false alarms. This particular problem limits the effectiveness of the algorithm. The number of false alarms can be reduced in the future by modifying the model such that it captures the drift more closely. The detection can be further improved by employing a more advanced model of eye physiology [36] that, for example, assumes refectory periods between saccades and natural rates of saccade occurrence, which can reduce the rate of false alarms. The statistics of eye movements [37] and statistics of eye movements in relation to the eye position [38,39] could also be implemented to improve the detection step. However, EOG does not contain any information about the environment or the intention of the participant (e.g., whether the saccade is voluntary or involuntary). Therefore any statistical model would have to take into account only eye physiology, natural tendencies of the eye movements, or factors which are not dependent on environment or task.
Thirdly, the integration step was a simplistic model of the eye physiology assuming a perfectly constant eye position during the fixation and a step-like change of eye position during a saccade. This introduced noise into the estimation process because the eye is never perfectly still. Further, the additional noise was introduced because the estimated saccades were delayed by approximately half of the duration of the window length. Implementing a statistical predictor of the eye position, however, could improve the integration step. A possible future predictor of the eye position is the head angle. The contributions of head movements to the eye gaze directions have been previously demonstrated, for instance, in a visual search task experiment [34] with stimuli distributed over 360° around the participant with unconstrained head movements. That experiment showed that the distributions of the eye gaze directions followed the distributions of the head vs. body orientation (e.g., when the head turns to left, the eye gaze is likely to be to the left). That means that if the saccade integration model had access to the actual head vs. body orientation, then it has an independent predictor of eye position. However, the head was fixed in the current experiment, and the positions of the visual targets were strictly controlled. Future experiments should, therefore, test the method in more realistic environments that include head movements.
Relation to previous work
In-ear EOG
The magnitude of the in-ear EOG signal was estimated to be 2.2 μV per 1° of visual angle, which is less than the measurements in the standard position and slightly less than the previously reported in-ear measurement of 3 μV /1° [18]. One of the five participants had an in-ear EOG magnitude of 2.8 μV/1° which is closer to the previous study. The relationship of the change in EOG to the change in target angle was linear for the observed transition magnitudes up to 61°. In our experiment, the saccade transitions were not uniformly distributed, and the eye positions with eccentricities larger than 30.5° were not tested. Therefore, further testing is necessary to establish whether linearity is preserved for extreme eye positions.
Saccade detection and saccade identification
Previously, the saccades were detected by analysing parameters of the continuous wavelet transformation [10]; the detector performed with small number of errors (F = 0.94). In that study, they detected the magnitudes of the saccades, but these were only classified as either small or large, and the actual eye angle was not estimated. Barea et al. [16] reported that their system was able to identify saccades with magnitudes greater than 10° and an error of 2°. A method of Iáñez et al. [14] was based on an analysis of the derivative of EOG. Detection performance was the same as [11] (F = 0.94). Another method based on continuous wavelet transformation and auto-calibration [21] claimed almost perfect detection of horizontal saccades, but the analysis was based on an offline artifact and drift removal step. When the output was compared to the eye tracker data, the performance diminished. Vidal et al. [20] based their method on feature extraction (velocity, acceleration, slope, parameters of polynomial fit) and also claimed almost perfect detection. The study did not report the length of the window that was used for the data analysis. Therefore, it was not clear whether the data were processed offline or online. These various methods usually achieved similar or better performance than our algorithm, but in all these studies, the EOG signal was measured at the peri-orbital positions which offer a substantially higher signal to noise ratio. The studies also tended to use signals from two or more channels (e.g., horizontal and vertical channels), and they disregarded the magnitudes of the saccades when evaluating the performance of the detection algorithm (e.g., a small saccade could be assigned as a large saccade). Thus, it is not possible to directly compare the current results with the performance of the mentioned methods.
Eye angle estimation
Several previous studies [18,22,23,40,41] have suggested the possibility of estimating eye gaze angle from EOG. These methods either relied on readings from multiple electrodes, or they used a camera for calibration. One approach [22] analysed the EOG signal from multiple electrodes, which allowed to decorrelate the noise components in different electrodes while preserving the EOG signal, thus allowing direct estimation of gaze angle. In another approach [23], gaze angle estimation was based on the EOG signal that was calibrated by saliency maps obtained from an external camera. The estimation RMS error of the current method was 12° which is greater than the multiple-electrode method [22] error of 4°, but less than the externally calibrated method [23] error of 15°. However, the current and the previous methods cannot be compared directly because neither of the previous experiments measured the EOG signal inside the ear canals with just two electrodes.
Hearing aids and other applications
The motivation for the current investigation was the development of a portable and unobtrusive eye gaze angle estimation technique for hearing aids. This could be used to steer directional microphones toward attended sounds [4]. This technology could engage the hearing-impaired listeners into dynamic conversations in noisy environments, and make such situations less challenging because the listener usually consciously looks at the attended talker(s). However, eye movements (as well as head movements) reflect exogenous attention which might have a detrimental effect on listening, if the acoustic beam of the directional microphone system was too narrow [42]. Therefore, future research is needed to estimate the effect of the parameters of such technology on their benefit in real listening scenarios.
Potentially, eye-movement information could provide information about the health, mood, or environment where the user currently is. For instance, previous research studied eye movements in connection with various types of neurodegenerative disease [43]. Further, this type of eye tracking could be incorporated in consumer headphones, virtual reality systems, or systems that monitor fatigue. Portable eye tracking techniques have been previously used in marketing [44], sport [45], sleep [13], and car driving [46] research. Less intrusive methods of eye tracking could ease the data collection in realistic environments.
Limitations
The most notable limitation of all EOG applications is the signal to noise ratio, which relates mainly to measurement artifacts: DC drift and muscular activity. The application is further constrained by the assumptions of the current model, specifically its saccade detection and integration. The algorithm is only capable of detecting (with relative reliability) saccades of large magnitude (> 8°). The magnitude estimation, though, is accurate; mean estimation error was 4.67°. The algorithm cannot detect smooth pursuit nor any other smooth eye movement. Smooth pursuit and DC drift appear identical in the EOG signal, which means that they are impossible to distinguish in an online analysis. Other limitations are the parameters of the saccade integration model. In this work, these were estimated in the training period but in real environments, they may change over time, and they need to be calibrated. The parameter Ci may change with external lighting conditions. One possible solution is to implement a light sensor and define the relationship between the change of lighting and in-ear EOG. However, that would require an investigation of whether lighting is the only factor that affects the EOG magnitude. A second way to calibrate Ci is to use the vestibulo-ocular reflex in connection with head movements. When the head moves, eyes often remain fixed during head movements, thus if the device was equipped with a gyroscope, then the strongly correlated outputs of the EOG and gyroscope can instantaneously calibrate the system. Two other parameters Nm and Nx,min are specific for this particular implementation, which means that they might need to be replaced in the future. Moreover, Nm and Nx,min are not likely to change over time.
Another limitation of the current approach is that our tests were conducted only in one controlled environment, with a small sample of participants, without deliberate body movements, and with targets uniformly spaced across the visual field. Nevertheless, several previous studies have demonstrated that EOG can be measured even in a mobile environment [9,10,23], and showed that specialized algorithms can diminish the effects of commonly occurring artifacts (e.g., the walking artifact). Certain artifacts cannot be filtered easily. For example, the artifacts related to the jaw or tongue movements would be difficult to filter, because they do not have any regular shape or frequency, and any EOG-to-gaze algorithm is vulnerable to them.
The current algorithm can be used online, but this analysis was run offline on a PC with high computational power. In this implementation, the most computationally demanding step is the non-linear fitting procedure, but a different fitting procedure may be more computationally efficient. One further limitation is that the current approach analysed the signal in a temporal window, which in practice would lead to a delay up to 200 ms, and this delay cannot be avoided.
Conclusion
The current work showed that it is possible to estimate the eye gaze angle with a single-channel in-ear EOG recording using EOGHP (r2 = 0.46) and a novel SACCINT (r2 = 0.54) method. The estimates were still noisy, but in theory the SACCINT could attain much better performance (r2 > 0.8). This difference between the theory and the actual performance of the SACCINT can be attributed mostly to the quality of the in-ear EOG signal, which lead to errors in the detection and integration steps. Therefore, further improvements of this method are necessary. A number of improvements have been proposed, including improving the design of the electrodes, improving the non-linear fitting procedure, modelling of eye physiology, incorporating gyroscope signals, or incorporating statistical models. Overall, our investigation suggests that in-ear EOG signals captured with conductive ear moulds could serve as a basis for light-weight, portable horizontal eye gaze angle estimation suitable for broad range of applications not limited to hearing aids.
Acknowledgments
We wish to acknowledge Dr. William ‘Bill’ Whitmer for his helpful suggestions and comments.
Data Availability
Raw data and the implementation of the algorithm are available from the Nottingham Research Data Management Repository (DOI: 10.17639/nott.334).
Funding Statement
This work was supported by grants from the Oticon Foundation, the UK Medical Research Council [grant nos. U135097131 and MC_UU_00010/4], and the Chief Scientist Office (Government of Scotland). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Grant KW, Walden BE, Seitz PF. Auditory-visual speech recognition by hearing-impaired subjects: Consonant recognition, sentence recognition, and auditory-visual integration. J Acoust Soc Am. 1998;103: 2677–2690. doi: 10.1121/1.422788 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bernstein LE, Demorest ME, Tucker PE. Speech perception without hearing. Percept Psychophys. 2000;62: 233–252. doi: 10.3758/BF03205546 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Desai S, Stickney G, Zeng F-G. Auditory-visual speech perception in normal-hearing and cochlear-implant listeners. J Acoust Soc Am. 2008;123: 428–440. doi: 10.1121/1.2816573 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kidd G Jr., Favrot S, Desloge JG, Streeter TM, Mason CR. Design and preliminary testing of a visually guided hearing aid. J Acoust Soc Am. Boston, MA, United States.: Acoustical Society of America; 2013;133: EL202–EL207. doi: 10.1121/1.4791710 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hart J, Onceanu D, Sohn C, Wightman D, Vertegaal R. The Attentive Hearing Aid: Eye Selection of Auditory Sources for Hearing Impaired Users. Lecture Notes in Computer Science (including subseries Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence and Lecture Notes in Bioinformatics). 2009. pp. 19–35. 10.1007/978-3-642-03655-2_4
- 6.Bulling A, Kunze K. Eyewear computers for human-computer interaction. interactions. 2016;23: 70–73. doi: 10.1145/2912886 [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ramli R, Arof H, Ibrahim F, Mokhtar N, Idris MYI. Using finite state machine and a hybrid of EEG signal and EOG artifacts for an asynchronous wheelchair navigation. Expert Syst Appl. Elsevier Ltd; 2015;42: 2451–2463. doi: 10.1016/j.eswa.2014.10.052 [Google Scholar]
- 8.Barea R, Boquete L, Mazo M, López E. Wheelchair guidance strategies using EOG. J Intell Robot Syst Theory Appl. 2002;34: 279–299. doi: 10.1023/A:1016359503796 [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bulling A, Roggen D, Tröster G. Wearable EOG goggles: Seamless sensing and context-awareness in everyday environments. J Ambient Intell Smart Environ. 2009;1: 157–171. doi: 10.3233/AIS-2009-0020 [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bulling A, Member S, Ward JA, Gellersen H, Tro G, Tröster G. Eye movement analysis for activity recognition using electrooculography. IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell. 2011;33: 741–753. doi: 10.1109/TPAMI.2010.86 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Brown M, Marmor M, Vaegan, Zrenner E, Brigell M, Bach M. ISCEV Standard for Clinical Electro-oculography (EOG) 2006. Doc Ophthalmol. 2006;113: 205–212. doi: 10.1007/s10633-006-9030-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Haslwanter T, Clarke AH. Eye movement measurement. electro-oculography and video-oculography [Internet]. 1st ed Handbook of Clinical Neurophysiology. Elsevier B.V.; 2010. doi: 10.1016/S1567-4231(10)09005-2 [Google Scholar]
- 13.McPartland RJ, Kupfer DJ. Computerised measures of electro-oculographic activity during sleep. Int J Biomed Comput. 1978;9: 409–419. doi: 10.1016/0020-7101(78)90048-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Iáñez E, Azorin JM, Perez-Vidal C. Using Eye Movement to Control a Computer: A Design for a Lightweight Electro-Oculogram Electrode Array and Computer Interface. PLoS One. 2013;8: 1–10. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Toivanen M, Pettersson K, Lukander K. A probabilistic real-time algorithm for detecting blinks, saccades, and fixations from EOG data. J Eye Mov Res. 2015;8: 1–14. doi: 10.16910/jemr.8.2.1 [Google Scholar]
- 16.Barea R, Boquete L, Ortega S, López E, Rodríguez-Ascariz JM. EOG-based eye movements codification for human computer interaction. Expert Syst Appl. Elsevier Ltd; 2012;39: 2677–2683. doi: 10.1016/j.eswa.2011.08.123 [Google Scholar]
- 17.Puthusserypady S, Ratnarajah T. Robust adaptive techniques for minimization of EOG artefacts from EEG signals. Signal Processing. 2006;86: 2351–2363. doi: 10.1016/j.sigpro.2005.10.018 [Google Scholar]
- 18.Manabe H, Fukumoto M. Using Earphones to Perform Gaze Detection for Warable Interfaces. NTT DOCOMO Tech J. 2006;12: 12–17. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Behrens F, MacKeben M, Schröder-Preikschat W. An improved algorithm for automatic detection of saccades in eye movement data and for calculating saccade parameters. Behav Res Methods. 2010;42: 701–708. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Vidal M, Bulling A, Gellersen H. Analysing EOG signal features for the discrimination of eye movements with wearable devices. Proceedings of the 1st international workshop on Pervasive eye tracking & mobile eye-based interaction—PETMEI ‘11. New York, New York, USA: ACM Press; 2011. p. 15. 10.1145/2029956.2029962
- 21.Pettersson K, Jagadeesan S, Lukander K, Henelius A, Haeggström E, Müller K, et al. Algorithm for automatic analysis of electro-oculographic data. Biomed Eng Online. 2013;12 doi: 10.1186/1475-925X-12-110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Manabe H, Fukumoto M, Yagi T. Direct Gaze Estimation Based on Nonlinearity of EOG. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2015;62: 1553–1562. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2015.2394409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Sugano Y, Bulling A. Self-Calibrating Head-Mounted Eye Trackers Using Egocentric Visual Saliency. Proceedings of the 28th Annual ACM Symposium on User Interface Software & Technology. 2015. pp. 363–372. 10.1145/2807442.2807445
- 24.Borji A, Itti L. State-of-the-Art in Visual Attention Modeling. IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell. 2013;35: 185–207. doi: 10.1109/TPAMI.2012.89 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Grimnes S, Martinsen ØG. Bioimpedance and Bioelectricity Basics. Academic Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Nakashima R, Fang Y, Hatori Y, Hiratani A, Matsumiya K, Kuriki I, et al. Saliency-based gaze prediction based on head direction. Vision Res. Elsevier Ltd; 2015;117: 59–66. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.de Bruijn NG. A combinatorial problem. Proc Sect Sci K Ned Akad van Wet te Amsterdam. 1946;49: 758–764. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kassner M, Patera W, Bulling A. Pupil: An Open Source Platform for Pervasive Eye Tracking and Mobile Gaze-based Interaction. 2014; http://arxiv.org/abs/1405.0006
- 29.Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D, Ingling A, Murray R, Broussard C. What’s new in psychtoolbox-3. Perception. 2007;36: 1–16. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brainard DH. The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997;10: 433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Pelli DG. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis. 1997;10: 437–442. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00366 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Krassanakis V, Filippakopoulou V, Nakos B. EyeMMV toolbox: An eye movement post-analysis tool based on a two-step spatial dispersion threshold for fixation identification. J Eye Mov Res. 2014;7(1): 1–10. Available: http://users.ntua.gr/bnakos/Data/Section5-7/Pub_5-7-19.pdf [Google Scholar]
- 33.Zhu C, Byrd RH, Lu P, Nocedal J. Algorithm 778: L-BFGS-B: Fortran subroutines for large-scale bound-constrained optimization. ACM Trans Math Softw. 1997;23: 550–560. doi: 10.1145/279232.279236 [Google Scholar]
- 34.Fang Y, Nakashima R, Matsumiya K, Kuriki I, Shioiri S. Eye-head coordination for visual cognitive processing. PLoS One. 2015;10: 1–17. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Glickman ME, Rao SR, Schultz MR. False discovery rate control is a recommended alternative to Bonferroni-type adjustments in health studies. J Clin Epidemiol. 2014;67: 850–857. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.03.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Komogortsev O V, Khan JI. Eye Movement Prediction by Kalman Filter with Integrated Linear Horizontal Oculomotor Plant Mechanical Model. Eye Track Res Appl Symp. 2008; 229–236. doi: 10.1145/1344471.1344525 [Google Scholar]
- 37.Boccignone G. Advanced statistical methods for eye movement analysis and modeling: a gentle introduction. 2015; http://arxiv.org/abs/1506.07194
- 38.Tatler BW, Vincent BT. The prominence of behavioural biases in eye guidance. Vis cogn. 2009;17: 1029–1054. doi: 10.1080/13506280902764539 [Google Scholar]
- 39.Le Meur O, Coutrot A. Introducing context-dependent and spatially-variant viewing biases in saccadic models. Vision Res. Elsevier Ltd; 2016;121: 72–84. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2016.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Manabe H, Fukumoto M. Full-time Wearable Headphone-Type Gaze Detector. 2006; 1073–1078.
- 41.Manabe H, Fukumoto M, Yagi T. Automatic drift calibration for EOG-based gaze input interface. Proc Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc EMBS. 2013; 53–56. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 42.Hládek Ľ, Porr B, Brimijoin WO. Effect of width of acoustic beam in eye-controlled beamforming in a dynamic “cocktail party.” BSA Basic Auditory Science, University of Nottingham, September 4–5. 2017.
- 43.Anderson TJ, MacAskill MR. Eye movements in patients with neurodegenerative disorders. Nat Rev Neurol. Nature Publishing Group; 2013;9: 74–85. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.273 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Higgins E, Leinenger M, Rayner K. Eye movements when viewing advertisements. Front Psychol. 2014;5: 1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Shank MD, Haywood KM. (University of Missouri-St. Louis) Eye movements while viewing a baseball pitch1. Percept Mot Skills. 1987; 1191–1197. doi: 10.2466/pms.1987.64.3c.1191 [Google Scholar]
- 46.Schleicher R, Galley N, Briest S, Galley L. Blinks and saccades as indicators of fatigue in sleepiness warnings: Looking tired? Ergonomics. 2008;51: 982–1010. doi: 10.1080/00140130701817062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Raw data and the implementation of the algorithm are available from the Nottingham Research Data Management Repository (DOI: 10.17639/nott.334).