Skip to main content
. 2017 Nov 20;17(Suppl 5):855. doi: 10.1186/s12889-017-4850-2

Table 3.

Association between sleep duration and cognitive development in children aged 0–4 years

No of studies Design Quality Assessment No of participants Absolute effect Quality
Risk of bias Inconsistency Indirectness Imprecision Other
Mean age ranged between 6 months and 4.9 years. Data were collected cross-sectionally and up to 3 years of follow-up. Sleep duration was assessed by actigraphy or parent report. Cognition was measured by various instruments including memory tasks, imitation tasks, neuropsychological tests, interviews, scales of intelligence or questionnaires.
1 Randomized triala No serious risk of bias No serious inconsistency No serious indirectness No serious imprecision None 23 The number of correct answers in an explicit recognition task was significantly higher in the nap (control) compared to the wake (sleep-restricted) condition, whereas implicit memory (priming task) did not differ between conditions [65]. HIGH
4 Longitudinal studyb No serious risk of bias No serious inconsistency No serious indirectness No serious imprecision None 438 Children getting higher proportions of their sleep at night as infants (i.e. 1 year) were found to perform better on executive functions, but did not show better general cognition [66].
Higher proportions of total sleep occurring at night time, at both 12 and 18 months, were associated with better performance on executive tasks, especially those involving a strong impulse control component. However, the total sleep duration at 12 and 18 months was not associated with executive functioning at 18 and 26 months. Sleep duration at 12 months was not correlated with 18 month working memory (r = −0.11, p > 0.05), 26 month conflict executive functioning (r = −0.10, p > 0.05) or 26 month impulse control (r = −0.06, p > 0.05). Sleep duration at 18 months was not correlated with 18 month working memory (r = −0.16, p > 0.05), 26 month conflict executive functioning (r = 0.09, p > 0.05) or 26 month impulse control (r = −0.16, p > 0.05) [67].
The number of daytimenaps was positively associated with both predicted expressive (p = 0.062) and receptive vocabulary growth (p = 0.006), whereas the length of nighttime sleep was negatively associated with rate of predicted expressive vocabulary growth (p = 0.045) [68].
Children who had 8 h or more of sleep had significantly higher General Conceptual Ability (GCA) scores than those with 7 h or less of sleep by 35.53 points at age 3. Children with more than 10 h of sleep had higher GCA scores at age 3 compared to children with 8–9 h or less of sleep (233.91 vs. 203.92, respectively) [69].
LOW
11 Cross-sectional studyc No serious risk of bias No serious inconsistency No serious indirectness No serious imprecision None 10,838 Out of 11 cross-sectional analyses, 7 reported null findings [38, 51, 55, 7073], 3 reported that shorter sleep duration was associated with poorer cognitive function [57, 74, 75], and 1 reported opposite associations [76]. LOW

Due to heterogeneity in the measurement of sleep and cognition, a meta-analysis was not possible

aRandomized cross-over study [65]

bIncludes 4 longitudinal studies [6669]

cIncludes 11 cross-sectional studies [38, 51, 55, 57, 7076]