Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2018 Jan 18;7:e32018. doi: 10.7554/eLife.32018

Inhibition of oxidative stress in cholinergic projection neurons fully rescues aging-associated olfactory circuit degeneration in Drosophila

Ashiq Hussain 1,, Atefeh Pooryasin 2,3,, Mo Zhang 4,, Laura F Loschek 4, Marco La Fortezza 5, Anja B Friedrich 1, Catherine-Marie Blais 1, Habibe K Üçpunar 4, Vicente A Yépez 6, Martin Lehmann 7, Nicolas Gompel 5, Julien Gagneur 6, Stephan J Sigrist 2,3, Ilona C Grunwald Kadow 1,4,8,
Editor: Patrik Verstreken9
PMCID: PMC5790380  PMID: 29345616

Abstract

Loss of the sense of smell is among the first signs of natural aging and neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s. Cellular and molecular mechanisms promoting this smell loss are not understood. Here, we show that Drosophila melanogaster also loses olfaction before vision with age. Within the olfactory circuit, cholinergic projection neurons show a reduced odor response accompanied by a defect in axonal integrity and reduction in synaptic marker proteins. Using behavioral functional screening, we pinpoint that expression of the mitochondrial reactive oxygen scavenger SOD2 in cholinergic projection neurons is necessary and sufficient to prevent smell degeneration in aging flies. Together, our data suggest that oxidative stress induced axonal degeneration in a single class of neurons drives the functional decline of an entire neural network and the behavior it controls. Given the important role of the cholinergic system in neurodegeneration, the fly olfactory system could be a useful model for the identification of drug targets.

Research organism: D. melanogaster


In order to exploit Drosophila to characterize the mechanisms of neural circuit degeneration, we first established that flies show a similar early aging-dependent decline in olfactory perception as humans (Doty et al., 1984). In olfactory T-maze assays, where the animal’s preference or aversion for an odor is recorded as an index of their approach or avoidance behavior toward the odorant, we found that the performance to eight different (three attractive, five aversive) odors gradually declined with age (Figure 1A and Figure 1—figure supplement 1A–D). This decline occurred also for behavior to odors that are detected independent of the canonical olfactory receptor ORCO (olfactory receptor co-receptor), and hence affected all tested odorants recognized by the three classes of olfactory receptors (ORs, ionotropic receptors (IRs), and gustatory receptors (GRs); Figure 1—figure supplement 1E–I). By contrast, the fly’s high attraction to blue light was not significantly different between 1 and 10 weeks of age (Figure 1B) indicating that the flies were healthy enough to move and to make decisions in this type of behavioral assay. These data argue that in flies, as observed in humans, the sense of smell declines before and/or faster than the sense of vision.

Figure 1. The sense of smell ages faster than the sense of vision.

(A) Olfactory preference index of aging Canton S flies to aversive (benzaldehyde, 3-octanol) and attractive odors (2,3-butanedione, putrescine) in the T-maze assay. There is a gradual and significant (p≤0.01) decrease in olfactory preference with aging (1–10 weeks). For additional odors see Figure 1—figure supplement 1. (B) Preference index (X-axis) of flies to blue light versus red light in the T-maze assay against age (Y-axis). There was no significant difference between the data points. Graphs show mean value ±SEM (n = 8 trials, 60 flies/trial 30 ♀ and 30 ♂). (C) Schematic illustration of the fly brain and antennal appendages with olfactory sensory neurons (OSNs). OSNs project into the antennal lobe (AL), where they innervate a specific glomerulus (green). Projection neurons (PNs, blue) send the information mainly to two higher brain centres, the mushroom body (MB) and the lateral horn (LH) (top).

Figure 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 1.
elife-32018-fig1-data1.xlsx (200.9KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.004

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Functional decline affects odors detected by all receptor classes.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A–D) Olfactory preference index of aging Canton S flies to attractive (pentanoic acid) and aversive odors (4-methyl cyclohexanol, Linalool, CO2) in the T-maze assay. Y-axis indicates the preference index (P.I.) to odors while X-axis denotes the age (weeks) of the tested flies. (E–G) Aging affects all classes of OSNs. Olfactory preference index of the odorant receptor co-receptor ORCO mutant aging flies in the T-maze assay in response to three attractive odors (pentanoic acid, putrescine, 2–3 butanedione). Y-axis indicates the preference index (P.I.s) to odors while X-axis denotes age (weeks) of the tested flies. Flies show response to putrescine (F), because it is detected by ionotropic receptors class (IR41a) instead of olfactory receptors (Hussain et al., 2016a). (H–L) Olfactory preference index of the aging Canton S flies in the T-maze assay in response to five aversive odors (Benzaldehyde, four methyl cyclohexanol, 3-octanol, Linalool, and CO2). Flies show response to CO2 (L), because it is detected by gustatory receptors (Gr21a, Gr63a) (Jones et al., 2007) instead of olfactory receptors. Box plot shows median and upper/lower quartiles (n = 8, 60 flies/trial 30 ♀ and 30 ♂). All p-values were calculated via two-way ANOVA with the Bonferroni multiple comparison posthoc test (ns >0.05, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001).

We next sought to identify the underpinning neuronal and genetic mechanisms. First, we asked whether all neurons were affected equally or whether perhaps particular neuron types within the olfactory circuit were primary targets of aging. The architecture of the fly olfactory system parallels that of vertebrates (Wilson, 2013). Olfactory sensory neuron (OSN) axons connect to the antennal lobe (AL), the equivalent of the olfactory bulb (OB), in the brain, where olfactory information is further processed and transferred by second order projection neurons (PNs; analogs of mitral and tufted cells) to higher brain centers (Figure 1C). A straightforward reason for a loss of sense of smell could be a loss of OSNs or a reduction in their odor responsiveness, for instance through diminished olfactory receptor expression. Yet, by counting the number of a subclass of OSNs we found no difference in the number of sensory neurons between young and older flies (e.g., 1 week vs. 10 weeks Or42b-Gal4; UAS-mCD8GFP flies; Figure 2—figure supplement 1A), suggesting that OSNs do not die during aging. Furthermore, we detected no significant difference in the size of OSN cell bodies between young and old flies (Figure 2—figure supplement 1B). In agreement with these data, RNA-sequencing (RNA-seq) of whole antennae demonstrated that olfactory receptor expression including the expression of those responding to the tested odors and the obligatory co-receptor ORCO, which is required for the detection of the majority of odors, was not significantly different between young and old flies (Figure 2—figure supplement 1C,D).

We next investigated possible changes in the OSN’s ability to respond to an odor, by recording spikes of neuronal activity in single sensillum recordings, where an electrode is inserted into individual olfactory sensilla (SSR; Figure 2—figure supplement 2A). While the response to two aversive odors, benzaldehyde and CO2 showed a significant decrease in the number of elicited spikes, none of the other recordings using other odorants (six odors) showed a similar decline (Figure 2—figure supplement 2B–H). This result did not change even when flies were first sorted by their response to the odor in the T-maze behavioral assay. Flies that reacted to the odorant and flies that appeared to not care showed similar SSR responses, which were also not significantly different between the age groups (Figure 2—figure supplement 2J,K). Given that the behavior to odors without a decline in SSR was similarly affected as the behavior to benzaldehyde and CO2, we concluded that a decrease in OSN sensitivity is unlikely to be the general reason for the observed ageing-associated decline in olfactory behavior. Altogether these experiments suggested that a degeneration of peripheral sensory neurons is not the primary reason for the observed ageing-associated phenotype.

OSNs transmit their information to PNs in the central brain (~30 OSNs to one PN; Figure 1C). The majority of PNs are cholinergic and can be labeled with the transgenic reporter line GH146-Gal4 (Figure 2A). To test whether PNs were functionally impaired, we used in vivo calcium imaging (GH146-Gal4; UAS-GCaMP3.0) as a proxy of neuronal activity. We first recorded fluorescence changes at the level of the AL, where the dendrites of these neurons receive input from the OSNs, using epifluorescence microscopy (Figure 2B). The PN odor response in the responsive glomeruli was significantly reduced at different concentrations including those used in the behavioral assays (1 mM, Figure 2B). With the exception of very few odors such as CO2, most odors activate multiple glomeruli. To better distinguish glomerulus-specific responses, we turned to two-photon microscopy and imaged GCaMP fluorescence changes at three different levels of the AL (Figure 2C,D). Here, we observed that not all glomeruli were equally affected. While for each odor, one, normally very responsive glomerulus showed a strong and significant decline, two other glomeruli were not significantly affected by age (Figure 2C,C’,D,D’). The reason for why different glomeruli are differentially affected is not know at this point. Differences in OSN number or olfactory receptor expression can, according to the data shown above, likely be excluded. Currently, we can only speculate that the degree of innervation by inhibitory or excitatory local neurons in the AL, other distinctive features, the activity of the OSN-PN synapses, interaction between sister PNs, or even the interaction with glia cells might play a role in these differences. Nonetheless, the current data strongly suggest that, in contrast to OSNs, PNs are less activated by odors in old as compared to young flies.

Figure 2. Cholinergic projection neurons functionally decay with age.

(A) Scheme of in vivo functional imaging preparation. The Ca2+ sensor GCaMP3 is expressed in projection neurons (PNs) under the control of GH146-Gal4 (GH146-Gal4;UAS-GCaMP3 or GH146 > GCaMP). (B1-3) In vivo calcium imaging in PNs (GH146-Gal4;UAS-GCaMP3.0) at the level of the AL using epifluorescence microscopy. The neural response of 1 week and 7 weeks old flies to increasing concentrations of benzaldehyde, 2,3-butanedione and 3-octanol was compared (n = 8 ± SEM). Graphs represent the quantification of neural peak ΔF responses (in %ΔF/F) in the strongest responding glomeruli to different concentrations of odors for 1 week and 7 weeks old flies (n = 8 ± SEM). All GCaMP3-fluorescence responses were calculated in %ΔF/F. All p-values were calculated via Student’s t-test (ns > 0.05, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01). (C, C´) GCaMP fluorescence changes are recorded in three different responsive glomeruli (DC2, DM6 and DP1) upon stimulation with 3-octanol (12 mM). (D-D´) GCaMP fluorescence changes were measured in three responsive glomeruli (DC1, DP1 und VC2) upon stimulation with 4-methylcyclohexanol (16 mM) in 1, 4 and 6 weeks old flies. (C, D) Maximum fluorescence changes of GCaMP3 upon odor stimulation in three different glomeruli. Scale bars: 20 µm. (C´, D´) Odor-induced fluorescence change of GCaMP3 is indicated as false color images (top row) for one representative animal. Fluorescence changes over time are shown in the lower row for each different glomerulus. The pink bars represent the time window of odor presentation. n = 9; one-way ANOVA with post hoc Bonferroni tests. ns, not significant (p>0.05). *p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001. (E–G) Expression of the Ca2+ sensor GCaMP3 in PNs under the control of GH146-Gal4 visualized in two focal planes in presynaptic boutons of projection neurons in calyces. Scale bars: 20 µm. (E) Representative image of in vivo two-photon imaging of fluorescence of GCaMP3 in PNs (GH146 > GCaMP) at their axonal extensions (boutons) in the mushroom body calyx is shown in the top image. Odor-induced fluorescence changes of GCaMP3 are indicated as false color images (bottom image) for one representative animal. (F) Maximal fluorescence changes of GCaMP3 in individual responsive boutons and (G) number of responsive boutons upon stimulation with 3-octanol (12 mM), 4-methylcyclohexanol (16 mM) or linalool (11 mM) in the two imaged focal planes. n = 9–11; one-way ANOVA with post hoc Bonferroni tests. ns, not significant (p>0.05). *p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001. All traces represent mean ±SEM of ΔF/F values. Box plots indicate means, medians, interquartile ranges, and 1–99% ranges.

Figure 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2 and Figure 2—figure supplements 1—3.
elife-32018-fig2-data1.xlsx (134.3KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.009

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Olfactory receptor neuron are not affected by aging.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Expression of a reporter construct for specific olfactory sensory neurons (OSN) in 1–10 weeks old flies with a transgenic reporter construct (Or42b-Gal4;UAS-mCD8GFP). Scale bars: 15 µm. Box plot shows median and upper/lower quartiles for the number of OR42b neurons in 1–10 weeks old flies. No difference in OSN number was detected between young (1 week) and older flies (10 weeks). P-value was calculated via one-way ANOVA (ns >0.05, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001). (B) The size of OSN cell bodies did not change during aging (OR42b > mCD8 GFP labeled cell bodies of 1 week vs. 10 weeks old flies; N = 20 flies per group, n = 71 and 72 neurons). P-value was calculated using Standard t-test. (C) Number of all ORs, IRs, and GRs upregulated and downregulated (non-significantly) in 7 weeks old fly antenna during aging. In addition, some receptors remained unchanged between the two conditions. (D) Volcano plot of RNA-sequencing data of selected olfactory receptor genes displaying the receptors that are involved in recognition of the tested odorants. Only genes above the cutoff of –log10 (p-value adjusted (padj)) are considered significantly changed. (E) Scatter plot displaying the correlation of gene expression between samples from 1 and 7 weeks old brains. The high correlation indicates that the majority of genes in the brain remains unchanged, while a smaller number of genes change their expression.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Olfactory receptor neurons still respond to odors in aged animals.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A) Schematic illustration of the electrophysiology setup. (B–H) Neural activity of olfactory sensory neurons (OSNs) in electrophysiological single sensilum recordings (SSR) in response to 10 mM of attractive (2,3-butanedione, hexanoic acid, 1-propanol) and aversive (acetophenone,1-octen-3-ol, Benzaldehyde, CO2, 3-octanol) odors. The responses were compared between 1 week and 5 weeks old flies (n = 8 ± SEM). Each graph shows responses (spike/sec) on the Y-axis while the X-axis indicates the age (weeks) of the flies. (n = 8). Sample response traces are displayed on the right side of each graph. (J) Neural activity response (spike/sec) of young (1 week) and old (7 weeks) flies that show normal aversion (responders) and no aversion (non-responders) in T-maze assay, to aversive odor 3-octanol. The flies were sorted by behavioral performances before the SSR experiments. Responders were flies that showed the expected young fly behavioural response to an odor, while non-responder flies did not respond to an odor as expected in the olfactory behaviour assay. Y-axis shows neural response (spike/sec) whereas X-axis indicates the concentration of the odor (n = 8). (K) Neural activity response (spike/sec) of young (1 week) and old (7 weeks) flies that show normal attraction (responders) and no attraction (non-responders) in the T-maze assay, to attractive odor 2,3-butanedione. Y-axis shows neural responses (spike/sec), whereas the X-axis indicates the concentration of the odor (n = 8). These data suggest that behavioral changes do not correlate with responses of OSNs to odors.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Older flies show strongly improved behavior to higher odor concentrations.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

(A-D) Schematic illustration of the T-maze olfactory assays (top, left). Olfactory preference index of the 1 week (orange) and 7 weeks (grey) old wildtype (Canton S) flies to standard 1 mM and increased 10 mM attractive (2,3-butanedione, putrescine) and aversive (benzaldehyde, 3-octanol) odors in the T-maze assay. Y-axis indicates the preference index (P.I.) to odors, while the X-axis denotes concentration of the tested odors. Note that the increase in odor concentration strongly improves the flies’ performance in the test suggesting that flies suffer from decreased sensitivity but not from failure to recognize and evaluate the odor. Box plot show median and upper/lower quartiles (n = 8, 60 flies/trial 30 ♀ and 30 ♂).

PNs transfer their information mainly to two higher brain centers, the MB calyx and the LH. To characterize PN function further, we also imaged GCaMP fluorescence changes at the level of PN boutons in the calyx (Figure 2E–G). Remarkably, here the response per PN bouton was not significantly weaker between flies of different ages (Figure 2F). However, the number of responsive boutons was strongly reduced in old (4, 6 weeks) as compared to young flies (1 week; Figure 2G. Therefore, PN function is affected by aging. Importantly, not only their dendritic response is reduced in a glomerulus-specific manner, their axonal output to higher brain centers drastically diminishes with age. The strength of this decline indicates that primarily axons and their synaptic output regions might degenerate functionally or anatomically due to aging-related mechanisms.

These cellular data suggested that flies suffer from reduced odor sensitivity possibly due to a decline of PN function and a subsequent reduction in information that arrives at higher brain centers. Hence, we wondered whether the animals were still capable of odor recognition and valuation, if compensated for the loss in sensitivity. Indeed, a 10-fold increase in odor concentration was sufficient to markedly improve the old flies’ olfactory choice behavior indicating that old flies albeit a decline in odor sensitivity still recognize and correctly valuate a given odor (Figure 2—figure supplement 3).

To unravel the mechanism of this functional decline, we analyzed PN morphology in more detail (Figure 3A). In contrast to OSNs, the number of PNs labeled by the reporter GH146 >mCD8-GFP was mildly but significantly reduced in old as compared to younger flies (Figure 3B). In addition to the mild loss of labeled PNs, their cell bodies had shrunk in older animals as compared to younger animals (Figure 3C). Similar observations were made for other aging neurons including mitral cells in aged humans (Sama-ul-Haq et al., 2008). Importantly, the pan-neuronally expressed gene elav was somewhat upregulated relative to other genes in older brains as compared to its relative expression in younger brains (Figure 3F) suggesting that neurons were not selectively lost as compared to other cell types (e.g. glia) in aging Drosophila brains. Nevertheless, we observed a significant decrease in ToPro staining, which labels cell nuclei in several areas including the AL (Figure 3G,H). Despite of this, the expression level of the neuropil marker N-cadherin (Ncad) in the AL and in the region of the LH as judged by antibody staining was unchanged (see methods; Figure 3D). Similarly, the expression of the GCaMP reporter (normalized to Ncad staining) used for calcium imaging, was comparable, or even slightly increased in PN axon terminals of old as compared to young flies (Figure 3E). Therefore, the changes observed in PN odor responses are not likely to be attributable to gross morphological changes in PN anatomy.

Figure 3. Changes in axon and synapse integrity could affect projection neuron function.

Figure 3.

(A) Projection neurons (PNs) of a 1 and a 7 weeks old brains are labeled with a reporter line (GH146-Gal4;UASGCaMP3 or GH146 >GCaMP3) and stained with an anti-GFP antibody (green). AL, antennal lobe; cx, mushroom body calyx; LH, lateral horn; lateral cell body cluster is shown in dotted box. Scale bar: 25 µm. (B) Average number of PNs in the lateral, dorsal clusters and the total of both clusters. Orange boxes represent young flies (1 week), while grey boxes represent old flies (7 weeks) in all figures. There is a mild but significant decrease in the number of reporter-labeled PNs in aged flies (Students t-test, n = 19–21) (C) Average diameter of projection neuron cell body sizes of 1 (orange) and 7 (grey) weeks old flies. The cell bodies of PNs of aged flies are significantly smaller (Students t-test, n = 19–21). (D) The box plot shows that there is no change in the expression of NCad in the defined areas for quantification (antibody staining against N-cadherin as a synaptic marker) in 1 (orange) and 7 (grey) weeks old flies AL and LH (Students t-test, n = 19–21). (E) Normalized expression levels of the GCaMP reporter protein in PNs (GH146 >GCaMP3) in young and old flies. The expression was normalized to Ncad antibody staining. There is no reduction of GCaMP expression in old as compared to young flies, but instead a slight but significant increase (Students t-test, n = 19–21). (F) Volcano plot of RNA-sequencing data of selected genes displaying the genes that are downregulated and upregulated in 7 weeks old brains compared to 1 week old brains, respectively. Only genes above the cutoff of –log10 (p-value adjusted (padj)) are considered significantly changed (above black line). While several AChR receptors were significantly downregulated in the brain, this was not the case in the antenna (Figure 4—figure supplement 2C). In addition, several aging-related genes are upregulated in older brains. Selected genes are displayed and were color-labeled by gene ontology analysis (orange: aging; green: neuronal function). (G) Reporter construct showing the localization of acetylcholine receptor (AChR) Dα7 (GH146-Gal4;UAS-Dα7-GFP, stained with anti-GFP antibody (green)) and ToPro nuclear marker (shown in pink) in the AL and lateral horn (LH). There is a decline at PN postsynaptic sites in the AL supporting an aging-related decline in the integrity of cholinergic synapses. For instance, the localization of Dα7 at presynaptic terminals and axons is lost in old flies (n = 20/20) in contrast to young animals (n = 0/20). See missing signal in axon and presynaptic terminals in the MB calyx and LH. Scale bar: 25 µm (H) Quantification of mean gray value (MGV) of ToPro staining of cell bodies in the area of the LH revealed a decrease in the number of cells in old as compared to young flies (n = 20). (I) A box plot shows a significant reduction in the AChR Dα7 reporter construct signal (mean grey value, MGV) of 7 weeks old flies (grey) compared to 1 week old flies (orange) at the level of the AL. Box plots show median and upper/lower quartiles. All p-values represent: ns > 0.05, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001. (J) Representative images of antennal lobes of 1 and 7 weeks old flies. Brains express the reporter mito-mcherry in PNs (GH146-Gal4;UAS-mito-mcherry; anti-RFP, red) and are stained for anti-ChAT (blue). (K) Representative images of the mushroom body calyx of 1 and 7 weeks old flies. (L) Quantification of relative expression of a mitochondria reporter (GH146-Gal4;UAS-mito-mcherry) and ChAT in AL, LH, and calyx. Note that mitochondria and ChAT staining are significantly reduced in the MB calyx as compared to an mito-mcherry or ChAT expression in other parts of the brain in old flies as compared to younger animals (see methods). This suggests that ChAT does not decrease equally in all brain parts, but in particular in areas such as the MB calyx. Graphs display mean relative levels ± SEM. Student’s t-test: ns > 0.05, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001. (M) Confocal and high-resolution STED microscopy images in the calyx of flies expressing BRP-shortGFP under control of GH146-Gal4 driver line. Green and magenta represent anti-GFP and anti-Drep2C-Term immunostaining, respectively. White squares in (M, left column) indicate the magnified region in (M, right column). Scale bars represent 2 μm in (M, left) and 0.5 μm in (M, right). (N) Number of active zones and (O) postsynaptic densities significantly decrease upon aging. n = 10–12; Student’s t-test. ***p<0.001. Box plots indicate means, medians, interquartile ranges, and 1–99% ranges.

Figure 3—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.011

We next focused on expression and localization of markers of neuronal function to pinpoint a cellular defect in the PNs that could explain the observed functional decline. As a reduction in cholinergic signaling accompanies most forms of neurodegeneration and natural aging (Doty, 2012), and because PNs are cholinergic, we analyzed the expression of genes involved in cholinergic neurotransmission with RNA-sequencing. Our analysis revealed a significant decline in mRNA abundance of several Acetylcholine receptor (AChR) subunits (Figure 3F). By contrast, some previously implicated marker genes of systemic aging and lifespan did not change or increased in their expression (Figure 3F). Expression levels are only one factor that might impact on cellular function. The proper transport and localization of relevant proteins represents another critical point. As a proxy for synaptic integrity of the PNs, we expressed transgenic reporter constructs producing synapse-localized fluorescent marker proteins. Such a reporter construct for the localization of Acetylcholine receptors expressed in PNs (GH146 > Dα7-GFP) revealed a moderate, but significant signal reduction of this postsynaptic marker at PN postsynaptic sites in the AL indicating an aging-related change at the post-synapse (Figure 3G,I).

It has been proposed that axonal degeneration precedes and possibly leads to eventual neuronal loss in neurodegenerative diseases (Kurowska et al., 2016). Indeed, we observed that the Dα7-GFP reporter construct, which in young animals localizes not only to the post-synapse, but also, albeit to a markedly lower extend, to the axon and presynaptic terminals of PNs (in 20/20 animals analyzed) was completely absent from presynaptic terminals and axons in old flies (0/20 flies showed reporter labeling in axon, MB calyx (cx) and lateral horn (LH)) (Figure 3G). In mammals, AChRs are also found post- and pre-synaptically, where they modulate and enhance synaptic signaling (MacDermott et al., 1999).

Given the dramatic reduction of responsive presynaptic PN boutons in the MB calyx and the reduced response in the AL observed by GCaMP imaging (see Figure 2), we employed antibody staining against the enzyme ChAT (Choline Acetyltransferase) required for the production of Acetylcholine at synapses of cholinergic neurons such as the PNs. Quantification of these stained brains indeed revealed a significant reduction in ChAT positive puncta in the AL and at the level of the MB calyx (Figure 3J–L). ChAT staining in the LH, by contrast, remained relatively stable (Figure 3L). Beyond a change in synaptic proteins in the aged synapse, several studies in animal models including flies implicated mitochondrial dysfunction in neuronal, in particular axonal degeneration (Court and Coleman, 2012; Humphrey et al., 2012; Valadas et al., 2015). The current hypotheses for why axonal mitochondria are more vulnerable include the remote location of mitochondria in presynapses from the cell body, and a high metabolic activity including calcium homeostasis (Court and Coleman, 2012). Indeed, we observed a strong reduction in the number of puncta that were positive for a reporter construct for mitochondria (GH146 > mito-mcherry; (Vagnoni and Bullock, 2016) at the level of the MB calyx (Figure 3K,L), but not in the AL or LH area (Figure 3J,L). These results suggests that mitochondria are indeed depleted, lost, or not replenished selectively close to the remote synapse in the calyx, while postsynaptic sites of the PNs in the AL are seemingly maintained. Why LH synapses are not affected as compared to calyx synapses, given that they are actually further away from the cell bodies than their calycal counterparts, remains currently unknown. Nevertheless, specific metabolic or synaptic characteristics of these different synapses and their postsynaptic partners could be part of the explanation.

In the light of an apparent reduction of ChAT and mitochondria (or at least a mitochondria reporter), we analyzed the integrity of these boutons in more detail using the high-resolution microscopy technique STED (Stimulated emission depletion; [Kittel et al., 2006; Willig et al., 2006]). To this end, the expression of a GFP-tagged version of the presynaptic protein bruchpilot was expressed in PNs (GH146 > BRP-shortGFP; Figure 3M). In line with the strong reduction of odor-responsive boutons, quantitative STED analysis of this reporter revealed a strong decrease in the density of presynaptic active zones over the MB calyx (Figure 3N). Correspondingly, the number of postsynaptic densities as revealed by antibody staining against the postsynaptic density marker Drep2 was reduced by a similar margin (Figure 3O) (Andlauer et al., 2014).

These data indicate that a degeneration of cholinergic PNs, in particular of their axon, their presynaptic boutons and the corresponding synapses (i.e. active zones and postsynaptic densities), resembling aspects of neurodegeneration in humans, could be involved in the loss of the sense of smell.

Having identified a potential neuronal target of aging in the olfactory system, we next addressed the genetic mechanisms underpinning aging-associated olfactory decline. Several key genes and mechanisms have been identified in different model systems that contribute to systemic aging (López-Otín et al., 2013). To assess the mechanism of olfactory aging in our case, we used in vivo RNAi to knock-down the expression of candidate genes previously implicated in systemic aging and lifespan in the entire nervous system using a pan-neural transgenic driver line, elav-Gal4, or loss of function mutants. Of the ~20 genes analyzed the knock-down of only one gene, SOD2 (mitochondrial form of superoxide dismutase) significantly affected olfactory behavior in young animals (Figure 4A; Figure 4—figure supplement 1A,B). By contrast, knock-down of the cytoplasmic form, SOD1, only resulted in a smaller but non-significant decrease in olfactory behavior (Figure 4A). Sirt2 RNAi knock-down led to a mild, but not significant increase in odor attraction, possibly reflecting the fact that Sirt2 is upregulated in older brains (Figure 4A). More generally, it will be interesting to test overexpression of genes upregulated in aged brain for their role in neurodegeneration in the future.

Figure 4. Superoxide dismutase two is selectively required in projection neurons.

(A) RNAi knockdown behavioral screening of selected candidate genes involved in systemic aging, in the T-maze assay. RNAi Knockdown of SOD2 (superoxide dismutase 2) pan-neuronally using the line elav-Gal4 (elav >SOD2 i) in 3 weeks old flies significantly reduced olfactory attraction to 2,3 butanedione. In addition, two mutants (3 weeks of age) of autophagy genes did not show a defect in olfactory preference behavior (see Figure 4—figure supplement 1A) (B) RNAi knock-down of SOD2 in OSNs had no effect on the flies’ olfactory preference suggesting that OSNs might be less sensitive to oxidative stress. Box plots show olfactory PIs of 1 week old flies expressing an RNAi knock-down construct for SOD2 under the control of ORCO, which is expressed broadly in OSNs (ORCO-Gal4;UAS-SOD2-RNAi) and their genetic controls to 2,3-butanedione, benzaldehyde and 3-octanol in the T-maze assay. (C) RNAi knock-down of SOD2 in PNs using the GH146-Gal4 (GH146-Gal4;UAS-SOD2-RNAi), results in strongly diminished olfactory preference of flies to 2,3-butanedione, benzaldehyde and 3-octanol. Box plots show median and upper/lower quartiles (n = 8, 60 flies/trial, 30 ♀ and 30 ♂). All p-values were calculated via one-way ANOVA with the Bonferroni multiple comparison posthoc test (ns >0.05, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001).

Figure 4—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4 and Figure 4—figure supplements 1 and 3.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.016

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Autophagy mutants show normal olfactory behavior.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) Olfactory preference index of two different autophagy gene mutant (atg-/-) flies (3 weeks old) to attractive (putrescine, 2,3-butanedione) and aversive odors (benzaldehyde, 3-octanol) in the T-maze assay suggests that autophagy is not involved in innate olfaction. Box plot shows median and upper/lower quartiles (n = 8, 60 flies/trial 30 ♀ and 30 ♂). (B) Box plots show olfactory preference index (P.I.) of 1 week old transgenic flies carrying an in vivo RNAi construct to knock-down SOD2 pan-neuronally under the control of elav-Gal4 (elav-Gal4;UAS-SOD2-RNAi) and their genetic controls to 2,3-butanedione, benzaldehyde and 3-octanol in the T-maze assay. P-values were calculated via one-way ANOVA with the Bonferroni multiple comparison posthoc test (A) or with the t-test (B) (ns >0.05, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001).

Figure 4—figure supplement 2. SOD1 and SOD2 are downregulated in aged brains.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(A) RNA-seq expression of the total number of genes expressed in the 1 week (orange) and 7 weeks (grey) fly antenna. Y-axis represents total number of genes expressed in the antenna, while the x-axis shows the age of flies in weeks. Doughnut chart represents the number of genes upregulated (green) and downregulated (light brown) in 7 weeks old antennae compared to one week old. (B) RNA-seq expression of the total number of genes expressed in 1 week (orange) and 7 weeks (grey) old fly brains. Y-axis represents total number of genes expressed in the brain, while the x-axis shows the age of flies in weeks. Doughnut chart represents the number of genes upregulated (green) and downregulated (light brown) in 7 weeks old brains, compared to one week old. (C) Volcano plot of RNA-sequencing data of selected genes displaying the genes that are downregulated and upregulated in 7 weeks old antennae and brains, respectively. Only genes above the cutoff of –log10 (p-value adjusted (padj)) are considered significantly changed. Note that SOD1 and 2 expression does not change significantly in the antenna. Both genes, however, are significantly downregulated in the brain of old flies. (D) Box plot show olfactory preference index (P.I.) of 1 week old transgenic flies expressing a knockdown-construct of SOD2 pan-neuronally under the mature neuron driver nsyb (nsyb-Gal4;UAS-SOD2-RNAi) and their genetic controls to 2,3-butanedione, benzaldehyde and 3-octanol in the T-maze assay. Box plot shows median and upper/lower quartiles (n = 8, 60 flies/trial 30 ♀ and 30 ♂). All p-values were calculated via two-way ANOVA with the Bonferroni multiple comparison posthoc test (ns >0.05, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001).

Figure 4—figure supplement 3. Lifespan of experimental and control groups.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3.

(A) Survivorship of flies with RNAi knockdown of SOD2 in projection neurons using the GH146-Gal4 (GH146-Gal4;UAS-SOD2-RNAi) and their genetic control (GH146-Gal4) shown in line graphs. Y-axis represents % survivorship, while the x-axis shows the age of flies in weeks (n = 200 ± SEM). (B) Survivorship of flies with overexpression of SOD2 in PNs using the GH146-Gal4 (GH146-Gal4;UAS-SOD2) and their genetic controls (GH146-Gal4) shown in the line graph. Y-axis represents % survivorship, while the x-axis shows the age of the flies in weeks (n = 100 ± SEM, three replicates). (C) Survivorship of flies raised from day one on Resveratrol (grey line), L. plantarum (lime green), and standard fly food (magenta). Y-axis represents % survivorship, while the x-axis shows the age of flies in weeks (n = 100 ± SEM, three replicates). Please note that these survival curves are not intended to test the effect of the treatment on lifespan, but rather to correlate the lifespan of individual test and control groups with their behavioral performance to exclude unspecific effects of general health.

SODs protect against reactive oxygen species (ROS) and SOD2 appears to be required for normal lifespan (Kirby et al., 2002; Oka et al., 2015). Our RNA-seq data revealed that while SOD1 and SOD2 expression remained unchanged in the antennae of old flies, both genes were expressed at lower levels in older brains (Figure 4—figure supplement 2A–C).

In addition to SOD1 and SOD2, other genes were significantly up- and downregulated in older brains. Among the most significant up-regulated GO terms were proteolysis and defense/immunity genes, while energy metabolism and oxidation process related genes ranked high in the list of downregulated genes. For instance, genes belonging to the GO term ‘oxidation-reduction process’ were found to be significantly downregulated (FDR < 0.01), which corresponds to 128% more genes than expected by chance (p=2.3e-14, Fisher Test). Similarly, 119 genes belonging to proteolysis GO term were found to be significantly upregulated (FDR < 0.01), which corresponds to 59% more genes than expected by chance (p=1.3e-8, Fisher Test) (Supplementary file 1 and 2). These results were also well in line with our finding of a reduction of mitochondria in PN synapses (see Figure 3).

We next tested whether any particular neuron type in the olfactory system was most vulnerable to oxidative stress, or whether this was a systemic effect. Notably, knock-down of SOD2 in OSNs (ORCO-Gal4) had no effect on the flies’ olfactory attraction or aversion compared to genetic controls (Figure 4B). By contrast, knock-down of SOD2 in PNs resulted in a significantly reduced odor attraction and odor aversion similar to the knock-down of SOD2 in all mature neurons (Figure 4C and Figure 4—figure supplement 2D). While ubiquitous or pan-neural knock-down of SOD2 with the same construct as used here is lethal (flies die after ~3 days; (Kirby et al., 2002) and our own observations), knock-down of SOD2 in PNs did not reduce the flies’ life span significantly as compared to the genetic controls, which were also used in the behavioral experiments (Figure 4—figure supplement 3A). It is important to note, however, that given the well-known effects of genetic background on lifespan, we limit our conclusion to the correlation of the lifespan of each individual test or control group to their respective olfactory behavioral performance. In other words, an extremely short- or long-lived animal might show unspecific reasons for behavioral deficits or improvements. Based on this, a reduced lifespan is unlikely to explain the strong behavioral phenotype, suggesting a high vulnerability of PNs and their central role in olfactory decline.

Does lack of SOD2 in PNs result in changes at the level of neuronal function and morphology similar to what we observed for natural aging? To answer this, we carried out some of the same analysis as done for aged flies (see Figures 2 and 3). First, we counted the number of PNs in 1 week old flies where SOD2 was knocked-down under the control of GH146-Gal4 (GH146 > SOD2 i). In contrast to the aged flies, the number of GCaMP reporter expressing PNs (GH146 > GCaMP; with or without SOD2i) was not significantly different between the experimental group and controls (Figure 5A,B). However, the size of the PN cell bodies was significantly smaller upon knock-down of SOD2 as compared to controls (Figure 5C), similar to the decline of cell body size in old flies (see Figure 3). Quantification of the expression of GCaMP stained with an anti-GFP antibody in the LH and calyx revealed a small reduction, which was significant in the LH but not in the calyx, suggesting that the relative expression level of GCaMP upon normalization to staining outside the LH and calyx, respectively, remained at a similar level as compared to controls (Figure 5D). By contrast, antibody staining against ChAT showed that the expression of this enzyme was significantly reduced in PN boutons in the MB calyx (Figure 5E) indicating a reduction of functional cholinergic synapses – again in line with the results in aged brains.

Figure 5. SOD2 deprived PNs resemble neurons in aged brains.

Figure 5.

(A) SOD2 RNAi (GH146-Gal4;UAS-SOD2i) expressing and SOD2 RNAi negative controls labeled with the reporter line (GH146-Gal4;UASGCaMP3 or GH146 >GCaMP3) and stained with an anti-GFP antibody (green). AL, antennal lobe; cx, mushroom body calyx; LH, lateral horn; lateral cell body cluster is shown in dotted box. Scale bar: 25 µm. (B) Average number of PNs in the lateral, dorsal clusters and the total of both clusters. Orange boxes represent 1 week control flies, while blue boxes represent flies carrying GH146 >SOD2 i in all figures. There is no significant decrease in the number of reporter-labeled PNs upon SOD2 knock-down (Students t-test, n = 19–21) (C) Average diameter of projection neuron cell body sizes of controls (orange) and SOD2 knock-down flies (blue). The cell bodies of PNs are significantly smaller when SOD2 is reduced exclusively in PNs (Student’s t-test, n = 19–21). (D) Scatter plots showing normalized GCaMP signal stained with α-GFP antibody (MGV). The intensity of staining within the LH (upper panel) and within the calyx (bottom panel) was normalized to the background signal in a non-GFP positive brain area of the same brain (Student’s t-test, n = 28). (E) Mean gray value (MGV) of anti-ChAT antibody staining in the MB calyx. Note that knocking-down SOD2 in PNs significantly reduced the ChAT signal (Student’s t-test, n = 28). (F) Representative image of in vivo two-photon imaging of fluorescence of GCaMP3 in PNs (GH146 >GCaMP) at their axonal extensions (boutons) in the mushroom body calyx for test (GH146 >GCaMP;SOD2i) and control flies (GH146 >GCaMP;+). Odor-induced fluorescence change of GCaMP3 are indicated as false color images (right column) for one representative animal of each genotype. Scale bars: 20 µm. (G) maximal fluorescence changes of GCaMP3 in individual responsive boutons and (H) number of responsive boutons upon stimulation with 3-octanol (12 mM) or 4-methylcyclohexanol (16 mM) in the two imaged focal planes. n = 10; Student’s t-test. (ns >0.05, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001). Box plots indicate means, medians, interquartile ranges, and 1–99% ranges.

Figure 5—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.018

Next, we tested what the consequences of SOD2 lack meant for PN function by using two photon in vivo calcium imaging of the PN boutons in the MB calyx (Figure 5F–H). While SOD2 knock-down did not affect the responses of individual PN boutons to odor stimulation, the number of responsive boutons was significantly reduced upon SOD2 knock-down in PNs (Figure 5G,H).

Taken together, reduction of SOD2 expression exclusively in PNs leads to behavioral, functional and anatomical phenotypes that resemble several of the phenotypes observed in naturally aged brains.

Systemic SOD2 overexpression can ameliorate memory deficits in a transgenic Alzheimer’s disease mouse model (Massaad et al., 2009). However, it is not known whether augmenting SOD2 exclusively in one neuron type, the so to speak potential seed or Achilles heel of degeneration, prevents aging-associated decline of an entire circuit and its ability to control and drive behavior. We found that overexpression of SOD2 exclusively in PNs fully rescued behavioral decline of 7 weeks old flies, which behaved just like their 1 week old genetic counterparts (Figure 6A and Figure 6—figure supplement 1A). Importantly, SOD2 overexpression in PNs did not significantly extend or shorten the average lifespan of this group of flies as compared to the used genetic controls, which were also used for behavioral analysis (Figure 4—figure supplement 3B). Although these results do not allow strong conclusions regarding the effect on lifespan, they do, nevertheless, indicate a more specific role of SOD2 in PNs, and that the behavioral improvement is unlikely to be a result of an improved overall fitness of these animals. Furthermore, SOD2 expression under the control of ORCO-Gal4 in OSNs did not rescue the behavior of old flies (Figure 6B and Figure 6—figure supplement 1B). We conclude that cholinergic PNs represent key targets and possibly a ‘seed neuron’ population in aging-associated decline of the sense of smell. These results suggest that aging-related degeneration and behavioral decline could be significantly delayed by preventing oxidative damage in only one or few neuron types (see also [Seeley, 2017]).

Figure 6. Expression of SOD2 in single neuron subtype fully rescues olfactory degeneration.

(A) Box plots show PIs of 1 week old (orange boxes) and 7 weeks old (grey boxes) transgenic flies overexpressing SOD2 exclusively in projection neurons (PNs) (GH146-Gal4;UAS-SOD2) and their genetic controls in response to attractive (2,3-butanedione) and aversive (3-octanol) odors. Note that expression of SOD2 exclusively in PNs fully rescues olfactory performance in 7 weeks old flies indicating that sensitivity to oxidative stress of PNs represents a key player in the aging-associated decline of the olfactory system. (B) Box plots show PIs of 1 week old (orange) and 7 weeks old (grey) transgenic flies overexpressing SOD2 under the control of ORCO-Gal4 in OSNs (ORCO-Gal4;UAS-SOD2) and their genetic controls in response to attractive (2,3-butanedione) and aversive (3-octanol) odors. Importantly, overexpression of SOD2 under the control of ORCO-Gal4 in OSNs had no effect on the behavior of old flies. (C) Box plots show preference of 1 week old and 7 weeks old flies raised on standard fly food (first two boxes) and 7 weeks old flies raised on standard fly food mixed with Resveratrol (third grey box) in response to attractive (2,3-butanedione) and aversive (3-octanol) odors. All p-values were calculated via t-test (ns >0.05, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001). In all figures, asterisks above a single bar refer to p-values of comparison to the control (7 weeks old flies in second bar). (D) Box plots show untreated and treated (flies were inoculated with the indicated bacterial strain after being pretreated to become germfree) 1 week old (light (treated) and dark orange (not germfree, standard conditions as in all other experiments before) and 7 weeks old (dark and light grey) flies. Treated flies were inoculated with Lactobacillus plantarum NI202877, L. plantarum WJL, Acetobacter pomorum (A.p.), while control flies were just raised on standard fly food. P.I.s of flies to the attractive odor 2,3-butanedione is shown. All p-values were calculated via two-way ANOVA with the Bonferroni multiple comparison posthoc test (ns >0.05, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001). Asterisks above a single box refer to p-values of comparison to the control (7 weeks old treated control). Box plot show median and upper/lower quartiles (n = 8, 60 flies/trial 30 ♀ and 30 ♂). (E) Summary and model of presented results. SOD2, the mitochondrial form of SOD, protects projection neurons (PN) from oxidative stress. Their vulnerability to oxidative stress and reactive oxygen species (ROS) appears to be the weak point of the olfactory system of Drosophila. The decline in function of PNs ultimately results in strongly reduced sensitivity to odors and accordingly diminishes behavioural responses.

Figure 6—source data 1. Source data for Figure 6 and Figure 6—figure supplement 1.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.021

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. SOD2 and antioxidants slow down aging-associated olfactory decline.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(A) Box plots show P.I.s of 1 week old (orange bars) and 7 weeks old (grey bars) transgenic flies overexpressing SOD2 exclusively in projection neurons (PNs) (GH146-Gal4;UAS-SOD2) and their genetic controls in response to attractive (putrescine) and aversive (benzaldehyde) odors. (B) Box plots show P.I.s of 1 week old (orange bars) and 7 weeks old (grey bars) transgenic flies overexpressing SOD2 under the control of ORCO-Gal4 in OSNs (ORCO-Gal4;UAS-SOD) and their genetic controls in response to attractive (putrescine) and aversive (3-octanol) odors. (C) Box plots show preference of 1 week old and 7 weeks old flies raised on standard fly food (first two bars) and 7 weeks old flies raised on standard fly food mixed with Resveratrol (third grey bar) in response to attractive (putrescine) and aversive (benzaldehyde) odors. All p-values were calculated via t-test (ns >0.05, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001). In all figures, asterisks above a single bar refer to p-values of comparison to the control (7 weeks old flies in second bar). (D) Box plots show 1–2 weeks old flies raised on standard food with solvent or on standard fly food supplemented with Resveratrol for 1 week. There is no significant difference between the treated and control flies (n = 8). (E) Box plots show untreated and treated (flies were inoculated with the indicated bacterial strain after being pretreated to become germfree) 1 week old (light (treated) and dark orange (not germfree, standard conditions as in all other experiments before) and 7 weeks old (dark and light grey) flies. Treated flies were inoculated with Lactobacillus plantarum NI202877, L. plantarum WJL, Acetobacter pomorum (A.p.), while control flies were just raised on standard fly food. P.I.s of flies to attractive (putrescine) and aversive (benzaldehyde, 3-octanol) odors is shown. All p-values were calculated via two-way ANOVA with the Bonferroni multiple comparison posthoc test (ns >0.05, *p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001). Asterisks above a single bar refer to p-values of comparison to the control (7 weeks old treated control). Box plot show median and upper/lower quartiles (n = 8, 60 flies/trial 30 ♀ and 30 ♂).

The overexpression of SOD2 does currently not provide a feasible way to protect the nervous system of humans during aging or during the onset or course of a neurodegenerative disease. A popular idea is that it might be possible to boost an organism’s ability to fight ROS by consuming a diet high in antioxidants (Vaiserman and Marotta, 2016). We found that a diet high in Resveratrol, a well-studied antioxidant shown to increase the expression of SOD2 in neurons (Fukui et al., 2010) and with potential benefits against AD (Granzotto and Zatta, 2014), protected from olfactory decline. The relatively small effect on the flies’ lifespan (Wood et al., 2004) (Figure 4—figure supplement 3C) is unlikely to explain the observed behavioral improvement (Figure 6C and Figure 6—figure supplement 1C). Notably, a 1 week Resveratrol treatment of younger flies did not affect olfactory behavior as compared to solvent fed flies (Figure 6—figure supplement 1D) indicating that Resveratrol might indeed counteract oxidative stress that builds up during aging. Thus, protection from oxidative stress, plausibly at least in part through protection of PNs, might help to maintain the function of the olfactory system in an aged individual.

Apart from certain diets and nutrients, the gut microbiome has been implicated in progression of Parkinson’s disease and aging (Kong et al., 2016; Scheperjans et al., 2015). Recent studies show that beneficial effects of microbiota are conserved between Drosophila and mouse (Schwarzer et al., 2016). For instance, the effects of malnutrition can be partially overcome by inoculating flies with a specific strain of Lactobacillus plantarum (L.p.WJL) or Acetobacter pomorum (A.p.) (Schwarzer et al., 2016). Importantly, another strain of L. plantarum (L.p.NI202877) did not produce the same effect suggesting a specific mechanism and not just the presence of any bacteria in the gut. Oral administration of L. plantarum, which colonizes the mammalian gut, led to a significant increase of SOD levels in serum and liver in a mouse aging model (Tang et al., 2016). Moreover, L.p.WJL modulates TOR and insulin signaling in Drosophila, both of which are regulators of lifespan (Storelli et al., 2011). We tested the effect of these microbiota on olfactory aging. L.p.WJL and A.p., but not L.p.NI202877, improved the old flies’ performance in olfactory preference assays significantly (Figure 6D and Figure 6—figure supplement 1E). We conclude that certain microbiota have a positive effect and slow-down aging-associated olfactory neurodegeneration.

Based on the data presented, we propose that due to their insufficient resistance to oxidative stress, which is facilitated by an aging-associated decrease in SOD levels, functional degeneration starting at the axon of specific cholinergic neurons is responsible for the decline of the olfactory circuit and the sense of smell (Figure 6E). A central role of cholinergic neurons in neurodegeneration seems conserved. In C. elegans, a decline in cholinergic signaling likely triggers an aging-associated decline in the sense of smell (Leinwand et al., 2015). In humans, cholinesterase inhibitors, which augment levels of acetylcholine in the brain, represent the main class of Alzheimer drugs (Canter et al., 2016). Using the Drosophila model, we showed that a specific type of cholinergic neurons plays a key role in the loss of the sense of smell. Our data therefore provides experimental evidence that declines in nervous system function are not due to a universal degeneration, but rather that specific neuronal subsets are primarily or even solely responsible and might trigger further degeneration throughout a neuronal network.

Why are certain neurons more sensitive than others? Similar to flies, axonal degeneration and decline of cholinergic neurotransmission play important roles in neurodegeneration in humans. Indeed, neurons with long axonal projections, broad input, high sensitivity and high action potential frequency, such as PNs (Wilson, 2013), might be particularly vulnerable to aging. The olfactory system of the fly could help to pinpoint the ‘Achilles heel’ of a neuron and aid the development of more targeted treatments by combining high-throughput genetic screening with drug or microbiota treatments.

Materials and methods

Key resources: Fly rearing and lines

Drosophila melanogaster stocks were raised on conventional cornmeal-agar medium at 25°C temperature and 60% humidity and a 12 hr light:12 hr dark cycle. The following fly lines were used to obtain experimental groups of flies for the different experiments:

  1. Canton S

  2. w1118

  3. Or42b-Gal4 (Bloomington stock (BL) # 9971)

  4. UAS-mCD8GFP (gift from L. Zipursky)

  5. GH146-Gal4 (gift from L. Zipursky)

  6. elav-Gal4;UAS-Dcr-2 (BL# 25750)

  7. RNAi: UAS-SOD2i (Bloomington stock (BL) # 24489; [Kirby et al., 2002]), UAS-SOD1i (BL# 24493), UAS-TORi (BL# 33627), UAS-InRi (VDRC ID 992), UAS-Sirt2i(a, BL#31613), UAS-Sirt2i(b, BL# 36868), UAS-Indyi (VDRC ID 9981), UAS-chicoi (BL# 28329), UAS-mthi (BL# 27495), UAS-FOXOi (VDRC ID 107786), UAS-FMR1i (BL# 27484)

  8. UAS-SOD2 (BL# 24494)

  9. nsyb-Gal4 (gift from L. Zipursky)

  10. UAS-GCaMP3 (gift from S. Sachse)

  11. ORCO-Gal4 (BL# 23292)

  12. Orco1 (BL# 23129)

  13. UAS-Dα7-GFP (gift from G. Tavosanis)

  14. UAS-BRP-shortGFP (Christiansen et al., 2011); Kremer et al., 2010)

  15. y[1], Atg8a[d4] (from [Pircs et al., 2012])

  16. w[1118]; Atg7[d77] (from [Juhász et al., 2007])

The lines were obtained from Bloomington (http://flystocks.bio.indiana.edu/) or the Vienna Drosophila Resource Center (VDRC) stock center (http://stockcenter.vdrc.at/control/main) unless indicated otherwise.

Further key resources

  1. Resveratrol (Sigma-Aldrich, R5010)

  2. Lactobacillus plantarum or Acetobacter pomorum (all strains were a gift of Francois Leulier)

  3. Antibodies and additional reagents used for molecular biology are described in the respective methods section.

Aging and recording of lifespan

100 flies (50 males and 50 females) were flipped onto fresh food every second day for up to 10 weeks. For lifespan recordings, flies were counted every other day or once a week. Please note that conclusions regarding lifespan might be confounded by genetic background etc. as flies have not been backcrossed for 10 generations or more. However, the lifespan experiments were carried out solely to be able to correlate the lifespan of each group of flies to its respective behavioral, physiological, or anatomical phenotype.

Olfactory T-Maze assay

1–10 weeks old flies collected on the same day were used for all experiments and tested on the same day side by side. For experiments with RNAi, experimental flies and genetic controls were raised at 30°C to enhance the effect of the RNAi. Flies were tested in groups of ~60 (30 females and 30 males) in a T-maze and were allowed 1 min to make a decision to go into either arm. Experimentation was carried out within climate-controlled boxes at 25°C and 60% rH in the dark. 50 μl of fresh odor solution (all odors were purchased at Sigma-Aldrich) at different concentrations diluted in distilled water or paraffin oil applied on Whatman chromatography paper was provided in the odor tube, except for 1% CO2, which was diluted using a custom-build setup with mass flow controller from pure CO2 and bottled air. Control tubes were filled with 50 μl odorant solvent or compressed air in the case of CO2. Unless otherwise indicated, 1 mM of odor dilution was used. After experimentation, the number of flies in each tube was counted. An olfactory preference index (P.I.) was calculated by subtracting the number of flies on the test odor site from the number of flies on the control site and normalizing by the total number of flies. Statistical analysis was performed using ANOVA and the Bonferroni multiple comparisons posthoc test using Prism GraphPad 6.

Visual T-maze assay

Experiments were essentially carried out as described above, but with a visual instead of an olfactory stimulus. A modified transparent T-maze apparatus was used to allow stimulation with light. The two arms of the T-maze were illuminated with blue LED emitting lights (465–470 nm) on one side or red LED lights (625–630 nm) on the other side. by subtracting the number of flies on the test odor site from the number of flies on the control site and normalizing by the total number of flies. Statistical analysis was performed using ANOVA and the Bonferroni multiple comparisons posthoc test using Prism GraphPad 6.

In some experiments flies were videotracked by using an infrared camera on the top of the climate box and analyzed with ctrax software.

Treatment with Lactobacillus plantarum or Acetobacter pomorum

200 fly embryos (12 hr AEL) were collected in a petri dish, washed with 10% bleach once, in 70% ethanol twice, in PBS once, and in sterile, distilled water twice. These embryos are considered germfree and are referred to as treated. Embryos were then transferred into new food bottles. An overnight L. plantarum culture was concentrated to OD:200 and 300 µl were added into the fly food every third day.

Treatment with resveratrol

For Resveratrol-supplemented diets, Resveratrol was dissolved in 100% ethanol and added to fly food to a final concentration of 100 μM. All diets contained 1.5% agar and equal amounts of ethanol.

Anatomy

Adult fly brains were dissected, fixed, and stained as described previously (Hartl et al., 2011). Briefly, brains were dissected in cold PBS, fixed with paraformaldehyde (2%, overnight at 4°C or for 2 hr at RT), washed in PBS, 0.1% Triton X-100, 10% donkey serum and stained overnight at 4° C or for 2 hr at RT with the primary and after washes in PBS, 0.1% Triton X-100 with the secondary antibody using the same conditions. All microscopic observations were made at an Olympus FV-1000 or at a Leica SP8 confocal microscope. Images were processed using ImageJ and Photoshop. The following antibodies were used: chicken anti-GFP and anti-RFP (molecular probes, 1:100), rat anti-N-cadherin (anti-N-cad DN-Ex #8, Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, 1:100), and mouse anti-ChAT (Yasuyama et al., 1995). Secondary antibodies used were: anti-chicken Alexa 488 (molecular probes, 1:250), a-mouse Alexa 633 (molecular probes, 1:250) and anti-rabbit Alexa 549 (molecular probes, 1:250), respectively.

For image quantification, all brains were processed at the same time using the same conditions. Images were taken at the exact same settings. All analysis was done blind to the genotype or age of the flies. For the quantification of cell bodies, neurons were counted section-by-section either directly at the confocal or using ImageJ/FIJI software. For antibody staining or reporter construct expression at the level of the lateral horn (LH), images were Z-projected into a single image. Regions of interest (ROI) were drawn around the LH in each image and quantification was carried out using FIJI ImageJ software. For quantification of stainings in the antennal lobe (AL) and calyx, three sections at similar levels of the structures were chosen. ROIs were drawn around the AL or calyx in each section and combined to quantify each individual brain. For each brain, only one LH, calyx and AL were chosen randomly for quantifications. For quantification of staining intensity, different strategies were used depending on the staining. For GCaMP staining, all stainings were normalized to Ncad antibody staining. For Dα7-GFP and ToPro staining, staining intensity was measured as mean grey value (MGV) and normalized to the size of the area that was measured. For quantification of ChAT and mito-RFP staining in Figure 3, ratios of stainings were calculated by dividing the MGV in the area of interest by another brain area of equal size next to it, that is, in a region of the brain just below the calyx that did not express the protein or the reporter or that did not belong to the region of interest. With the aim to calculate how brain areas were affected by aging relative to each other. A decrease in the ratio showed that the MGV in for instance the calyx was more strongly decreased as compared to another brain area. For Figure 5, ChAT staining was quantified as MGV. Measurements of cell body size diameter were carried out in ImageJ/FIJI. 3 cell bodies were measured per section in several sections of the brain or antenna. The averages of the measured diameters were used for statistical analysis.

All statistical analysis was carried out with GraphPad Prism software. The exact statistics used in each experiment are indicated in the respective figure legends. Of note, different absolute values are the result of the use of two different confocal microscopes; the newer Leica SP8 was significantly more sensitive as compared to the older Olympus FV1000. As the result, individual settings for each microscope were used and absolute number can only be compared within each individual graph.

Immunostaining for STED microscopy

Brains were dissected in Ringer’s solution (pH 7.3, 290–310 mOsm) containing 5 mM HEPES-NaOH, 130 mM NaCl, 5 mM KCl, 2 mM MgCl2, 2 mM CaCl2, and 36 mM sucrose, fixed in 4% PFA for 2 hr at 4°C and washed three times for 10 min each in PBS containing 0.6% Triton X-100 (PBT) at room temperature. Samples were incubated for 2 hr in PBT containing 2% BSA and 5% normal goat serum. Subsequently, the samples were incubated in the primary antibody diluted in block solution at 4°C overnight. For staining BRP-shortGFP, FluoTag-X4 anti-GFP, Abberior Star 635P-conjugated (1:100, NanoTag Biotechnologies, N0304-Ab635P) and for staining postsynaptic densities of Kenyon cells, rabbit anti-Drep- 2C-Term (1: 500, Andlauer et al., 2014) were used. Samples were washed three times for at least 30 min each in PBT containing 2% BSA (PAT) at room temperature, subsequently incubated with secondary antibody diluted in PAT overnight at 4°C. As secondary antibody AlexaFluor-594-coupled goat anti-rabbit (1: 200, Thermo Fisher Scientific, A-11012) was used. Brains were washed at least six times for 30 min each in PBT and embedded in Prolong Gold Antifade (Invitrogen). Samples were stored for 24 hr at room temperature followed by 48 hr at 4°C, before STED microscopy.

Two-Color STED imaging

STED imaging with time-gated detection was performed on a Leica SP8 TCS STED microscope (Leica Microsystems) equipped with a pulsed white light excitation laser (NKT Photonics). Dual-channel STED imaging was performed by sequentially exciting Abberior Star 635P and Alexa 594 at 646 nm and 598 nm, respectively. Both dyes were depleted with a 775 nm STED laser. Three optical sections at a distance of 250 nm were acquired with an HC PL APO CS2 100×/1.40 N.A. oil objective (Leica Microsystems), a scanning format of 1024 × 1024 pixel, eight bit sampling and six fold zoom, yielding a pixel dimension of 18.9 × 18.9 nm. Time-gated detection was set from 0.3 to 6 ns for all dyes. To minimize thermal drift, the microscope was housed in a heatable incubation chamber (LIS Life Imaging Services).

For quantifications, raw data obtained from STED imaging were de-convoluted using Huygens Professional (Scientific Volume Imaging, Netherlands) and analyzed by a custom-written ImageJ macro. Briefly, z-stacks of de-convoluted images were projected and each channel was segmented using the auto local threshold method ‘Phansalkar’ followed by watershed separation of touching segmented particles. The number, area fraction, and feret diameter from all segmented particles with sizes from 5 to 200 pixels were measured in >4 separate images per calyx region.

In vivo calcium imaging

For calcium imaging experiments with an epifluorescence microscope, GCaMP3 was expressed under the control of GH146-Gal4 (flies were heterozygous for both transgenes). Female flies were prepared in a modified setup according to a method previously reported (Hussain et al., 2016b2016b). In vivo preparations were imaged using a Leica DM6000FS fluorescent microscope equipped with a 40x water immersion objective and a Leica DFC360 FX fluorescent camera. All images were acquired with the Leica LAS AF E6000 image acquisition suit. Images were acquired for 20 s at a rate of 20 frames per second with 4 × 4 binning mode. To calculate the normalized change in the relative fluorescence intensity, we used the following formula: ∆F/F = 100(Fn-F0)/F0, where Fn is the nth frame after stimulation and F0 is the averaged basal fluorescence of 15 frames before stimulation. The peak fluorescence intensity change is calculated as the mean of normalized trace over a 2 s time window during the stimulation period. During all measurements, the exposure time was kept constant at 20 ms. For all experiments with odor stimulation, the stimulus was applied 5 s after the start of each measurement. A continuous and humidified airstream (2000 ml/min) was delivered to the fly throughout the experiment via an 8 mm diameter glass tube positioned 10 mm away from the preparation. A custom-made odor delivery system (Smartec, Martinsried), consisting of mass flow controllers (MFC) and solenoid valves, was used for delivering a continuous airstream and stimuli in all experiments. In all experiments, stimuli were delivered for 500 ms, and during stimulations the continuous flow was maintained at 2000 ml/min. For odorant stimulations, 1 ml of a precise concentration was filled in the odor delivery cup and the collected airspace odor was injected into the main airstream to give 0 mM, 0.1 mM, 1 mM, and 10 mM final concentrations for 500 ms without changing airstream strength. To measure the fluorescent intensity change, the region of interest was delineated by hand and the resulting time trace was used for further analysis. To calculate the normalized change in the relative fluorescence intensity, we used the following formula: ΔF/F = 100(FnF0)/F0, where Fn is the nth frame after stimulation and F0 is the averaged basal fluorescence of 15 frames before stimulation. The peak fluorescence intensity change is calculated as the mean of normalized trace over a 2 s time window during the stimulation period. The pseudo-colored images were generated in MATLAB using a custom written program. All analysis and statistical tests were done using Excel and GraphPad6 Prism software, respectively.

For live imaging using two-photon microscopy, flies were homozygous for both GH146-Gal4 and the UAS-GCaMP3 construct. Female transgenic flies (5–6 day, 4 week and 6 weeks old) were used for imaging experiments. Imaging was performed using two-photon microscope (Leica) equipped with a 20x water-immersion objective (NA = 1, Leica). GCaMP3 was excited at 920 nm. A custom-built device was used as odor delivery system to supply odors with a constant flow rate of 1 ml/s to the fly’s antennae for 2 s. Onset and duration of the odor stimulus were controlled using a custom-written LABVIEW program. Images were recorded at 5 Hz. Image processing and analysis was performed using Fiji software. For correcting the potential slight movements in x-y direction, recorded images were aligned using TurboReg plugin (Thévenaz et al., 1998). Afterwards, regions of interest (ROIs) were manually defined. In the antennal lobe, individual glomeruli and in the calyx, individual presynaptic boutons were selected as ROIs. For signal quantification, the average pixel intensity of five frames before stimulus onset was determined as F. ΔF is the difference between fluorescence and F, and resulting values were divided by F and displayed as percent.

In vivo electrophysiology

Single sensillum recordings (SSR) are extracellular recordings performed from antennal olfactory sensilla as previously described (Hartl et al., 2011). A single fly was wedged into narrow end of a truncated 200 µl pipette tip and placed on a slide under the objective. The fly head was exposed and stabilized on top of a glass coverslip. The antennae were hold by the tip of a glass capillary. An odor delivery pipette blew continuous air-streams to the antenna providing odor stimulations of different concentrations (SYNTECH, the Netherlands). A glass reference electrode filled with ringer (0.01 mM KCl) was inserted into the fly eye gently by a micromanipulator (Sutter instruments). And a glass recording electrode filled with ringer (0.01 mM KCl) was pushed against a sensillum until it pierced the cuticular wall of the sensillum. The recording of action potentials (APs) was started after an observation of spontaneous responses of olfactory neurons. The AC signals (10–2800 Hz) of the responses were amplified 500x (Multiclamp 700B, United States) and were analyzed with Clampex10.3 software (Digidata 1440A, the United States). The signals to a particular stimulus were recorded 5 s before giving the odor stimulation. The responses of neurons were calculated by counting the number of APs for 0.5 s during the response minus the number of APs for 0.5 s before the response (spikes/s). Statistical analysis was performed by one-way ANOVA using GraphPad Prism software.

RNA-sequencing

RNA was extracted and sequencing was carried out using standard methods and as previously described (Hussain et al., 2016a). RNA-sequencing was performed using the Illumina HiSeq 2000/2500 sequencer suite.

Statistical methods for RNA-seq and gene ontology analysis

Gene expression data was normalized by size factors and tested for differential expression using DESeq2 package (v. 1.16.1) (Love et al., 2014) in R (v. 3.4.0). Significant up and down regulated genes were classified according to their log2 fold change and adjusted p-value<0.01. Then, up and down regulated genes were analyzed separately to obtain enriched gene ontology terms using topGO package (v. 2.28.0) in R and the D. melanogaster gene ontology annotation database gene_association.fb (http://www.flybase.org).

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank members of the Grunwald Kadow lab for critically reading the manuscript. We thank Francois Leulier for bacterial strains and for consultation and advice. We thank Jonathan Landry and the EMBL sequencing core facility for RNA-sequencing and initial analysis, and Vicente Yepez for help in deeper transcriptome analysis. We would like to acknowledge the contribution of Kaida Ning in the initial analysis of the RNA-sequencing data of the fly antenna. This work was funded by the Max-Planck-Society, an EMBO young investigator grant and an ERC starting grant to IGK, and by the Juniorverbund in der Systemmedizin ‘mitOmics’ (FKZ 01Z × 1405A JG and VAY).

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Ilona C Grunwald Kadow, Email: ilona.grunwald@tum.de.

Patrik Verstreken, VIB-KU Leuven, Belgium.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung Smartage, 01GQ1420A to Atefeh Pooryasin, Stephan J Sigrist.

  • Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung Juniorverbund in der Systemmedizin ‘mitOmics’, FKZ 01Z × 1405A to Vicente A Yépez, Julien Gagneur.

  • Forschungszentrum für neurodegenerative Erkrankungen to Stephan J Sigrist.

  • H2020 European Research Council FlyContext to Ilona C Grunwald Kadow.

  • European Molecular Biology Organization EMBO Young Investigator Small Grant to Ilona C Grunwald Kadow.

  • Max-Planck-Gesellschaft Open-access funding to Ilona C Grunwald Kadow.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization.

Formal analysis, Methodology.

Formal analysis, Methodology.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology.

Software, Methodology.

Resources, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Software, Supervision, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Resources, Supervision, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Additional files

Source data 1. RNA sequencing all figures.
elife-32018-data1.xlsx (860.8KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.022
Supplementary file 1. Genes upregulated in brains of 7 weeks old flies vs. brains of 1 week old flies organized by GO (gene ontology) terms.
elife-32018-supp1.docx (56.2KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.023
Supplementary file 2. Genes downregulated in brains of 7 weeks old flies vs. brains of 1 week old flies organized by GO (gene ontology) terms.
elife-32018-supp2.docx (251.1KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.024
Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.025

References

  1. Andlauer TFM, Scholz-Kornehl S, Tian R, Kirchner M, Babikir HA, Depner H, Loll B, Quentin C, Gupta VK, Holt MG, Dipt S, Cressy M, Wahl MC, Fiala A, Selbach M, Schwärzel M, Sigrist SJ. Drep-2 is a novel synaptic protein important for learning and memory. eLife. 2014;3:e03895. doi: 10.7554/eLife.03895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Canter RG, Penney J, Tsai LH. The road to restoring neural circuits for the treatment of Alzheimer's disease. Nature. 2016;539:187–196. doi: 10.1038/nature20412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Christiansen F, Zube C, Andlauer TF, Wichmann C, Fouquet W, Owald D, Mertel S, Leiss F, Tavosanis G, Luna AJ, Fiala A, Sigrist SJ. Presynapses in Kenyon cell dendrites in the mushroom body calyx of Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:9696–9707. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6542-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Court FA, Coleman MP. Mitochondria as a central sensor for axonal degenerative stimuli. Trends in Neurosciences. 2012;35:364–372. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Doty RL, Shaman P, Applebaum SL, Giberson R, Siksorski L, Rosenberg L. Smell identification ability: changes with age. Science. 1984;226:1441–1443. doi: 10.1126/science.6505700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Doty RL. Olfaction in Parkinson's disease and related disorders. Neurobiology of Disease. 2012;46:527–552. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.10.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Fukui M, Choi HJ, Zhu BT. Mechanism for the protective effect of resveratrol against oxidative stress-induced neuronal death. Free Radical Biology and Medicine. 2010;49:800–813. doi: 10.1016/j.freeradbiomed.2010.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Granzotto A, Zatta P. Resveratrol and Alzheimer's disease: message in a bottle on red wine and cognition. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2014;6:95. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Hartl M, Loschek LF, Stephan D, Siju KP, Knappmeyer C, Kadow IC. A new Prospero and microRNA-279 pathway restricts CO2 receptor neuron formation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:15660–15673. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2592-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Humphrey DM, Parsons RB, Ludlow ZN, Riemensperger T, Esposito G, Verstreken P, Jacobs HT, Birman S, Hirth F. Alternative oxidase rescues mitochondria-mediated dopaminergic cell loss in Drosophila. Human Molecular Genetics. 2012;21:2698–2712. doi: 10.1093/hmg/dds096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Hussain A, Üçpunar HK, Zhang M, Loschek LF, Grunwald Kadow IC. Neuropeptides modulate female chemosensory processing upon mating in Drosophila. PLOS Biology. 2016a;14:e1002455. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hussain A, Zhang M, Üçpunar HK, Svensson T, Quillery E, Gompel N, Ignell R, Grunwald Kadow IC. Ionotropic chemosensory receptors mediate the taste and smell of polyamines. PLOS Biology. 2016b;14:e1002454. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Jones WD, Cayirlioglu P, Kadow IG, Vosshall LB. Two chemosensory receptors together mediate carbon dioxide detection in Drosophila. Nature. 2007;445:86–90. doi: 10.1038/nature05466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Juhász G, Erdi B, Sass M, Neufeld TP. Atg7-dependent autophagy promotes neuronal health, stress tolerance, and longevity but is dispensable for metamorphosis in Drosophila. Genes & Development. 2007;21:3061–3066. doi: 10.1101/gad.1600707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Kirby K, Hu J, Hilliker AJ, Phillips JP. RNA interference-mediated silencing of Sod2 in Drosophila leads to early adult-onset mortality and elevated endogenous oxidative stress. PNAS. 2002;99:16162–16167. doi: 10.1073/pnas.252342899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kittel RJ, Wichmann C, Rasse TM, Fouquet W, Schmidt M, Schmid A, Wagh DA, Pawlu C, Kellner RR, Willig KI, Hell SW, Buchner E, Heckmann M, Sigrist SJ. Bruchpilot promotes active zone assembly, Ca2+ channel clustering, and vesicle release. Science. 2006;312:1051–1054. doi: 10.1126/science.1126308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kong F, Hua Y, Zeng B, Ning R, Li Y, Zhao J. Gut microbiota signatures of longevity. Current Biology. 2016;26:R832–R833. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kremer MC, Christiansen F, Leiss F, Paehler M, Knapek S, Andlauer TF, Förstner F, Kloppenburg P, Sigrist SJ, Tavosanis G. Structural long-term changes at mushroom body input synapses. Current Biology. 2010;20:1938–1944. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kurowska Z, Kordower JH, Stoessl AJ, Burke RE, Brundin P, Yue Z, Brady ST, Milbrandt J, Trapp BD, Sherer TB, Medicetty S. Is axonal degeneration a key early event in Parkinson's disease? Journal of Parkinson's Disease. 2016;6:703–707. doi: 10.3233/JPD-160881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Leinwand SG, Yang CJ, Bazopoulou D, Chronis N, Srinivasan J, Chalasani SH. Circuit mechanisms encoding odors and driving aging-associated behavioral declines in Caenorhabditis elegans. eLife. 2015;4:e10181. doi: 10.7554/eLife.10181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. López-Otín C, Blasco MA, Partridge L, Serrano M, Kroemer G. The hallmarks of aging. Cell. 2013;153:1194–1217. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.05.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Love MI, Huber W, Anders S. Moderated estimation of fold change and dispersion for RNA-seq data with DESeq2. Genome Biology. 2014;15:550. doi: 10.1186/s13059-014-0550-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. MacDermott AB, Role LW, Siegelbaum SA. Presynaptic ionotropic receptors and the control of transmitter release. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1999;22:443–485. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Massaad CA, Washington TM, Pautler RG, Klann E. Overexpression of SOD-2 reduces hippocampal superoxide and prevents memory deficits in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. PNAS. 2009;106:13576–13581. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902714106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Oka S, Hirai J, Yasukawa T, Nakahara Y, Inoue YH. A correlation of reactive oxygen species accumulation by depletion of superoxide dismutases with age-dependent impairment in the nervous system and muscles of Drosophila adults. Biogerontology. 2015;16:485–501. doi: 10.1007/s10522-015-9570-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Pircs K, Nagy P, Varga A, Venkei Z, Erdi B, Hegedus K, Juhasz G. Advantages and limitations of different p62-based assays for estimating autophagic activity in Drosophila. PLoS One. 2012;7:e44214. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Sama-ul-Haq A, Tahir M, Lone KP. Age and gender-related differences in mitral cells of olfactory bulb. Journal of the College of Physicians and Surgeons--Pakistan. 2008;18:669–673. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Scheperjans F, Aho V, Pereira PA, Koskinen K, Paulin L, Pekkonen E, Haapaniemi E, Kaakkola S, Eerola-Rautio J, Pohja M, Kinnunen E, Murros K, Auvinen P. Gut microbiota are related to Parkinson's disease and clinical phenotype. Movement Disorders. 2015;30:350–358. doi: 10.1002/mds.26069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Schwarzer M, Makki K, Storelli G, Machuca-Gayet I, Srutkova D, Hermanova P, Martino ME, Balmand S, Hudcovic T, Heddi A, Rieusset J, Kozakova H, Vidal H, Leulier F. Lactobacillus plantarum strain maintains growth of infant mice during chronic undernutrition. Science. 2016;351:854–857. doi: 10.1126/science.aad8588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Seeley WW. Mapping neurodegenerative disease onset and progression. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 2017;9:a023622. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a023622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Storelli G, Defaye A, Erkosar B, Hols P, Royet J, Leulier F. Lactobacillus plantarum promotes Drosophila systemic growth by modulating hormonal signals through TOR-dependent nutrient sensing. Cell Metabolism. 2011;14:403–414. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2011.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Tang W, Xing Z, Hu W, Li C, Wang J, Wang Y. Antioxidative effects in vivo and colonization of Lactobacillus plantarum MA2 in the murine intestinal tract. Applied Microbiology and Biotechnology. 2016;100:7193–7202. doi: 10.1007/s00253-016-7581-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Thévenaz P, Ruttimann UE, Unser M. A pyramid approach to subpixel registration based on intensity. IEEE Transactions on Image Processing. 1998;7:27–41. doi: 10.1109/83.650848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Vagnoni A, Bullock SL. A simple method for imaging axonal transport in aging neurons using the adult Drosophila wing. Nature Protocols. 2016;11:1711–1723. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2016.112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Vaiserman AM, Marotta F. Longevity-promoting pharmaceuticals: is it a time for implementation? Trends in Pharmacological Sciences. 2016;37:331–333. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2016.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Valadas JS, Vos M, Verstreken P. Therapeutic strategies in Parkinson's disease: what we have learned from animal models. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2015;1338:16–37. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Willig KI, Kellner RR, Medda R, Hein B, Jakobs S, Hell SW. Nanoscale resolution in GFP-based microscopy. Nature Methods. 2006;3:721–723. doi: 10.1038/nmeth922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Wilson RI. Early olfactory processing in Drosophila: mechanisms and principles. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2013;36:217–241. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Wood JG, Rogina B, Lavu S, Howitz K, Helfand SL, Tatar M, Sinclair D. Sirtuin activators mimic caloric restriction and delay ageing in metazoans. Nature. 2004;430:686–689. doi: 10.1038/nature02789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Yasuyama K, Kitamoto T, Salvaterra PM. Localization of choline acetyltransferase-expressing neurons in the larval visual system of Drosophila melanogaster. Cell & Tissue Research. 1995;282:193–202. doi: 10.1007/BF00319111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Patrik Verstreken1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Inhibition of oxidative stress in cholinergic projection neurons rescues aging-associated olfactory circuit degeneration" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by K VijayRaghavan (Senior Editor) and three reviewers, one of whom, Patrik Verstreken, is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

Hussein et al. describe a series of experiments that identify deterioration in olfactory projection neuron function as a primary cause of aging-related behavioral and olfactory deficits. For this they use Drosophila and study the age and neurodegeneration-induced loss of olfactory sensitivity. They show that this phenomenon is not mainly attributable to an age-induced peripheral deficit in sensory reception, but that the function of the aged olfactory circuit (as measured by behavioral performance) is fully rescued by protecting a small population of cholinergic olfactory interneurons in the brain by scavenging oxidative stress. They then extend their approach and show that rescue is not only achieved by genetic manipulation in specific cells, but also by feeding anti-oxidants or manipulating the gut microbiome; clearly these are out of the box approaches! The study will be interesting to a broad field of researchers, as flies and mammals share not only features of architecture and function of the olfactory circuit, but also fundamental processes leading to aging and neurodegenerative diseases.

In general, the reviewers were excited about the work and appreciated that a careful examination of aging-related changes in neuron function in the fly is highly welcomed, given the importance of this model system for studies of neurodegeneration and aging.

Essential revisions:

I have summarized the essential items we felt should be addressed in a revised manuscript:

* Figure 3: labeling and/or description should be improved. It is not clear if GCaMP/Dα7-GFP is stained by an antibody here for display and for quantification. The way of quantification displayed in 3D, E and H is not explained sufficiently (what/where/how many: single boutons? large regions? single optical plane? sum of several z-planes?).

* The authors find that somata of projection neurons shrink with age. This has been described for other cell types, e.g., dopaminergic neurons. It would be nice to see, whether shrinking is specific for "vulnerable" cells, like projection neurons or dopaminergic neurons, or whether this is a general phenomenon, e.g., observed also in the olfactory sensory neurons. This can be easily quantified in the existing data displayed in Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

* The number of GH146-positive somata is found to be decreased in aged flies. We would suggest you formulate this more cautiously, i.e., reformulate more precisely. The experiments only show that fewer cell bodies express GCaMP, the authors cannot differentiate whether cells are lost or whether cells do not express the marker (anymore). Although we agree that complete loss of neurons is not a broad phenomenon in aged Drosophila brains, we would reformulate the arguments in the seventh paragraph of the main text more cautiously, as, for example, the ToPro staining in Figure 3G clearly indicates "holes" in the 7 week old brains.

* It would be nice (and add to the impact of the study) to show some more significantly changed genes rather than AChR and SODs. This could be done, for example, by adding the SODs to Figure 3F and creating a graph with "non-relevant" genes in Figure 4—figure supplement 6.

* The relocalization of Dα7-GFP in old flies is a very interesting finding. The connection to neurodegeneration is, however, rather vague. Here some data on number (and possibly degeneration) of branches or synaptic sites would be good to add. A degeneration of synaptic sites would likely also explain the reduced responses in the AL/number of responding boutons in the calyx (Figure 2).

* A citation (or experiment) for the efficacy of (at least) the SOD2i construct would complement this otherwise so well controlled study.

* Additional analysis of the aged GH146>SOD2 flies is maybe beyond the scope of this report. Nevertheless it raises the question whether or which of the described functional and cellular phenotypes in Figure 2 and 3 are also rescued and, therefore, critical to maintain overall circuit function.

* The careful investigation of how aging influences cholinergic function, as summarized in Figure 2, is a strength, and is quite convincing. Notably, however, none of this functional work was executed in flies where genetic or environmental manipulation was shown to have reversed the phenotypic deficit at old ages. Can we be sure that the described functional deficits were equally rescued? This might be a reasonable argument for the targeted SOD expression, but it is certainly much more tenuous for the drug and microbiome experiments. Is a small or modest functional rescue sufficient to completely rescue behavior? One might argue that while the behavioral effects are very interesting, a detailed understanding of the extent to which the functional parameters are preserved are more important.

* It is well known in the aging field that lifespan and all sorts of behavioral and health-related phenotypes exhibit extreme variation among different genotypes. Indeed, the standard in the field is that all transgenic stocks be backcrossed at least 10 generations to a common background control within 6-12 months of the experiments in question. Alternatively, inducible expression systems (e.g., Geneswitch) are often used to allow comparisons between identical backgrounds with and without/drug. Apparently, this type of background control was not followed for the experiments in this manuscript. As a result, claims about lifespan differences (e.g., Figure 4—figure supplement 7) are not valid because of well-established situations in which false inference was obtained. Strain variability in cellular or neuronal functional decline is largely unknown, and to some extent, a lack of background control may have also worked a bit in the authors' favor. In mice, certain aspects of sensory decline are known to be characteristic of some mouse strains but not others (e.g., Blk6 mice go deaf quite young while other strains do not). By observing effects in "wild-type" and unrelated GAL4/UAS stocks it appears that the olfactory decline described by the authors is quite robust to strain variation. Resolving this issue is not straightforward in our opinion. We are not requesting that the authors repeat all of the experiments after backcrossing all of the genetic strains; we do believe the data in Figures 1 and 2, for example. But they must address this issue in the manuscript and resolve the situations where known confounding occurs (e.g., comparing lifespans of non-backcrossed GAL4 and UAS lines).

* For the experiments involving resveratrol and microbiome manipulations, there is a critical control missing: the manipulation in question measured in young animals. For example, does short-term administration of resveratrol modify response behavior? Put another way, the proper statistical question is whether there is a significant interaction between age and treatment, which would indicate a difference in the rate of change over the lifespan. It is formally possible, for example, that resveratrol improves t-maze behavior in young animals and a similar magnitude of decline occurs from week 1 to week 7. Such a result would have a very different interpretation than that currently put forward by the authors.

* The authors show that knock-down of SOD2 levels in PN's significantly affected olfactory behavior in young flies. However, some additional experiments to fully support this finding should be performed. For instance: To what extent was SOD2 down-regulated? The RNA or protein levels should be determined. – What consequence is observed with in the neurons where SOD2 is down-regulated? Is there an overall increase in ROS levels; And when rescue/overexpression experiments with SOD2 are performed, are the ROS levels different/decreased?

eLife. 2018 Jan 18;7:e32018. doi: 10.7554/eLife.32018.030

Author response


Essential revisions:

I have summarized the essential items we felt should be addressed in a revised manuscript:

* Figure 3: labeling and/or description should be improved. It is not clear if GCaMP/Dα7-GFP is stained by an antibody here for display and for quantification.

We have improved the labelling and the description in the figure legend and methods. For display and for quantification, we have stained GCaMP and Dα7-GFP with an anti-GFP antibody.

The way of quantification displayed in 3D, E and H is not explained sufficiently (what/where/how many: single boutons? large regions? single optical plane? sum of several z-planes?).

We have now included a longer and more detailed description of how quantifications have been carried out in the Materials and methods section, and where appropriate also in the main text.

* The authors find that somata of projection neurons shrink with age. This has been described for other cell types, e.g., dopaminergic neurons. It would be nice to see, whether shrinking is specific for "vulnerable" cells, like projection neurons or dopaminergic neurons, or whether this is a general phenomenon, e.g., observed also in the olfactory sensory neurons. This can be easily quantified in the existing data displayed in Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

This is indeed an important point. We have followed the suggestion of the reviewers and have measured the size of the OSNs. We did not find a significant difference between the size of the cell bodies of the OSNs of young as compared to old flies. These data are included as Figure 2—figure supplement 1B.

* The number of GH146-positive somata is found to be decreased in aged flies. We would suggest you formulate this more cautiously, i.e., reformulate more precisely. The experiments only show that fewer cell bodies express GCaMP, the authors cannot differentiate whether cells are lost or whether cells do not express the marker (anymore). Although we agree that complete loss of neurons is not a broad phenomenon in aged Drosophila brains, we would reformulate the arguments in the seventh paragraph of the main text more cautiously, as, for example, the ToPro staining in Figure 3G clearly indicates "holes" in the 7 week old brains.

We agree and have now reformulated this. In addition, we have included a quantification for ToPro staining, which indeed shows some reduction (included in main Figure 3). Nevertheless, other important markers such as Ncad were not reduced.

* It would be nice (and add to the impact of the study) to show some more significantly changed genes rather than AChR and SODs. This could be done, for example, by adding the SODs to Figure 3F and creating a graph with "non-relevant" genes in Figure 4—figure supplement 6.

We have followed this suggestion and have updated the graph in Figure 3F. In addition, we have included two tables of genes that are up- or downregulated in aged brains.

* The relocalization of Dα7-GFP in old flies is a very interesting finding. The connection to neurodegeneration is, however, rather vague. Here some data on number (and possibly degeneration) of branches or synaptic sites would be good to add. A degeneration of synaptic sites would likely also explain the reduced responses in the AL/number of responding boutons in the calyx (Figure 2).

We thank the reviewers for this suggestion. We have included more quantifications, which have been placed as additional graphs into Figure 3. These include a quantification of an antibody staining against ChAT and a reporter construct for mitochondria. In addition, we have carried out high-resolution STED microscopy with pre- and postsynaptic markers on PN boutons. In summary, we found that the number of PN boutons in the mushroom body calyx is reduced, which explains the reduction of functional boutons found in functional calcium imaging.

* A citation (or experiment) for the efficacy of (at least) the SOD2i construct would complement this otherwise so well controlled study.

We have now added this citation as requested: “RNAi: UAS-SOD2i (Bloomington stock (BL) # 24489; (Kirby et al., 2002))”. It provides evidence of the efficacy of the used UAS-SOD2 RNAi construct. The same construct was also successfully used in Oka et al., Biogerontology 2015 and other publications.

* Additional analysis of the aged GH146>SOD2 flies is maybe beyond the scope of this report. Nevertheless it raises the question whether or which of the described functional and cellular phenotypes in Figure 2 and 3 are also rescued and, therefore, critical to maintain overall circuit function.

We completely agree and we plan to address these points in the future. Unfortunately, these experiments are very time consuming and it was not possible to carry them out in the two months that were given to us by the editors. In particular, we would want to carry behavioral experiments in parallel to directly correlate phenotype in anatomy with phenotype in behaviour. Such experiments require a large number of animals (every single box in every graph represents ~500 flies or more). The underlying fly husbandry is very demanding as we need to maintain the flies until they are 7 weeks of age. At this age, a large proportion of the flies have already died meaning that we have to start with ~5 times the number of flies that are required for the actual experiment. We foresee that carrying out meaningful and well controlled functional experiments with 7 weeks old flies that overexpress SOD2 in PNs as well as in other neuron types such as OSNs will require a minimum of several months. Therefore, we hope that the reviewers will agree that this is currently beyond the scope of the present study.

We have, however, carried out anatomical analysis of flies where SOD2 was knocked-down in PNs. These flies not only show the same behavioral deficit as aged flies (as shown in the previous version of the manuscript; Figure 4), but also a very similar type and degree of anatomical and functional deficits as 7 weeks old flies. Together, these data, which have been included as new Figure 5 provide further evidence that SOD2 is indeed critical for the aging process.

* The careful investigation of how aging influences cholinergic function, as summarized in Figure 2, is a strength, and is quite convincing. Notably, however, none of this functional work was executed in flies where genetic or environmental manipulation was shown to have reversed the phenotypic deficit at old ages. Can we be sure that the described functional deficits were equally rescued? This might be a reasonable argument for the targeted SOD expression, but it is certainly much more tenuous for the drug and microbiome experiments. Is a small or modest functional rescue sufficient to completely rescue behavior? One might argue that while the behavioral effects are very interesting, a detailed understanding of the extent to which the functional parameters are preserved are more important.

We fully agree (see above), and we plan to carry out these experiments in the future, because this is a very important question. Nevertheless, we think that the current manuscript offers several interesting insights even without these additional experiments.

Finally, while we agree that it would be nice to understand the exact mechanisms of SOD2 function in aging, we would argue that the functional rescue of a behavioral deficit is the main and arguably the most important readout of a treatment. We think our data makes a strong point in showing that one localized genetic intervention can be sufficient to rescue behavioural function fully. And it provides an exciting starting point for launching further screens and functional analysis of the detailed mechanisms and potential treatments.

* It is well known in the aging field that lifespan and all sorts of behavioral and health-related phenotypes exhibit extreme variation among different genotypes. Indeed, the standard in the field is that all transgenic stocks be backcrossed at least 10 generations to a common background control within 6-12 months of the experiments in question. Alternatively, inducible expression systems (e.g., Geneswitch) are often used to allow comparisons between identical backgrounds with and without/drug. Apparently, this type of background control was not followed for the experiments in this manuscript. As a result, claims about lifespan differences (e.g., Figure 4—figure supplement 7) are not valid because of well-established situations in which false inference was obtained. Strain variability in cellular or neuronal functional decline is largely unknown, and to some extent, a lack of background control may have also worked a bit in the authors' favor. In mice, certain aspects of sensory decline are known to be characteristic of some mouse strains but not others (e.g., Blk6 mice go deaf quite young while other strains do not). By observing effects in "wild-type" and unrelated GAL4/UAS stocks it appears that the olfactory decline described by the authors is quite robust to strain variation. Resolving this issue is not straightforward in our opinion. We are not requesting that the authors repeat all of the experiments after backcrossing all of the genetic strains; we do believe the data in Figures 1 and 2, for example. But they must address this issue in the manuscript and resolve the situations where known confounding occurs (e.g., comparing lifespans of non-backcrossed GAL4 and UAS lines).

We agree with these comments. Nevertheless, we would like to point out that we are not comparing lifespan between Gal4 lines, UAS lines and the combination of the two, but rather lifespan of one particular genotype to a behavioral phenotype of flies of the same genotype and therefore genetic background. The lifespan experiments were done solely to be able to correlate the lifespan of the very flies that were analysed for their behavior to their behavioral performance. The argument is that, if the flies have a very short lifespan, a strong reduction in behavioral performance of the same flies could be due to a general decline in the state of health. By contrast, we are observing relatively small effects on lifespan, but large effects in behaviour. In other words, we do not mean to imply that we are measuring lifespan to conclude how a manipulation of PNs affects lifespan. We are measuring it solely to exclude that a short or extremely long lifespan of a particular experimental or control group might explain the behavioral phenotypes. Therefore, we think these lifespan data are very important. In order to avoid misunderstandings, we have now made our motivation for including lifespan measurements clearer in the text.

* For the experiments involving resveratrol and microbiome manipulations, there is a critical control missing: the manipulation in question measured in young animals. For example, does short-term administration of resveratrol modify response behavior? Put another way, the proper statistical question is whether there is a significant interaction between age and treatment, which would indicate a difference in the rate of change over the lifespan. It is formally possible, for example, that resveratrol improves t-maze behavior in young animals and a similar magnitude of decline occurs from week 1 to week 7. Such a result would have a very different interpretation than that currently put forward by the authors.

We agree. We have included resveratrol feeding of young flies as a supplementary graph into Figure 6—figure supplement 1D. Feeding resveratrol for an entire week had no positive or negative effect on the young flies’ olfactory performance.

* The authors show that knock-down of SOD2 levels in PN's significantly affected olfactory behavior in young flies. However, some additional experiments to fully support this finding should be performed. For instance: To what extent was SOD2 down-regulated?

We appreciate the concern of the reviewers. We have used a line that was previously used several times and where SOD2 levels where measured in entire embryos, larvae, pupae and adults (Kirby et al., 2002).

We have also used an alternative construct from Bloomington, which was not included in the presented results (Stock 32983 (y1 sc* v1; P{TRiP.HMS00783}attP2), but produced the same behavioral phenotype. In addition, we have expressed the first construct, which we then used throughout the study (“RNAi: UAS-SOD2i (Bloomington stock (BL) # 24489; (Kirby et al., 2002))” under the control of the pan-neuronal driver elav-Gal4, which – as previously described – led to the death of the flies ~3-5 days after eclosion.

Author response image 1. Figure shows olfactory preference index of 1 week old flies to 2,3-butanedione.

Author response image 1.

The effect of this alternative RNAi construct of SOD2 (Stock #32983) is similar to the effect observed for the construct used throughout our study. Please note that for this experiment, the flies also carried UAS-GCaMP3.0.

Author response image 2. Figure shows lifespan measurements of flies overexpressing UAS-SOD2-RNAi (Stock #24489) under the control of a Pan-neuronal driver elav-Gal4 and the Gal4/+ control.

Author response image 2.

The results confirm previous results by Kirby et al., 2002.

The RNA or protein levels should be determined.

It is very difficult to carry out RNA or protein measurements selectively on PNs. We do not have an antibody against SOD2 that functions in wholemount immunohistochemistry. To extract RNA just from these neurons is very challenging. Given that two independent lines showed the same behavioral phenotype and both lines have been used previously, we hope that the reviewers will agree that a proof beyond doubt that SOD2 is truly downregulated specifically in PNs is beyond the scope of the study.

What consequence is observed with in the neurons where SOD2 is down-regulated? Is there an overall increase in ROS levels; And when rescue/overexpression experiments with SOD2 are performed, are the ROS levels different/decreased?

We have analysed the consequence of SOD2 knockdown on PN morphology and function. We find that, while SOD2 knockdown does not reduce the number of PNs significantly, it has a significant impact on the size of PN cell bodies. These are reduced resembling the phenotype observed in PNs of old flies.

Furthermore, knockdown of SOD2 in PNs had a strong effect on the responses of PNs in calcium imaging. Similar to what we found in brains of old flies, the response per bouton is not reduced, but the number of responsive boutons is strongly diminished. These data have been included in a new Figure 5.

Regarding the ROS stainings, Oka et al., 2015 have used a reporter gene GstD1-GFP expressed under the control of elav-Gal4 to monitor ROS in all neurons in the brain. Their results demonstrate that knock-down of SOD2 using the same construct from Kirby et al. as used in this study increases ROS in the whole brain.

We have previously attempted to carry out ROS stainings using Dihydroethidium (DHE) on old brains with little success. We have followed the protocol as described in Owusu-Ansah et al. in Protocol Exchange. While we did see staining in old brains, we also observed stainings in younger brains occasionally. By doing different kinds of controls (staining procedure, person who dissected and stained, movements such as shaking etc.), we finally came to the conclusion that the staining is extremely sensitive to all kinds of perturbations. In particular, we found that confocal scanning increased the staining, possibly because of the effect of the strong laser light on the living tissue. We, therefore, concluded that this staining is too unreliable in our hands to analyse neuron-specific effects, and discontinued our efforts in this direction.

In light of the reviewers’ comments, we aimed at analysing ROS upon SOD2 knockdown in PNs in 1 week old brains. Unfortunately, this led to the same situation and we, once again, do not feel comfortable to conclude anything from these results, because of the variable stainings. While we do not doubt that this procedure works in other labs, we have not been able to develop a staining and imaging protocol that reliably produces consistent results independent of the experimenter, way of dissection, way of staining etc.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 1.
    elife-32018-fig1-data1.xlsx (200.9KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.004
    Figure 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2 and Figure 2—figure supplements 1—3.
    elife-32018-fig2-data1.xlsx (134.3KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.009
    Figure 3—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.011
    Figure 4—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4 and Figure 4—figure supplements 1 and 3.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.016
    Figure 5—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.018
    Figure 6—source data 1. Source data for Figure 6 and Figure 6—figure supplement 1.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.021
    Source data 1. RNA sequencing all figures.
    elife-32018-data1.xlsx (860.8KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.022
    Supplementary file 1. Genes upregulated in brains of 7 weeks old flies vs. brains of 1 week old flies organized by GO (gene ontology) terms.
    elife-32018-supp1.docx (56.2KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.023
    Supplementary file 2. Genes downregulated in brains of 7 weeks old flies vs. brains of 1 week old flies organized by GO (gene ontology) terms.
    elife-32018-supp2.docx (251.1KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.024
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.32018.025

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES