Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017 Nov 28;88:105–114. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.11.015

Reproductive Steroids and ADHD Symptoms Across the Menstrual Cycle

Bethan Roberts 1, Tory Eisenlohr-Moul 2, Michelle M Martel 3,
PMCID: PMC5803442  NIHMSID: NIHMS925910  PMID: 29197795

Abstract

Although Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder shows (ADHD) male predominance, females are significantly impaired and exhibit additional comorbid disorders during adolescence. However, no empirical work has examined the influence of cyclical fluctuating steroids on ADHD symptoms in women. The present study examined estradiol (E2), progesterone (P4), and testosterone (T) associations with ADHD symptoms across the menstrual cycle in regularly-cycling young women (N=32), examining trait impulsivity as a moderator. Women completed a baseline measure of trait impulsivity, provided saliva samples each morning, and completed an ADHD symptom checklist every evening for 35 days. Results indicated decreased levels of E2 in the context of increased levels of either P4 or T was associated with higher ADHD symptoms on the following day, particularly for those with high trait impulsivity. Phase analyses suggested both an early follicular and early luteal, or post-ovulatory, increase in ADHD symptoms. Therefore, ADHD symptoms may change across the menstrual cycle in response to endogenous steroid changes.

Keywords: reproductive steroid hormones, estradiol, progesterone, testosterone, ADHD

1. Introduction

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a common and impairing childhood neurodevelopmental disorder (APA, 2013; Bernfort et al., 2008; Pelham et al., 2007, Polanczyk et al., 2007; Sayal et al. 2017; Thomas et al., 2015) that often persists into adolescence and adulthood with a prevalence of approximately 3% in adults (Faraone & Biederman, 2005; Fayyad et al., 2017; Kessler et al., 2006; Wilcutt, 2012). ADHD is a heterogeneous condition, currently conceptualized theoretically using multiple pathway models (Nigg et al., 2004; Sonuga-Barke, 2005) and most accurately described using two continuous symptom dimensions of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity (Haslam et al., 2006; Larsson et al., 2012; Marcus & Barry, 2011). Although ADHD is more frequently diagnosed in males, females with ADHD often become particularly impaired and exhibit comorbid disorders beginning during adolescence (Biederman et al., 1999; Hinshaw et al., 2012; Hosain, et al., 2012; Lahey et al. 1994; Quinn, 2005; Robison et al., 2008). The mechanisms of such sex differences remain unclear yet fundamental to understanding ADHD and sex differences in ADHD.

Prior work suggests a role for organizational testosterone (T) in ADHD symptoms in both sexes (Wang et al., 2017), but particularly males (Martel, 2009; McFadden et al., 2005). Women and girls are under-studied and in particular, activational hormonal effects remain essentially unstudied in ADHD. Case studies have suggested that ADHD symptoms may worsen the week before menstruation (during declining estrogen and progesterone; Quinn, 2005) and improve during pregnancy (during elevated estrogen and progesterone (Nadeau & Quinn, 2002), but empirical work is lacking.

Higher estradiol (E2) and progesterone (P4) have been generally linked to enhanced executive function (EF) and attention (e.g., Hatta & Nagaya, 2009), which appear to improve during cycle phases characterized by elevated E2 and P4 (Gogos, 2013; Howard et al., 1988; Jacobs & D’Esposito, 2011; Lord & Taylor, 1991; Rosenberg & Park, 2002; Segal, 2012; Vranic´ & Hromatko, 2008; Solís-Ortiz & Corsi-Cabrera, 2008; Solis-Ortiz et al., 2004). Further, verbal fluency, verbal learning and memory appear to improve with administration of T in women (Davison et al., 2011; Drake et al., 2000), whereas response inhibition performance decreases (Bjork et al., 2001). Given that weak EF is a prominent correlate and possible marker of ADHD, this prior work suggests a potential role for cycling reproductive steroids in daily ADHD symptom expression.

During adulthood, impulsivity appears to be a particularly prominent marker of ADHD that is underpinned by cyclical steroid effects (Barkley et al., 2010). Studies of adult ADHD suggest robust associations with components of impulsivity (Lynam et al., 2006), including Negative Urgency (negative affect-driven rash action), Lack of Perseverance (inability to persist on a task through completion), and Lack of Planning (action without careful thinking; Miller et al., 2010; Pedersen et al., 2016; reviewed by Berg et al., 2015) with Negative Urgency potentially related to fluctuations in E2 and P4 through its links with affect (Eisenlohr-Moul et al., 2015). Sensation-Seeking (tendency to seek adventure) may be more specifically associated with hyperactivity-impulsivity (Lopez et al., 2015), as well as related to higher T (Campbell et al., 2010; Daitzman & Zuckerman, 1980; Roberti, 2004). Overall, trait impulsivity appears prominently associated with both adult ADHD symptoms and steroid effects. Therefore, in line with rodent research (Löfgren et al., 2009), impulsivity may be a marker or a route to adult ADHD that is particularly sensitive to steroid effects. Thus, the possibility that steroid effects on ADHD may be apparent particularly for women with high trait impulsivity (particularly Negative Urgency and Sensation-Seeking) was examined herein.

1.1 Goals of the Present Study

The present study was conducted with the intention of examining the within-person covariations of reproductive steroids, including E2, P4, and T, with daily ADHD symptoms across the menstrual cycle. Based on prior work on E2 associations with EF, it was hypothesized that 1) between-women, lower average E2 across the entire menstrual cycle would be associated with higher ADHD symptoms, 2) within-person declines in E2 (i.e., lower-than-average E2 for a given woman) would be associated with increases in ADHD symptoms, and that 3) within-person declines in E2 would be more strongly associated with increased ADHD symptoms, particularly for those with higher levels of trait impulsivity. Additionally, given the evidence for interactive effects of E2 and P4 in other externalizing psychopathologies (Eisenlohr-Moul et al., 2015; Klump et al., 2008; 2013). P4 was included as an exploratory moderating variable given established antagonistic effects on E2, and T was also included as an exploratory moderating variable due to associations of T with ADHD symptoms (Martel, 2009; McFadden et al., 2005) and sensation seeking (Campbell et al., 2010; Daitzman & Zuckerman, 1980; Roberti, 2004). Finally, 4) cycle phase effects were examined to more easily compare results to previous studies and to evaluate whether ADHD risk may be heightened during particular parts of the menstrual cycle exhibiting characteristic hormone profiles, such as the early luteal phase and the perimenstrual timeframe (see Appendix 1).

2. Methods and Materials

2.1 Participants

Participants were 32 naturally-cycling young females between the ages of 18 and 22 (M = 19.43, SD = 1.38) recruited from the local university and surrounding community. The participants’ ethnic background broadly matched that of the community: 70.6% Caucasian, 17.6% African American, and 11.8% other ethnic minority. Exclusionary criteria included primary sensorimotor handicap, neurological disorder, pervasive developmental disorder or intellectual disability, history of psychosis, current gynecological or thyroid disorders, irregular cycles (<21 days or >35 days), and current medical use of hormones, psychostimulants, or antipsychotic medications. 3 women reported prescribed SSRIs. Height and weight were measured since body mass index impacts reproductive steroids (Lukanova et al., 2004; BMI Mean = 24.37, SD = 6.33). Participants who completed any data collection received $50, those who completed >50% of participation received $75, and those who completed 100% of data collection received $100.

2.2 Procedure

At an initial laboratory visit, participants provided informed consent via IRB-approved procedures and completed measure of study eligibility and trait measures of impulsivity. Eligible women then completed 35 days of morning saliva collection and evening online symptom reports, after which participants returned frozen samples to the laboratory. The number of measures administered was kept to a minimum to reduce participant burden during the lengthy data collection period. E2, P4, and T were assayed every other day, resulting in an average of 17 days of data (steroids and symptoms) per woman.

2.3 Measures

2.3.1. Trait Impulsivity

The UPPS-P Trait Impulsivity Scale, a 59-item questionnaire, assesses five components of trait impulsivity: Positive Urgency (positive affect-driven rash action), Negative Urgency (negative affect-driven rash action), Lack of Premeditation (action without careful thinking), Lack of Perseverance (inability to remain with a task through completion), and Sensation Seeking (tendency to seek adventure; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001) The scales demonstrate excellent reliability and validity (Lynam et al., 2006; Cyders et al., 2007). In the current sample, Lack of Perseverance exhibited fair reliability (α = .56) and Lack of Premeditation, Negative Urgency, Positive Urgency and Sensation Seeking exhibited good reliability (α = .82–.93).

2.3.2. Daily ADHD Symptoms

Daily symptoms were measured using the Current ADHD Symptoms Scale (CSS): Self-report, a symptom checklist for ADHD symptoms and impairment (Barkley & Murphy, 2006). Participants were instructed to rate their symptoms for the day every day in the evening. Symptom counts were used for analysis rather than diagnosis, as the sample was community-recruited, and ADHD appears best characterized as a continuum of symptoms (Haslam et al., 2006; Marcus & Barry, 2011). Total Hyperactive-Impulsive symptoms ranged from 0 to 9 (M = .32, SD = .66; α = .66), and total Inattentive symptoms ranged from 0 to 9 (M = .20, SD = .65; α = .72). Log transformations were applied and resulted in normally distributed residuals in all analyses.

2.3.3 Reproductive Steroids: 17β-Estradiol (E2), Progesterone (P4), and Testosterone (T)

Saliva samples were collected via passive drool by participants in the morning thirty minutes after waking and were subsequently frozen. Participants were instructed not to eat, drink, brush teeth, or smoke before saliva collection. No participant reported violation of this morning protocol in daily diaries. Samples from every other day were analyzed. Serum E2 (pg/mL), P4 (pg/mL), and T (pg/mL) were determined using enzyme immunoassay kits available through Salimetrics. For E2, the Salimetrics 17β-Estradiol immunoassay kit had a sensitivity of 0.1 pg/mL and high precision (% coefficient of variation ranging from .7 to 14.5). For P4, the Salimetrics immunoassay kits had a sensitivity of 5 pg/mL (from 0) and a precision of percent coefficient of variation between 1.05 and 14.8. For T, the Salimetrics immunoassay kits had a sensitivity of 1pg/dL and precision of percent coefficient of variation between 1.2 and 18.9 All participants showed peak P4 levels consistent with an ovulatory cycle (Howards et al., 2009). Finally, steroids generally showed expected trajectories across the menstrual cycle: E2 demonstrated a midcycle peak and secondary peak during the midluteal phase, P4 was low during the follicular phase and peaked during the luteal phase, and T showed a small mid-cycle peak.

The following predictors were defined for E2, P4, and T: (1) steroid averages across all observations within a participant, representing between-person differences in total exposure across the cycle, (2) steroid standard deviation across all observations within a participant, representing between-person differences in within-person variance across the cycle, and (3) recent steroid levels relative to one’s own average and standard deviation across all observations (within-person levels; Klump et al., 2008)1, defined as yesterday’s value minus the person average, divided by the person standard deviation. The one-day lag for the impact on ADHD symptoms was chosen to correspond to the 24 hour lag for ovarian steroid effects on behavior in animals (Bless, McGinnis, Mitchell, Hartwell, & Mitchell, 1997).

2.3.4. Cycle Phase Coding

Menstrual cycle phase was coded as described by Edler, Lipson, & Keel (2006). We then created an additional phase variable to differentiate pre- and post-ovulatory phases. To do this, we identified four pre-E2-peak and four post-E2 peak days; after identifying the ovulatory E2 peak, the day of the ovulatory E2 peak and the three days prior comprised the “pre-ovulatory” phase, while the four days after the ovulatory peak comprised the “post-ovulatory” phase. Five total phases were coded, with no overlap: Follicular, Preovulatory, Postovulatory, Midluteal, and Premenstrual.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

First, steroid means and standard deviations were correlated with impulsivity and average symptoms using spearman rank correlations (due to skewed distributions) partialling age and BMI. Next, linear mixed-effect models examined cycle phase and within-person steroid variation as predictors. In steroid models, we predicted today’s symptoms from BMI, yesterday’s person-standardized E2, P4, and T, and the two-way interactions between all steroids. Trait impulsivity variables were considered as moderators. Random slopes were specified for within-person predictors, and an additional term specified an autoregressive covariance structure (day – 1). Coefficients and odds ratios for within-person hormonal effects can be interpreted as the effect of a one person-SD increase in the predictor above one’s mean. Visual inspection of distributions and residuals indicated that assumptions of normality and homoscedasticity were met. In order to evaluate their robustness, moderation analyses were also carried out excluding the highest and lowest 10% of observations on the moderator. Using the Intraclass Correlations (ICCs) for each outcome and equations provided by Snijders and Bosker (1999), calculations indicated 80% power to detect conventionally small-to-medium effects (Klump et al., 2013; smallest detectible effects for cross-level interaction were f2 = .085 – .091).

3. Results

ICCs revealed that the majority of variance in symptoms was at the within-person level for Inattentive Symptoms (15% stable between-person variance; ICC = .15, NMI = .18, NMISE = .048) and Hyperactive/Impulsive symptoms (10% stable between-person variance; ICC = .10, NMI = .13, NMISE = .031). Therefore, ADHD symptoms were more state-like than trait-like, and analyses focused on within-person variance in symptoms were therefore most relevant to explaining symptom variance.

1) Association between Average Hormone Levels and ADHD Symptoms

Table 1 presents correlations of steroid means and variances with risk variables. Higher average E2 predicted less inattention, and greater E2 variance was associated with lower Negative Urgency. Higher average P4 correlated with lower inattention, Positive Urgency, Sensation Seeking, and Lack of Premeditation, and greater P4 variance predicted lower Negative Urgency, Positive Urgency, Sensation Seeking, and inattention. Greater variance in T predicted higher Positive Urgency and Sensation Seeking.

Table 1.

Partial (Controlling for Age and BMI) Spearman Rank Correlations Between Average Steroids, Variance in Steroids, Impulsivity Variables, and ADHD symptoms Across Observations (N=32)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
1. Negative Urgency
2. Positive Urgency .85***
3. Sensation Seeking .62*** .66***
4. Lack Premeditation .49** .59*** .37*
5. Lack Perseverance .54** .51** .11 .67***
6. Testosterone Average .04 .02 .05 −.20 −.10
7. Estradiol Average −.13 −.05 −.05 −.20 −.19 .22
8. Progesterone Average −.28 −.43* −.37* −.42* −.18 .11 .55**
9. Testosterone SD .28 .37* .42* .07 .03 .71*** .30 −.02
10. Estradiol SD −.30* −.19 −.10 −.11 −.07 .12 .45* .17 .01
11. Progesterone SD −.27* −.39* −.30* −.25 −.07 .07 .30* .87*** .01 .12
12. Hyp/Imp Sx Average .61*** .42* .50** .51** .46* −.22 −.23 −.18 −.09 −.20 −.14
13. Inattentive Sx Average .58*** .60*** .55** .51** .41* −.22 −.36* −.44* .01 −.23 −.39* .79***

Note.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001.

2) Do Higher or Lower Recent Steroid Levels Impact ADHD Symptoms?

Results of models predicting daily ADHD symptoms from recent steroids and their interactions revealed no significant associations (see Table 2). However, the existence of random effects of E2, P4, and T indicated that the impact of steroids varied as a function of between-person factors.

Table 2.

Models Predicting Daily ADHD Symptoms Person-Standardized Recent Ovarian Steroid Levels

Daily Outcome

Parameter Hyperactive/Impulsive
Symptoms
Inattentive
Symptoms
Estimate SE Estimate SE
Intercept 0.13 0.03 0.16 0.05
BMI 0.04 0.03 0.04 0.05
E2 −0.02 0.02 0.00 0.02
P4 0.02 0.02 −0.01 0.02
T 0.00 0.02 0.02 0.02
E2 × P4 −0.01 0.02 0.00 0.02
E2 × T −0.02 0.01 −0.01 0.02
P4 × T 0.01 0.02 0.00 0.02
Models Predicting Daily ADHD Symptoms from the Interaction of Negative Urgency and Recent
Ovarian Steroid Levels

Daily Outcome

Parameter Hyperactive/Impulsive
Symptoms
Inattentive
Symptoms
Estimate SE Estimate SE
Intercept 0.12 0.03 0.17 0.04
BMI 0.00 0.03 −0.01 0.04
E2 −0.02 0.01 −0.01 0.02
P4 0.04** 0.01 0.00 0.02
T −0.01 0.01 0.00 0.02
E2 × P4 −0.03* 0.01 −0.05** 0.02
E2 × T 0.00 0.01 −0.01 0.02
P4 × T 0.00 0.01 0.04 0.03
Negative Urgency (NU) 0.10** 0.03 0.14** 0.05
NU × E2 −0.03* 0.02 −0.01 0.02
NU × P4 0.05** 0.02 0.00 0.02
NU × T 0.01 0.01 0.02 0.02
NU × E2 × P4 −0.04* 0.01 −0.05* 0.02
NU × E2 × T 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.02
NU × P4 × 0.00 0.02 0.01 0.02
Models Predicting Daily ADHD Symptoms from the Interaction of Positive Urgency and Recent
Ovarian Steroid Levels

Daily Outcome

Parameter Hyperactive/Impulsive
Symptoms
Inattentive
Symptoms
Estimate SE Estimate SE
Intercept 0.13 0.03 0.18 0.04
BMI 0.02 0.03 −0.02 0.04
E2 −0.02 0.02 0.01 0.02
P4 0.02 0.02 −0.01 0.02
T −0.00 0.02 0.00 0.02
E2 × P4 −0.01 0.02 −0.05** 0.02
E2 × T −0.02 0.01 −0.02 0.02
P4 × T 0.01 0.02 0.03 0.02
Positive Urgency (PU) 0.08** 0.03 0.17*** 0.04
PU × E2 −0.04* 0.02 0.01 0.02
PU × P4 0.03* 0.02 0.00 0.02
PU × T 0.01 0.02 0.00 0.02
PU × E2 × P4 −0.03* 0.01 −0.04* 0.02
PU × E2 × 0.01 0.02 −0.02 0.02
PU × P4 × 0.02 0.02 0.00 0.02
Models Predicting Daily ADHD Symptoms from the Interaction of Sensation Seeking and
Recent Ovarian Steroid Levels

Daily Outcome

Parameter Hyperactive/Impulsive
Symptoms
Inattentive
Symptoms
Estimate SE Estimate SE
Intercept 0.13 0.03 0.17 0.04
BMI 0.04 0.03 0.04 0.04
E2 −0.01 0.01 0.01 0.02
P4 0.03* 0.02 −0.01 0.02
T −0.01 0.01 0.02 0.02
E2 × P4 −0.03* 0.01 −0.05* 0.02
E2 × T 0.00 0.01 −0.01 0.02
P4 × T 0.01 0.01 0.03 0.02
Sensation Seeking (SS) 0.09* 0.03 0.13 0.04
SS × E2 −0.01 0.02 −0.02 0.02
SS × P4 0.03 0.02 0.00 0.02
SS × T 0.03* 0.01 0.02 0.02
SS × E2 × P4 −0.03** 0.01 −0.04* 0.02
SS × E2 × 0.00 0.01 −0.04* 0.02
SS × P4 × 0.02 0.02 0.01 0.02

Note.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001.

E2 = Estradiol, P4 = Progesterone. T = Testosterone.

Significant fixed effects are shown in bold.

3) Do Impulsivity Traits Moderate the Impact of Daily Steroids on ADHD Symptoms?

Table 2 provides results of moderator analysis of the UPPS-P subscales on steroid-symptom links. All five UPPS-P subscales moderated at least one steroid-symptom association. However, sensitivity analyses revealed that moderation of steroid effects by Lack of Premeditation and Lack of Perseverance were not robust to elimination of extreme observations. Therefore, we report and discuss only the more robust moderating effects of Negative Urgency, Positive Urgency, and Sensation Seeking.

3a) Moderated Interactive Effects of E2 and P4

Only among women high in Negative Urgency, Positive Urgency, and Sensation Seeking (+1 SD), there were significant interactive effects of E2 and P4 on both inattention and hyperactivity (Figures 1, 2, and 3). These interactions took a similar form in each case: in the context of higher-than-average P4 (the luteal phase), lower-than-average E2 predicted higher symptoms.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Moderating Influence of Sensation Seeking on (A) the Effect of Testosterone and (B) the Interactive Effect of Estradiol and Testosterone on Daily ADHD Symptoms

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Moderating Influence of Negative Urgency (A), Positive Urgency (B), and Sensation Seeking (C) on the Interactive Effect of E2 and P4 on Daily Hyperactive/Impulsive ADHD Symptoms.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Moderating Influence of Negative Urgency (A), Positive Urgency (B), and Sensation Seeking (C) on the Interactive Effect of E2 and P4 on Daily Inattentive ADHD Symptoms

3b) Moderated Effects of T and E2 X T

Among women high in Sensation Seeking (+1 SD), higher-than-average T predicted increased hyperactivity/impulsivity; among women low in Sensation Seeking (−1 SD), higher-than-average T predicted decreased hyperactivity/impulsivity. Only among women high in Sensation Seeking, E2 and T interacted to predict inattention; when T was higher than usual, higher E2 predicted lower symptoms of inattention; when T was lower than usual, higher E2 predicted higher symptoms of inattention.

In Figure 3, we provide means plots to illustrate the interactive effects of E2 with both P4 and T in predicting inattention across the cycle; smoothed levels of hormones and symptoms are plotted across the cycle among the top 33% of sensation seeking scorers. In Figure 4, we similarly illustrate the impact of T on hyperactivity/impulsivity across the cycle.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Means plots illustrating the interactive effects of estradiol with testosterone and progesterone in predicting inattentive symptoms across the menstrual cycle among women high (A) and low (B) in sensation seeking.

4) Does Cycle Phase Impact ADHD Symptoms?

Next, we examined the influence of cycle phase on daily symptoms (see Appendix 1). Within-person analysis revealed no significant differences between phases for hyperactivity/impulsivity, but the postovulatory phase was characterized by higher inattentive symptoms than all other phases (all p’s < .01).

4a) Do Impulsive Traits Alter the Impact of Cycle Phase on ADHD Symptoms?

We then examined the moderating influence of UPPS-P impulsivity variables on within-person associations of cycle phase with daily symptoms. Only in women high (+1 SD) in Positive Urgency or Sensation Seeking, the follicular and postovulatory phases were characterized by greater hyperactivity/impulsivity than the midluteal, premenstrual, and preovulatory phases (all p’s < .05). Only in women high on Sensation Seeking, Positive Urgency, and Lack of Premeditation (+1 SD), the follicular and postovulatory phases were each characterized by greater inattentive symptoms relative to all other phases (all p’s < .05). Only for women high in Positive Urgency (+1 SD), symptoms in the postovulatory phase were higher than the follicular phase (p < .05).

4. Discussion

In the first study to examine daily reproductive hormone levels with subsequent daily ADHD symptom expression, lower-than-average E2 in the context of higher-than-average P4 or T predicted next day increase in ADHD symptoms. The effect was magnified by high baseline trait impulsivity, suggesting that high trait impulsivity may be a route to ADHD symptoms in young adult women that is particularly sensitive to steroid influence. The within-person effects of decreased E2 during elevated P4 or T were complimented by our cycle phase findings, which suggested postovulatory and follicular elevations in symptoms among vulnerable women. Between-person associations of average E2 and P4 with average ADHD symptoms further indicated that low (and low variance in) E2 and P4 predicted greater symptoms. In addition, greater variation in T was linked with increased ADHD symptoms, whereas average levels were not.

Associations between low E2 and increased ADHD symptoms are in line with evidence that decreased E2 reduces EF (Jacobs et al., 1998; Schmidt et al., 1996; Sherwin, 1997), perhaps particularly among women with genotypic risk for low prefrontal dopamine and elevated trait impulsivity (Jacobs & D’Esposito, 2011). E2 effects on dopaminergic striatal circuits by reducing dopamine receptors and increasing dopamine release may therefore mediate these effects (Archer, 1999; Gibbs, 2010), particularly in the prefrontal cortex (Berman et al., 1997).

Our work replicates antagonistic effects of E2 and P4 in externalizing disorders (Eisenlohr-Moul et al., 2015; Klump et al., 2008; 2013; Singh, Su, & Ng, 2013) and is the first to indicate an antagonistic relationship of E2 and T. Notably, although our interactive steroid findings (high P4, low E2) might be initially seen as suggestive of a premenstrual exacerbation such as those described in case studies (Quinn, 2005), moderated cycle phase findings instead suggest strong postovulatory (rather than premenstrual) worsening of symptoms with a secondary early follicular peak. To our knowledge, this is also the first study to identify post-ovulatory risk for ADHD symptoms.

Notably, these effects of low E2 on ADHD symptoms were only present during periods of high P4 and T. P4 and T may work in slightly different ways to increase the salience of environmental cues that may facilitate distractibility and risk taking; elevated or increasing P4 heightens attention to social cues relevant to social affiliation in women (Maner & Miller, 2014), while elevated or increasing T facilitates attention to social dominance opportunities (Albert et al., 1993; Eisenegger et al., 2011; Mazur & Booth, 1998). It is possible, then, that heightened levels of either P4 or T set the stage for vulnerability to (social) environmental distractions and opportunities for impulsive behavior, and concurrent drops in E2, with their associated reductions in EF, further increase risk for symptoms. This could function to increase the probability of indiscriminate mating and conception due to increases in impulsive behavior just post-ovulation.

Although this study was the first to directly examine steroids and ADHD symptoms, it is not without limitations. A one-day lag was assumed for the impact of hormones on ADHD symptoms based on a study of rats (Bless et al., 1997). An important area of future work is verifying the time period of effects in humans. In addition, use of a nonclinical sample may limit generalizability of these findings, despite a consensus that ADHD is best conceptualized dimensionally (Barkley, 2006; Haslam et al., 2006; Larsson et al., 2012; Marcus & Barry, 2011). Further, the present study had limited diagnostic information (i.e., one measure of ADHD) due to practical constraints and also did not explore clinical comorbidities (e.g., Borderline Personality Disorder; DeSoto et al., 2003; Eisenlohr-Moul et al., 2015), which might have impacted study findings. Future studies should utilize multiple measures of ADHD symptoms, use or develop a measure designed to measure daily changes in symptoms, and explore or control for potential comorbidities. The present study also relied on self-report. Future studies should utilize other-report and/or neuropsychological testing. Finally, analyses utilized data collected every other day (vs. every day) due to expense of assays; more fine-grained analysis is an important extension of current results. Finally, rodent models suggest that progesterone injection in intact rats leads to hypoactivity (Rodier 1971), and administration of estradiol reverses hypoactivity in ovariectomized rats (Gorzek et al., 2007), effects that appear to be in the opposite direction of the present study’s findings. Additional comparative work will be important, although -- in this case -- it might be argued that the study of naturally-occurring cyclic variations in hormones may lead to a very different pattern of results compared to more rigorous examination of ovariectomization and substitution effects on behavior.

5. Conclusions

Results demonstrating steroid effects across the menstrual cycle on ADHD symptoms for vulnerable women suggests the possibility that ADHD symptoms may be more state-like and variable within women than previously thought. If results that ADHD symptoms vary across the menstrual cycle in tandem with steroid changes are replicated, this may suggest the need for clinicians to ask for information about cycle phase, hormonal profiles, use of hormonal birth control, and/or stage of life (i.e., pre- or post-puberty) during ADHD assessment in women. These results also suggest the possibility that EF and psychostimulant response might vary across the cycle. These represent critical directions for future work.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Smoothed means plots illustrating the effects of testosterone among women high in sensation seeking (A) and the interactive effects of estradiol with progesterone in predicting hyperactive/impulsive symptoms across the menstrual cycle among women high in negative urgency (B).

Highlights.

  • ADHD symptoms are higher when estradiol decreases

  • Low estradiol risky for ADHD in context of high progesterone or testosterone

  • Associations are stronger for highly impulsive women

  • ADHD symptoms increased during early follicular and early luteal phases

  • ADHD symptoms change across menstrual cycle in young adult women

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (M.M., UL1R000117), National Institute of Mental Health (T.E., T32MH093315; K99MH109667), and National Institute on Drug Abuse (M.M., DA005312; DA035150). We thank the participants who made this work possible.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

1

Values on this variable are centered around one’s personal average for that steroid; therefore, negative values indicate lower-than-average recent levels, and positive values indicate higher-than-average recent levels.

Conflicts of interest: none

All co-authors have seen and agree with the contents of the manuscript and there is no financial interest to report or conflicts of interest. We certify that the submission meets the guidelines for ethical conduct and report of research, is original work, and is not under review at any other journal.

Contributor Information

Bethan Roberts, University of Kentucky

Tory Eisenlohr-Moul, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

Michelle M. Martel, University of Kentucky

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM 5. Bookpoint US; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Albert DJ, Walsh ML, Jonik RH. Aggression in humans: What is its biological foundation? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1993;17:85–97. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(05)80117-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Archer JS. Relationship between estrogen, serotonin, and depression. Physiol Behav. 1999;6(1):71–78. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barkley RA, Murphy KR, Fischer M. ADHD in adults: What the science says. Guilford Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  5. Barkley RA, Murphy KR. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A clinical workbook. 3. New York: The Guilford Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  6. Berg JM, Latzman RD, Bliwise NG, Lilienfeld SO. Parsing the heterogeneity of impulsivity: A meta-analytic review of the behavioral implications of the UPPS for psychopathology. Psychol Assess. 2015;27(4):1129–1146. doi: 10.1037/pas0000111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berman KF, Schmidt PJ, Rubinow DR, Danaceau MA, Van Horn JD, Esposito G, et al. Modulation of cognition-specific cortical activity by gonadal steroids: A positron-emission tomography study in women. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 1997;94(16):8836–8841. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.16.8836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bernfort L, Nordfeldt S, Persson J. ADHD from a socio-economic perspective. Acta Paediatr. 2008;97:239–245. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00611.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Biederman J, Faraone SV, Mick E, et al. Clinical correlates of ADHD in females: findings from a large group of girls ascertained from pediatric and psychiatric referral sources. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38(8):966–975. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199908000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bjork JM, Moeller FG, Dougherty DM, Swann AC. Endogenous plasma testosterone levels and commission errors in women: A preliminary report. Physiol Behav. 2001;73(1):217–221. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00474-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bless EP, McGinnis KA, Mitchell AL, Hartwell A, Mitchell JB. The effects of gonadal steroids on brain stimulation reward in female rats. Behav Brain Res. 1997;82(2):235–244. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(96)00129-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Campbell BC, Dreber A, Apicella CL, Eisenberg DT, Gray PB, Little AC, Lum JK. Testosterone exposure, dopaminergic reward, and sensation-seeking in young men. Physiol Behav. 2010;99(4):451–456. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cyders MA, Smith GT, Spillane NS, Fischer S, Annus AM, Peterson C. Integration of impulsivity and positive mood to predict risky behavior: Development and validation of a measure of positive urgency. Psychol Assess. 2007;19:107–118. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.1.107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Daitzman R, Zuckerman M. Disinhibitory sensation seeking, personality and gonadal hormones. Pers Ind Dif. 1980;1(2):103–110. [Google Scholar]
  15. Davison SL, Bell RJ, Gavrilescu M, Searle K, Maruff P, Gogos A, Rossell SL, Adams J, Egan GF, Davis SR. Testosterone improves verbal learning and memory in postmenopausal women: Results from a pilot study. Maturitas. 2011;70(3):307–311. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2011.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. DeSoto MC, Geary DC, Hoard MK, Sheldon MS, Cooper L. Estrogen fluctuations, oral contraceptives and borderline personality. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2003;28(6):751–766. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(02)00068-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Drake EB, Henderson VW, Stanczyk FZ, McCleary CA, Brown WS, Smith CA, Rizzo AA, Murdock GA, Buckwalter JG. Associations between circulating sex steroid hormones and cognition in normal elderly women. Neurology. 2000;54(3):599–599. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.3.599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Edler C, Lipson SF, Keel PK. Ovarian hormones and binge eating in bulimia nervosa. Psychol Med. 2007;37(1):131–141. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706008956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Eisenegger C, Haushofer J, Fehr E. The role of testosterone in social interaction. Trends Cogni Sci. 2011;15(6):263–271. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Eisenlohr-Moul TA, DeWall CN, Girdler SS, Segerstrom SC. Ovarian hormones and borderline personality disorder features: preliminary evidence for interactive effects of estradiol and progesterone. Biol Psychol. 2015;109:37–52. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.03.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Faraone SV, Biederman J. What is the prevalence of adult ADHD? Results of a population screen of 966 adults. J Atten Disord. 2005;9(2):384–391. doi: 10.1177/1087054705281478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fayyad J, Sampson NA, Hwang I, Adamowski T, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Al-Hamzawi A, Gureje O. The descriptive epidemiology of DSM-IV Adult ADHD in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. ADHD Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2017;9(1):47–65. doi: 10.1007/s12402-016-0208-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gibbs RB. Estrogen therapy and cognition: a review of the cholinergic hypothesis. Endocr Rev. 2010;31(2):224–253. doi: 10.1210/er.2009-0036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gogos A. Natural and synthetic sex hormones: Effects on higher order cognitive function and prepulse inhibition. Biol Psychol. 2013;93(1):17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Haslam N, Williams B, Prior M, Haslam R, Graetz B, Sawyer M. Latent structure of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a taxometric analysis. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2006;40:639–647. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2006.01863.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hatta T, Nagaya K. Menstrual cycle phase effects on memory and Stroop task performance. Arch Sex Behav. 2009;38(5):821–827. doi: 10.1007/s10508-008-9445-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hosain GM, Berenson AB, Tennen H, Bauer LO, Wu ZH. Attention deficit hyperactivity symptoms and risky sexual behavior in young adult women. J Women's Health. 2012;21(4):463–468. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2011.2825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Howard R, Gifford M, Lumsden J. Changes in an electrocortical measure of impulsivity during the menstrual cycle. Pers Individ Dif. 1988;9(5):917–918. [Google Scholar]
  29. Howards PP, Schisterman EF, Wactawski-Wende J, Reschke JE, Frazer AA, Hovey KM. Timing clinic visits to phases of the menstrual cycle by using a fertility monitor: the BioCycle Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2009;169(1):105–112. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Jacobs DM, Tang MX, Stern Y, Sano M, Marder K, Bell KL, Schofield P, Dooneief G, Gurland B, Mayeux R. Cognitive function in nondemented older women who took estrogen after menopause. Neurology. 1998;50(2):368–373. doi: 10.1212/wnl.50.2.368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Jacobs E, D’Esposito M. Estrogen shapes dopamine-dependent cognitive processes: Implications for women’s health. J Neurosci. 2011;31(14):5286–5293. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6394-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kessler R, Adler L, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners C, Demler O, Zaslavsky A. The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(4):716–723. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.4.716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Klump KL, Keel PK, Culbert KM, Edler C. Ovarian hormones and binge eating: exploring associations in community samples. Psychol Med. 2008;38(12):1749–1757. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708002997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Klump KK, Keel PK, Racine SE, Burt SA, Neale M, Sisk CL, et al. The interactive effects of estrogen and progesterone on changes in emotional eating across the menstrual cycle. J Abnorm Psychol. 2013;122(1):131–137. doi: 10.1037/a0029524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lahey BB, Applegate B, McBurnett K, Biederman J, Greenhill L, Hynd GW, Barkley RA, Newcorn J, Jensen P, Richters J, Garfinkel B. DMS-IV field trials for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. The Am J Psychiatry. 1994 doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.11.1673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Larsson H, Anckarsater H, Råstam M, Chang Z, Lichtenstein P. Childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: a quantitative genetic study of 8,500 twin pairs. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53(1):73–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02467.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Löfgren Mm, Johansson IM, Meyerson B, Turkmen S, Bäckström T. Withdrawal effects from progesterone and estradiol relate to individual risk-taking and explorative behavior in female rats. Physiol Behav. 2009;96(1):91–97. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lopez R, Dauvilliers Y, Jaussent I, Billieux J, Bayard S. A multidimensional approach of impulsivity in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2015;227(2):290–295. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lord T, Taylor K. Monthly fluctuation in task concentration in female college students. Percept and Mot Skills. 1991;72(2):435–439. doi: 10.2466/pms.1991.72.2.435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lukanova A, Lundin E, Zeleniuch-Jacquotte A, Muti P, Mure A, et al. Body mass index, circulating levels of sex-steroid hormones, IGF-I and IGF-binding protein-3: a cross-sectional study in healthy women. Eur J of Endocrinol. 2004;150:161–171. doi: 10.1530/eje.0.1500161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lynam DR, Smith GT, Whiteside SP, Cyders MA. The UPPS-P: Assessing five personality pathways to impulsive behavior. West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  42. Maner JK, Miller SL. Hormones and social monitoring: Menstrual cycle shifts in progesterone underlie women's sensitivity to social information. Evol Hum Behav. 2014;35(1):9–16. [Google Scholar]
  43. Marcus DK, Barry TD. Does attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder have a dimensional latent structure? A taxometric analysis. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011;120(2):427. doi: 10.1037/a0021405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Martel MM. Conscientiousness as a mediator of the association between masculinized finger-length ratios and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(7):790–798. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02065.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Mazur A, Booth A. Testosterone and dominance in men. Behav. Brain Sci. 1998;21:353–397. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. McFadden D, Westhafer JG, Pasanen EG, Carlson CL, Tucker DM. Physiological evidence of hypermasculinization in boys with the inattentive type of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Clin Neurosci Res. 2005;5(5):233–245. [Google Scholar]
  47. Miller DJ, Derefinko KJ, Lynam DR, Milich R, Fillmore MT. Impulsivity and attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: subtype classification using the UPPS impulsive behavior scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2010;32(3):323–332. doi: 10.1007/s10862-009-9155-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Nadeau KG, Quinn PO. Gender and history of ADHD: an unexamined gender bias. In: Quinn PO, Nadeau KG, editors. Gender Issues and ADHD: Research, Diagnosis, and Treatment. Silver Spring, MD: Advantage Books; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  49. Nigg JT, Goldsmith HH, Sachek J. Temperament and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: The development of a multiple pathway model. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2004;33(1):42–53. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Pedersen SL, Walther CA, Harty SC, Gnagy EM, Pelham WE, Molina BS. The Indirect Effects of Childhood ADHD on Alcohol Problems in Adulthood through Unique Facets of Impulsivity. Addiction (Abingdon, England) 2016 doi: 10.1111/add.13398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Pelham WE, Foster EM, Robb JA. The economic impact of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in children and adolescents. Ambul Pediatr. 2007;7:121–131. doi: 10.1016/j.ambp.2006.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2007 doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Quinn PO. Treating adolescent girls and women with ADHD: gender-specific issues. J Clin Psychol. 2005;61(5):579–587. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Roberti JW. A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking. Journal Res Pers. 2004;38(3):256–279. [Google Scholar]
  55. Robison RJ, Reimherr FW, Marchant BK, Faraone SV, Adler LA, West SA. Gender differences in 2 clinical trials of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a retrospective data analysis. J Clin Psychiatry. 2008;69(2):213–221. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v69n0207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Rosenberg L, Park S. Verbal and spatial functions across the menstrual cycle in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002;27(7):835–841. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(01)00083-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Sayal K, Prasad V, Daley D, Ford T, Coghill D. ADHD in children and young people: prevalence, care pathways & service provision. Lancet Psychiatry. 2017 doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30167-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Schmidt R, Fazekas F, Reinhart B, Kapeller P, Fazekas G, Offenbacher H, Eber B, Schumacher M, Freidl W. Estrogen replacement therapy in older women: a neuropsychological and brain MRI study. J Am Geriatr. 1996;44(11):1307–1313. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1996.tb01400.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Segal MT. Effect of menstrual cycle related hormone fluctuations on working memory. London, Ontario: The University of Western Ontario (Doctoral dissertation); 2012. [Google Scholar]
  60. Sherwin B. Estrogen Effects on cognition in menopausal women. Neurology. 1997;48:S21–26. doi: 10.1212/wnl.48.5_suppl_7.21s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Singh M, Su C, Ng S. Non-genomic mechanisms of progesterone action in the brain. Frontiers Neurosci. 2013;7:159. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Snijders T, Bosker R. Multilevel modeling: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling 1999 [Google Scholar]
  63. Solís-Ortiz S, Corsi-Cabrera M. Sustained attention is favored by progesterone during early luteal phase and visuo-spatial memory by estrogens during ovulatory phase in young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33(7):989–998. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Solis-Ortiz S, Guevara MA, Corsi-Cabrera M. Performance in a test demanding prefrontal functions is favored by early luteal phase progesterone: An electroencephalographic study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29(8):1047–1057. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2003.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Sonuga-Barke EJ. Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: from common simple deficits to multiple developmental pathways. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1231–1238. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Thomas R, Sanders S, Doust J, Beller E, Glasziou P. Prevalence of attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2015;135:994–1001. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-3482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Vranic´ A, Hromatko I. Content-specific activational effects of estrogen on working memory performance. J Gen Psychol. 2008;135(3):323–336. doi: 10.3200/GENP.135.3.323-336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Wang LJ, Chou MC, Chou WJ, Lee MJ, Lee SY, Lin PY, Yen CF. Potential role of pre-and postnatal testosterone levels in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: is there a sex difference? Neuropsychiatr Dis and Treat. 2017;13:1331. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S136717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Whiteside SP, Lynam DR. The five factor model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Pers Individ Dif. 2001;30(4):669–689. [Google Scholar]
  70. Wilcutt EG. The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9(3):490–499. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0135-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES