Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2017 Mar 9;26(12):e67–e80. doi: 10.1002/hec.3477

The Effects of Exercise and Relaxation on Health and Wellbeing

Hannah Forbes 1,, Eleonora Fichera 1, Anne Rogers 2, Matt Sutton 1
PMCID: PMC5811789  PMID: 28276112

Summary

Better management by individuals of their long‐term conditions is promoted to improve health and reduce healthcare expenditure. However, there is limited evidence on the determinants and consequences of self‐management activity. We investigate the determinants of two forms of self‐management, exercise and relaxation, and their impact on the health and wellbeing of 3472 individuals with long‐term health conditions over a 1‐year period. We use simultaneous recursive trivariate models to estimate the effects of these two inputs on three health and wellbeing outcomes: the EuroQol five‐dimensional (EQ‐5D) score, self‐assessed health and happiness. We reflect the opportunity cost of time and knowledge with employment status and education and find that employment reduces relaxation and education increases exercise. We find that neither exercise nor relaxation affects the EuroQol five‐dimensional score, but exercise increases self‐assessed health and relaxation increases happiness. Our findings show that individuals tailor their self‐management activities to their economic constraints, with effects on different aspects of their utility. Interventions to encourage self‐management should take account of heterogeneous effects and constraints. © 2017 The Authors. Health Economics Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Keywords: self‐management, long‐term conditions, panel data, simultaneous recursive trivariate regression

1. Introduction

Healthcare systems in Organisation for Economic Co‐operation and Development (OECD) countries are endeavouring to rise to the challenges emerging from ageing populations, growing levels of chronic illness, advancing technical possibilities and rising public expectations. These challenges have resulted in healthcare expenditure growing more rapidly than GDP in all OECD countries over the past 30 years (Pammolli et al., 2011). The downturn in the world economy has added to this and raised concerns regarding the sustainability of expenditure on healthcare systems (Tooke, 2011). In the UK, spending on the National Health Service (NHS) as a proportion of GDP is expected to fall from 8% to just over 6% by 2021 (Appleby et al., 2014). This could result in a funding gap of approximately £30 billion by 2020/21 (Illmann).

One way to reduce pressure on formal health services is to reduce demand, especially amongst people with long‐term conditions. According to Roberts et al. (2012), pressure on NHS costs from people with long‐term conditions is at least equal to, if not more than, that from the expanding and ageing population. It is estimated that there are more than 15 million people in the UK with at least one long‐term condition, and the number of people with multiple long‐term conditions has been predicted to rise by over a third over the next 10 years (Department of Health, 2012). There is a similar trend across OECD countries; in 1990, average spending on long‐term conditions was 0.7% of GDP, and this had risen to 1.5% of GDP by 2012 (Gershlick, 2015).

A sustainable way to manage demand on services used by people with long‐term conditions is to encourage people to engage in their own health and healthcare. This was described by Wanless (2002) as the ‘fully engaged scenario’. This vision has resulted in heightened policy emphasis on encouraging patients with long‐term conditions towards ‘self‐management’ through schemes such as the Expert Patients Programme (Department of Health, 2001). Self‐management has been defined by the Department of Health as ‘[t]he care taken by individuals towards their own health and wellbeing: it comprises of the actions they take to lead a healthy lifestyle; to meet their social, emotional and psychological needs; to take care of their long‐term condition; and to prevent further illness or accidents’ (Department of Health, 2005). There has been increasing advocacy of self‐management in the UK and internationally over the last two decades (Rogers et al., 2009).

Recently, Naylor et al. (2015) have argued that active self‐management support is one of the key priorities for healthcare commissioners to focus on helping to sustain the NHS. Self‐management support aims to increase ‘the capacity, confidence and efficacy of the individual’ to manage their condition (Kennedy et al., 2013). There are two core models of self‐management support: a provider‐based model with support embedded in clinical practice and a patient‐based model, which enables patients through individual or group‐based education, telehealth or telecare (Kennedy et al., 2013). Despite the growing policy emphasis on the importance of self‐management support, there is modest evidence of the effects on health and healthcare utilization (Griffiths et al., 2007; Kennedy et al., 2013; Richardson et al., 2008). More notably, evaluations of interventions to increase self‐management activity focus on self‐efficacy (the confidence of the individual to manage their condition through self‐management) and changes in health and healthcare utilization in the short term. Little attention has been paid to the self‐management activities per se, which are treated as intervening variables. In this paper, we aim to bridge the gap between self‐management literature and economic literature to enhance understanding of self‐management behaviour.

Previous studies have shown that the allocation of time to a health input such as self‐management depends on its associated opportunity cost. Brown and Roberts (2011) analysed the frequency of participation in physical activity by using six waves of data from Household Income and Labour Dynamics of Australia. They measured opportunity cost through employment status and found that full‐time employment had a significant negative impact on exercise for women. Mullahy and Robert (2010) hypothesized that the opportunity cost of time differs by level of human capital endowment (measured through education). Using data from the American Time Use Survey, they found that human capital endowments affect the way participants use their time to produce different health outcomes. For example, more educated individuals were more likely to participate in physical exercise. These studies indicate that differences in the cost of time can change the way that people choose to invest in self‐management.

Brown and Roberts (2011) and Mullahy and Robert (2010) both used general population data. One study that focuses on people with long‐term conditions is by Ettner et al. (2009). Unlike Mullahy and Robert (2010), Ettner et al. found that education reduced the amount of time spent on self‐management activities in a sample of 11 927 patients with at least one long‐term condition. This implies that time constraints may affect the choice of health inputs differently amongst people with long‐term conditions.

Changes in health inputs as a result of time constraints can also affect health outcomes (Balia and Jones, 2008; Contoyannis and Jones, 2004). Despite the clear link between inputs to health and health outcomes, we have found only one paper that investigates the link between opportunity cost and health through the choice of health inputs. Kuvaja‐Köllner et al. (2013) used 2 years of data from a randomized controlled trial in Finland to link the effect of the opportunity cost of time spent exercising with the effect of exercise on health. They assume physical exercise to be endogenous and so conduct a two‐stage least squares analysis, using motivation to exercise as the instrumental variable for exercise. Kuvuja‐Köllner et al. find that a lower opportunity cost of time (measured through labour market status) has a significant effect on the amount of time spent exercising. This increase in time spent on physical exercise was found to have a positive effect on the short form 6D.

The Kuvaja‐Köllner et al. (2013) paper shows that the opportunity cost of time affects health inputs and health outcomes. However, their instrumental variable is a short‐term decision that could be correlated with the outcome. It could be that enjoyment of physical exercise can change and therefore is not a suitable instrument for exercise. A further limitation of Kuvaja‐Köllner et al.'s (2013) analysis is that lagged measures of health are not included in the model. Previous endowments of health are likely to affect people's allocation to time spent on exercise as well as people's future health. Therefore, the results could be affected by omitted variable bias. In addition, the input (exercise) and output (health) equations were not modelled simultaneously, even though it is likely that the errors between these equations could be correlated.

Kuvaja‐Köllner et al. (2013) paved the way for linking a health input to a health output within the same dataset, but they did not explain how the opportunity cost of time could affect the specific self‐management activities that policy is aiming to encourage. In this paper, we describe how the opportunity cost of time (measured through employment and education) can impact on the mix of self‐management activities and estimate the effects of self‐management activities on future health and wellbeing. Our study is distinct because we study a population of individuals with long‐term conditions, precisely those for whom self‐management potential is greatest. As we use data from a clinical trial, we have rich data on health and wellbeing. We improve on Kuvaja‐Köllner et al.'s (2013) study by modelling inputs and outputs simultaneously and by using more plausible identifying restrictions. We estimate exercise, relaxation and health as part of a simultaneous model and allow for the possibility that there is correlation between these equations.

2. Methods

2.1. Theoretical motivation

We motivate our empirical estimation of the relationship between the opportunity cost of time and self‐management (measured through exercise and relaxation) with a modified time allocation framework. The time allocation framework is based upon Becker's (1965) theory of allocation of time and the Cawley (2004) ‘SLOTH’ (sleep, leisure, occupation, transportation and home‐based activities) framework. We can think of patients who demand health for two reasons: (i) as a consumption commodity, because it directly enters their preference functions, and (ii) as an investment commodity, because it determines the amount of time available for other activities. Individuals choose how to allocate their time amongst work, exercise, relaxation and other activities to maximize their utility subject to a budget and time constraint. The budget constraint includes a time price of ‘self‐management’ which is dependent on wages and educational qualifications. The time price indicates the opportunity cost of participating in exercise and relaxation. The budget constraint includes the money price of exercise (e.g. gym membership), income and the time price of working. Health output is determined by two inputs, exercise and relaxation. If the time price of exercise increases due to the individual entering employment, then the opportunity cost of time spent exercising increases. But as the individual becomes richer, he or she is able to afford the money price of exercise. The overall effect of a change in the time price of exercise on its demand is ambiguous and depends on the relative size of the income and substitution effects.

2.2. Data

We use data collected in the trial of Whole System Informing Self‐Management Engagement, a provider‐based model of self‐management support (Bower et al., 2012; Kennedy et al., 2014, 2013). The trial was based in the north‐west of England between the years of 2009 and 2012. The population in the local area was predominantly white and socioeconomically deprived. The objective of the trial was to determine the effectiveness of a provider‐based model of self‐management support. No statistically significant differences were found between the patients receiving the provider‐based intervention and those in the control group. However, the trial highlighted the need to elicit and understand the active components required for effective self‐management support (Kennedy et al., 2014).

Patients were eligible for the trial if they had any of three long‐term conditions: diabetes, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease and irritable bowel syndrome. These were selected as exemplar conditions that could be influenced by self‐management interventions and where there was also published evidence of effectiveness (Deakin et al., 2005; Richardson et al., 2006). Patients were identified from electronic health records and checked for any exclusion criteria (under 18 years of age, insufficient English language, receiving palliative care or insufficient capacity to give written consent) before being sent a postal invitation to take part in the trial. Forty‐four general practices were recruited.

2.2.1. Health variables

We consider three measures of health and wellbeing: the EuroQol five‐dimensional three‐level (EQ‐5D‐3L) score, self‐assessed health (SAH) and the amount of time the respondent had spent being happy over the previous 4 weeks. The five dimensions in the EQ‐5D are mobility, self‐care, usual activities, pain/discomfort and anxiety/depression. Each dimension provides three options of response: no problems, some problems and extreme problems. SAH was collected by asking the following question: ‘In general, would you say your health is excellent, very good, good, fair or poor?’ Happiness was collected by using the following question: ‘How much time during the past 4 weeks have you been a happy person?’ The individual was offered six response categories: none of the time, a little bit of the time, some of the time, a good bit of the time, most of the time and all of the time.

2.2.2. Measures of self‐management (health inputs)

The respondents were asked to report the self‐management activities they adopted to help manage their conditions through the question: ‘To help manage my condition, I…’ For each listed self‐management activity, the respondent could select whether it was very true, partly true or not true. A full list of self‐management activities and associated summary statistics is presented in Table 2. Amongst the 12 listed activities are ‘do exercises’ and ‘rest and relax’. We focus on these inputs because both activities are time consuming and therefore economically interesting. They are also not highly correlated to one another (ρ = 0.07) and demonstrate good variation in the sample. In preliminary analysis, we also studied responses to ‘I follow an eating plan or special diet’, but we did not find any significant effects on the outcome variables.

2.2.3. Education and economic status

The participants were asked to report their highest educational qualification. We created a dummy variable for higher education. A value of 0 captured those with no qualifications, O levels or A levels, which constituted 51.3% of the participants. A value of 1 represents those with a professional qualification (36.7%) or a degree (12.0%).

The respondents were also asked to report their employment status from a list of eight options. We created a dummy variable. A value of 0 captured those who were unemployed, retired from paid work, voluntary workers and others. This comprised 65.1% of participants. A value of 1 represented those who were in full‐time or part‐time work, self‐employed, looking after the family or home or in full‐time education, capturing the other 34.9% of the participants. We combined the categories in this way because we found the effects of being in full‐time education and looking after the family or home to be very similar to the effects of full‐time work. They are equally as influential intuitively in terms of time availability.

2.2.4. Covariates

To control for the previous health of the participants, we used the respondent's SAH score and EQ‐5D‐3L score at baseline and at 6 months, as well as the time spent being happy at 6 months. We also controlled for socioeconomic and demographic characteristics, including gender, age, ethnicity, type of accommodation (defined as dummy variable with a value of 1 indicating whether the respondent owns a house) and type of long‐term condition. We further controlled for deprivation by using the index of multiple deprivation score [Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD), 2007] of the respondent's area of residence.

2.2.5. Analysis sample

The data were collected from patients via postal questionnaires at baseline and 6 and 12 months. We use all three waves of the data. Initially, 5599 patients (2546 with diabetes, 1634 chronic obstructive pulmonary disease and 1419 with irritable bowel syndrome) were recruited to the trial. Four thousand five hundred thirty‐three patients (81.0%) completed the 6‐month follow‐up questionnaire, and 4076 (72.8%) completed the 12‐month questionnaire.

We follow the missing data strategy adopted by Kennedy et al. (2013) who collected the dataset. This strategy was based on the findings of a simulation study by Groenwold et al. (2012), who showed that covariate adjustment is as efficient as multiple imputation at controlling for bias with the advantage of improving precision in estimating the treatment effect. We did not impute missing values for the outcome variables at 6 and 12 months but addressed potential bias due to missingness with covariate adjustment. We used logistic regression to model missingness in the outcome variables as a function of demographic characteristics, employment status, ethnicity, type of health condition, type of accommodation, the index of multiple deprivation, EQ‐5D‐3L and SAH measured at baseline. We then controlled for the baseline covariates that were predictive of missingness at the two follow‐ups.

There were small numbers (less than 3% of the sample) of missing values in the baseline values of EQ‐5D‐3L, SAH, gender, age, ethnicity, type of housing tenure and diabetes, heart problems/blood pressure and anxiety/depression. Missing values for these baseline variables were imputed by using a multiple imputation through chained‐equation technique as in Kennedy et al. (2013). Three thousand four hundred and seventy‐two observations were available following the imputation of missing data on the baseline variables.

2.3. Empirical strategy

Our empirical strategy aims to investigate the determinants of self‐management activities and the effect of these health inputs on health outputs. Self‐management activities such as exercise and relaxation have the potential to be endogenous if there is an unobserved variable that is correlated with both these inputs and health outputs or there could be unobserved individual characteristics that drive both the extent of self‐management activity and health. This potential for endogeneity raises the concern that if we estimate these equations separately, then there would be unobserved correlation between the error terms. Therefore, we adopt a simultaneous recursive trivariate model that controls for the potential endogeneity of exercise and relaxation and for unobserved heterogeneity between the equations (Maddala, 1983). Our model simultaneously estimates three equations, one output equation (health) and two input equations (exercise and relaxation):

ht=α1+β1h0+δ1ht1+γ1xt+μ1et+σ1rt+u1t (1)
et=α2+β2h0+δ2ht1+γ2xt+ϕ2et1+ω2rt1+ϑ2w0+τ2ed0+u2t (2)
rt=α3+β3h0+δ3ht1+γ3xt+ϕ3et1+ω3rt1+ϑ3w0+τ3ed0+u3t (3)

Health at baseline and 6 months is denoted by h 0 and h t − 1, and x denotes sociodemographic variables. Equations 2 and 3 show that we exclude employment (w 0) and education (ed 0) from the health equation. Thus, we assume that, conditional on baseline and lagged health and the other covariates, employment and education affect health indirectly through their influence on exercise and relaxation.

The variance–covariance matrix Ω can be written as follows:

Ω=1ρ12ρ13ρ121ρ23ρ13ρ231

where ρ jk with j, k = 1, 2,3 ∀ j ≠ k is the correlation between the error terms. Unobserved heterogeneity can be allowed with the potentially non‐zero values of the off‐diagonal elements of the matrix. We use Roodman's (2011) conditional recursive mixed process model to estimate the system of equations (1–3) in stata and report them in Table 4.

There has been much discussion of the best estimation technique for EQ‐5D given its bimodal distribution. Hernández Alava et al. (2012) argue that estimation of EQ‐5D should take into account its bimodal distribution by using an adjusted limited dependent variable mixture model. Other researchers have also used two‐part Tobit models (Acaster et al., 2015). However, numerous studies find that when EQ‐5D is treated as continuous, the model performs just as efficiently as any other estimation strategy, if not better (Kim et al., 2014).

To check for robustness of our results, we also run our analysis treating SAH and happiness as ordered probits to reflect the ordinal nature of these dependent variables. We present average marginal effects for each possible response category (none of the time, a little of the time, etc.).

Exercise and relaxation are ordinal variables on a three‐point scale. We include these variables in two different ways. When we model the health/wellbeing variables using linear models, we also treat the self‐management inputs as continuous. When we model SAH and happiness as ordinal variables, we include the self‐management input as binary variables, separating those who reported ‘very true’ from those who reported ‘partly true’ or ‘not true’.

We also test the independence of the equations and the validity of the over‐identifying restrictions. We test the independence of the equations with a likelihood ratio test that compares an unrestricted equation with all correlations between equations and a restricted one with correlations set to zero. We conduct an F‐test for over‐identification by setting the coefficients of the instruments equal to 0, excluding lagged exercise for exercise and lagged relaxation for relaxation.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive statistics

At baseline, the average EQ‐5D score reported by the respondents was 0.655, ranging from −0.426 to 1 (Table I). Also at baseline, 40% of the respondents reported their SAH as ‘poor’ or ‘fair’, and 43% stated that they were happy ‘none of the time’, ‘a little bit of the time’ or ‘some of the time’.

Table I.

Descriptive statistics

Variable At baseline At 6 months At 12 months
EQ‐5D score (mean, SD) 0.655 (0.307) 0.637 (0.312) 0.636 (0.317)
Self‐assessed health (n, %)
Poor 302 (8.70%) 380 (10.94%) 405 (11.66%)
Fair 1108 (31.91%) 1044(30.07%) 1082 (31.16%)
Good 1365 (39.31%) 1266 (36.46%) 1238 (35.66%)
Very good 607 (17.48%) 688 (19.82%) 636 (18.32%)
Excellent 90 (2.59%) 94 (2.71%) 111 (3.20%)
Happiness (n, %)
None of the time Not available 238 (6.85%) 218 (6.28%)
A little bit of the time 493 (14.20%) 475 (13.68%)
Some of the time 761 (21.92%) 836 (24.08%)
A good bit of the time 656 (18.89%) 639 (18.40%)
Most of the time 1022 (29.44%) 1002 (28.86%)
All of the time 302 (8.70%) 302 (8.70%)
Higher education (n, %) 1688 (48.62%)
Working full time (n, %) 1158 (33.35%)
White ethnic background (n, %) 3387 (97.55%)
Owns accommodation (n, %) 2443 (70.36)
Long‐term condition (n, %)
Diabetes 1736 (50.00%)
Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease 1247 (35.92%)
Irritable bowel syndrome 1014 (29.21%)
Arthritis 1586 (45.68%)
Heart 1547 (44.56%)
Fatigue 121 (3.49%)
Anxiety 678 (19.53%)
Multiple sclerosis 13 (0.37%)
Other 686 (19.76%)
Number of individuals 3472

EQ‐5D score, EuroQol five‐dimensional score.

The average age of the participants was 63 years (ranging from 18 to 102 years). The sample was predominantly from a White ethnic group (97%), slightly higher than the 95% across England as a whole for the population aged 65 years (see Office for National Statistics, 2009) and reflecting the population composition of Salford. A large proportion of the sample (71%) owned their own accommodation. Diabetes is the most common long‐term condition of the participants, with 50% suffering the condition. Arthritis and heart conditions were the next most frequent at 46% and 45% respectively.

At 12 months, 30.2% of the respondents said that it was ‘not true’ that they used exercise to manage their condition, 46.6% said that it was ‘partly true’, and 23.2% said that it was ‘very true’ (Table II). Of the respondents, 9.5% said that it was ‘not true’ that they rested and relaxed, 47.6% said that it was ‘partly true’, and 42.3% said that it was ‘very true’. The descriptive statistics for the 10 other self‐management activities are also provided in Table II. We investigated the correlations among the 12 self‐management activities and found that exercise was most strongly correlated with diet (ρ = 0.24) and relaxation was most strongly correlated with stress management (ρ = 0.39) and medication (ρ = 0.20). Exercise and relaxation were not highly correlated with each other (ρ = 0.07).

Table II.

Descriptive statistics for all 12 self‐management activities in the questionnaire

Variable Description At 6 months At 12 months
Exercise
Not true Do exercises 1062 (30.59%) 1047 (30.16%)
Partly true 1563 (45.02%) 1618 (46.60%)
Very true 847 (24.40%) 807 (23.24%)
Relaxation
Not true Rest and relax 341 (9.82%) 330 (9.50%)
Partly true 1638 (47.18%) 1653 (47.61%)
Very true 1493 (43.00%) 1489 (42.89%)
Diet
Not true Follow an eating plan or special diet 1064 (29.09) 1043 (28.55%)
Partly true 1762 (48.17%) 1833 (50.18%)
Very true 832 (22.74%) 777 (21/27%)
Stress management
Not true Reduce or avoid stress 407 (11.07%) 409 (11.12%)
Partly true 1863 (50.65%) 1851 (50.31%)
Very true 1408 (38.28%) 1419 (38.57%)
Medication
Not true Take medication as prescribed 126 (3.44%) 134 (3.66%)
Partly true 162 (7.16%) 277 (6.57%)
Very true 3271 (89.40%) 3249 (88.77%)
Information
Not true Seek information about condition 605 (16.48%) 633 (17.27%)
Partly true 1528 (41.62%) 1506 (41.09%)
Very true 1538 (41.90%) 1526 (41.64%)
Alternative treatments
Not true Try alternative treatments such as health products 2694 (73.49%) 2722 (74.39%)
Partly true 557 (15.19%) 547 (14.95%)
Very true 415 (24.40%) 390 (10.66%)
Meditation
Not true Meditation or prayers 2896 (79.32%) 2892 (79.43%)
Partly true 452 (12.38%) 434 (11.92%)
Very true 303 (8.30%) 315 (8.65%)
Advice from family/friends
Not true Seek advice from friends or family 2098 (57.14%) 2090 (57.15%)
Partly true 1248 (33.99%) 1251 (34.21%)
Very true 326 (8.88%) 316 (8.64%)
Advice from patients
Not true Seek advice from someone with the same condition 2382 (64.96%) 2379 (65.11%)
Partly true 990 (27.00%) 1008 (27.59%)
Very true 295 (8.04%) 267 (7.31%)
Self‐help groups
Not true Member of self‐help or support group 3449 (94.36%) 3430 (94.05%)
Partly true 92 (2.52%) 98 (2.69%)
Very true 114 (3.12%) 119 (3.26%)
Living adjustments
Not true Make adjustments to living conditions 1078 (29.35%) 1084 (29.55%)
Partly true
Very true 1799 (48.98%) 1801 (49.10%)
796 (21.67%) 783 (21.35%)
Number of individuals 3472

3.2. Simultaneous trivariate recursive regression results

Being in full‐time employment decreases the probability that the respondent uses relaxation to manage their condition by 11 percentage points (Table III). Table III also shows that having a degree or other professional qualification increases the probability that the individual will use exercise to manage their condition by 5 percentage points respectively.

Table III.

Simultaneous recursive estimation of the effects of health investments on health and wellbeing

Outcomes at 12 months
EQ‐5D score Self‐assessed health Time a happy person
Outcome equation
Exercise at 12 months 0.0018 0.0770*** 0.0396
(0.24) (3.42) (0.84)
Relaxation at 12 months 0.0074 −0.0423 0.2159***
(0.63) (−1.22) (2.95)
Input equation (exercise at 12 months)
Exercise at 6 months 0.5797*** 0.5798*** 0.5799***
(42.49) (42.51) (42.52)
Relaxation at 6 months 0.0141 0.0146 0.0147
(0.90) (0.93) (0.93)
Working full time −0.0231 −0.0215 −0.0208
(−0.86) (−0.80) (−0.77)
Higher education 0.0546** 0.0548** 0.0551**
(2.67) (2.68) (2.70)
Input equation (relaxation at 12 months)
Exercise at 6 months 0.0110 0.0114 0.0110
(0.82) (0.85) (0.82)
Relaxation at 6 months 0.4150*** 0.4152*** 0.4150***
(26.86) (26.89) (26.86)
Working full time −0.1108*** −0.1121*** −0.1105***
(−4.18) (−4.24) (−4.17)
Higher education −0.0356 −0.0355 −0.0354
(−1.77) (−1.77) (−1.76)
ρ 12 0.0500 −0.0043 −0.0245
(1.75) (−0.15) (−0.86)
ρ 13 −0.0109 0.0588 −0.0200
(−0.28) (1.55) (−0.52)
ρ 23 0.0819*** 0.0819*** 0.0819***
(4.78) (4.78) (4.78)
No. of observations 3472 3472 3472

EQ‐5D score, EuroQol five‐dimensional score.

Exercise and relaxation are measured as continuous variables: 0 = ‘not true’, 1 = ‘partly true’ and 2 = ‘very true’. All models are estimated on five datasets with imputed values for the missing variables. All equations also include the following covariates: ethnicity, housing tenure, index of multiple deprivation, main health condition, age, age squared, baseline self‐assessed health, self‐assessed health at 6 months, happiness at 6 months, EQ‐5D at baseline and EQ‐5D at 6 months.

***

p < 0.001.

**

p < 0.05. t‐stats are in parentheses.

Exercise and relaxation are found to have no statistically significant effect on EQ‐5D. A one‐unit increase in exercise, which represents a change from ‘not true’ to ‘partly true’ or ‘partly true’ to ‘very true’ that this form of self‐management was used by the respondent, increases SAH by 0.08 units on a five‐point scale. The same change in the use of rest and relaxation increases happiness by 0.22 units on a six‐point scale.

The value for ρ 23 is positive and significant (Table III). This implies that those who have a higher propensity to exercise also have a higher propensity to relax (and vice versa).

3.3. Robustness checks

The results of the simultaneous recursive trivariate models where SAH and happiness are treated as ordinal variables are presented in Table IV. Exercise increases the probability that SAH is good, very good or excellent and reduces the probability that it is either poor or fair. Relaxation instead decreases the probability that SAH is good, very good or excellent and increases the probability that it is either poor or fair. Relaxation significantly increases the probability of being happy a good bit of the time, most or all of the time.

Table IV.

Marginal effects of exercise and relaxation on self‐assessed health (SAH) and happiness

Self‐assessed health Happiness
Poor Fair Good Very good Excellent None of the time A little of the time Some of the time A good bit of the time Most of the time All of the time
Exercise −0.025*** −0.022*** 0.007* 0.029*** 0.011*** −0.007 −0.007 −0.006 0.001 0.011 0.008
Relaxation 0.018 0.016 −0.005 −0.021 −0.008 −0.054* −0.059** −0.047* 0.006* 0.088* 0.066*

Estimated with simultaneous trivariate recursive equations treating SAH and happiness as ordered probits and exercise and relaxation as binary variables, 1 = ‘very true’, 0 = ‘partly true’ or ‘not true’. All equations also include the following covariates: ethnicity, housing tenure, index of multiple deprivation, main health condition, age, age squared, baseline self‐assessed health, self‐assessed health at 6 months, happiness at 6 months, EuroQol five‐dimensional score (EQ‐5D) at baseline and EQ‐5D at 6 months.

***

p < 0.001.

**

p < 0.01.

*

p < 0.05.

3.4. Test statistics

The test statistics in Table V show that the equations are not independent. The p‐values from a likelihood ratio test imply that we should reject the null hypothesis and conduct a joint analysis. The results of an F‐test for over‐identification indicate that we can reject the null hypothesis; the model is not over‐identified.

Table V.

Tests of over‐identification and residual independence in the simultaneous recursive trivariate model

Health/wellbeing outcome
Test EQ‐5D score Self‐assessed health Happiness
Over‐identification χ 2 (2) = 31.43 χ 2 (2) = 32.86 χ 2 (2) = 31.44
Prob > χ 2 < 0.0001 Prob > χ 2 < 0.0001 Prob > χ 2 < 0.0001
Independence of residuals χ 2 (3) = 26.06 χ 2 (3) = 25.30 χ 2 (3) = 23.79
Prob > χ 2 < 0.0001 Prob > χ 2 < 0.0001 Prob > χ 2 < 0.0001

EQ‐5D score, EuroQol five‐dimensional score.

4. Discussion

In this paper, we have examined the determinants and consequences of two self‐management activities, relaxation and exercise. We have motivated our empirical specification with a time allocation framework where a change in the opportunity cost of time has an ambiguous effect on the choice of these health inputs because of the relative size of the income and substitution effects. As a result, the effect of exercise and relaxation on health is ambiguous as well. We apply this framework to a unique longitudinal trial of 3472 individuals observed at three points in time. This group is representative of populations with long‐term conditions, exactly those at whom self‐management support schemes are targeted.

We use education and employment status as measures of the opportunity cost of time of health investments. If the substitution effect is dominant, then increases in the opportunity cost of time (being in full‐time employment or having a degree) should reduce the amount of time participating in these activities. An increase in the opportunity cost of time also means an increase in income (if you are working or have higher education, then you are likely to have a higher income). Individuals are more likely to trade off labour for leisure as real income increases as long as leisure is a normal good.

Our results indicate that those who work full time are less likely to spend time resting and relaxing as a form of self‐management compared with those who do not work full time. This implies that the substitution effect is dominating. People are substituting self‐management for working as the opportunity cost of self‐management is increasing. This finding coincides with Humphrey and Ruseski's (2011) paper.

Education increases the probability of exercising. This implies that the income effect is dominating (when the shadow price of time is higher, the respondents have higher income and therefore do not need to spend as much time working). This finding is consistent with research on exercise within the general population (Eberth and Smith, 2010).

Time preference has been suggested as a channel through which health and education are correlated (Grossman, 2006). Van der Pol (2011) found that the relationship between education and health can be partly explained through time preference. Our finding that education increases exercise and decreases relaxation could also be partly explained by time preference. People with higher education could have a lower time preference and therefore are more willing to engage in an activity that benefits health in the long term, whereas people with lower education might prefer to engage in a self‐management that has a more instantaneous payoff.

Employment status and education change the way that people allocate their time to health investments. This finding supports similar findings in the literature (Brown and Roberts, 2011; Kuvaja‐Köllner et al., 2013; Mullahy and Robert, 2010). We find that the way in which people decide to allocate their time to self‐management behaviours affects their health. For example, as found by Kuvaja‐Köllner et al. (2013), exercise has a positive effect on SAH. Our findings could explain why researchers have found self‐management support schemes to affect efficacy but to only have modest effects on health and patient outcomes. One way to encourage people to engage in their health could be to attempt to make health‐promoting behaviours less costly to the individual.

One of our key findings is that the effect of exercise and relaxation on health and wellbeing depended on which measures of health and wellbeing we used. We found that both self‐management activities had no significant effect on EQ‐5D score, whereas exercise had a positive effect on SAH and relaxation had a positive effect on happiness. This is likely to be because different measures capture different aspects of health and wellbeing. For example, Sun et al. (2016) found that the anxiety/depression domain of the EQ‐5D was most strongly associated with subjective wellbeing. Böckerman et al. (2011) found that psychiatric disorders had a large effect on wellbeing even after controlling for the EQ‐5D (Böckerman et al., 2011). This could explain the stronger relationship shown between relaxation and happiness, due to its conceivably greater effect on mental health.

This may also be the explanation for the positive effect of exercise on SAH, which may be more focused on physical health. Exercise may positively affect physical health more than any other aspect, and hence show up most significantly in SAH. The lack of significant effect of either exercise or relaxation on EQ‐5D scores over time, however, may be explained by its insensitivity to changes over a short period of time. Clearly, the potential differences among EQ‐5D, SAH and happiness could have important ramifications for policy and allocation of resources depending on what measure we take to best capture utility.

Our test statistics indicate that there is enough correlation between the errors to justify modelling the equations simultaneously. Through modelling the equations simultaneously, we are also controlling for two sources of unobserved heterogeneity. First, we control for the possibility that exercise and relaxation are endogenous. We also control for the possibility that there is unobservable heterogeneity between the equations in our model. Through conducting a joint analysis, we found that the propensity to participate in relaxation is positively correlated with the propensity to participate in exercise. This could be picking up a measure of unobserved motivation to engage in health‐promoting behaviour.

To our knowledge, this is the first study in the UK that links the effect of employment and education on health inputs and health. We are also the first study to our knowledge to model exercise, relaxation and health simultaneously. However, there are several limitations to this study. First, there is a relatively short follow‐up. We look at the effect that self‐management has on health after just 6 months. One explanation for our modest findings for the effect of self‐management on EQ‐5D could be that 6 months is too short to pick up most of the effects of self‐management behaviour. Another potential limitation of the study is that we assume that exercise and relaxation are separate inputs with additive effects. We also note that our results are confined to a group of older people with an ethnic composition that is not representative of the younger population in England.

A further limitation relates to the available measures of exercise and relaxation. A change from ‘partly true’ to ‘very true’ is not an easily transferable measure of health input. However, further analysis (Table IV) shows that our results do not differ when using binary variables for whether the respondents reported ‘very true’ versus ‘partly true’ or ‘not true’.

Overall, our study demonstrates that constraints on time can change the way individuals engage in self‐management, with effects on health and wellbeing. We find no effect of exercise and relaxation on EQ‐5D, but we find significant positive effects of exercise on SAH and relaxation on happiness. These results are relevant for the National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence guidelines on best practice management of long‐term conditions (NICE Guidelines, 2009; NICE Guidelines, 2015). These guidelines promote a combination of drug treatment and behaviour change interventions targeting exercise but do not mention the importance of relaxation. Our findings indicate that initiatives designed to encourage self‐management should take account of heterogeneous constraints on an individual's potential to use different inputs and the potential for different effects on alternative aspects of their health and wellbeing. These results support the provision of individualized care and shared decisions between doctors and patients on the best behaviour change interventions depending on the individual's time constraints (NICE Guidelines, 2015).

Acknowledgements

This is an independent research supported by the National Institute for Health Research (Research Methods Fellowship MET 12–22) and the Medical Research Council (Early Career Fellowship in Economics of Health MR/K021583/1). The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the National Institute for Health Research or the Department of Health.

Forbes, H. , Fichera, E. , Rogers, A. , and Sutton, M. (2017) The Effects of Exercise and Relaxation on Health and Wellbeing. Health Econ., 26: e67–e80. doi: 10.1002/hec.3477.

References

  1. Acaster S, Pinder B, Mukuria C, Copans A. 2015. Mapping the EQ‐5D index from the cystic fibrosis questionnaire‐revised using multiple modelling approaches. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes 13: 33 DOI:10.1186/s12955-015-0224-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Appleby J, Galea A, Murray R. 2014. The NHS Productivity Challenge. The King's Fund. http://www.kingsfund.org.uk/publications/nhs‐productivity‐challenge (accessed 7.29.15). [Google Scholar]
  3. Balia S, Jones AM. 2008. Mortality, lifestyle and socio‐economic status. Journal of Health Economics 27: 1–26. DOI:10.1016/j.jhealeco.2007.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Becker GS. 1965. A theory of the allocation of time. The Economic Journal 75(299): 493–517. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bower P, Kennedy A, Reeves D, Rogers A, Blakeman T, Chew‐Graham C, Bowen R, Eden M, Gardner C, Hann M, Lee V, Morris R, Protheroe J, Richardson G, Sanders C, Swallow A, Thompson D. 2012. A cluster randomised controlled trial of the clinical and cost‐effectiveness of a “whole systems” model of self‐management support for the management of long‐term conditions in primary care: trial protocol. Implementation Science 7: 7 DOI:10.1186/1748-5908-7-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Böckerman P, Pekka I, Johansson E. 2011. Do established health‐related quality‐of‐life measures adequately capture the impact of chronic conditions on subjective well‐being? Health Policy 100(1): 91–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brown H, Roberts J. 2011. Exercising choice: the economic determinants of physical activity behaviour of an employed population. Social Science & Medicine 1982(73): 383–390. DOI:10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cawley J. 2004. An economic framework for understanding physical activity and eating behaviors. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 27(3S): 117–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Contoyannis P, Jones AM. 2004. Socio‐economic status, health and lifestyle. Journal of Health Economics 23: 965–995. DOI:10.1016/j.jhealeco.2004.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Deakin T, McShane CE, Cade JE, Williams RD. 2005. Group based training for self‐management strategies in people with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Cochrane Database Systematic Reviews 18(2): CD003417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Department of Health . 2001. The expert patient: a new approach to chronic disease management for the 21st century [WWW document]. URL http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/+/www.dh.gov.uk/en/Publicationsandstatistics/Publications/PublicationsPolicyandGuidance/DH_4006801 (accessed 6.30.15).
  12. Department of Health . 2005. Self care — a real choice. http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20130107105354/http://www.dh.gov.uk/prod_consum_dh/groups/dh_digitalassets/@dh/@en/documents/digitalasset/dh_4101702.pdf (accessed 6.30.15).
  13. Department of Health . 2012. Long Term Conditions Compendium of Information: Third Edition https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/long‐term‐conditions‐compendium‐of‐information‐third‐edition (accessed 7.29.15). [Google Scholar]
  14. Eberth B, Smith MD. 2010. Modelling the participation decision and duration of sporting activity in Scotland. Economic Modelling 27: 822–834. DOI:10.1016/j.econmod.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ettner SL, Cadwell BL, Russell LB, Brown A, Karter AJ, Safford M, Mangione C, Beckles G, Herman WH, Thompson TJ. 2009. Investing time in health: do socioeconomically disadvantaged patients spend more or less extra time on diabetes self‐care? Health Economics 18: 645–663. DOI:10.1002/hec.1394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gershlick, B. 2015. International comparisons. [WWW Document], (accessed 1.26.16) (accessed 6.30.15).
  17. Griffiths C, Foster G, Ramsay J, Eldridge S, Taylor S. 2007. How effective are expert patient (lay led) education programmes for chronic disease? BMJ 334: 1254–1256. DOI:10.1136/bmj.39227.698785.47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Groenwold R, Donders A, Roes K, Harrell F Jr, Moons K. 2012. Dealing with missing outcome data in randomized trials and observational studies. American Journal of Epidemiology 175: 210–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Grossman M. 2006. Education and nonmarket outcomes In Handbook of the Economics of Education, Hanushek E, Welch F. (eds.), North‐Holland: Amsterdam. [Google Scholar]
  20. Hernández Alava M, Wailoo AJ, Ara R. 2012 May. Tails from the peak district: adjusted limited dependent variable mixture models of EQ‐5D questionnaire health state utility values. Value in Health 15(3): 550–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Humphreys BR, Ruseski JE. 2011. An economic analysis of participation and time spent in physical activity. The B.E. Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy 11(1): 1–38. [Google Scholar]
  22. Illmann J. Kelsey: NHS faces £30bn funding gap by 2020 [WWW document]. URL http://www.hsj.co.uk/news/kelsey‐nhs‐faces‐30bn‐funding‐gap‐by‐2020/5060745.article (accessed 6.30.15).
  23. Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) 2007. — Datasets [WWW Document], n.d. URL https://data.gov.uk/dataset/index_of_multiple_deprivation_imd_2007 (accessed 2.2.16).
  24. Kennedy A, Bower P, Reeves D, Blakeman T, Bowen R, Chew‐Graham C, Eden M, Fullwood C, Gaffney H, Gardner C, Lee V, Morris R, Protheroe J, Richardson G, Sanders C, Swallow A, Thompson D, Rogers A. 2013. Implementation of self management support for long term conditions in routine primary care settings: cluster randomised controlled trial. The BMJ 346: f2882 DOI:10.1136/bmj.f2882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kennedy A, Rogers A, Chew‐Graham C, Blakeman T, Bowen R, Gardner C, Lee V, Morris R, Protheroe J. 2014. Implementation of a self‐management support approach (WISE) across a health system: a process evaluation explaining what did and did not work for organisations, clinicians and patients. Implementation Science 9: 129 DOI:10.1186/s13012-014-0129-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kim S‐H, Kim S‐O, Lee S, Jo M‐W. 2014. Deriving a mapping algorithm for converting SF‐36 scores to EQ‐5D utility score in a Korean population. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes 12: 145 DOI:10.1186/s12955-014-0145-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kuvaja‐Köllner V, Valtonen H, Komulainen P, Hassinen M, Rauramaa R. 2013. The impact of time cost of physical exercise on health outcomes by older adults: the DR's EXTRA study. The European Journal Health Economics HEPAC Health Economics Prevenzione Care 14: 471–479. DOI:10.1007/s10198-012-0390-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Maddala GS. 1983. Limited dependent and qualitative variables in econometrics. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge [Cambridgeshire]; New York. [Google Scholar]
  29. Mullahy J, Robert SA. 2010. No time to lose: Time constraints and physical activity in the production of health. Review of Economics of the Household 8: 409–432. [Google Scholar]
  30. Naylor C, Imison C, Addicott R, Buck D, Goodwin N, Harrison T, Ross S, Sonola L, Tian Y, Curry N. 2015. Transforming our health care system Kings Fund. http://www.kingsfund.org.uk/publications/articles/transforming‐our‐health‐care‐system‐ten‐priorities‐commissioners (accessed 7.29.15). [Google Scholar]
  31. NICE Guidelines , 2009. NICE guidelines type 2 diabetes: the management of type 2 diabetes (2009) Retrieved from: http://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/cg87/ifp/chapter/cardiovascular‐disease
  32. NICE Guidelines , 2015. NICE guidelines type 2 diabetes in adults: management. NICE NG28 Retrieved from: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng28/chapter/1‐recommendations
  33. Office for National Statistics . 2009. Archived data Retrieved from: https://docs.google.com/spreadsheets/d/1yc8W1SiCbWd9V4I9KmTlY_Rk_qullL828Qxbsvth93w/edit?hl=en#gid=1.
  34. Pammolli F, Riccaboni M, Magazzini L. 2011. The sustainability of European health care systems: beyond income and aging. The European Journal of Health Economics 13: 623–634. DOI:10.1007/s10198-011-0337-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Richardson G, Kennedy A, Reeves D, Bower P, Lee V, Middleton E, Gardner C, Gately C, Rogers A. 2008. Cost effectiveness of the Expert Patients Programme (EPP) for patients with chronic conditions. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 62: 361–367. DOI:10.1136/jech.2006.057430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Richardson G, Sculpher M, Kennedy A, Nelson E, Reeves D, Roberts C, Robinson A, Rogers A, Thompson D. 2006. Is self‐care a cost‐effective use of resources? Evidence from a randomized trial in inflammatory bowel disease. Journal of Health Services Research & Policy 11(4): 225–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rogers A, Bury M, Kennedy A. 2009. Rationality, rhetoric, and religiosity in health care: the case of England's Expert Patients Programme. International Journal of Health Services 39: 725–747. DOI:10.2190/HS.39.4.h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Roberts A, Marshall L, Charlesworth A. 2012. A decade of austerity? Nuffield Trust, http://www.nuffieldtrust.org.uk/sites/files/nuffield/121203_a_decade_of_austerity_full_report_1.pdf.
  39. Roodman D. 2011. Fitting fully observed recursive mixed‐process models with cmp. The Stata Journal 11(2): 159–206. [Google Scholar]
  40. Sun S, Chen J, Johannesson M, Kind P, Burström K. 2016. Subjective well‐being and its association with subjective health status, age, sex, region, and socio‐economic characteristics in a Chinese population study. Journal of Happiness Studies 17(2): 833–873. [Google Scholar]
  41. Tooke J. 2011. The future of healthcare in Europe. University College London. https://www.ucl.ac.uk/european‐institute/analysis‐publications/publications/FHE_FINAL_online.pdf.
  42. van der Pol M. 2011. Health, education and time preference. Health Economics 20: 917–929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Wanless D. 2002. Securing our future health: taking a long‐term view. HM Treasury. http://si.easp.es/derechosciudadania/wp‐content/uploads/2009/10/4.Informe‐Wanless.pdf. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Health Economics are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES