Abstract
Background
High quality, longitudinal data describing young people’s screen use across a number of distinct forms of screen activity is missing from the literature. This study tracked multiple screen use activities (passive screen use, gaming, social networking, web searching) amongst 10- to 17-year-old adolescents across 24 months.
Methods
This study tracked the screen use of 1948 Australian students in Grade 5 (n = 636), Grade 7 (n = 672), and Grade 9 (n = 640) for 24 months. At approximately six-month intervals, students reported their total screen time as well as time spent on social networking, passive screen use, gaming, and web use. Patterns of screen use were determined using latent growth curve modelling.
Results
In the Grades 7 and 9 cohorts, girls generally reported more screen use than boys (by approximately one hour a day), though all cohorts of boys reported more gaming. The different forms of screen use were remarkably stable, though specific cohorts showed change for certain forms of screen activity.
Conclusion
These results highlight the diverse nature of adolescent screen use and emphasise the need to consider both grade and sex in future research and policy.
Keywords: Adolescents, Electronic screen use, Guidelines
Background
Electronic screens are now a ubiquitous part of the adolescent landscape, occupying a large amount of their daily time, [1–10] and becoming an important aspect of their lives [11]. Today, virtually all 8 to 17-year-olds in developed countries access the internet at home, school or at their friends’ homes and increasingly through mobile devices outside of these settings [4, 9, 11]. Data suggest US children older than eight years of age spend on average 6.4 h per day on screen-based activities [see 9]. In Australia, electronic media use amongst 11–13-year-olds averages 4.8 h on a week day and 5.8 h on a weekend day [5], with an average of 3.8 h a day spent on sedentary screen time [12]. Access to multiple portable and constantly connected devices continues to proliferate, and today’s mobile phones can be simultaneously an internet, gaming and social media device. The consumption of multiple screen based activities contributes towards a high prevalence of screen use amongst children and adolescents [4, 5, 9, 13, 14].
Some studies suggest screen use has been associated with a range of adverse physical [15–19] and mental health outcomes [20–23]. However, the more recent research literature has debated the extent to which screen use is associated with these health related issues. For example, two separate meta-analyses have reported only small effects of video game use on behavioural and mental health outcomes [24] and no effects of TV and video game use with body fatness [25]. However, the relationships between screen use and positive and negative behavioural related outcomes may be more complex. That is, screen use up to one hour per day can be beneficial, but more extensive use may be detrimental [26]. In line with this, four linked studies have suggested that young people need to reach critical cut-off points before negative effects are observed [27]. The American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) recently recognised both the positive and negative effects of screen use on young people by recommending that parents develop personalised media use plans with their children so as to achieve a balance in lifestyle choices [28, 29].
A related issue concerns the extent to which national guidelines for youth ignore the diversity of screen use [30]. Typically, electronic media for entertainment has focused on use for recreation, combining TV viewing, gaming, and computer use as non-interactive activities [9]. This neglects the social active-engagement that now co-exists with a wide range of screen-based activities [9]. Given the plethora of devices, content, and connection, and normalisation of screen use, understanding young people’s use of screens over time is crucial. High quality, contemporary, longitudinal data which describes young people’s screen use across a number of distinct forms of screen activity is very limited in the literature. This is not to say that no longitudinal evidence exists. Higher amounts of initial screen time were found to be significantly associated with higher initial symptoms of depression in one study [20], whereas no predictive main effect of screen time on depressive symptoms (one year later) was found in another [31]. While such information is important when trying to understand screen use among youth, more research is needed to obtain a comprehensive picture about trends in screen use and separate screen use activities over time. Therefore, the purpose of this present study was to track patterns of multiple screen use activities (passive screen use, gaming, social networking, web searching) amongst 10- to 17-year-old adolescents using a latent growth curve model.
Method
Research design
This research utilised an accelerated longitudinal cohort sequential design (ALCSRD) to assess and represent change in screen use spanning approximately 7 years (10- to 17-years of age). The ALCSRD links adjacent segments of limited longitudinal data from different age cohorts to estimate a common long-term developmental trend, or growth curve. Thus, researchers can approximate a long-term longitudinal study by conducting several simultaneous short-term longitudinal studies of different age cohorts [32]. The validity of the ALCSRD as a valid and effective means of constructing the full longitudinal growth curve has been demonstrated for youth [33–37] and health-related behaviours, including physical activity [32, 38].
Participants
As shown in Table 1, three cohorts of participants were randomly recruited at Time 1 (T1) from Grade 5 (10/11 years of age: 346 males, 307 females), Grade 7 (12/13 years of age: 370 males, 324 females), and Grade 9 (14/15 years of age: 356 males, 312 females). These cohorts were retested on a further five occasions over three academic/calendar school years (2013–2015). To be included in the final analysis participants were required to have completed the initial survey (T1). Consequently, a total of 1948 children and adolescents (1030 males and 918 females) were retained for analysis.
Table 1.
Screen Use Measure | Time 11 | Time 22 | Time 33 | Time 44 | Time 55 | Time 66 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Overall | ||||||
Grade 5 | 3.05 (2.32) | 2.65 (2.16) | 2.60 (2.13) | 2.60 (2.12) | 2.89 (2.22) | 3.02 (2.20) |
Grade 7 | 3.92 (2.44) | 3.78 (2.61) | 3.67 (2.28) | 3.89 (2.47) | 3.92 (2.38) | 4.04 (2.43) |
Grade 9 | 4.95 (2.70) | 4.53 (2.47) | 4.57 (2.44) | 4.66 (2.63) | 4.63 (2.60) | 4.48 (2.47) |
Social Networking | ||||||
Grade 5 | 0.88 (1.77) | 0.86 (1.84) | 0.76 (1.40) | 0.93 (1.75) | 1.18 (2.00) | 1.30 (1.99) |
Grade 7 | 1.70 (2.45) | 1.71 (2.51) | 1.58 (2.17) | 1.72 (2.41) | 1.82 (2.33) | 1.93 (2.32) |
Grade 9 | 2.52 (2.99) | 2.17 (2.58) | 2.20 (2.56) | 2.28 (2.71) | 2.48 (2.69) | 2.19 (2.50) |
Gaming | ||||||
Grade 5 | 2.09 (2.28) | 1.86 (2.26) | 1.64 (2.06) | 1.60 (1.94) | 1.41 (2.02) | 1.38 (1.97) |
Grade 7 | 1.88 (2.16) | 1.71 (2.43) | 1.27 (1.85) | 1.43 (2.10) | 1.29 (2.24) | 1.23 (2.10) |
Grade 9 | 1.62 (2.33) | 1.57 (2.37) | 1.40 (2.22) | 1.42 (2.43) | 1.31 (2.57) | 1.17 (2.29) |
Web | ||||||
Grade 5 | 1.46 (1.93) | 1.35 (1.81) | 1.26 (1.65) | 1.25 (1.64) | 1.46 (1.64) | 1.34 (1.47) |
Grade 7 | 1.73 (1.99) | 1.90 (2.15) | 1.64 (1.64) | 1.84 (2.10) | 1.78 (1.94) | 1.79 (1.95) |
Grade 9 | 2.09 (2.17) | 2.04 (2.14) | 1.87 (2.25) | 2.17 (2.20) | 2.42 (2.50) | 2.15 (2.42) |
Passive | ||||||
Grade 5 | 2.45 (2.28) | 2.18 (2.17) | 2.09 (1.96) | 2.14 (2.11) | 2.06 (1.99) | 2.04 (1.89) |
Grade 7 | 2.73 (2.41) | 2.80 (2.52) | 2.35 (2.01) | 2.47 (2.33) | 2.41 (2.27) | 2.48 (2.28) |
Grade 9 | 3.02 (2.59) | 2.84 (2.32) | 2.69 (2.43) | 2.73 (2.53) | 2.59 (2.57) | 2.56 (2.65) |
1N = 590 to 652. 2N = 481 to 589. 3N = 461 to 539. 4N = 476 to 541. 5N = 373 to 483. 6N = 378 to 462
Participants were recruited from 25 randomly selected schools comprising state government primary schools (K-6) and high schools (7–12) (4 in rural locations), and non-government schools (K-12). All schools were located across a range of socio-economic status (SES) areas as indexed by their Socio-Economic Index for Areas (SEIFA), Australia, 2011 [39] and spread across metropolitan and regional areas. Initially schools were identified from different rural and metro geographical locations and SES areas across Western Australia in an attempt to ensure that any sample generated would be representative of the wider WA school and student population. A number of schools from each of these locations and SES areas were invited to participate. Of the 25 schools contacted, all agreed to involved.
Instrumentation
The Screen Based Media Use Scale (SBMUS), detailed in Houghton et al. (2015) [9] is an online instrument designed to collect data on “total time spent on screens” and types of screen activities. What ‘screens’ refer to and what they are typically used for is first clarified in the SBMUS (images are shown of iPod Touch, iPad, Mobile Phone, TV, Laptop, Computer, Xbox). Examples of screen activities are then provided. Participants are asked to estimate their overall use of screens on a typical week day from waking up until the time they go to bed (including use both during and outside of school) using an interactive slide bar that measures screen use in hours and minutes. This is then repeated for a typical weekend day. An overall screen use score for average weekly use was created by using a weighted average.
Four separate sections of questions on gaming, social networking and instant messenger, TV/Videos/Music (passive screen use), and web use are then presented. Each section requires participants to use an interactive slide bar to estimate their SBMU in hours and minutes [as previously for total time spent on screens].
Previous research [9] has demonstrated the SBMUS has satisfactory test-retest reliability across a 6-month period: Overall reliability (r = .50, N = 174) and this did not differ by sex (r(boys) = .51, n = 91; r(girls) = .53, n = 82). To ensure comprehensive coverage of screen use across the six administrations, surveys were administered at different times: T1 August/September 2013; T2 November/December 2013; T3 March/April 2014; T4 August/September 2014; T5 March/April 2015; and T6 August/September 2015.
Procedure
Permission to conduct this research was obtained from the Human Research Ethics Committees of the University of Western Australia and the State Department of Education. Data collection was carried out following the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki. We obtained written consent from principals, parents and students and reminded students (assent) on the day of participation via their teacher that if they wished they could withdraw from the study without prejudice. The SBMUS was administered to participants via an online survey during regular school hours. If a student was absent on any of the data collection days, an alternative time was arranged by the school to ensure survey completion.
Data analysis
MPlus version 7.31 [40] was used to estimate all latent growth curve models (LGCMs). This technique estimates latent variables corresponding to growth parameters (slope and intercept) [32, 34, 41]. Full information maximum likelihood (FIML) was used in the analyses to address missing data and the MLR estimator was used in order to address non-normality.
The analyses described here were repeated five times, i.e., for total screen time, gaming, social networking, web use, and passive screen use. First, a cohort-sequential LGCM [34] was conducted where the three cohorts were used to estimate a common intercept and slope, modelling screen use from mid-Grade 5 to mid-Grade 11. This was an unconditional growth model with random intercept and random slope: Each cohort accounted for a different section of the overall slope, and overlap in sampling (at Grades 7 and 9) allowing the adequacy of a model describing a common developmental trend to be assessed (see Fig. 1). The loadings of observed variables on the slope variable were constrained to reflect the timing (in months) of each data collection point, had a true longitudinal design been used. These loadings are divided by 10 in order to avoid convergence problems associated with large time scores, hence they varied from 0 to 7.2 instead of 0 to 72 [40]. The overlapping data collection points (mid-Grade 7, mid-Grade 9) were loaded as the same weight in both cohorts sharing that time point. In this way, a common LGCM was estimated from mid-Grade 5 to mid-Grade 11.
A cohort-specific LGCM approach (see Fig. 2) then assessed whether individual LGCMs for each cohort better accounted for the data than the common slope estimated in the cohort-sequential LGCM. This allowed for the possibility of differences in slopes and intercepts across the three cohorts (Grade 5, Grade 7, and Grade 9). Loadings of observed variables on the intercept latent variable were constrained to 1. Loadings of the observed variables on the slope latent variable were constrained to 0, 0.3, 0.8, 1.2, 2.0, and 2.4 to reflect the timing (in months) of each data collection point (again, divided by 10). This was again a random intercepts, random slopes model. Here, the means and variances of the slope and intercept were allowed to vary across the three cohorts, as was the slope-intercept covariance. The cohort-sequential LGCMs and the cohort-specific LGCMs were compared using the chi-square difference test (Δχ2): Significant differences indicate that the model with additional constraints (the cohort-sequential LGCM) should be rejected.
For all five measures of screen use, the cohort-specific model was a better fit to the data than the cohort-sequential model and there was significant variation in both intercept and slope [41]. We further evaluated a final model where the effects of sex upon slope and intercept were assessed. This model was a conditional cohort-specific LGCM, with both intercept and slope predicted by sex.
Results
Loss to follow-up
There were 1948 participants at Time 1. Across subsequent data collection points, this reduced to 1705 at T2 (12.5% attrition), 1597 at T3 (18.0% attrition), 1578 at T4 (19.0% attrition), 1337 at T5 (31.4% attrition), and 1303 at T6 (33.1% attrition). To understand if attrition was associated with sex, Grade, or rural/urban status, we conducted a logistic regression predicting participant presence or absence at T6. Nagelkerke R2 for the regression model was .010. Participants lost to follow-up were more likely to be girls than boys (AOR = 1.37, 95% CI [1.14, 1.66], p = .001), no other predictors were detected.
Descriptive statistics
Reported time spent on each form of screen use differed quite substantially over time (see Table 1). Total screen time was estimated at approximately 3 h per day by the Grade 5 cohort, 4 h per day by the Grade 7 cohort, and 4.5 to 5 h a day in the Grade 9 cohort. Social networking generally appeared to increase from T1 to T6, gaming and passive screen use decreased, and web use was relatively stable.
Results of latent growth curve analyses
The fit of the cohort-sequential latent growth curve models (LGCM) were relatively poor across all five measures, with weaknesses on certain indices of fit (see Table 2). In addition, the model failed to converge for social networking. The fit indices for the cohort-specific LGCMs were good across all five measures and the variances for all intercept and slope estimates were significant (all p < .001). The Δχ2 tests supported the superiority of the second models over the first. The conditional cohort-specific LGCMs which included sex also demonstrated good fit, and these were selected as the final models. Parameter estimates of the conditional cohort-specific LGCMs are reported in Table 3. Since five models were estimated, we applied a Bonferroni correction, hence only effects significant at the 99% level (p < .01) were interpreted.
Table 2.
Variable | Model | χ2 (df) | CFI | RMSEA (90% CI) | SRMR | Δχ2 (df) to previous |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Overall | (1) Full Accelerated LGCM | 286.53 (60), p < .001 | .858 | .076 (.068, .085) | .113 | N/A |
(2) Multi-Group LGCM | 98.20 (48), p < .001 | .969 | .040 (.029, .051) | .048 | 188.33 (12), p < .01 | |
(3) Sex Added to (2) | 121.55 (60), p < .001 | .968 | .040 (.030, .050) | .044 | N/A | |
Social Networking | (1) Full Accelerated LGCM | Does not converge* | ||||
(2) Multi-Group LGCM | 79.30 (48), p = .003 | .968 | .032 (.019, .044) | .045 | N/A | |
(3) Sex Added to (2) | 105.36 (60), p < .001 | .964 | .034 (.023, .045) | .042 | N/A | |
Gaming | (1) Full Accelerated LGCM | 160.40 (60), p < .001 | .887 | .051 (.041, .060) | .098 | N/A |
(2) Multi-Group LGCM | 81.61 (48), p < .001 | .962 | .033 (.020, .045) | .053 | 78.79 (12), p < .01 | |
(3) Sex Added to (2) | 98.44 (60), p = .001 | .967 | .031 (.020, .042) | .049 | N/A | |
Web use | (1) Full Accelerated LGCM | 151.08 (60), p < .001 | .883 | .048 (.039, .058) | .106 | N/A |
(2) Multi-Group LGCM | 74.51 (48), p = .008 | .966 | .029 (.015, .042) | .052 | 76.57 (12), p < .01 | |
(3) Sex Added to (2) | 94.29 (60), p = .003 | .965 | .030 (.017, .041) | .048 | N/A | |
Passive | (1) Full Accelerated LGCM | 185.52 (60), p < .001 | .872 | .057 (.048, .066) | .092 | N/A |
(2) Multi-Group LGCM | 75.48 (48), p = .007 | .972 | .030 (.016, .042) | .045 | 110.04 (12), p < .01 | |
(3) Sex Added to (2) | 93.23 (60), p = .004 | .972 | .029 (.017, .040) | .041 | N/A |
*Covariance matrix (PSI) in one or more groups is not positive definite
Good model fit = CFI values above .95; RMSEA scores of .06 or less; SRMR <.05
Table 3.
Grade and Parameter Estimate | Measure of screen use | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Overall | Social Networking | Gaming | Web | Passive | |
Grade 5 | |||||
Intercept | 2.77 (0.11)*** | 0.70 (0.08)*** | 2.32 (0.11)*** | 1.29 (0.08)*** | 2.19 (0.09)*** |
Slope | 0.05 (0.06) | 0.12 (0.05)** | −0.20 (0.06)** | 0.00 (0.04) | − 0.11 (0.05) |
Intercept* Sex | −0.08 (0.17) | 0.15 (0.12) | −0.58 (0.16)*** | 0.12 (0.12) | 0.22 (0.15) |
Slope* Sex | 0.02 (0.09) | 0.14 (0.07) | − 0.17 (0.09) | 0.01 (0.06) | − 0.02 (0.07) |
Intercept/Slope | −0.55 (0.16)** | −0.03 (0.09) | − 0.65 (0.16)*** | −0.12 (0.12) | − 0.15 (0.12) |
Grade 7 | |||||
Intercept | 3.17 (0.11)*** | 1.13 (0.10)*** | 2.06 (0.11)*** | 1.38 (0.09)*** | 2.26 (0.10)*** |
Slope | 0.24 (0.06)*** | 0.13 (0.05)** | −0.11 (0.06) | 0.10 (0.05) | 0.01 (0.06) |
Intercept* Sex | 1.26 (0.17)*** | 1.11 (0.18)*** | −0.71 (0.15)*** | 0.76 (0.14)*** | 0.83 (0.16)*** |
Slope* Sex | −0.18 (0.09) | 0.14 (0.08) | −0.26 (0.08)** | −0.09 (0.08) | − 0.11 (0.09) |
Intercept/Slope | −0.45 (0.13)*** | − 0.50 (0.18)** | −0.48 (0.15)** | − 0.40 (0.14)** | −0.46 (0.16)** |
Grade 9 | |||||
Intercept | 3.93 (0.12)*** | 1.74 (0.12)*** | 2.09 (0.12)*** | 1.67 (0.09)*** | 2.44 (0.11)*** |
Slope | 0.14 (0.07) | 0.18 (0.07) | −0.17 (0.07) | 0.16 (0.07) | −0.00 (0.07) |
Intercept* Sex | 1.73 (0.18)*** | 1.35 (0.21)*** | −1.08 (0.16)*** | 0.86 (0.16)*** | 1.03 (0.17)*** |
Slope* Sex | −0.39 (0.10)*** | −0.24 (0.10) | 0.08 (0.10) | −0.15 (0.10) | − 0.21 (0.11) |
Intercept/Slope | −0.56 (0.15)*** | − 0.82 (0.21)*** | −0.61 (0.20)** | − 0.61 (0.19)** | −0.43 (0.16)** |
** = p < .01; *** = p < .001
Footnote: Reference group for sex is male
Overall screen time
Sex had no effect on either the slope or intercept amongst the Grade 5 cohort, with overall screen time estimated at 2 h 46 min at the start of the study, remaining flat across the subsequent 24 months (see Fig. 3). Both boys and girls in the Grade 7 cohort reported increasing overall screen time across the 24 months, although at all time points girls reported an average of 1 h 16 min more than boys (girlsT1 = 4 h 26 min, boysT1 = 3 h 10 min). Among the Grade 9 cohort, girls reported the highest overall screen use of any sub-group (5 h 40 min) which then decreased across the 24 months, whereas boys started at a lower level than girls (3 h 56 min) and remained at that level across the data collection period.
Social networking
Sex had no effect on either the slope or intercept amongst the Grade 5 cohort, and while the time spent on social networking started at 42 min per day this increased by 7 min per month across T1 to T6 (see Fig. 4). Among the Grade 7 cohort, girls reported more frequent daily social networking at T1 (2 h 14 min) than boys (1 h 8 min) and both boys’ and girls’ social networking subsequently increased by 8 min per month. For the Grade 9 cohort, girls (3 h 5 min) again reported more frequent social networking than boys (1 h 44 min) at T1, though there was no subsequent increase in levels of social networking reported.
Gaming
In the Grade 5 cohort, boys reported gaming more than girls at T1 (2 h 14 min and 1 h 39 min daily, respectively), and this decreased at an equal rate for both sexes across T1 to T6 (see Fig. 5). Boys in the Grade 7 cohort also reported more time spent gaming than girls at T1 (2 h 4 min and 1 h 21 min, respectively), and girls (but not boys) decreased their subsequent time spend gaming. In the Grade 9 cohort, boys again reported more frequent gaming than girls (2 h 5 min and 1 h 1 min, respectively) and neither sex reported subsequent changes in time spent gaming.
Web use
Levels of web use remained stable from T1 to T6 within each cohort. In the Grade 5 cohort, both boys and girls reported using the web for 1 h 17 min per day (see Fig. 6). Girls reported more frequent web use than boys for both the Grade 7 cohort (2 h 8 min and 1 h 23 min, respectively) and the Grade 9 cohort (3 h 17 min and 2 h 26 min, respectively).
Passive screen use
As for web use, levels of passive screen use remained stable from T1 to T6 within each cohort. There was no sex difference in frequency of passive screen use in the Grade 5 cohort (2 h 11 min) (see Fig. 7). In the Grade 7 cohort, girls reported more frequent passive screen use than boys (3 h 5 min and 2 h 16 min daily, respectively). In the Grade 9 cohort, girls again reported more frequent passive screen use than boys (3 h 28 min and 2 h 26 min, respectively).
Discussion
This study has identified important nuances in patterns of screen use at different ages, emphasizing the heterogeneous nature of adolescent screen use. Screen access is ubiquitous amongst today’s adolescents [28, 42–44] and our results reinforce the high level of screen time 10–17 year olds engage on a daily basis [9, 21]. In the present study, we observed several different forms of screen use to be remarkably stable, though specific cohorts showed change for certain forms of screen activity. This stable pattern has not previously been reported in the literature, and may reflect important differences in the ways in which the rapidly changing electronic landscape is negotiated by each new set of young people [45]. In Grades 7 and 9 girls engage more than boys in all activities except gaming. Although Grade 9 boys’ estimates of total screen time remains stable for 24 months this is not the case for Grade 9 girls who have higher screen use which decreases over time. These results suggest that Grades 7 to 11 are periods when screen use is most fluid, implying screen use may be more amenable to change at these times, and efforts to change or moderate screen behaviours may be best targeted before these periods of significant increase in screen use.
The accelerated longitudinal cohort sequential research design utilised here was employed in the expectation that a unitary rate of change for each form of screen use could be estimated across the grade levels sampled (Grades 5 to 11). However, our results indicate that there were important cohort effects meaning that, for example, when the Grade 5 baseline cohort reached Year 7 they displayed different patterns of screen use to the Grade 7 baseline cohort. These cohort effects are likely to reflect the rapidly changing and evolving technologies available to young people: smartphone ownership among adolescents increased from 23% in 2011, to 37% in 2013 [45], and then 73% in 2015 [46]. Similarly, tablet computing has increasingly become a common activity in school classrooms [47]. Such developments can fundamentally change the ways in which young people use screens. For example, adolescents with smartphones (as compared to adolescents with cell phones) text more, and rely on social media and phone calls less, when contacting close friends [48]. The pace of change in technology therefore makes it very likely that each successive cohort of young people will experience and interact with the digital landscape in new and novel ways. In this rapidly evolving environment, clearly documenting young people’s changing screen use is important.
The pattern of screen use observed amongst participants in this study suggests that time spent in each main screen activity is relatively stable, across 24 months, within each cohort. The exceptions are declines in gaming amongst all Year 5 s and Year 7 girls, and increases in social networking among all Grade 5 s and Grade 7 s. The overall screen use trends resonate with previous findings that gaming declines with age, particularly for girls [49]. Our results support findings that social networking is more heavily used by girls than by boys [50] though suggest that this emerges only in Grade 7 (around 12 or 13 years old). Certainly, there were high levels of social networking reported, especially by girls, suggesting it is a highly normative and compelling activity. This is likely to make any effort to reduce or moderate screen use relating to this activity particularly challenging. The average time spent on each type of screen use summed across the four categories used in this study was substantially higher than the average total time spent using screens in each cohort, and for each time period. This reflects a high degree of multi-tasking and multiple screen use. Young people frequently multitask, using several screens at the same time (e.g. playing a game of using social media on a smartphone or tablet at the same time as watching TV or videos on the web.
Screen use amongst adolescents is now an entrenched and dominant sedentary behaviour [9, 28, 51] with data from this study suggesting that older adolescents spend considerable portions of their waking time engaged in screen activity. In fact, screen use may account for approximately 35% of girls’, and 25% of boys’, waking time (based on an average of 8 h sleep). However, contemporary research argues for a focus on a range of forms of screen use [9] rather than focussing on a single form (e.g., gaming) or on a single measurement of overall time spent on screens. The differences in screen time we have documented here support such an approach in future.
This study has a number of advantages when compared to the existing literature, including explicit assessment of the development of screen use across multiple cohorts across a two-year period. This design is unique in this literature and has highlighted important cohort effects which have not been documented before. However, limitations of the study should be considered when interpreting the results. The study relied upon self-report of screen use and activities, and it was not feasible to conduct objective validation of these. While such validation is desirable, it is extremely challenging for a number of reasons including the use of screens at school, the number of screens used by young people, and the use of screens which are not the participants’ own (e.g., friends’ or other family members’ smartphones, tablets, TVs etc.).
There was some disparity between the participants estimations of the total time they spent using screens compared to the time they spent on each of the four screen activities. Young people often undertake more than one screen activity simultaneously (e.g. using social media on their phone at the same time as watching TV) and therefore by simply adding each screen activity estimate to generate a total screen time is not an accurate method of estimation.
Conclusions
In conclusion, these results highlight the heterogeneity of adolescent screen use, and in particular the merits of delineating the type of screen activity, rather than just focusing on overall screen time [9], as presented in the AAP updated recommendations [29]. Future guidelines may consider significant changes in screen media consumption patterns across adolescence. If effective public health communications are to be developed, future refinement of screen use guidelines and recommendations need to directly consider how they can be flexible enough to respond to both cohort-level and shifts in young people’s screen based behaviours and experiences.
Acknowledgements
We are indebted to the Department of Education (Western Australia) for providing access to schools and the students who agreed to participate in the study.
Funding
This study was funded by a grant through the Western Australian Health Promotion Foundation (Healthway) (#22951).
Availability of data and materials
The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- AAP
American Academy of Pediatrics
- ALCSRD
Accelerated longitudinal cohort sequential design
- FIML
Full information maximum likelihood
- K-12
Kindergarten to Year 12
- K-6
Kindergarten to Year 6
- LGCM
Latent growth curve models
- SBMUS
Screen based Media Use Scale
- SEIFA
Socio-Economic Index for Areas
- SES
Socio-economic status
Authors’ contributions
MR, conceptualised the study, developed the instruments and drafted the initial manuscript. SH conceptualised the study, developed the instruments, coordinated and supervised data collection and drafted the initial manuscript. SCH conceptualised the study, developed the instruments, carried out the data analysis and drafted the initial manuscript. TS conceptualised the study and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content. LW conceptualised the study, developed the instruments and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript. CZ developed the instruments, coordinated and supervised the data collection, and drafted the initial manuscript. DL contributed to drafting the data analytic plan, conducted the longitudinal analysis and interpretation of data with SCH, drafted the results sections of the manuscript with SCH and critically reviewed the manuscript prior to submission. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Ethics approval was provided for this study by the University of Western Australia’s Human Research Ethics Committee and the State Department of Education Ethics Committee. Active Principal consent was obtained for school participation. Active Parental/Guardian consent and Child assent was obtained for child participation in this study.
Consent for publication
Not applicable
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Michael Rosenberg, Email: Michael.rosenberg@uwa.edu.au.
Stephen Houghton, Email: Stephen.Houghton@uwa.edu.au.
Simon C. Hunter, Email: simon.hunter@strath.ac.uk
Corinne Zadow, Email: Corinne.Zadow@uwa.edu.au.
Trevor Shilton, Email: Trevor.Shilton@heartfoundation.org.au.
Lisa Wood, Email: Lisa.Wood@uwa.edu.au.
David Lawrence, Email: David.Lawrence@uwa.edu.au.
References
- 1.Anderson SE, Economos CD. Must a: active play and screen time in US children aged 4 to 11 years in relation to sociodemographic and weight status characteristics: a nationally representative cross-sectional analysis. BMC Public Health. 2008;8(1):1. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-8-366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Biddle SJ, Pearson N, Ross GM, Braithwaite R. Tracking of sedentary behaviours of young people: a systematic review. Prev Med. 2010;51(5):345–351. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: a review. Sleep Med. 2010;11(8):735–742. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Lenhart A, Purcell K, Smith A, Zickuhr K. Social Media & Mobile Internet use among teens and young adults. Washington, DC: Millennials: Pew Internet & American Life Project; 2010.
- 5.Granich J, Rosenberg M, Knuiman MW, Timperio A. Individual, social, and physical environment factors associated with electronic media use among children: sedentary behavior at home. J Phys Act Health. 2011;8(5):613. doi: 10.1123/jpah.8.5.613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hinkley T, Salmon J, Okely AD, Crawford D, Hesketh K. Preschoolers’ physical activity, screen time, and compliance with recommendations. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2012;44(3):458–465. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e318233763b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Fakhouri TH, Hughes JP, Brody DJ, Kit BK, Ogden CL. Physical activity and screen-time viewing among elementary school–aged children in the United States from 2009 to 2010. JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167(3):223–229. doi: 10.1001/2013.jamapediatrics.122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Yang F, Helgason AR, Sigfusdottir ID, Kristjansson AL. Electronic screen use and mental well-being of 10–12-year-old children. The European Journal of Public Health. 2013;23(3):492–498. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cks102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Houghton S, Hunter SC, Rosenberg M, Wood L, Zadow C, Martin K, Shilton T. Virtually impossible: limiting Australian children and adolescents daily screen based media use. BMC Public Health. 2015;15(1):1. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-15-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Chassiakos YLR, Radesky J, Christakis D, Moreno MA, Cross C. Children And adolescents and digital media. Pediatrics. 2016:e20162593. 10.1542/peds.2016-2593. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 11.Australian Communication and Media Authority . Like, post, share: young Australians’ experience of social media. Canberra: Australian Government; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Olds TS, Maher CA, Ridley K, Kittel DM. Descriptive epidemiology of screen and non-screen sedentary time in adolescents: a cross sectional study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010;7(1):1–9. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-7-92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Pea R, Nass C, Meheula L, Rance M, Kumar A, Bamford H, Nass M, Simha A, Stillerman B, Yang S. Media use, face-to-face communication, media multitasking, and social well-being among 8-to 12-year-old girls. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(2):327. doi: 10.1037/a0027030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sampasa-Kanyinga H, Chaput J-P. Use of social networking sites and adherence to physical activity and screen time recommendations in adolescents. J Phys Act Health. 2016;13(5):474-480. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 15.Suchert V, Hanewinkel R, Isensee B. Screen time, weight status and the self-concept of physical attractiveness in adolescents. J Adolesc. 2016;48:11–17. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Byun W, Dowda M, Pate RR. Associations between screen-based sedentary behavior and cardiovascular disease risk factors in Korean youth. J Korean Med Sci. 2012;27:388–394. doi: 10.3346/jkms.2012.27.4.388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kristiansen H, Júlíusson PB, Eide GE, Roelants M, Bjerknes R. TV viewing and obesity among Norwegian children: the importance of parental education. Acta Paediatr. 2013;102:199–205. doi: 10.1111/apa.12066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Christofaro DG, De Andrade SM, Mesas AE, Fernandes RA, Farias Júnior JC. Higher screen time is associated with overweight, poor dietary habits and physical inactivity in Brazilian adolescents, mainly among girls. European Journal of Sport Science. 2016;16(4):498–506. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2015.1068868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Gomes TN, dos Santos FK, Santos D, Pereira S, Chaves R, Katzmarzyk PT, Maia J. Correlates of sedentary time in children: a multilevel modelling approach. BMC Public Health. 2014;14(1):890–898. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-890. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gunnell KE, Flament MF, Buchholz A, Henderson KA, Obeid N, Schubert N, Goldfield GS. Examining the bidirectional relationship between physical activity, screen time, and symptoms of anxiety and depression over time during adolescence. Prev Med. 2016;88:147–152. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Maras D, Flament MF, Murray M, Buchholz A, Henderson KA, Obeid N, Goldfield GS. Screen time is associated with depression and anxiety in Canadian youth. Prev Med. 2015;73:133–138. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2015.01.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Chen SY, Lu L. After-school time use in Taiwan: effects on educational achievement and well-being. Adolescence. 2009;44(176):891–909. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.McHale SM, Crouter AC, Tucker CJ. Free-time activities in middle childhood: links with adjustment in early adolescence. Child Dev. 2001;72(6):1764–1778. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ferguson CJ. Do angry birds make for angry children? A meta-analysis of video game influences on children’s and adolescents’ aggression, mental health, prosocial behavior, and academic performance. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10(5):646–666. doi: 10.1177/1745691615592234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Marshall SJ, Biddle SJ, Gorely T, Cameron N, Murdey I. Relationships between media use, body fatness and physical activity in children and youth: a meta-analysis. Int J Obes. 2004;28(10):1238–1246. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Liu M, Wu L, Yao S. Dose–response association of screen time-based sedentary behaviour in children and adolescents and depression: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Br J Sports Med. 2016;50:1252–1258. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2015-095084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Przybylski AK, Weinstein N, Murayama K. Internet gaming disorder: investigating the clinical relevance of a new phenomenon. Am J Psychiatr. 2016;174(3):230–236. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16020224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hill D, Ameenuddin N, Chassiakos YLR, Cross C, Radesky J, Hutchinson J, Levine A, Boyd R, Mendelson R, Moreno M. Media use in school-aged children and adolescents. Pediatrics. 2016;e20162592. 10.1542/peds.2016-2593. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 29.AAP Council on Communications and Media Media use in school-aged children and adolescents. Pediatrics. 2016;138(5):1–6. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Parrish A-M, Vella S, Okely AD, Cliff D. Physical activity and sedentary guidelines; what are the similarities and differences across the globe? 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Selfhout MH, Branje SJ, Delsing M, ter Bogt TF, Meeus WH. Different types of internet use, depression, and social anxiety: the role of perceived friendship quality. J Adolesc. 2009;32:819–833. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Duncan SC, Duncan TE. Larson B, Little TD, Card NA, editors. Acceleratd longitudinal designs. In: Handbook of Developmental Research Methods. New York: Guilford Press. (in press)
- 33.Baer J, Schmitz MF. Latent growth curve modeling with a cohort sequential design. Soc Work Res. 2000;24(4):243. doi: 10.1093/swr/24.4.243. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Duncan SC, Duncan TE, Strycker LA. Alcohol use from ages 9 to 16: a cohort-sequential latent growth model. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2006;81(1):71–81. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.McArdle JJ, Ferrer-Caja E, Hamagami F, Woodcock RW. Comparative longitudinal structural analyses of the growth and decline of multiple intellectual abilities over the life span. Dev Psychol. 2002;38(1):115. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.1.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Tildesley EA, Andrews JA. The development of children's intentions to use alcohol: direct and indirect effects of parent alcohol use and parenting behaviors. Psychol Addict Behav. 2008;22(3):326. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.22.3.326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Watt HMA. Latent growth curve modeling approach using an accelerated longitudinal design: the ontogeny of boys’ and girls’ talent perceptions and intrinsic values through adolescence. Educ Res Eval. 2008;14(4):287–304. doi: 10.1080/13803610802249316. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Duncan SC, Duncan TE, Strycker LA, Chaumeton NR. A cohort-sequential latent growth model of physical activity from ages 12 to 17 years. Ann Behav Med. 2007;33(1):80–89. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm3301_9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Statistics ABS. Canberra: Australian bureau of statistics. 2011. Census of population and housing: socio-economic index of areas (SEIFA) Australia state suburb indexes. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Muthen LK, Muthen B O, . Mplus User's guide. Seventh edition. Los Angeles: Muthen & Muthen; 1998-2015.
- 41.Preacher KL, Wichman AL, MacCallum RC, Briggs NE. Latent growth curve modeling. USA: Sage; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lenhart A. Teens, Social Media & Technology Overview 2015: Pew Research Center; 2015.
- 43.Steeves V. Young Canadians in a wired world, phase III: life online. In. MediaSmarts: Ottawa; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Authority ACaM . Australian government. 2014. Communications report 2013-2014. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Madden M, Lenhart A, Duggan M, Cortesi S, Gasser U. Teens and technology 2013. Washington, DC: Pew Internet & American Life Project; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lenhart A, Smith A, Anderson M, Duggan M, Perrin A. Teens, technology and friendships. 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Blackwell CK, Wartella E, Lauricella AR, Robb MB. Northwestern School of Communication. 2015. Technology in the lives of educators and early childhood programs. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Lenhart A, Ling R, Campbell S, Purcell K. Teens and mobile phones: text messaging explodes as teens embrace it as the centerpiece of their communication strategies with friends. Washington, DC: Pew Internet & American Life Project; 2010.
- 49.Lenhart A, Kahne J, Middaugh E, Macgill AR, Evans C, Vitak J. Teens, video games, and civics: teens’ gaming experiences are diverse and include significant social interaction and civic engagement. Washington, DC: Pew Internet & American Life Project; 2008.
- 50.Tsitsika AK, Tzavela EC, Janikian M, Ólafsson K, Iordache A, Schoenmakers TM, Tzavara C, Richardson C. Online social networking in adolescence: patterns of use in six European countries and links with psychosocial functioning. J Adolesc Health. 2014;55(1):141–147. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Carson V, Staiano AE, Katzmarzyk PT. Physical activity, screen time, and sitting among US adolescents. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2015;27(1):151. doi: 10.1123/pes.2014-0022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.