Abstract
Similar phenotypic changes occur across many species as a result of domestication, e.g. in pigmentation and snout size. Experimental studies of domestication have concentrated on intense and directed selection regimes, while conditions that approximate the commensal and indirect interactions with humans have not been explored. We examine long-term data on a free-living population of wild house mice that have been indirectly selected for tameness by regular exposure to humans. In the course of a decade, this mouse population exhibited significantly increased occurrence of white patches of fur and decreased head length. These phenotypic changes fit to the predictions of the ‘domestication syndrome'.
Keywords: commensalism, tameness, pigmentation, domestication syndrome, human, evolutionary rate
1. Introduction
The outcomes of domestication in the diversity of breeds are familiar, but much less is known about the effects of the initial phases of the domestication process. These include alternative pathways: ‘commensal', ‘prey’ and ‘directed' [1–3]. The commensal pathway is characterized by wild animals, such as wolves, entering an anthropogenic habitat, and eventually becoming habituated and tame. The prey pathway concerns increasingly more intensively managed prey species. The directed pathway describes a process in which humans intentionally domesticate wild species, based on knowledge of previous domestication processes, such as mink and silver fox [4].
Although the distribution of traits across domesticated forms of mammalian species is not universal [5], some traits do commonly appear, irrespective of phylogenetic relatedness. The ‘domestication syndrome' includes the appearance of white patches of fur and a reduced relative brain and snout size. The leading mechanistic explanation is that selection for tameness results in developmental changes in the neural crest that produce this cascade of features [6]. Experimental selection for tameness in silver foxes [7], rats [8,9] and mink [10] have shown the power of the directed pathway of domestication. The commensal pathway of domestication remains unexplored. In this study, we describe phenotypic changes in a population of wild house mouse (Mus musculus domesticus) that has experienced an environment similar to that hypothesized for the early phases of the commensal pathway: frequent exposure to humans, without deliberate artificial selection. We investigate if domestication-associated traits that have been scored in this population (occurrence of white patches of fur and head length) are changing in this mouse population and compare the rate of evolution of these traits.
2. Material and methods
The study population was established in 2002 in an uninhabited barn in Illnau, Switzerland, with the introduction of 12 wild-caught individuals [11] trapped in cattle and sheep sheds at two neighbouring small working farms. Mice in these small working farms are considered as unwanted pests. The founder individuals reproduced successfully and the study population increased considerably in size and now comprises 250–430 individuals at any given time [12]. The floor area of the study barn is 72 m2, resulting in 3.5–6 mice m−2 in the barn, a density that is below the 10 mice m−2 that can occur in stable, commensal house mouse populations [13]. The sex ratio is approximately equal [11] and inbreeding levels did not increase over time in the study population [14]. Mice are free to enter and leave through numerous openings which are too small for predators to pass through (e.g. domestic cats, martens, foxes, owls). Other small rodents living outside occasionally enter but have never colonized the barn. The population is subject to diseases [15] and parasites. Commensal mice on farms typically have abundant food resources and a human-made environment [16], which is also the case in the study population. A standardized ad libitum feeding regime is followed with a 50 : 50 oat and commercial rodent food (Haefliger AG) mixture.
Owing to regular experimental handling of mice and monitoring of nests over 14 years, we hypothesize that these wild mice have been habituated and unintentionally selected for tameness for approximately 20 generations [14]. All nest-boxes and hiding places are monitored at least every 10–13 days. Every mouse is usually first handled during these controls and subsequently, a second time when pups reach 13 days of postnatal age (±1 day, see [11]; electronic supplementary material, table S1 for details) to record sex, body weight and head length. Some pups were first discovered at the age of 13 ± 1 days, and then were handled once rather than twice as a pup. Every seven weeks, on average, comprehensive capture events are conducted in which all pups are handled, and all subadults and adults are captured and inspected in jars [11]. During these events, newly mature adults are also handled, inspected for white patches of fur and implanted with transponders [11]. Thus, in an average mouse lifespan in the study population of 196 days (28 weeks) [14], mice are handled two to three times and captured an additional three to four times. Capture success of transpondered adults is about 80% per capture event, thus some mice will be captured less often. This contrasts with virtually no human handling at all in sympatric commensal house mouse populations. Handling procedures correspond to good laboratory practice, i.e. mice are held gently but firmly in the hand and no mice are injured during the handling procedure. Duration of handling varies with age and temper of individuals and comprises 1–2 min per individual.
Two datasets inform the present study: (i) the occurrence of white patches and spots in the fur of adult mice from 2010 to 2016 (n = 2727) which was analysed with a binary logistic regression using white (1) and wild-type (0) as outcome and the date of monitoring (day/month/year) as the predictor variable. Approximately eight white hairs make up a small white spot. Individuals were systematically examined in the hand and tagged the first time they were caught as adults, here considered as 18 g or more [11], and therefore our dataset is restricted to first capture events of each adult (repeat examinations have been excluded). Only capture events with more than 10 recorded specimens were considered. (ii) Measurements of head length and body weight of 13-day-old (±1 day) mice from 2007 to 2016 (n = 2633), which were analysed with linear regressions. The age of pups was estimated by the developmental stage of hair growth, and ear and eye development. The eyes open at age 14 days [11]. Variation in age of measurement results mainly from non-daily visits to the field site. Head length of pups was measured in mm with digital callipers from the back of the skull to the tip of the snout (figure 1). Body weight (to the nearest 0.1 g measured by Mettler digital balances) was used as a proxy for body size. Pups of the same age found in the same nest were assigned a unique litter ID. To reduce variation in measurement error due to differences between observers, we only used measurements conducted by either of two highly trained people that collected data throughout the study period. Specimens described as runts were excluded. Time constraints in whole population capture events (in which up to 700 animals were handled in a day) precluded the measurement of head length in adults. Head length relative to body size (using residuals from least-squares regression of body weight on head length) was set as the response variable in a generalized linear mixed model with year of birth and sex as fixed effects and age, observer ID, litter ID and temperature as random effects. Age was used as a random effect because the age range of 2 days in the investigated mice (12–14 days old) could constitute a potential bias due to ontogenetic variation. Observer ID was included as a random effect because of possible systematic differences in measurements between observers. Litter ID was used as a random effect because measurements from individuals of the same litter are not independent due to a similar genetic, environmental and maternal background. Temperature (as measured at the day of pup examination in the barn) was used as a random effect because directed changes of temperature over the years might have had an influence on body weight and skull length [17–19]. The time intervals in these datasets differ because the initial purpose of the study was to investigate behaviour, disease transmission and genetics in house mice [15,20–24]: white spotting was very rare and, therefore, not systematically recorded before 2010 and head length information was not collected prior to 2007.
Evolutionary rates of head length change were calculated in darwins (d) [25] and haldanes (h) [26] (for a review, see [27]). Darwins were calculated as d = [ln(x2) − ln(x1)]/[t2 − t1], where ln(x2) − ln(x1) is the difference between the ln-transformed sample means of head length at time t2 and t1, and t2 − t1 is the elapsed time (in Myr) between t2 and t1. Head length changes (x1, x2) between 2007 (t1) and 2016 (t2) were calculated. Haldanes were calculated as , where ln(x1) and ln(x2) are the ln-transformed sample means of head length at the beginning and the end of the study period, respectively, and are the pooled standard deviations of ln(x1) and ln(x2), respectively, and g is the number of generations between the beginning and the end of the study period (years divided by generation length). Generation time in the study population has been estimated to be 263 days [14]. Species mean evolutionary rates of head and skull measurements in eight rodent species (and subspecies in the case of Peromyscus maniculatus) were used for comparison [28–31] (table 1). A one-sample Wilcoxon's signed-rank test was used to compare the median evolutionary rates in darwins from the literature to that of the study population. Statistical comparisons of haldane estimates could not be conducted because the literature data were only available for two species (table 1). Analyses were conducted using Microsoft Excel 2010, R v. 3.1.3 [32], and RStudio v. 0.98.1103 [33].
Table 1.
species | study area | timeframe (years) | traits showing significant change | darwins (d) | haldanes (h) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Akodon cursor [28] | EPTEA Mato do Paraíso (Brazil) | 15.5 | least interorbital width | 1622.03 | n.a. |
height of skull | 1127.18 | n.a. | |||
Cerradomys subflavus [28] | EPTEA Mato do Paraíso (Brazil) | 15.5 | rostrum width | 2022.72 | n.a. |
length of incisive foramina | 4308.62 | n.a. | |||
Oligorysomys nigripes [28] | EPTEA Mato do Paraíso (Brazil) | 15.5 | nasal length | 1945.81 | n.a. |
rostrum length | 1915.23 | n.a. | |||
Peromyscus maniculatus anacapae [29] | Anacapa (Channel Island) | 38 | intermeatus width | 1730.00 | n.a. |
breadth of rostrum | 461.00 | n.a. | |||
depth of braincase | 702.00 | n.a. | |||
length of incisive foramen | 688.00 | n.a. | |||
snout width | 603.00 | n.a. | |||
breadth of zygomatic plate | 685.00 | n.a. | |||
P. m. santacruzae [29] | Santa Cruz (Channel Island) | 38 | intermeatus width | 2682.00 | n.a. |
depth of braincase | 792.00 | n.a. | |||
P. m. elusus [29] | Santa Barbara (Channel Island) | 44 | length of nasals | 916.00 | n.a. |
depth of braincase | 619.00 | n.a. | |||
P. leucopus [30] | Chicago (Illinois) | 27.5 | breadth of rostrum | 2134 | 0.106 |
depth of braincase | 915 | 0.038 | |||
greatest length of skull | 1005 | 0.017 | |||
length of braincase | 1041 | 0.023 | |||
length of incisive foramen | 4772 | 0.125 | |||
length of palate plus incisor | 1631 | 0.035 | |||
length from supraorbitals to nasals | 1422 | 0.032 | |||
zygomatic breadth | 1527 | 0.041 | |||
Rattus rattus [31] | Anacapa (Channel Island) | 60 | zygomatic breadth | 1912 | 0.145 |
greatest length of skull | 1892 | 0.060 | |||
interorbital breadth | 1183 | 0.433 | |||
breadth of braincase | 1470 | 0.192 | |||
length of palate plus incisor | 2567 | 0.127 | |||
length of braincase | 1953 | 0.098 | |||
length of incisive foramen | 2074 | 0.323 | |||
depth of braincase | 1084 | 0.209 | |||
summary (of species means) | average rate | 1670 | 0.125 | ||
median rate | 1752 | 0.125 | |||
minimum rate | 768 | 0.052 | |||
maximum rate | 3166 | 0.198 |
3. Results
The proportion of adult mice with white patches of fur increased more than twofold from 2.5% in 2010 to 5.4% in 2016 (z = 4.61, p < 0.0001; figure 2). Head length in 13-day-old mice decreased significantly between 2007 and 2016 (F1,2631 = 195.00, p < 0.0001), as did body weight (F1, 2631 = 78.77, p < 0.0001). Head length relative to body weight also decreased significantly (t = −4.42, p < 0.0001, figure 1). Sex did not significantly influence head length (t = 1.59, p = 0.113). The random effects explained about 45% of the total variance (electronic supplementary material, table S2 and figure S1). Raw data are available as electronic supplementary material, S2 and S3.
Absolute head length decreased by 4017 darwins and 0.499 haldanes. Although the rate in darwins lies in the range of microevolutionary rates reported for some skull measurements in some rodent species (table 1), it is significantly higher (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, V = 0, p = 0.0078). The rate in haldanes exceeds the maximum values reported in the literature by more than twofold (table 1).
4. Discussion
The increase in the occurrence of white fur patches in adults (figure 2) and the decrease in absolute and relative head length and body size in young mice (figure 1) in our study demonstrates that within few generations detectable phenotypic changes can occur that are known from many different domesticated species and are part of the so-called ‘domestication syndrome' [6,7,34,35]. However, we cannot exclude that these phenotypic changes, consistent with selection for tameness, have arisen by other processes.
We hypothesize that our procedures of regular capture and handling of mice and exposure to humans have led to the dispersal of anthropophobic mice from the study population, while more anthropophilic ones have remained and bred (or possibly immigrated from other populations, which would suggest that the influence of the experimental set-up would be even stronger than inferred). If such tolerance towards human disturbance, an aspect of tameness, is heritable, then tolerance will increase over time. Tameness in domesticates is associated with a downregulation of the fear/stress system, specifically the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis and serotonin levels [6]. In laboratory mice, tameness is variable and influenced by genetic background [36,37]. A recent hypothesis [6] suggests that selection for tameness leads to reduced neural crest cell input which in turn gives rise to the ‘domestication syndrome', affecting stress and fight or flight responses as well as shortening the snout and causing white spots. The high evolutionary rates we report suggest that such selection can be strong. However, the link between tameness and white spotting is probably not straightforward given the lack of quantitative trait loci influencing both in rats [38].
Commensalism in M. m. domesticus is probably the result of human development of sedentary ways of life about 15 000 years ago in the Near East [39]. Subsequently, house mice spread towards Europe, following human migration, and reached western Europe (and Switzerland) less than 3000 years ago [40]. The house mouse populations of Switzerland are thus all descended from a lineage that can be considered anthropophilic (or even anthro-dependent [41]) and thus already habituated—to a certain degree—to interactions with humans, a first step on a commensal pathway towards domestication [1,2]. Commensal house mice have been described as an intermediate form between non-commensal wild and laboratory house mice [42]. They have been found to be less agonistic towards conspecifics than non-commensal house mice [43] and to have shorter skulls than non-commensal conspecifics [44,45]—as predicted by the ‘domestication syndrome’ hypothesis [5,6].
Alternative explanations for the observed phenotypic changes include altered environmental conditions [16,35], and inbreeding and genetic drift [34]. In this study population, interspecific competition and predation at nests and feeding sites can be excluded and population density is within the range of natural populations [12,13]. Thus, frequent interaction with humans and reduced predation are likely the most important factors that distinguish the study population from sympatric commensal house mouse populations [16]. The lack of predation may play a role in the occurrence of pigmentation-related domestication characters [46]: mutations leading to white patches of fur occur in the wild as well as under domestication but might be selected against in the wild and also in commensal populations, whereas the relaxed selection on these traits in domestication (and also in the barn) allows them to persist or increase. If reduced head length relative to body size is associated with decreased risk of predation, then predator release could also contribute to this phenotype change, but we know of no evidence for this scenario. Genetic drift and inbreeding are likely to influence genetic variation underlying white spots and reduced head length in house mice, including in the study population. However, the only analysis to date (2003–2008) showed no change in inbreeding level in the study population [14]. Genetic drift is a possible explanation for some or all of the changes in head length, especially for the later increases (figure 1). Furthermore, morphological alterations of the skull due to adaptations to different food items [47] in the study population (see Material and methods) relative to the source populations (probably spillovers from harvest and fodder on farms), cannot be ruled out as a source of the observed decrease in head length, but also cannot explain the reversed trend in later years. Further work is required to rule out these alternative explanations.
5. Conclusion
Previous studies on foxes [7] and rats [8,9] have shown that strong selection for tameness can be associated with the peculiar phenotypic changes that are typical for domesticated animals. Our study shows that unintentional selection for tameness is associated with the same phenotypic changes in a wild house mouse population within few generations.
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgement
We thank Barbara König for access to data, comments and support, and all those who contributed to data collection. Further, we thank anonymous reviewers for useful comments to improve this manuscript.
Ethics
Procedures were approved by the Veterinary Office of the Kanton of Zurich under permits 215/2006, 51/2010, 56/2013 and ZH091/16.
Data accessibility
Data are available as electronic supplementary material, S2 and S3.
Authors' contributions
A.K.L. and M.R.S.-V. conceived the study; A.K.L. and M.G. designed the study and interpreted the data; A.K.L. provided and M.G. analysed the data; M.G. drafted the manuscript and all authors critically revised the manuscript for intellectual content and gave final approval for publication.
Competing interests
The authors have no competing interests.
Funding
This study has been funded by the University of Zurich and Swiss National Science Foundation grants P2ZHP3_171977 to M.G., 31003A_169395 to M.R.S.-V., and 31003A_120444 and 310030M_138389 to A.K.L.
References
- 1.Zeder M. 2012. Pathways to animal domestication. In Biodiversity in agriculture: domestication, evolution, and sustainability (eds Gepts P, Famula TR, Bettinger RL, Brush SB, Damania AB, McGuire PE, Qualset CO), pp. 227–259. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Vigne JD. 2011. The origins of animal domestication and husbandry: a major change in the history of humanity and the biosphere. C. R. Biol. 334, 171–181. (doi:10.1016/j.crvi.2010.12.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Larson G, Fuller D. 2014. The evolution of animal domestication. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. 45, 115–136. (doi:10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110512-135813) [Google Scholar]
- 4.Zeder MA. 2015. Core questions in domestication research. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 3191–3198. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1501711112) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Sánchez-Villagra MR, Geiger M, Schneider RA. 2016. The taming of the neural crest: a developmental perspective on the origins of morphological covariation in domesticated mammals. R. Soc. open sci. 3, 160107 (doi:10.1098/rsos.160107) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Wilkins AS, Wrangham RW, Tecumseh Fitch W. 2014. The ‘domestication syndrome’ in mammals: a unified explanation based on neural crest cell behavior and genetics. Genetics 197, 795–808. (doi:10.1534/genetics.114.165423) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Trut L, Oskina I, Kharlamova A. 2009. Animal evolution during domestication: the domesticated fox as a model. Bioessays 31, 349–360. (doi:10.1002/bies.200800070) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Trut LN, Pliusnina IZ, Prasolova LA. 2000. Mutations hooded and nonagouti in the gray rat Rattus norvegicus: effect of selection on behavior and photoperiod. Russ. J. Genet. 36, 668–676. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Singh N, Albert FW, Plyusnina I, Trut L, Pääbo S, Harvati K. 2017. Facial shape differences between rats selected for tame and aggressive behaviors. PLoS ONE 12, e0175043 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0175043) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kharlamova AV, Faleev VI, Trapezov OV. 2000. Effect of selection for behavior on the cranial traits of the American mink (Mustela vison). Russ. J. Genet. 36, 677–681. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.König B, Lindholm AK. 2012. The complex social environment of female house mice (Mus domesticus). In Evolution of the house mouse (eds Macholán M, Baird SJE, Munlinger P, Piálek J), pp. 114–134. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 12.König B, Lindholm AK, Lopes PC, Dobay A, Steinert S, Buschmann FJ-U. 2015. A system for automatic recording of social behavior in a free-living wild house mouse population. Anim. Biotelem. 3, 39 (doi:10.1186/s40317-015-0069-0) [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bronson FH. 1979. The reproductive ecology of the house mouse. Q. Rev. Biol. 54, 265–299. (doi:10.1086/411295) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Manser A, Lindholm AK, König B, Bagheri HC. 2011. Polyandry and the decrease of a selfish genetic element in a wild house mouse population. Evolution 65, 2435–2447. (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01336.x) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dobay A, Pilo P, Lindholm AK, Origgi F, Bagheri HC, König B, Howarth R, Walker L. 2015. Dynamics of a tularemia outbreak in a closely monitored free-roaming population of wild house mice. PLoS ONE 10, e0141103 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0141103) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Pocock MJO, Searle JB, White PC. 2004. Adaptations of animals to commensal habitats: population dynamics of house mice Mus musculus domesticus on farms. J. Anim. Ecol. 73, 878–888. (doi:10.2307/3505523) [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ogle C. 1934. Climatic influence on the growth of the male albino mouse. Am. J. Physiol. 107, 635–640. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Caumul R, Polly PD. 2005. Phylogenetic and environmental components of morphological variation: skull, mandible, and molar shape in marmots (Marmota, Rodentia). Evolution 59, 2460–2472. (doi:10.1554/05-117.1) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Renaud S, Hardouin EA, Quéré JP, Chevret P. 2017. Morphometric variations at an ecological scale: seasonal and local variations in feral and commensal house mice. Mamm. Biol. 87, 1–12. (doi:10.1016/j.mambio.2017.04.004) [Google Scholar]
- 20.Perony N, Tessone CJ, König B, Schweitzer F. 2012. How random is social behaviour? Disentangling social complexity through the study of a wild house mouse population. PLoS Comput. Biol. 8, e1002786 (doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002786) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lindholm AK, Musolf K, Weidt A, König B. 2013. Mate choice for genetic compatibility in the house mouse. Ecol. Evol. 3, 1231–1247. (doi:10.1002/ece3.534) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Auclair Y, König B, Ferrari M, Perony N, Lindholm AK. 2014. Nest attendance of lactating females in a wild house mouse population: benefits associated with communal nesting. Anim. Behav. 92, 143–149. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.03.008) [Google Scholar]
- 23.Weidt A, Lindholm AK, König B. 2014. Communal nursing in wild house mice is not a by-product of group living: females choose. Naturwissenschaften 101, 73–76. (doi:10.1007/s00114-013-1130-6) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lopes PC, Block P, König B. 2016. Infection-induced behavioural changes reduce connectivity and the potential for disease spread in wild mice contact networks. Sci. Rep. 6, 31790 (doi:10.1038/srep31790) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Haldane JBS. 1949. Suggestions as to quantitative measurement of rates of evolution. Evolution 3, 51–56. (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.1949.tb00004.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gingerich PD. 1993. Quantification and comparison of evolutionary rates. Am. J. Sci. 293, 453–478. (doi:10.2475/ajs.293.A.453) [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hendry AP, Kinnison MT. 1999. Perspective: the pace of modern life: measuring rates of contemporary microevolution. Evolution 53, 1637–1653. (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.1999.tb04550.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Stumpp R, Fuzessy L, Paglia AP. 2016. Environment drivers acting on rodent rapid morphological change. J. Mamm. Evol. 60, 2016, 255 (doi:10.1007/s10914-016-9369-2) [Google Scholar]
- 29.Pergams ORW, Ashley MV. 1999. Rapid morphological change in Channel Island deer mice. Evolution 53, 1573–1581. (doi:10.2307/2640902) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Pergams ORW, Lacy RC. 2007. Rapid morphological and genetic change in Chicago-area Peromyscus. Mol. Ecol. 17, 450–463. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2007.03517.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Pergams ORW, Byrn D, Lee KLY, Jackson R. 2015. Rapid morphological change in black rats (Rattus rattus) after an island introduction. PeerJ 3, e812 (doi:10.7717/peerj.812) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.R Core Team. 2012. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- 33.RStudio. 2012. RStudio: integrated development environment for R.
- 34.Clutton-Brock J. 1999. A natural history of domesticated mammals. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Tchernov E, Horwitz LK. 1991. Body size diminution under domestication: unconscious selection in primeval domesticates. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 10, 54–75. (doi:10.1016/0278-4165(91)90021-O) [Google Scholar]
- 36.Goto T, Tanave A, Moriwaki K, Shiroishi T, Koide T. 2013. Selection for reluctance to avoid humans during the domestication of mice. Genes, Brain Behav. 12, 760–770. (doi:10.1111/gbb.12088) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wahlsten D, Metten P, Crabbe JC. 2003. A rating scale for wildness and ease of handling laboratory mice: results for 21 inbred strains tested in two laboratories. Genes Brain Behav. 2, 71–79. (doi:10.1034/j.1601-183X.2003.00012.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Albert FW, et al. 2009. Genetic architecture of tameness in a rat model of animal domestication. Genetics 182, 541–554. (doi:10.1534/genetics.109.102186) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Weissbrod L, Marshall FB, Valla FR, Khalaily H, Bar-Oz G, Auffray J-C, Vigne J-D, Cucchi T. 2017. Origins of house mice in ecological niches created by settled hunter-gatherers in the Levant 15 000 y ago. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 4099–4104. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1619137114) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Cucchi T, Vigne JD, Auffray JC. 2005. First occurrence of the house mouse (Mus musculus domesticus Schwarz & Schwarz, 1943) in the Western Mediterranean: a zooarchaeological revision of subfossil occurrences. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 84, 429–445. (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2005.00445.x) [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hulme-Beaman A, Dobney K, Cucchi T, Searle JB. 2016. An ecological and evolutionary framework for commensalism in anthropogenic environments. Trends Ecol. Evol. 31, 633–645. (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2016.05.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Frick H, Nord H. 1963. Domestikation und Hirngewicht. Anat. Anz. 113, 307–316. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Frynta D, Slábová M, Váchová H, Volfová R, Munclinger P. 2005. Aggression and commensalism in house mouse: a comparative study across Europe and the Near East. Aggress. Behav. 31, 283–293. (doi:10.1002/ab.15555) [Google Scholar]
- 44.Schwarz E, Schwarz HK. 1943. The wild and commensal stocks of the house mouse, Mus musculus Linnaeus. J. Mammal. 24, 59–72. (doi:10.2307/1374781) [Google Scholar]
- 45.Slábová M, Frynta D. 2007. Morphometric variation in nearly unstudied populations of the most studied mammal: the non-commensal house mouse (Mus musculus domesticus) in the Near East and Northern Africa. Zool. Anz. 246, 91–101. (doi:10.1016/j.jcz.2007.02.003) [Google Scholar]
- 46.Herre W, Röhrs M. 1990. Haustiere — zoologisch gesehen. Stuttgart, Germany: Gustav Fischer Verlag. [Google Scholar]
- 47.O'Regan HJ, Kitchener AC. 2005. The effects of captivity on the morphology of captive, domesticated and feral mammals. Mamm. Rev. 35, 215–230. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2907.2005.00070.x) [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data are available as electronic supplementary material, S2 and S3.