Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Autism. 2017 Nov 3;23(1):100–111. doi: 10.1177/1362361317722030

Race influences parent report of concerns about symptoms of autism spectrum disorder

Meghan Rose Donohue 1, Amber W Childs 2, Megan Richards 3, Diana L Robins 4
PMCID: PMC5930138  NIHMSID: NIHMS926275  PMID: 29100475

Abstract

Racial differences in parent report of concerns about their child’s development to healthcare providers may contribute to delayed autism spectrum disorder diagnoses in Black children. We tested the hypotheses that compared to White parents, Black parents of children with autism spectrum disorder would report fewer concerns about autism symptoms and would be more likely to report concerns about disruptive behaviors. A sample of 18–40 month old toddlers (N = 174) with autism spectrum disorder and their parent participated. After screening positive for autism spectrum disorder risk, but prior to a diagnostic evaluation, parents completed free-response questions soliciting concerns about their child’s development. Parent responses were coded for the presence or absence of 10 possible concerns, which were grouped into autism concerns (e.g., social and restricted and repetitive behavior concerns) or non-autism concerns (e.g., general developmental and disruptive behavior concerns). Compared to White parents, Black parents reported significantly fewer autism concerns and fewer social and restricted and repetitive behavior concerns. However, Black parents did not report significantly fewer non-autism concerns. Race did not influence parent report of disruptive behavior concerns. Lower reporting of autism concerns by Black parents may impact providers’ abilities to identify children in need of further screening or evaluation.

Keywords: Autism, autism spectrum disorder, parent concerns, child development, race and ethnicity, diagnostic disparities

Introduction

Although the majority of parents of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) report concerns about their child’s development during the first two years of the child’s life (Herlihy et al., 2013; Richards et al., 2016; Zuckerman et al., 2015) the median age of diagnosis is after the child’s fourth birthday (CDC, 2016). Early detection of ASD facilitates early intervention, which is associated with better outcomes (Anderson et al., 2014; Linstead et al., 2016; MacDonald et al., 2014; Orinstein et al., 2014; Smith et al., 2015; Vivanti et al., 2016). Despite progress in using standardized screening to identify children at risk for ASD (Pierce et al., 2011; Robins et al., 2014), Black children are diagnosed with ASD at older ages than White children and children of other races (Mandell et al., 2002, 2007, 2009; Valicenti-McDermott et al., 2012). To date, little research has been devoted to elucidating potential reasons for such diagnostic disparity. The purpose of this study is to offer insight into delayed ASD diagnosis for Black children by examining whether concerns that parents communicated to healthcare providers about their child’s development prior to diagnosis differed as a function of race.

Diagnostic disparity in Black children with ASD

In light of the implications of delayed or missed diagnosis, it is crucial to address ASD diagnostic disparity in Black children. Although the ASD phenotype is not thought to differ by race, studies have found that Black children had a lower incidence of documented ASD diagnoses than White children (Kogan et al., 2009; Mandell et al., 2009; Valicenti-McDermott et al., 2012). Compared to White peers, Black children were diagnosed with ASD 1.4 years later (Mandell et al., 2002), required threefold more specialty care visits and spent eight months longer in mental health treatment prior to receiving a diagnosis (Mandell et al., 2007). One study found significantly fewer documented mild-to-moderate ASD diagnoses among Black than White children five years and older, suggesting potential under-identification of older Black children with mild-to-moderate ASD (Jo et al., 2015). Many possible causes of such diagnostic disparity have been proposed, including unequal access to healthcare (Liptak et al., 2008; Montes and Halterman, 2011), clinician prejudice (Institute of Medicine, 2002; Mandell et al., 2002) and parent interpretation and report of symptoms to healthcare providers (Barton, Dumont-Mathieu, et al., 2012b; Mandell et al., 2007).

Mandell and colleagues (2007) found that Black children were nearly twice as likely as children of other races to be misdiagnosed with conduct disorders and oppositional defiant disorders prior to an ASD diagnosis. They posited that Black parents may relate concerns about their child’s development to healthcare providers in ways that deemphasize ASD symptoms and emphasize disruptive behaviors, which may sway clinicians from considering ASD and other diagnoses. An important next step is to directly test the hypotheses that prior to receiving an ASD diagnosis, Black parents report fewer ASD symptom specific concerns about their child than White parents and that Black parents are more likely to report concerns about disruptive behaviors.

Parent concerns about child development

Parent report of concerns about their child’s behaviors and development to healthcare providers often initiates referrals for evaluation of ASD and other developmental delays. While the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) recommends ASD-specific screening during well-child visits at 18 and 24 months (Johnson and Myers, 2007), ASD screening is not widely utilized (Sices, 2007) to initiate further evaluation of a child. Indeed, recent estimates of compliance rates for ASD-specific screenings were only 42.9% (Zuckerman et al., 2013) and 55% (Daniels and Mandell, 2013). Thus, caregivers’ reports of concerns to providers, including communicating about delayed or otherwise non-typical behaviors, is critical as these voiced concerns likely trigger referrals.

Studies have found that parent concerns accurately reflect delays and predict diagnoses. For instance, greater parent concerns about their child’s development have been found to be related to lower scores on cognitive and language measures (McMahon et al., 2007) and higher scores on ASD severity measures (Richards et al., 2016) in toddlers at-risk for ASD. In this same study, Richards and colleagues (2016) found that although parents of children with and without an ASD diagnosis did not differ in the mean number of concerns about their child’s development prior to their child being evaluated, parents of children diagnosed with ASD were more likely to report concerns specific to ASD criteria than parents of children without ASD, demonstrating specificity in parent concerns. By the time a child is 12 months old, parent concerns about ASD symptoms are predictive of later ASD diagnosis (Ozonoff et al., 2009). Given the demonstrated utility of parent concerns, it is important to examine how parents’ characteristics may influence the extent and/or content of their reported concerns. For example, parents of lower socioeconomic status (SES) reported fewer concerns about their child’s development than parents of higher SES (Sun et al., 2014), and parents of boys reported concerns about their child’s development later than parents of girls (Horovitz et al., 2012).

Racial and ethnic differences in parent concerns

Parent race/ethnicity is one understudied factor that may influence the extent or content of parent report of concerns about their child’s development. The only known study examining differences in concerns among Black and White parents of toddlers with ASD (Jang et al., 2014), found no effect of race on the age at which parents reported that they first became concerned about their child’s development. Yet, race and ethnicity may be more relevant to the extent and content of parent concerns. For example, parents of preschool children in Singapore reported significant concerns about their child’s development at much higher rates than American and Australian parents (Kiing et al., 2012), suggesting potentially different thresholds for perceiving and reporting concerns among different cultures. Similarly, Hispanic mothers reported significantly fewer concerns specific to ASD criteria than non-Hispanic mothers, despite the fact that ultimately, Hispanic children diagnosed with ASD demonstrated greater ASD severity than the non-Hispanic children diagnosed with ASD (Blacher et al., 2014). Moreover, when faced with similar symptom presentations in children with ASD, parents from distinct cultural backgrounds may emphasize different symptoms depending on the cultural importance of a particular delayed skill (Mandell and Novak, 2005). For example, in studies of parents of children with ASD, American and Japanese parents reported that they were first concerned about delayed speech (Giacomo and Fombonne, 1998; Ohta et al., 1987), whereas urban Indian parents reported that they were primarily concerned about their child’s impaired social functioning (Daley, 2004), which may reflect the emphasis placed on social relatedness and conformity in traditional Indian culture.

Studies finding differences in parent report and perceptions of Attention- Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) between Black and White parents may offer additional insight into differences in concerns among Black and White parents of children with ASD. One study found that Black parents of children at-risk for ADHD were less likely to endorse several specific symptoms than parents of White children with comparable levels of hyperactivity (Hillemeier et al., 2007). Fewer reported ADHD symptoms by Black parents may in part reflect less parental knowledge about the disorder; indeed, one study found that when compared to White parents, Black parents reported that they had less knowledge about ADHD (Bussing et al., 2007, 2012). Moreover, when attributing the cause of their child’s ADHD symptoms, Black parents were four times more likely to label their child as “bad” and three times less likely to use a genetic label for the symptoms than White parents (Bussing et al., 1998). Similarly, parents of Black girls with ADHD were particularly likely to characterize their child as a “misbehaving child” (Bussing et al., 2005).

Taken together, this research suggests potential underreporting of ADHD symptoms and interpretations of children’s ADHD symptoms as disruptive behavior among Black parents; this phenomenon may extend to other neurodevelopmental disorders, such as ASD. In a qualitative study, Black families of children with ASD reported a belief that there were gaps in knowledge about ASD in the Black community (Burkett et al., 2015), which may lead to under-reporting ASD concerns to healthcare providers. Moreover, Black parents may emphasize children’s disruptive behaviors to healthcare providers and/or misinterpret children’s ASD symptoms as indicators of disruptive behavior problems. Indeed, disruptive behaviors may be particularly salient to Black parents. Black parents have been found to endorse stricter discipline practices (e.g., non-abusive physical punishment; Ibanez et al., 2006; Ispa and Halgunseth, 2004; Kelley et al., 1992), which may reflect efforts to respond to less neighborhood safety (Barajas-Gonzalez and Brooks-Gunn, 2014) and/or protect children from experiences of racism, prejudice and discrimination (Thomas and Dettlaff, 2011). Like ADHD, ASD symptoms may be easily interpreted as “bad behavior.” For example, a child may not comply with a parent’s instructions or directives due to delayed receptive language, lack of concern for pleasing the parent reflecting social deficits, or the interference of restricted and repetitive behaviors.

Study hypotheses

In summary, although missed and delayed diagnoses are well documented in Black children with ASD, less is known about factors that may contribute to diagnostic disparities such as parent report of concerns about their child’s development. The purpose of the present study was to investigate racial differences in parent report of concerns about their child’s behavior to healthcare providers before their child received an ASD diagnosis. Prior to children’s participation in a diagnostic evaluation – but after showing risk on an ASD screening tool – parents answered free-response questions in which they indicated concerns about their child’s behavior and development. Researchers coded parent responses into categories of concerns specific to ASD diagnostic criteria as well as other, non-ASD concerns. The hypotheses were that compared to White parents, Black parents would 1) report fewer ASD concerns, but not non-ASD concerns; 2) be less likely than White parents to report a concern within each individual category of ASD concerns (speech/communication, restricted and repetitive behavior, social deficits and directly naming autism/ASD as a concern); and 3) be more likely to report a disruptive behavior concern.

Method

Participants

Participants were from two multi-site studies validating the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (-Revised), with Follow-Up (M-CHAT(-R)/F; Chlebowski et al., 2013; Robins et al., 2014). Parents completed the M-CHAT(-R; Robins et al., 1999, 2009) during toddler well-child visits at participating pediatricians’ offices. Consistent with the larger studies, participants from both study sites (metro-Atlanta, Connecticut) were examined. Researchers called families of children who screened positive for risk of ASD and completed the M-CHAT(-R) Follow-Up to obtain additional information regarding the at-risk responses on the initial questionnaire. If the child screened positive on the Follow-Up, the family was offered a free diagnostic evaluation (3.24% of the screened sample). Parents were told that their responses on the screener indicated risk for developmental delay in their child and that an evaluation would be beneficial; ASD was not explicitly mentioned. The majority of children who screened positive (62.2%) completed the evaluation. In the larger study, families with less maternal education were less likely to complete the evaluation after screening positive; race did not significantly predict evaluation completion (Khowaja et al., 2015). Parents completed a history questionnaire eliciting parent concerns about their child’s development prior to the evaluation.

Children were excluded from the larger studies if they: 1) had an ASD diagnosis prior to screening or 2) had sensory or motor disabilities that prevented completion of the evaluation. Criteria for inclusion in the current study were: 1) diagnosed with ASD, 2) completion of the history questionnaire, in English, by the child’s parent, 3) the parent respondent of the history questionnaire self-reported a race of either White or Black, and 4) the parent respondent self-reported their level of education.

The current sample included 174 toddlers (Autistic Disorder n = 98, PDD-NOS n = 76) according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text rev.; DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) and their parent who completed the history form. The sample is a subset of that reported in Richards and colleagues (2016) and was used in the current study to answer a novel question regarding racial differences in parent concerns; see Table 1 for demographics. Regarding parents, 116 self-identified as non-Hispanic White (“White,” 66.7%), whereas 58 self-identified as non-Hispanic Black (“Black,” 33.3%). Parents identified their child’s race; 106 were identified as White (60.9%), 57 as Black (32.8%), nine as Biracial (4.3%), one as Asian (.6%) and one parent did not report the child’s race (.6%).

Table 1.

Demographic Variables

Percent or Mean (SD)

N = 174 n = 105 n = 69
Overall Sample Atlanta Site Connecticut Site

Child Variables
 Sex (% male) 71.8% 70.5%a 73.9%a
 Sex (% female) 28.2% 29.5%a 26.1%a
 Age (months) 25.77 (4.77) 26.24 (4.64)a 25.06 (4.93)a
 Race (% White) 60.9% 47.6%a 81.2%b
 Race (% Black) 39.1% 44.8%a 14.5%b
Parent Variables
 Sex (% mothers) 93.7% 92.4%a 95.7%a
 Sex (% fathers) 6.3% 7.6%a 4.3%a
 Education (years1) 14.25 (2.49) 14.69 (2.60)a 13.59 (2.18)b
 Race (% White) 66.7% 54.3%a 82.6%b
 Race (% Black) 33.3% 44.8%a 13.0%b

Note. Means/percentages in each row with differing superscripts are significantly different from each other (p = <.05).

1

Self-reported education level was converted to years of education, capped at 18 years to indicate graduate degree (some high school = 10, high school degree = 12, some college = 14, college degree = 16, graduate degree = 18).

Diagnosis

The evaluation measured cognitive, adaptive, motor and language functioning, and ASD symptomatology. Measures included the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL; Mullen, 1995), the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales-II (VABS-II; Sparrow and Cicchetti, 1985; Sparrow et al., 2005), Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS(-2); Lord et al., 1999, 2012), the Toddler Autism Symptom Interview (TASI; Barton, Boorstein, Herlihy, Dumont-Mathieu, & Fein, 2012a) and the Childhood Autism Rating Scales(-2) (CARS(-2); Schopler et al., 1980, 2010). A licensed clinician made the diagnosis, integrating all available information using DSM-IV criteria for any ASD.

Measures

History questionnaire

Parents completed a history questionnaire with multiple-choice and free-response questions related to demographics, family history, pre- and perinatal history, medical history, developmental milestones and behavioral details and parent concerns (Herlihy et al., 2013; Richards et al., 2016). Parents’ free responses to three items that probed for concerns were coded for the current study: ‘Please briefly list any concerns/worries you have about how the child has been developing (walking, speaking, playing) or behaving,’ ‘Concerns about your child’s development’ and ‘What were the first things that made you concerned about your child’s development?’

Coding of parent concerns

The coding scheme, reported in Richards and colleagues (2016) and adapted from Ozonoff and colleagues (2009), classified responses to the three concerns questions from the history questionnaire into 10 different concern categories (Table 2). The current study’s concern categories differed from those used by Richards and colleagues (2016) in that a disruptive behavior concern category was separated from other behavioral/temperament concerns. The disruptive behavior category reflected conduct and oppositional symptoms that are developmentally relevant to toddlers such as aggression, anger, and noncompliance to discipline.

Table 2.

Coded Categories of Parent Concerns

Concern Description Example
ASD concerns
Speech/Communication Related to production and comprehension of speech; gestures; reciprocity “Doesn’t speak”
Restricted and repetitive behavior Restricted and repetitive behaviors in sensory, motor, or object use “Flaps her arms”
Social Social interest; social attention; play; reciprocity “Doesn’t interact with other children”
Autism Label Directly named autism “Worried about ASD”
Non-ASD concerns
Motor Motor difficulties; clumsiness “Balance, frequent falls”
Behavior/Temperament Related to temperament (inattention, shyness); related to behavior (activity level, self- injurious behavior) “Has a short attention span”
Medical/Regulatory Medical condition; related to a regulatory behavior (sleeping, toileting) “Not sleeping through the night”
General Developmental General development concerns “Worried about general development”
Feeding/Eating Related to feeding/eating “Picky eater”
Disruptive Behavior Tantrums, aggression, anger, non- compliance “Aggressive toward other children”

The 10 individual concern categories were grouped into two broad concern categories for analysis based on previous research (Ozonoff et al., 2009; Richards et al., 2016). The ASD concerns category was composed of the sum of concerns specific to DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for ASD: speech/communication, social, restricted and repetitive behavior (RRB) and the autism label (i.e., the parent directly named “autism/ASD” as a concern) categories; (range: 0–4). The Non-ASD concerns category was composed of the sum of concerns falling in the remaining categories: motor (excluding repetitive motor behaviors), behavioral/temperament, medical/regulatory, general developmental, feeding/eating and disruptive behavior concerns (range: 0–6). If the parent described more than one type of concern, all relevant categories were coded.

Coding was completed by two trained researchers who were blind to child diagnosis and demographic characteristics. Cohen’s Kappa values of .70 or higher were achieved for all concern categories. Discrepancies were resolved by consulting with a team of experts and joint consensus was used.

ADOS Calibrated Severity Scores

Calibrated severity scores (ADOS-CSS) were calculated to compare ASD symptoms across ADOS modules (Gotham et al., 2009; Hus et al., 2014). These variables were used as covariates in the tests of hypotheses to control for possibility that racial differences in parent concerns about symptoms of ASD are due to racial differences in symptom severity in our sample. The current study uses the overall CSS (Overall-CSS), the CSS for the Social Affect domain (SA-CSS) and the CSS for the RRB domain (RRB-CSS). Higher scores indicate greater severity (range: 1–10).

Procedure

All parents provided informed consent. Prior to the evaluation (but after learning that the child was at risk for developmental delay based on screening), parents completed the history questionnaire. Parents and children then completed the diagnostic evaluation. Institutional Review Boards at Georgia State University and the University of Connecticut (H05185, H12139) approved all data collection procedures.

Data Analytic Plan

Two separate Analyses of Covariance (ANCOVA) were conducted to examine the effect of parent race on both ASD concerns and Non-ASD concerns. Five logistic regression analyses were conducted to examine the effect of race on each of the four individual categories of ASD concerns and the disruptive behavior concerns category.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Overall, White parents (M = 14.55, SD = 2.22) had more years of education than Black parents (M = 13.66, SD = 2.91), t(91.16) = 2.07, p = .04, η2 = .04. Higher numbers of Black parents, χ2(1) = 18.27, p < .001, and Black children, χ2(1) = 21.95, p < .001, came from the Atlanta versus the Connecticut site, and Atlanta parents had more years of education (M = 14.69, SD = 2.60) than Connecticut parents (M = 13.59, SD = 2.18), t(161.79) = 2.99, p < .001, η2 = .05.

Correlations and t-tests between MSEL and Vineland standard scores and study variables were examined, considering the higher prevalence of ASD with intellectual disability (ID) in Black than White children (CDC, 2016). Lower MSEL scores were associated with lower Vineland scores, r = .65, p < .001. Black children had lower MSEL scores (M = 55.28, SD = 8.23) than White children (M = 63.85, SD = 14.42), t(147.95) = −4.64, p < .001, η2 = .05. Black children also had lower Vineland scores (M = 73.89, SD = 9.37) than White children (M = 80.48, SD = 10.48), t(95) = −3.26, p = .002, η2 = .05. Lower SES was associated with lower MSEL r = .19, p = .02 and Vineland scores, r = .27, p = .01. Importantly, neither MSEL nor Vineland scores were related to any outcome variable (i.e., either broad concern category or any specific concern category), suggesting that they do not need to be included as covariates in tests of the hypotheses. Further, researchers have cautioned against using variables such as IQ as covariates when they are attributes of a particular neurodevelopmental disorder and therefore cannot be separated from the disorder’s effects (Dennis et al., 2009).

Correlations between ADOS-CSS scores and parent and child demographic variables indicated that fewer years of parental education were associated with higher Overall-CSS and SA-CSS, but not RRB-CSS (Table 3). Older children had greater severity scores on all three CSS variables than younger children. Lower MSEL and Vineland scores were related to greater severity scores on all three CSS variables. Neither the overall-CSS nor the RRB-CSS differed significantly between children of Black and White parents. However, children of Black parents (M = 6.89, SD = 1.94) had significantly greater SA-CSS than children of White parents (M = 6.24, SD = 1.73), t(160) = 2.21, p = .03, η2 = .03.

Table 3.

Correlations among Continuous Variables

Variables 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9.
1. Child age
2. Parent respondent’s education -.02
3. MSEL composite score −.06 .19*
4. VABS-II composite score −.17 .27** .65**
5. ADOS CSS severity score .27** −.16* −.33** −.27**
6. ADOS SA severity score .19* −.16* −.30** −.25** .90**
7. ADOS RRB severity score .25** −.06 −.29** −.24* .69** .44**
8. Total ASD Concerns .02 .17* .02 .07 .02 −.01 .10
9. Total Non-ASD Concerns .04 −.02 .10 −.17 −.10 −.08 −.02 .06
10. Total Concerns .04 .09 .09 −.08 −.06 −.07 .05 .68** .77**

Notes.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01.

MSEL = Mullen Scales of Early Learning; VABS-II = Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales-II; ADOS = Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; CSS = Calibrated Severity Score; SA = Social affect; RRB = Restricted, repetitive behavior.

We also tested whether child and parent demographic variables or study site were related to outcome variables. More parental years of education were significantly and positively correlated with more ASD concerns, r = .17, p = .03. Connecticut parents (M = 1.12, SD = .95) reported significantly more Non-ASD concerns than Atlanta parents (M = .80, SD = .85), t(172)= −2.29, p = .02, η2 = .03. A greater proportion of parents of boys than girls reported an autism label concern, χ2(1) = 4.16, p = .04. A greater proportion of parents from Connecticut than Atlanta reported a disruptive behavior concern, χ2(1) = 4.94, p = .03. As a result of these analyses, child sex was included as a covariate in analyses of the autism label concern variable, and study site was included as a covariate in analyses of the Non-ASD concerns and disruptive behaviors concerns variables. Education of the parent respondent, a common proxy for SES, was included as a covariate in all analyses. Finally, the Overall-CSS was included as a covariate in analyses of the ASD concerns and Non-ASD concerns variables, the SA-CSS was included as a covariate in the analysis of the social concerns variable and the RRB-CSS was included as a covariate in the analysis of the RRB concerns variable.

White parents (M = 2.52, SD = 1.32) endorsed a greater number of distinct categories of concerns about their children’s development (i.e., the sum of all 10 individual concern categories) than Black parents (M = 1.97, SD = .92), t(153.87) = −3.27, p < .001, η2 = .06. Across parents, concerns about speech/communication were the most common concern (Table 4), followed by motor and social concerns. The pattern was similar when examining parent concerns by race. For White parents, speech concerns were the most common, followed by social and motor concerns. For Black parents, speech concerns were the most common, followed by motor concerns as second most common and social and medical concerns as third most common.

Table 4.

Descriptive Statistics of CSS Severity and Concern Variables: Frequencies, Means and Standard Deviations

Percent or Mean (SD)

N = 174 n = 58 n = 116
Overall Sample Black White

MSEL Standard Score 60.83 (13.53) 55.28 (8.23) 63.85 (14.42)
VABS Standard Score 77.42 (10.42) 73.89 (9.37) 80.48 (10.48)
ADOS CSS Severity Score 6.38 (2.02) 6.60 (2.07) 6.27 (2.00)
ADOS SA Severity Score 6.49 (1.79) 6.95 (1.86) 6.26 (1.72)
ADOS RRB Severity Score 6.54 (2.45) 6.37 (2.46) 6.63 (2.44)
ASD concerns (% yes)
 Speech/communication 87.4% 82.8% 89.7%
 RRB 19.0% 6.9% 25.0%
 Social 29.3% 17.2% 35.3%
 Autism label 5.7% 5.2% 6.0%
Total ASD concernsa 1.41 (.77) 1.12 (.70) 1.56 (.77)
Non-ASD concerns (% yes)
 Motor 31.0% 31.0% 31.0%
 Behavior/temperament 13.2% 5.2% 17.2%
 Medical/regulatory 12.6% 17.2% 10.3%
 General developmental 12.1% 10.3% 11.9%
 Feeding/eating 11.5% 12.1% 11.2%
 Disruptive behavior 12.1% 8.6% 13.8%
Total Non-ASD concernsb .93 (.90) .84 (.77) .97 (.96)

Note. RRB = Restricted, repetitive behavior

a

Sum of ASD concern categories (range = 0–4)

b

Sum of Non-ASD Concern categories (range = 0–6)

Tests of study hypotheses

ASD concerns

There was a statistically significant effect of race on the total number of ASD concerns, F(1,167) = 10.04, p < .001, partial η2 = .06. Black parents had significantly fewer ASD concerns than White parents (Mdiff = .42, SE = .13).

Non-ASD concerns

There was not a statistically significant effect of race on the number of Non-ASD concerns, F(1,167) = 1.19, p = .28, partial η2 = .007.

Individual categories of ASD concerns

Figure 1 displays the percent endorsement of each individual category of ASD concerns by parent race. Race significantly affected the likelihood that parents would report a social concern, χ2(3) = 8.217, p = .04 (Table 5). White parents had 2.61 times (95% confidence interval (CI) [1.17, 5.831]) the odds of reporting a social concern than Black parents. Race also significantly affected the likelihood that parents would report a RRB concern, χ2(3) = 11.35, p = .01 (Table 6). White parents had 4.12 times (95% CI [1.36, 12.46]) the odds of reporting a RRB concern than Black parents. Race did not significantly predict the likelihood that parents would report a speech/communication (χ2(2) = 1.61, p = .45) or autism label (χ2(2) = 1.59, p = .45) concern.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Percent Endorsement of ASD-Specific Concerns by Race.

Notes. *p = < .05 RRB = Restricted and repetitive behavior

Table 5.

Logistic Regression Predicting Likelihood of Reporting Social Concerns Based on Parent Race, Parent Education, and SA-CSS

B SE Wald df p Odds Ratio 95% CI for Odds Ratio
Lower Upper
Parent education .08 .07 1.28 1 .26 1.09 .94 1.25
SA Severity .10 .10 1.01 1 .32 1.11 .91 1.34
Parent race .96 .41 5.50 1 .02 2.61 1.17 5.83
Constant −3.39 1.37 6.15 1 .01 .03

Note: Race is for White parents compared to Black parents.

Table 6.

Logistic Regression Predicting Likelihood of Reporting RRB Concerns Based on Parent Race, Parent Education, and RRB-CSS

B SE Wald df p Odds Ratio 95% CI for Odds Ratio
Lower Upper
Parent education .13 .09 2.12 1 .15 1.14 .96 1.18
RRB Severity .01 .08 .003 1 .95 1.01 .85 1.18
Parent race 1.42 .57 6.29 1 .01 4.12 1.36 12.46
Constant −4.43 1.52 8.47 1 .004 .01

Note: Race is for White parents compared to Black parents.

Disruptive behavior concerns

Race did not significantly affect the likelihood that parents would report a disruptive behavior concern, χ2(3) = 5.02, p = .17.

Discussion

This study found racial differences in the concerns that parents reported to healthcare providers about children’s behaviors before children received an ASD diagnosis. Black parents reported fewer concerns about their child’s ASD-specific behaviors than White parents. Specifically, Black parents were less likely than White parents to report concerns about two ASD symptoms – social and RRB – in their child with ASD. Importantly, these differences cannot be explained by lower ASD symptom severity in Black children. In fact, Black parents were less likely to report a social concern than White parents even though their children’s social affect symptoms were significantly more severe than the children of White parents. Consistent with findings of higher prevalence of ASD with ID in Black than White children (CDC, 2016), Black children had lower scores on measures of cognitive and adaptive functioning; however, neither measure was related to parent concerns, suggesting that racial differences in parent concerns cannot be explained by perceived or actual deficits in children’s cognitive and adaptive abilities. As expected, race did not impact the number of non-ASD concerns that parents reported, suggesting that the effect was specific to concerns about ASD symptoms. Contrary to expectations, there were no racial differences in parents’ disruptive behavior concerns.

Consistent with previous research (Giacomo and Fombonne, 1998; Hess and Landa, 2012; Ozonoff et al., 2009), concerns about speech/communication were most common across both Black and White parents. Social difficulties, typically the second most common parent concern (Hess and Landa, 2012; Young et al., 2003), were slightly less common than motor concerns in our sample. White and Black parents differed in their second and third most commonly reported concerns, suggesting some racial differences in the most frequently reported parent concerns about children’s development. These differences appear primarily driven by differences in social concerns; 35.3% of White parents reported social concerns compared to 17.2% of Black parents.

Previous studies have demonstrated decreased help-seeking patterns (e.g., seeking services; amount of information shared with providers) in Black individuals, even after controlling for insurance and SES (Dobalian and Rivers, 2008; McMiller and Weisz, 1996). In a qualitative study, Black families of children with ASD described incidents in which they believed that providers did not listen or take action in response to their concerns about their child’s development (Burkett et al., 2015); these experiences may contribute to families reporting fewer concerns to providers. However, in the current sample, there were no racial differences in the number of Non-ASD concerns reported by parents, suggesting that underreporting is specific to ASD symptoms among Black parents of children with ASD and does not simply reflect reduced help-seeking or distrust of healthcare providers. One factor that may explain report of fewer ASD concerns by Black parents of children diagnosed with ASD is less knowledge of the disorder, perhaps particularly less knowledge about social and RRB symptoms. Indeed, some Black parents of children with ASD have expressed the belief that they have less knowledge of ASD and access to information about ASD than White parents (Burkett et al., 2015). In the same study, Black parents of children with ASD reported that they rarely reached out to other families affected by ASD and knew few other Black families with a child with ASD. Thus, cultural norms surrounding discussion of ASD within Black communities may also impact parents’ knowledge about the disorder. Parents of different cultural backgrounds may also have different expectations or interpretations of a child’s behavior, which may affect their report of concerns. For instance, there may be differences between White and Black parents in perceptions of whether ASD-specific behaviors indicate serious delays and are important to disclose to providers; indeed, thresholds of determining whether certain behaviors are problematic vary as a function of culture (Weisz and Weiss, 1991; Weisz et al., 1988).

Most critically, the findings have important clinical implications. While the use of screening tools has improved (Arunyanart et al., 2012), pediatricians still rely heavily on parent report of concerns to trigger referrals for evaluations. Parent under-reporting of concerns specific to ASD symptoms may contribute to providers missing symptoms of ASD and may thus influence their decisions about whether to administer ASD-specific screeners and/or make referrals for diagnostic evaluations. Reduced reporting of children’s ASD symptoms by Black parents may contribute to providers disproportionately missing symptoms in Black children, and thus may contribute to missed or delayed diagnosis. Lesser reporting of ASD concerns by Black parents may be particularly detrimental in combination with clinician factors such as prejudices and statistical discrimination that likely also contribute to diagnostic disparities in Black children (Institute of Medicine, 2002). Reassuring and/or passive provider responses to parent concerns (e.g., stating that nothing was wrong or that the child might “grow out of it”) may also contribute to delays between a parent’s first conversation with the provider and the child’s ASD diagnosis (Zuckerman et al., 2015). Future studies examining potential differences in the extent of passive provider responses based on the child’s race may provide additional valuable insight into diagnostic disparity for Black children with ASD.

In the current study, Black parents were less likely than White parents to report concerns about their child’s social deficits and RRBs. These symptoms are more specific features of ASD in very young children than speech/communication concerns, which are also characteristic of other early childhood disorders such as global developmental delay and developmental language delay (Ventola et al., 2007). Thus, not reporting concerns about these particular symptoms may be especially problematic in that it may hinder providers from adequately considering ASD in Black children. The current findings may inform recommendations to pediatricians aimed at garnering crucial information from parents including culturally sensitive interviewing techniques and underscore the importance of increasing the use of ASD-specific screening instruments that explicitly probe for ASD symptoms and rely less heavily on parents’ knowledge of problematic behaviors.

It has been theorized that Black parents may emphasize children’s disruptive behaviors to healthcare providers (Mandell et al., 2007), which may contribute to the over diagnosis of disruptive behavior disorders in Black children with ASD. There was not a racial difference in parent report of concerns about their children’s disruptive behaviors in our sample; in contrast, disruptive behaviors were reported by parents of children with ASD regardless of racial identity and at rates similar to report of other Non-ASD symptoms. The study by Mandell and colleagues (2007) examined older children (mean 6.7 years at their first specialty provider visit); future research should examine whether children’s disruptive behaviors become increasingly salient to, and reported by Black parents, with children’s increasing age. Although determining reasons for over-diagnosis of conduct disorder in Black children with ASD is beyond the scope of this study, current study findings suggest that future research should also investigate factors other than parent report of symptoms, such as clinicians’ biased interpretation of symptoms as disruptive behavior, in Black children with ASD.

Limitations and Future Research

One limitation is that the sample was highly educated and the majority of parents had completed college. Future research should increase participation of participants with fewer years of formal education, perhaps through oversampling from community clinics, to increase the generalizability of the findings. Researchers should investigate whether Black parents’ report of fewer ASD concerns than White parents is related to limited access to knowledge about the disorder, particularly the social deficits and RRB that are characteristic of ASD. Evidence of less knowledge of social and RRB symptoms could inform interventions aimed to increase Black parents’ knowledge of warning signs of ASD, such as the Autism Speaks “Maybe” ad campaign (Autism Speaks, 2013). Such findings may also inform interventions to help parents articulate concerns about such symptoms to healthcare providers. Research should also investigate whether potentially different thresholds of perceiving ASD concerns exist between White and Black parents. Finally, studies should examine whether Black parents’ report of fewer ASD concerns is predictive of children’s misdiagnosis or delayed diagnosis. In spite of limitations, the current study provides evidence for the need to account for racial differences that impact the diagnostic process for children and provides a framework for novel investigations of factors that account for diagnostic disparity in Black children with ASD.

Acknowledgments

We would like to give special thanks to Diana Thao for help coding history questionnaire responses, Deborah Fein for support conducting this secondary analysis, and Kiauhna Haynes for extraction of the data set. We also thank all of the children, parents, and physicians that participated in the study. We thank the Ronald E. McNair Post Baccalaureate Achievement Program’s Summer Research Program for supporting Amber Childs’ effort on this study. Finally, we are thankful to the Early Detection and Intervention team at the A.J. Drexel Autism Institute for their support during this project. Finally, we thank our funding sources.

Funding This work was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [R01HD039961]; the Maternal and Child Health Bureau [R40MC00270]; a U.S. Department of Education Student-Initiated Research Grant; a University of Connecticut’s Research Foundation Faculty Grant; the National Alliance of Autism Research; and a National Institute of Mental Health Predoctoral Fellowship [F31MH12550].

Footnotes

Declaration of conflicting interests

Diana Robins is co-owner of M-CHAT LLC, which licenses use of the M-CHAT in electronic products. However, data used in the current study were collected using the free version of the M-CHAT and no royalties are associated with this study.

Contributor Information

Meghan Rose Donohue, Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, GA, USA.

Amber W Childs, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, CT, USA.

Megan Richards, Department of Epidemiology, Drexel University, PA, USA; Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Indiana University, IN, USA.

Diana L Robins, Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, GA, USA; AJ Drexel Autism Institute, Drexel University, PA, USA.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. Washington, DC: Author; 2000. text rev. [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson DK, Liang JW, Lord C. Predicting young adult outcome among more and less cognitively able individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2014;55(5):485–494. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arunyanart W, Fenick A, Ukritchon S, et al. Developmental and autism ccreening. Infants & Young Children. 2012;25(3):175–187. [Google Scholar]
  4. Autism Speaks. Autism Speaks launches new ad council campaign – ‘Maybe’. 2013 Available from: www.autismspeaks.org/news/news-item/autism-speaks-ad-council-campaign-maybe-signs.
  5. Barajas-Gonzalez RG, Brooks-Gunn J. Income, neighborhood stressors, and harsh parenting: Test of moderation by ethnicity, age, and gender. Journal of Family Psychology. 2014;28(6):855–866. doi: 10.1037/a0038242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barton ML, Boorstein H, Herlihy L, et al. Toddler ASD symptom interview. Self-published 2012a [Google Scholar]
  7. Barton ML, Dumont-Mathieu T, Fein D. Screening young children for autism spectrum disorders in primary practice. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2012b;42(6):1165–1174. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1343-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blacher J, Cohen SR, Azad G. In the eye of the beholder: Reports of autism symptoms by Anglo and Latino mothers. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Elsevier Ltd. 2014;8(12):1648–1656. [Google Scholar]
  9. Burkett K, Morris E, Manning-Courtney P, et al. African American families on autism diagnosis and treatment: The influence of culture. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Springer US. 2015;45(10):3244–3254. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2482-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bussing R, Schoenberg NE, Perwien AR. Knowledge and information about ADHD: Evidence of cultural differences among African-American and white parents. Social Science and Medicine. 1998;46(7):919–928. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(97)00219-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bussing R, Koro-Ljungberg ME, Gary F, et al. Exploring help-seeking for ADHD symptoms: a mixed-methods approach. Harvard review of psychiatry. 2005;13(2):85–101. doi: 10.1080/10673220590956465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bussing R, Gary FA, Mills TL, et al. Cultural variations in parental health beliefs, knowledge, and information sources related to Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Family Issues. 2007;28(3):291–318. [Google Scholar]
  13. Bussing R, Zima BT, Mason DM, et al. Journal of Adolescent Health. 6. Vol. 51. Elsevier Inc; 2012. ADHD knowledge, perceptions, and information sources: Perspectives from a community sample of adolescents and their parents; pp. 593–600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. CDC. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network. Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, Un. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2016;65(3):1–23. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6503a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chlebowski C, Robins DL, Barton ML, et al. Large-scale use of the Modified Checklist for Autism in low-risk toddlers. Pediatrics. 2013;131(4):e1121–e1127. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-1525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Daley TC. From symptom recognition to diagnosis: Children with autism in urban India. Social Science and Medicine. 2004;58(7):1323–1335. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00330-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Daniels AM, Mandell DS. Children’s compliance with american academy of pediatrics' well-child care visit guidelines and the early detection of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2013;43(12):2844–2854. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1831-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dennis M, Francis D, Cirino P, et al. Why IQ is not a covatiate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society. 2009;15(3):331–343. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dobalian A, Rivers Pa. Racial and ethnic disparities in the use of mental health services. The Journal of Behavioral Health Services & Research. 2008;35(2):128–141. doi: 10.1007/s11414-007-9097-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. De Giacomo A, Fombonne E. Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. 1998;136:131–137. doi: 10.1007/s007870050058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gotham K, Pickles A, Lord C. Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(5):693–705. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0674-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Herlihy L, Knoch K, Vibert B, et al. Parents’ first concerns about toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: Effect of sibling status. Autism. 2013;19(1):20–28. doi: 10.1177/1362361313509731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hess CR, Landa RJ. Predictive and concurrent validity of parent concern about young children at risk for autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2012;42(4):575–584. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1282-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hillemeier MM, Foster EM, Heinrichs B, et al. Racial differences in parental reports of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder behaviors. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2007;28(5):353–361. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31811ff8b8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Horovitz M, Matson JL, Turygin N, et al. The relationship between gender and age of first concern in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Elsevier Ltd. 2012;6(1):466–471. [Google Scholar]
  26. Hus V, Gotham K, Lord C. Standardizing ADOS domain scores: Separating severity of social affect and restricted and repetitive behaviors. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2014;44(10):2400–2412. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1719-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ibanez ES, Borrego J, Pemberton JR, et al. Cultural factors in decision-making about child physical abuse: Identifying reporter characteristics influencing reporting tendencies. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2006;30(12):1365–1379. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Institute of Medicine. Unequal Treatment. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  29. Ispa JM, Halgunseth LC. Talking about corporal punishment: Nine low-income African American mothers’ perspectives. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2004;19(3):463–484. [Google Scholar]
  30. Jang J, Matson JL, Cervantes PE, et al. The relationship between ethnicity and age of first concern in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Elsevier Ltd. 2014;8(7):925–932. [Google Scholar]
  31. Jo H, Schieve La, Rice CE, et al. Age at autism spectrum disorder (ASD) diagnosis by race, ethnicity, and primary household language among children with special health care needs, United States, 2009–2010. Maternal and Child Health Journal. 2015;19(8):1687–1697. doi: 10.1007/s10995-015-1683-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Johnson CP, Myers SM. Identification and evaluation of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics. 2007;120(5):1183–1215. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kelley ML, Power TG, Wimbush DD. Determinants of disciplinary practices in low-income black mothers. Child Development. 1992;63(3):573–582. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01647.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Khowaja MK, Hazzard AP, Robins DL. Sociodemographic barriers to early detection of autism: Screening and evaluation using the M-CHAT, M-CHAT-R, and Follow-Up. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Springer US. 2015;45(6):1797–1808. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2339-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kiing JSH, Low PS, Chan YH, et al. Interpreting Parents' Concerns About Their Children's Development With the Parents Evaluation of Developmental Status: Culture Matters. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2012;33(2):1. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31823f686e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kogan MD, Blumberg SJ, Schieve La, et al. Prevalence of parent-reported diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder among children in the US, 2007. Pediatrics. 2009;124(5):1395–403. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-1522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Linstead E, Dixon DR, French R, et al. Intensity and learning outcomes in the treatment of children with autism spectrum disorder. Behavior Modification. 2016 doi: 10.1177/0145445516667059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Liptak GS, Benzoni LB, Mruzek DW, et al. Disparities in diagnosis and access to health services for children with autism: Data from the National Survey of Children’s Health. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2008;29(3):152–160. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e318165c7a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lord C, Rutter M, Divalore PC, et al. Autism diagnostic observation schedule-WPS edition. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  40. Lord C, Rutter M, Dilavore PC, et al. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2) Manual (Part I): Modules 1–4. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  41. MacDonald R, Parry-Cruwys D, Dupere S, et al. Assessing progress and outcome of early intensive behavioral intervention for toddlers with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2014;35(12):3632–44. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.08.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Mandell DS, Novak M. The role of culture in families’ treatment decisions for children with autism spectrum disorders. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2005 doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mandell DS, Listerud J, Levy SE, et al. Race differences in the age at diagnosis among medicaid-eligible children with autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, The American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41(12):1447–1453. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200212000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mandell DS, Ittenbach RF, Levy SE, et al. Disparities in diagnoses received prior to a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37(9):1795–1802. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0314-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Mandell DS, Wiggins LD, Carpenter LA, et al. Racial/ethnic disparities in the identification of children with autism spectrum disorders. American Journal of Public Health. 2009;99(3):493–498. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2007.131243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. McMahon CR, Malesa EE, Yoder PJ, et al. Parents of Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders Have Merited Concerns About Their Later-Born Infants. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities. 2007;32(2):154–160. [Google Scholar]
  47. McMiller WP, Weisz JR. Help-seeking preceding mental health clinic intake among African-American, Latino, and Caucasian youths. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, The American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35(8):1086–1094. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199608000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Montes G, Halterman JS. White-Black disparities in family-centered care among children with autism in the United States: Evidence from the NS-CSHCN 2005–2006. Academic Pediatrics. 2011;11(4):297–304. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2011.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mullen EM. Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  50. Ohta M, Nagai Y, Hara H, et al. Parental perception of behavioral symptoms in Japanese autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1987;17(4):549–563. doi: 10.1007/BF01486970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Orinstein AJ, Helt M, Troyb E, et al. Intervention for optimal outcome in children and adolescents with a history of autism. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2014;35(4):247–256. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Ozonoff S, Young GS, Steinfeld MB, et al. How early do parent concerns predict later autism diagnosis? Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP. 2009;30(5):367–375. doi: 10.1097/dbp.0b013e3181ba0fcf. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Pierce K, Carter C, Weinfeld M, et al. Detecting, studying, and treating autism early: The one-year well-baby check-up approach. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2011;159(3):458–465. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2011.02.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Richards M, Mossey J, Robins DL. Parents’ concerns as they relate to their child’s development and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2016;37(7):532–540. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Robins DL, Fein D, Barton ML. Self-published. 1999. Modified checklist for autism in toddlers. [Google Scholar]
  56. Robins DL, Fein D, Barton ML. Self-published. 2009. Modified checklist for autism in toddlers, revised with follow-up (M-CHAT-R/F) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Robins DL, Casagrande K, Barton ML, et al. Validation of the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers, Revised with Follow-up (M-CHAT-R/F) Pediatrics. 2014;133(1):37–45. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-1813. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Schopler E, Reichler R, DeVellis R, et al. Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) for diagnostic screening and classification of autism. New York: Irvington; 1980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Schopler E, Van Bourgondien M, Wellman G, et al. Childhood Autism Rating Scale-Second Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Association; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  60. Sices L. Developmental Screening in Primary Care: The Effectiveness of Current Practice and Recommendations for Improvement. Screening 2007 [Google Scholar]
  61. Smith T, Klorman R, Mruzek DW. Predicting outcome of community-based early intensive behavioral intervention for children with autism. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2015;43(7):1271–1282. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0002-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sparrow SS, Cicchetti DV. Diagnostic uses of the vineland adaptive behavior scales. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 1985;10(2):215–225. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/10.2.215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sparrow SS, Cicchetti DV, Balla D. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. 2. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  64. Sun X, Allison C, Auyeung B, et al. Research in developmental disabilities. 12. Vol. 35. Elsevier Ltd; 2014. Parental concerns, socioeconomic status, and the risk of autism spectrum conditions in a population-based study; pp. 3678–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Thomas KA, Dettlaff AJ. African American families and the role of physical discipline: Witnessing the past in the present. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment 2011 [Google Scholar]
  66. Valicenti-McDermott M, Hottinger K, Seijo R, et al. Age at diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Pediatrics, Elsevier. 2012;161(3):554–556. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ventola P, Kleinman J, Pandey J, et al. Differentiating between autism spectrum disorders and other developmental disabilities in children who failed a screening instrument for ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37(3):425–436. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0177-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Vivanti G, Dissanayake C, Victorian ASELCC. Outcome for children receiving the Early Start Denver Model before and after 48 months. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Springer US. 2016;46(7):2441–2449. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2777-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Weisz JR, Weiss B. Studying the ‘referability’ of child clinical problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1991;59(2):266–73. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.59.2.266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Weisz JR, Suwanlert S, Chaiyasit W, et al. Thai and American perspectives on over- and undercontrolled child behavior problems: Exploring the threshold model among parents, teachers, and psychologists. Journal of Consulting and Cinical Psychology. 1988;56(4):601–9. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.56.4.601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Young RL, Brewer N, Pattison C. Parental identification of early behavioural abnormalities in children with autistic disorder. Autism : the international journal of research and practice. 2003;7(2):125–143. doi: 10.1177/1362361303007002002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Zuckerman KE, Mattox K, Donelan K, et al. Pediatrician identification of Latino children at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics. 2013;132(3):445–53. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-0383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Zuckerman KE, Lindly OJ, Sinche BK. Parental concerns, provider response, and timeliness of autism spectrum disorder diagnosis. Journal of Pediatrics, Elsevier Inc. 2015;166(6):1431–1439. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES