Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 May 3.
Published in final edited form as: Lancet Psychiatry. 2017 Feb 16;4(4):310–319. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30049-4

Subcortical brain volume differences of participants with ADHD across the lifespan: an ENIGMA collaboration

Martine Hoogman 1,2, Janita Bralten 1,2, Derrek P Hibar 3, Maarten Mennes 4, Marcel P Zwiers 4, Lizanne Schweren 5, Kimm JE van Hulzen 1,2, Sarah E Medland 6, Elena Shumskaya 1,2, Neda Jahanshad 3, Patrick de Zeeuw 7, Eszter Szekely 8, Gustavo Sudre 8, Thomas Wolfers 1,9, Alberdingk MH Onnink 1,2, Janneke T Dammers 2,10, Jeanette C Mostert 1,9, Yolanda Vives-Gilabert 11, Gregor Kohls 12, Eileen Oberwelland 12, Jochen Seitz 13, Martin Schulte-Rüther 12,14, Sara Ambrosino di Bruttopilo 7, Alysa E Doyle 15,16, Marie F Høvik 17, Margaretha Dramsdahl 18, Leanne Tamm 19, Theo GM van Erp 20, Anders Dale 21,22, Andrew Schork 23, Annette Conzelmann 24, Kathrin Zierhut 25, Ramona Baur 26, Hazel McCarthy 27, Yuliya N Yoncheva 28, Ana Cubillo 29, Kaylita Chantiluke 29, Mitul A Mehta 30, Yannis Paloyelis 30, Sarah Hohmann 31, Sarah Baumeister 31, Ivanei Bramati 32, Paulo Mattos 32,33, Fernanda Tovar-Moll 32,34, Pamela Douglas 35, Tobias Banaschewski 31, Daniel Brandeis 31,36,37,38, Jonna Kuntsi 39, Phil Asherson 39, Katya Rubia 29, Clare Kelly 27,28,40,41, Adriana Di Martino 28, Michael P Milham 42,43, Francisco X Castellanos 28,44, Thomas Frodl 27,45, Mariam Zentis 45, Klaus-Peter Lesch 46,47, Andreas Reif 48, Paul Pauli 26, Terry Jernigan 49,50, Jan Haavik 51,52, Kerstin J Plessen 51,53, Astri J Lundervold 51,54, Kenneth Hugdahl 52,54, Larry J Seidman 55,56, Joseph Biederman 55,57, Nanda Rommelse 10,58, Dirk J Heslenfeld 59,60, Catharina Hartman 5, Pieter J Hoekstra 5, Jaap Oosterlaan 60, Georg von Polier 12, Kerstin Konrad 12, Oscar Vilarroya 62,63, Josep-Antoni Ramos-Quiroga 62,64, Joan Carles Soliva 62, Sarah Durston 7, Jan K Buitelaar 2,58,65, Stephen V Faraone 51,66, Philip Shaw 8,67, Paul Thompson 3, Barbara Franke 1,2,10
PMCID: PMC5933934  NIHMSID: NIHMS854669  PMID: 28219628

Abstract

BACKGROUND

Neuroimaging studies show structural alterations in several brain regions in children and adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Through the formation of the worldwide ENIGMA ADHD Working Group, we addressed weaknesses of prior imaging studies and meta-analyses in sample size and methodological heterogeneity.

METHODS

Our sample comprised 1713 participants with ADHD and 1529 controls from 23 sites (age range: 4–63 years; 66% males). Individual sites analyzed magnetic resonance imaging brain scans with harmonized protocols. Case-control differences in subcortical structures and intracranial volume (ICV) were assessed through mega-and meta-analysis.

FINDINGS

The volumes of the accumbens (Cohen’s d=−0.15), amygdala (d=−0.19), caudate (d=−0.11), hippocampus (d=−0.11), putamen (d=−0.14), and ICV (d=−0.10) were found to be smaller in cases relative to controls. Effect sizes were highest in children, case-control differences were not present in adults. Explorative lifespan modeling suggested a delay of maturation and a delay of degeneration. Psychostimulant medication use or presence of comorbid psychiatric disorders did not influence results, nor did symptom scores correlate with brain volume.

INTERPRETATION

Using the largest data set to date, we extend the brain maturation delay theory for ADHD to include subcortical structures and refute medication effects on brain volume suggested by earlier meta-analyses. We add new knowledge about bilateral amygdala, accumbens, and hippocampus reductions in ADHD, and provide unprecedented precision in effect size estimates. Lifespan analyses suggest that, in the absence of well-powered longitudinal studies, the ENIGMA cross-sectional sample across six decades of life provides a means to generate hypotheses about lifespan trajectories in brain phenotypes.

FUNDING

National Institutes of Health

Keywords: ADHD, Subcortical brain volumes, imaging, lifespan, meta-analysis, amygdala

INTRODUCTION

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common neuropsychiatric disorder with a prevalence of 5.3% in childhood1. Two-thirds of patients with an ADHD diagnosis in childhood continue to have persistent, impairing symptoms in adulthood2. ADHD is characterized by age-inappropriate symptoms of inattention and/or hyperactivity and impulsivity3. Many imaging studies, often in small samples, have reported brain structural and functional differences between individuals with ADHD and controls, both in childhood and adulthood. Five meta-analyses of structural neuroimaging studies in ADHD have been published (Table 1). The first meta-analysis pooled region of interest brain volumes studies4, while the others pooled voxel-based morphometry (VBM) studies58. Most consistent results across studies were for reduced volumes of (parts of) the basal ganglia for patients compared with healthy controls. Two metaanalyses showed that, with increasing age, basal ganglia structural differences between cases and controls tended to decrease, and that stimulant treatment was associated with normalization of these brain structures5,6.

Table 1.

Overview of published structural neuroimaging meta-analyses in ADHD.

Study Subjects Sample size (case/controls) Image analysis method Main results
Valera et al., 2007 Children only 565/583 Brain volumetry Reduced volume of splenium of the corpus callosum, cerebral volume, and right caudate nucleus in patients.
Ellison-Wright et al., 2008 Children only 114/143 VBM Smaller right putamen/pallidum region in patients.
Nakao et al., 2011 Children and adults 378/344 VBM Smaller basal ganglia in patients. Increasing age and long-term medication use associated with reduced case-control differences.
Frodl et al., 2012 Children and adults 320/288 VBM Right globus pallidus, right putamen, and caudate are reduced in patients. Increasing age and treatment tended to be associated with reduced deficits in patients.
Norman et al, 2016 Children and adults 931/822 VBM Decreased grey matter volume in right basal ganglia, insula, ventromedial orbitofrontal cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, right anterior cingulate cortex. No association between the grey matter abnormalities and longterm stimulant use

VBM = voxel-based morphometry

Brain volumes have also been associated with clinical features of ADHD; smaller volumes of caudate, cerebellum, and frontal and temporal gray matter have been associated with greater symptom severity9. Also in the general population, ADHD symptoms correlated with volumetric brain measures10,11.

Identifying structural brain differences in people with ADHD is important to further our insights into the nature of ADHD. So far, analyses of brain structures in ADHD have been limited in size and statistical power (Table 1); the sample size of the largest published meta-analysis on brain volume (n=565 cases and n=583 controls) allowed the identification of differences in brain volume with Cohen’s d effect sizes of ≥0.15 with 80% power (G*Power12). Analyses of other psychiatric disorders show that smaller effects are likely13. Existing meta-analyses for ADHD only used published data as source material, which limited their ability to address covariates that may vary among studies, like age and medication5,6. In addition, the existing meta-analyses included studies using variable methods and protocols such as the segmentation software and quality control.

To overcome such issues and perform collaborative studies of maximal power, we founded the ENIGMA ADHD Working Group. This worldwide collaboration enabled analyses of existing individual data, improving upon earlier meta-analyses by basing analyses on the use of harmonized segmentation and quality control protocols. Our increased sample size compared to all earlier studies supported both mega-and meta-analysis (sMethods, appendix) designs across 60 years of the lifespan. We selected subcortical brain volumes as our target, because of neurodevelopmental theories hypothesizing ADHD to be linked to early-emerging, persistent subcortical abnormalities14 and building on the results of earlier meta-analyses, which showed that deviations in these volumes were most consistently observed. In addition, we investigated intracranial volume (ICV) as a measure of total brain volume. Analyzing data from 23 cohorts with a sample size of n=3200 enabled us to detect the case-control effect sizes observed in other psychiatric disorders. In addition, the mega-analysis design also allowed investigation of associations with symptom scores, age, psychostimulant medication use, and comorbidity with other psychiatric disorders.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Contributing studies

The ENIGMA ADHD Working Group was formed in 2013 to aggregate structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data from participants with ADHD and healthy controls across the lifespan. Details about the diagnostic procedures for each site are listed in the appendix (sTable1). The group adopted a rolling inclusion design, in which new groups can join at any time, but data-freezes allow analysis at fixed time points. The data-freeze for the current subcortical analysis was set at February 8, 2015. The analyzed sample comprised 23 cohorts, for details see Table 2. Each participating site had approval from its local ethics committee to perform the study and to share de-identified, anonymized individual data.

Table 2.

Overview of cohort characteristics. For a more detailed description and references for the assessments and neuroimaging procedures, see sTable 1 in the appendix.

Sample name Site, country of origin N Total N Cases (M/F) N Controls (M/F) Age ± SD
ADHD-WUE Würzburg, GER 118 32/30 26/30 39.68±11.44
ADHD-DUB1 Dublin, IRL 75 27/9 31/8 22.29±5.23
ADHD-DUB2 Dublin, IRL 20 16/4 33.65±10.15
ADHD-Mattos Rio de Janeiro, BRA 17 10/7 22.94±1.39
ADHD200-KKI Baltimore, USA 94 15/10 41/28 10.22±1.34
ADHD200-NYU* New York, USA 260 115/36 54/55 11.47±2.92
ADHD200-Peking Peking, CHN 245 90/12 84/59 11.70±1.96
ADHD200-OHSU Oregon, USA 109 29/13 30/37 9.13±1.25
ADHD-UKA Aachen, GER 181 95/7 53/26 11.21±2.68
Bergen-adultADHD Bergen, NOR 81 21/17 16/27 31.21±6.74
Bergen-SVG Bergen, NOR 54 20/5 20/9 10.05±1.20
DAT-London London, GBR 56 27/0 29/0 15.78±2.10
IMpACT-NL Nijmegen, NLD 245 49/76 49/71 35.49±11.39
MGH-ADHD New York, USA 148 42/37 29/40 35.76±12.03
NICHE Utrecht, NLD 158 68/10 67/13 10.42±1.95
NYU ADHD New York, USA 80 22/18 22/18 31.58±9.44
UAB-ADHD Barcelona, SPA 198 82/21 64/31 25.80±13.02
ZI-CAPS Mannheim, GER 35 17/5 7/6 12.73±1.23
ADHD-Rubia London, GBR 77 44/0 33/0 13.95±2.19
NeuroImage-ADAM Amsterdam, NLD 182 73/24 57/28 17.16±3.19
NeuroImage-NIJM Nijmegen, NLD 178 89/50 23/16 16.89±3.41
NIH Bethesda, USA 502 168/83 168/83 9.97±3.09
MTA Irvine, USA 129 73/15 31/10 24.6±1.4
Total 3242 1713 1529 18.6±11.81
*

One subject was excluded because of missing gender status

Neuroimaging

Structural T1-weighted brain MRI data were acquired and processed at the individual sites. The images were analyzed using standardized protocols to harmonize analysis and quality control processes (sMethods, appendix, and http://enigma.ini.usc.edu/protocols/imaging-protocols/). Fully-automated and validated neuroimaging segmentation algorithms based on FreeSurfer versions 5.1 or 5.3 were used (sTable1, appendix). To make sure no effects of FreeSurfer version influenced the results13, we performed an additional analysis, adding version number as a covariate to our main model (see below). For each participant, we computed ICV and left and right volumes of the accumbens, putamen, pallidum, caudate, thalamus, amygdala, and hippocampus. For further analysis, we used the mean of the left and right volume ((R+L)/2). For an overview of single site subcortical structures, see appendix (sFigure1). Outliers were determined at above and below 1.5-times the interquartile range per cohort and group (case/control) and were excluded (sFigure1, appendix)15.

Case-control differences of subcortical brain volumes and ICV

By pooling all available individual data from all cohorts, a mega-analysis (for explanation see the sMethods, appendix), we investigated the differences between cases and controls on subcortical volumes and ICV. After excluding collinearity of age, sex, and intracranial volume (ICV) (variance inflation factor <1.2) and normality testing, the mega-analysis of each subcortical volume was performed using a linear mixed model (lme) by running the package nlme in R (version3.1–117). The model included diagnosis (case=1 and control=0) as factor of interest, age, sex, and ICV as fixed factors, and site as random factor. In the analysis of ICV, ICV was omitted as covariate from the model. Handedness was added to the model to correct for possible effects of lateralization, but was excluded from the model when there was no significant contribution of this factor. To calculate Cohen’s d effect size estimates, adjusted for age, sex, site, and ICV, we used the t-statistic from the factor diagnosis in the model. In a post-hoc analysis, left and right volumes were studied separately.

To make sure that no unobserved factor biased our analysis of case-control differences, meta-analysis was also performed by linear regression analysis for each volume and for each sample separately, taking age, sex, and ICV into account. The R-package “metaphor” (version 1.9–116) was used to perform an inverse variance-weighted, random-effects meta-analysis, in accordance with other ENIGMA Working Groups13,15 (sMethods, appendix).

Effects of age

The effect of age on subcortical volume and ICV was studied by running the above described model for groups stratified by age: in children aged 14 or younger, adolescents aged 15 until 21 years of age, and in adults, aged 22 and older. We removed samples that were left with 10 subjects or less due to the stratification. As it is likely that the effects of age do not strictly follow a linear model, we only report linear effects of age and the effect of age*diagnosis for the sake of being complete. In addition, more explorative modeling was done to better understand the effects of age, by plotting moving averages and using fractional polynomials to fit non-linear models to the data (sMethods, appendix).

Significance threshold

Multiple comparisons correction for 32 tests (8 volumes and 4 groups: all, children, adolescents, and adults) was applied by using a false discovery rate with q=0.05 resulting in a p-value significance threshold of p=0.156.

Exploration of effects of sex, psychostimulant medication, and clinical measures

To explore the effects of sex on brain volume, the results of the term sex from the main model are reported. To examine associations between prior psychostimulant treatment and regional brain volume, the mega-analysis model was run again, including only patients with medication information available (sTable1, appendix). To test, whether acute effects of psychostimulant medication confounded possible brain volume differences between participants with ADHD and healthy controls, we excluded subjects treated with stimulants at the time of their participation in the study (participants receiving other types of treatment were retained). In addition, as previous meta-analyses had found an association between stimulants and brain volumes5,6, we compared patients, who had ever used stimulant medication, to patients, who were lifetime stimulant-naïve. We explored the effects of ADHD symptom scores and presence/absence of co-morbid disorders on those brain volumes that differed significantly between participants with ADHD and healthy controls, for details see appendix (sMethods and sTable2, appendix).

Role of the funding sources

The funders had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report. The corresponding author had full access to all of the data and the final responsibility to submit for publication.

RESULTS

We included data from 1713 participants with ADHD and 1529 healthy controls (Table 2) with a median age of 140 (range 4–63) years.

Case-control differences in subcortical volumes and ICV

As shown in Table 3, the mega-analysis indicated that participants with ADHD had significantly smaller volumes for the accumbens, amygdala, caudate, hippocampus, putamen, and ICV. Post-hoc analyses for the subcortical regions showed these effects to be bilateral (sTable3, appendix). No effect of FreeSurfer version of handedness was found (sTable4&5, appendix).

Table 3.

Results of the mega-analysis of subcortical brain volumes in the total sample.

N Cases/Controls Adjusted mean volume estimate (SEM)1 Cases/Controls p-value for Diagnosis Cohen’s d (95%CI) Other significant terms in the model
Accumbens 1652/1471 656.5 (1.2)/673.7(2.1) 4.98×10−9 −0.15 (−0.22 – −0.08) ICV, Site, Age
Amygdala 1598/1463 1554.1 (3.7)/1577.8(3.7) 3.69×10−7 −019 (−0.26 – −0.11) Sex, ICV, Site
Caudate 1659/1489 3927.8 (8.2)/3964.6(8.6) 0.001 −011 (−0.18 – −0.03) ICV, Site, Age
Hippocampus 1599/1436 4147.8(8.1)/4163.7(8.5) 0.004 −0.11 (−0.18 – −0.03) Sex, ICV, Site
Pallidum 1651/1471 1764.8 (4.6)/1763.7(4.9) 0.95 −0.00 (−0.07 – 0) Sex, ICV, Site, Age
Putamen 1660/1497 6025.7 (13.7)/6100.6(14.6) 6.36×10−9 −0.14 (−0.21 – −0.07) Sex, ICV, Site, Age
Thalamus# 1405/1242 7683.3 (17.8)/7611.6(18.5) 0.39 −0.03 (0.03 – −0.10) Sex, ICV, Site, Age
ICV 1693/1513 1513597.3 (2741.3)/1501680.7(2924.7) 0.006 −0.10 (0.04 – −0.16) Sex, Site, Age

Bold p-values are significant at the FDR-corrected threshold of p=0.0156, italic p-values nominally significant at p<0.05

#

thalamus volume was not available from the NIH sample.

1

Adjusted mean volume estimate and standard error of the mean, corrected for age, sex, ICV, and site.

Results of the case-control meta-analysis were largely comparable to those of the mega-analysis, but volume differences for accumbens and hippocampus were not significant (sTable6, appendix). Heterogeneity (I2) across samples was low to moderate; heterogeneity was highest for hippocampus (sTable6, appendix) and might be indicative of non-linear effects of site for this structure.

Effect of age

Age-stratified analyses revealed significant case-control differences in children for the accumbens, amygdala, caudate, hippocampus, putamen, and ICV (Table 4 and Figure 1). Effect sizes were higher than those for the entire sample. In the adolescent group, there was a significant case-control difference in the hippocampus. In adults, none of the case-control comparisons remained significant. Figure 1 suggested an interaction effect for age-group and diagnosis on hippocampus volume; this was nominally supported by linear interaction statistics (p=0.03; sTable7, appendix). Explorative modeling using moving averages (Figure 2) also showed the age effects to cluster early in life, with higher age of attaining peak volumes in the ADHD group. The moving averages also hinted at potential later onset of volume decrease in the ADHD group, most clearly seen in accumbens and putamen. Sample sizes after age 50 years were limited (sFigure3, appendix), and resulted in wider confidence intervals in the moving average analyses. The fractional polynomial analyses also supported different developmental models for ADHD cases and controls for amygdala, hippocampus, putamen, thalamus, and ICV (sFigure4 & sTable8, appendix).

Table 4.

Results of the mega-analysis of subcortical brain volumes in the stratified age groups

Children (<15) Adolescents (15–21) Adults (21>)
N Cases/Controls p-value for Diagnosis Cohen’s d
(95%CI)
N Cases/Controls p-value for Diagnosis Cohen’s d
(95%CI)
N Cases/Controls p-value for Diagnosis Cohen’s d
(95%CI)
Accumbens 810/827 0.0001 −019
(−0.29 – 0.10)
323/224 0.61 −0.04
(−0.22 – 0.12)
510/415 0.12 −0.10
(−0.23 – −0.03)
Amygdala 767/820 0.0003 −018
(−028 – −0.08)
321/226 0.12 −014
(−0.31 – 0.03)
500/412 0.03 −0.14
(−0.27 – −0.01)
Caudate 825/840 0.006 −013
(−0.23 – −0.04)
324/224 0.28 −0.10
(−0.27 – 0.07)
502/420 0.30 −0.07
(−0.20 – 0.05)
Hippocampus 764/802 0.012 −0.12
(−0.22 – −0.03)
320/225 0.006 −0.24
(−0.42 – −0.08)
506/404 0.38 0.06
(−0.07 – 0.19)
Pallidum 816/831 0.79 −001
(−011 – 0.08)
321/223 0.78 002
(−0.15 – 0.20)
506/412 0.51 0.04
(−0.08 – 0.17)
Putamen 836/854 0.0002 −018
(−028 – −0.09)
329/228 0.83 −0.02
(−0.19 – 0.15)
499/416 0.23 −0.08
(−0.21 – 0.05)
Thalamus# 604/616 0.89 001
(0.06 – −0.10)
288/202 0.74 0.03
(−0.15 – 0.21)
503/416 0.28 −0.07
(−0.20 – −0.06)
ICV 837/854 0.003 −014
(0.04 – −0.24)
330/229 0.13 −0.13
(−0.30 – 0.04)
515/422 0.91 0.01
(0.06 – −0.12)

Bold p-values are significant at the FDR-corrected threshold of p=0.0156, italic p-values nominally significant at p<0.05

#

thalamus volume was not available from the NIH sample.

Due to a sample size lower than 10, the data for the following cohorts in analysis of the adolescent group were omitted: ADHD-Mattos (n=2), ADHD-WUE (n=2), BergenAdultADHD (n=4), MTA (n=2), Niche (n=7), ZI-CAPS (n=2).

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Displayed are the Cohens d effect sizes of differences between patients with ADHD and healthy controlsfor subcortical volumes and ICV, for 4 separate groups:1.) all subjects, 2.) children only (<15years), 3.) adolescents only (15–21 years), and 4.) adults only (>21 years). *significant after false discovery rate (FDR) correction; nominal significant at p<0.05

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Displayed are the moving averages, corrected for age, sex, ICV and site for the subcortical volumes.

Effect of sex

Consistent with literature documenting smaller brains in females17, all but two subcortical structures, accumbens and caudate, showed main effects of sex in the mega-analysis (Table 3). None of the volumes showed differential sex effects for participants with ADHD and controls.

Effect of medication

Information on current medication use was available for 1254 participants with ADHD; 455 participants with ADHD were on psychostimulant medication (methylphenidate or amphetamine) at the time of scanning, with over half of the studies using a washout period of 24/48 hours (sTable1, appendix); 799 participants with ADHD were not taking stimulant medication at scan time. Case-control differences in brain volumes after excluding participants currently on stimulant medication (Table 5) were comparable in effect sizes to those observed in the main analysis.

Table 5.

Results of the exploration of the effect of medication on case-control differences

Patients currently not taking stimulants versus controls* Stimulant use in patients: positive versus negative lifetime history
n Cases/Controls Cohen’s d (95%CI) p-value for Diagnosis n Never/ever stimulant use in patients only p-value for positive versus negative for lifetime stimulant use
Accumbens 776/1484 −0.12 (−0.21 – −0.03) 0.007 79/625 0.32
Amygdala 753/1474 −0.18 (−0.27 – −0.10) 4.90 ×10−9 80/590 0.41
Caudate 777/1502 −0.10 (−0.19 – −0.01) 0.02 80/627 0.15
Hippocampus 757/1446 −0.08 (−0.17 – 0.003) 0.06 80/593 0.69
Pallidum 776/1484 0.01 (−0.07 – 0.10) 0.74 79/621 0.26
Putamen 784/1508 −0.13 (−0.22 – −0.04) 0.004 81/627 0.29
Thalamus 692/1253 −0.03 (0.04 – −0.12) 0.53 80/458 0.29
ICV 793/1512 −0.06 (0.04 – −0.16) 0.15 81/632 0.92
*

within this group, 152 subjects were lifetime positive for the use of stimulant medication, 82 were lifetime negative; for 565 no lifetime information was available.

For 719 participants with ADHD, information was available on lifetime usage of stimulant medication. Of these, 82 participants (11%) had never taken stimulant medication, compared to 637 patients, who used stimulant medication somewhere in their lifetime for a period of more than 4 weeks. No differences in any of the volumes were found by directly comparing these two groups.

Association of clinical measures with subcortical brain volumes and ICV

Meta-analysis of the correlation between ADHD symptom scores in cases and brain volumes revealed no significant effects; only a nominally significant effect (p=0.02) was observed for caudate volume (sTable9 & sFigure6, appendix). Neither were there any significant correlations when only the childhood samples were entered in the meta-analysis. Also, the observed case-control brain volume differences were not explained by the presence of another comorbid psychiatric disorder (sTable10, appendix).

DISCUSSION

Here, we report the largest study to date of brain volume differences between participants with ADHD and healthy individuals. Through worldwide collaboration in the ENIGMA ADHD Working Group, data on 1713 participants with ADHD and 1529 healthy controls were newly analyzed, using harmonized quality control and segmentation procedures. Compared to previous meta-analyses, our study newly identified amygdala, accumbens, and hippocampus volumes to be smaller in participants with ADHD, and extended earlier findings for reduced caudate and putamen volumes by showing those to be bilateral rather than unilateral5,7. Significant volume differences had small effect sizes (ranging from d=−0.10 to d=−0.19). Meta-analysis confirmed these results. Age-stratification showed volume differences to cluster in childhood, no differences were seen in adulthood. The volume differences were equally apparent in those treated with psychostimulant medication and those naïve to psychostimulants. Finally, no correlations with quantitative scores of ADHD symptoms were found in cases, nor did comorbidity with other psychiatric disorders explain the findings.

The work presented here carries several important messages for the clinical field. First, our results coming from highly powered analysis, confirm that ADHD patients truly have altered brains, i.e. that ADHD is a disorder of the brain. This is a clear message for clinicians to convey to parents and patients, which can help to reduce the stigma that ADHD is just a label for difficult kids, and caused by incompetent parenting. We hope this work will contribute to a better understanding of ADHD in the general public, and that it becomes just as apparent as for major depressive disorder, for example, that we label ADHD as a brain disorder. Second, finding the most pronounced effects in childhood and showing delayed peaks of subcortical volume maturation provides a relevant model of ADHD as a disorder of brain maturation delay. Third, the brain differences we have found are not caused by any co-morbid disorders, medication effects, or ADHD symptom severity, but are exclusively related to the ADHD diagnosis. Lastly, finding the largest effect in the amygdala is another important message, as it links ADHD to emotional regulation problems. Those are often present in patients with ADHD, but these disease characteristics have not (yet) been included into the official DSM-criteria. Our work shows neurobiological support for the inclusion of this domain in the core ADHD phenotype, asking for more acknowledgement of the importance of emotion regulation problems in the ADHD patient.

Our findings for striatum volume reduction are in line with current models of ADHD18. Differences in caudate volume are the most consistent finding for ADHD46, and also smaller putamen volumes have been frequently reported57. Our study now provides robust effect size estimates for those structural differences and shows that effects are bilateral. Although identified before in a single study19, our findings extend the meta-analytic literature to the third striatal volume, nucleus accumbens. Novel meta-analytic findings of our study are for amygdala and hippocampus. Previous work in single studies had found effects in these structures2022, but also failed to replicate in others e.g.23,24. For amygdala volume, which showed the largest effect size in our study (d=−0.19; d=−0.18 in children), and for accumbens, the lack of earlier meta-analytic evidence for its role in ADHD might be due to the fact that these are small structures, for which automatic segmentation performs less well25. A more highly powered analysis may therefore have been necessary to overcome the experimental inaccuracy of these measures. Prior work provides functional evidence for a role of amygdala, accumbens, and hippocampus in ADHD. Dysfunction of the amygdala is associated with difficulties recognizing emotional stimuli, callous unemotional traits, and with emotion regulation in general26,27. Difficulties in recognizing emotional stimuli, diminished emotional reactions to pleasant stimuli, and higher levels of callous unemotional traits have all been linked to ADHD2831, and amygdala volume has been associated with hyperactivity20. The accumbens, with its prominent role in reward processing, is central to motivational and emotional dysfunction in ADHD18. The results of the hippocampus are less straight-forward, as there is not so much evidence for a deficit in long-term memory in ADHD patients the hippocampus’ main function32. However, there are also reports on the hippocampus playing a role in the regulation of motivation and emotion, which is impaired in ADHD33.

Importantly, effect sizes observed in our study were similar to those found for other psychiatric disorders analysed using the ENIGMA procedures, in particular major depression and bipolar disorder13,34. The scale of the effects is consistent with expectations for a heterogeneous disorder like ADHD. The specific pattern of findings may partially differentiate ADHD from the other psychiatric disorders analysed using similar procedures, i.e. schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depressive disorder13,15,34. Especially effects on caudate and putamen seem to be ADHD-specific among the four. However, as mostly adults were investigated for the other three disorders, formal analyses taking age into account will need to be performed to make valid statements.

The results of the age-stratified analysis indicate that subcortical volume differences in ADHD are most prominent in children, and non-existent in adults. Our additional exploratory models suggest that this is not the entire story on age effects, though care in interpreting this result is needed because of the crosssectional design of this study. Based on our findings across different approaches, we propose a model of altered trajectories of subcortical volume in ADHD. Our data suggest a delayed peak volume in participants with ADHD, which is reminiscent of earlier reports of altered velocity of cortical development in a longitudinal sample35. This model should be confirmed by longitudinal analyses, especially since the childhood and adult ADHD samples included in this study represent different subgroups of the population: childhood ADHD samples include those who will later remit and those who will persist having ADHD in adulthood, the adult ADHD samples include only the latter. In addition to the delays in subcortical brain maturation at early age, our exploratory work also tentatively suggest later onset of decreases in subcortical volumes beyond the 4th decade of life in ADHD. However, since sample sizes in our analysis dropped dramatically above age 25 years, and we had insufficient data to study age effects after 60 years, this work is still hampered by not having sufficient subjects per site to rule out site-biases in those age ranges. As long as ADHD in old age is still a blind spot in ADHD research, it will be difficult to test the validity of such findings.

Prior meta-analyses found associations between the percentage of treated patients and right caudate and amygdala/uncus volumes5,6. In our analysis, in which we were able to directly compare treated to non-treated participants with ADHD in a sample exceeding the size included in the two previous metaanalyses 4-fold, we did not confirm such associations with brain volume. This is in line with the most recent meta-analysis8. However, since our study had a non-randomized, cross-sectional design, some caution to interpreting these results is warranted, as the design of this study was not optimal to test medication effects. Also, as both prior meta-analyses used voxel-wise maps, there is a possibility that the observed normalizing effects of medication were too local to be picked up by volumetry.

We did not observe associations of brain volumes with clinical measures, i.e. comorbidity or ADHD symptom scores. The absence of an association with comorbidity suggests that the brain volume reductions are robustly linked to ADHD itself, rather than being a secondary phenomenon caused by comorbidity. The absence of significant associations between brain volumes and symptom ratings is not surprising, given that brain function is based on distributed networks of brain regions rather than individual brain regions36. Still, previous studies did find single volume-function associations9,37, which we do not replicate here. We also could not replicate an earlier reported (modest) correlation of a total brain volume measure highly related to ICV with ADHD symptom severity in a similarly sized population sample10. In addition to the above, not finding effects of symptom scores might also be due to the heterogeneity of the instruments used by different cohorts in our study and/or differences in raters (clinicians, teachers, parents). In addition, the sample size was halved in this case-only analysis, and the distribution of scores was skewed to the clinical range. In line with models of fronto-striatal dysfunction in ADHD, one could hypothesize that cortical structures might play a more important role in the severity of symptoms in ADHD patients than the subcortical structures14.

This study has several strengths and limitations. A clear strength is the sample size, being the largest mega-/meta-analysis to date, with enough power to detect effects as small as d=0.08. Another strength is the harmonization of segmentation protocols across all contribution sites, reducing imprecision caused by differences in methods. Nonetheless, diagnostic routines and acquisition of imaging data still differed between sites, a limitation contributing to heterogeneity across samples. A strength was also the opportunity for mega-analysis. While effect sizes were similar to the meta-analysis, the mega-analysis allowed a more powerful detection of case-control volume differences. Mega-analysis also enabled effects of age, sex, comorbidity, and medication to be studied, although accounting for site in these analyses might have somewhat masked age effects (as many studies had a restricted age range). Modeling age in a cross-sectional study is challenging but we have used several approaches to understand the effects of age, however, we should be cautious and interpret our findings as hypothesisgenerating for future studies.

To conclude, this first result of our world-wide collaboration confirms and extends previous findings of reduced striatal volume in ADHD. Optimizing sample size and harmonizing methods across studies allowed us to identify additional differences in amygdala and hippocampal volumes potentially contributing to problems in emotion regulation, motivation, and memory in ADHD. Brain volume differences were most prominent in children. We invite interested researchers to join the next studies of the ENIGMA ADHD Working Group. In this way, we may optimally benefit from efforts already invested in individual studies to better understand this common yet still vexing disorder.

Supplementary Material

supplement

Research in context.

Evidence before this study

After searching for all prior meta-analysis performed on brain volume differences in ADHD including the subcortical regions until the 1st of February 2015 using the search terms ‘ADHD’, ‘structural’, ‘brain’, and ‘meta-analysis [Title]’ and ‘english’ [Language] in Pubmed, we found four published meta-analyses. The largest of those meta-analysed data on 565 cases and 583 controls (children only). The published meta-analyses had three major limitations: 1. Power was only sufficient to detect effect sizes of Cohen’s d of 0.15 and higher, which we know to be insufficient based on results in other psychiatric disorders. 2. Existing studies only used published data as source material, which limited their ability to address covariates that may vary among studies, like age, and medication. 3. The existing meta-analyses included studies using different segmentation software and quality control procedures, a limitation contributing to heterogeneity across samples.

Added value of this study

The current multi-site study, with data on 1713 cases and 1529 controls included, is by far the largest and best-powered study to date on brain volumes in ADHD. Data of all sites were newly analyzed using harmonized methods. Our work implicates new structures, amygdala and hippocampus, in ADHD, and provides unprecedented precision in effect size estimates. Our results, covering most part of the lifespan, showed most pronounced effects in childhood.

Implications of all the available evidence

We confirm, with high powered analysis, that ADHD patients truly have altered brains, i.e. that ADHD is a disorder of the brain. This is a clear message for clinicians to convey to parents and patients, which can help to reduce the stigma of ADHD and get a better understanding of ADHD. This way, it will become just as apparent as for major depressive disorder, for example, that we label ADHD as a brain disorder. Also, finding the most pronounced effects in childhood provides a relevant model of ADHD as a disorder of brain maturation delay.

Finding the biggest effect in the amygdala is another important message, as it links ADHD to emotional regulation problems. Those are frequently found in patients with ADHD, but these disease characteristics have not (yet) made it into the official DSM-criteria. Our work shows neurobiological support for the inclusion of this domain in the core ADHD phenotype.

Acknowledgments

ENIGMA received funding from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) Consortium grant U54 EB020403, supported by a cross-NIH alliance that funds Big Data to Knowledge Centers of Excellence (BD2K). We also are supported by the European College for Neuropsychopharmacology (ECNP) by a grant for the ECNP Network ADHD across the lifespan.

ADHD-WUE: Data collection and analysis was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (KFO 125, TRR 58/A1 and A5, TRR SFB 58 B06, SFB-TRR 58/B01, and Z02, RE1632/5-1) and the research leading to these results also received funding from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme for research, technological development and demonstration under grant agreement no 602805 (“Aggressotype”).

ADHD-DUB1 and DUB2: The ADHD-DUB1 and DUB2 studies received funding from the Health Research Board Ireland.

ADHD-Mattos: Ivanei Bramati, Paolo Mattos and Fernanda Tovar-Moll were supported by an IDOR intramural grant.

ADHD200-KKI, ADHD200NYU, ADHD200Peking, ADHD200OHSU: http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/adhd200/)(p)ADHD-UKA: KFO-112 and IRTG1328 was supported by the German Research Foundation (DFG).

DAT-London: This work was supported in part by UK Medical Research, Council Grant G03001896 to J Kuntsi and NIH grants, R01MH62873 and R01MH081803 to SV Faraone.

IMpACT: The IMpACT study was supported by a grant from the Brain & Cognition Excellence Program and a Vici grant (to Barbara Franke) of the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO, grant numbers 433-09-229 and 016-130-669) and in part by the Netherlands Brain Foundation (grant number, 15F07[2]27)and the and BBMRI-NL (grant CP2010-33). The research leading to these results also received funding from the European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) under grant agreement no. 602805 (Aggressotype), no. 278948 (TACTICS) and no. 602450 (IMAGEMEND). In addition, the project received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie grant agreement no. 643051 (MiND) and under grant agreement no. 667302 (CoCA).

Niche: The structural neuroimaging studies of NICHE were supported by VIDI and VICI grants from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek, NWO) to Sarah Durston (grant numbers Vidi-91.776.384 and Vici-453-10-005).

NYU ADHD: NYU data collection and sharing was suported by NIH grants T32MH67763, R01MH083246, K23MH087770, R01MH094639, and U01MH099059 and a grant from the Stavros S. Niarchos Foundation.

UAB-ADHD: The study and its contributors received funding from the Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad under research grant SAF2012-32362 and : PI12/01139 from the Department of Health of the Government of Catalonia. Additional funding was obtained from the Generalitat de Catalunya.

ZI-CAPS: The Neurofeedback study was partly funded by the project D8 of the Deutsche Forschungsgesellschaft collaborative research center 636.

ADHD-Rubia: The study was funded by the UK Department of Health via the National Institute of Health Research Centre (BRC) for Mental health t South London and the Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London.

NeuroIMAGE: This work was supported by NIH Grant R01MH62873, NWO Large Investment Grant 1750102007010 and grants from Radboud university medical center, University Medical Center Groningen and Accare, and VU University Amsterdam. This work was also supported by a grant from NWO Brain & Cognition (433-09-242). Further support was received from the European Union FP7 programmes TACTICS (278948) and IMAGEMEND (602450).

MTA: Data collection and sharing for this project was funded by the NIDA MTA Neuroimaging Study (National Institute on Drug Abuse Grant Contract #: HHSN271200800009C). The Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA) was a National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) cooperative agreement randomized clinical trial, continued under an NIMH contract as a follow-up study and finally under a National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) contract.

Maarten Mennes: supported by a Marie Curie International Incoming Fellowship within the 7th European Community Framework Programme, grant agreement n° 327340.

Sarah Medland: supported by a Future Fellowship FT110100548 from the Australian Research Council. Pamela Douglas: Klingenstein Third Generation Foundation ADHD Grant Jan Haavik: K.G.Jebsen Foundation.

Larry Seidman: RO1 MH62152 and R21 MH091461.

Steve Faraone: K.G. Jebsen Centre for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURES

These authors all declare no conflicts of interest:

Martine Hoogman, Janita Bralten, Derrek Hibar, Maarten Mennes, Marcel Zwiers, Lizanne Schweren, Kimm van Hulzen, Sarah Medland, Elena Shumskaya, Neda Jahanshad, Eszter Szekely, Gustavo Sudre, Thomas Wolfers, Alberdingk Marten Onnink, Janneke Dammers, Jeanette Mostert, Yolanda Vives-Gilabert, Gregor Kohls, Ellen Oberwelland, Jochen Seitz, Martin Schulte-Rüther, Patrick de Zeeuw, Sara Ambrosino di Bruttopilo, Alysa Doyle, Marie Høvik, Margaretha Dramsdahl, Andrew Schork, Annette Conzelmann, Kathrin Zierhut, Ramona Baur, Hazel McCarthy, Yuliya Yoncheva, Ana Cubillo, Kaylita Chantiluke, Mitul Metha, Yannis Paloyelis, Sarah Hohmann, Sarah Baumeister, Ivanei Bramati, Fernanda Tovar-Moll, Daniel Brandeis, Jonna Kuntsi, Phil Asherson, Clare Kelly, Adriana Di Martino, Michael Milham, Francisco Castellanos, Thomas Frodl, Mariam Zentis, Klaus-Peter Lesch, Andreas Reif, Paul Pauli, Terry Jernigan, Kerstin Plessen, Astri Lundervold, Kenneth Hugdahl, Larry Seidman, Sarah Durston, Georg von Polier, Oscar Vilarroya, Joan Carles Soliva, Nanda Rommelse, Dirk Heslenfeld, Catharina Hartman, Jaap Oosterlaan, Philip Shaw, Paul Thompson.

Potential conflicts of interest for the following authors are reported:

Theo Van Erp consulted for Roche Pharmaceuticals and has a contract with Otsuka Pharmaceutical, Ltd.

Anders Dale is a Founder of CorTechs Labs, Inc. He serves on the Scientific Advisory Boards of CorTechs Labs and Human Longevity, Inc., and receives research funding through a Research Agreement with General Electric Healhcare.

Paulo Mattos was on the speakers’ bureau and/or acted as consultant for Janssen-Cilag, Novartis, and Shire in the previous five years; he also received travel awards to participate in scientific meetings from those companies. The ADHD outpatient program (Grupo de Estudos do Déficit de Atenção/Institute of Psychiatry) chaired by Dr. Mattos has also received research support from Novartis and Shire. The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data; or preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Tobias Banaschewski served in an advisory or consultancy role for Hexal Pharma, Lilly, Medice, Novartis, Oxford outcomes, PCM scientific, Shire and Viforpharma. He received conference support or speaker’s fee by Janssen McNeil, Lilly, Medice, Novartis and Shire. He is/has been involved in clinical trials conducted by Shire & Viforpharma. The present work is unrelated to the above grants and relationships.

Katya Rubia received speaker’s fees form Shire, Medice and a grant from Lilly for another project.

Jan Haavik has received speaker fees from Lilly, Novartis and Janssen Cilag.

Steve Faraone has received income, travel expenses and/or research support from, and/or has been on an Advisory Board for, and/or participated in continuing medical education programs sponsored by: Pfizer, Ironshore, Shire, Akili Interactive Labs, CogCubed, Alcobra, VAYA Pharma, Neurovance, Impax, NeuroLifeSciences, Otsuka, McNeil, Janssen, Novartis, Eli Lilly and the NIH. With his institution, he has US patent US20130217707 A1 for the use of sodium-hydrogen exchange inhibitors in the treatment of ADHD. He receives royalties from books published by Guilford Press: Straight Talk about Your Child’s Mental Health; Oxford University Press: Schizophrenia: The Facts; Elsevier, ADHD: Non-Pharmacologic Treatments

Joseph Biederman is currently receiving research support from the following sources: The Department of Defense, Food & Drug Administration, Ironshore, Lundbeck, Magceutics Inc., Merck, PamLab, Pfizer, Shire Pharmaceuticals Inc., SPRITES, Sunovion, Vaya Pharma/Enzymotec, and NIH. In 2015, Dr. Joseph Biederman received honoraria from the MGH Psychiatry Academy for tuition-funded CME courses. He has a US Patent Application pending (Provisional Number #61/233,686) through MGH corporate licensing, on a method to prevent stimulant abuse. In 2014, Dr. Joseph Biederman received honoraria from the MGH Psychiatry Academy for tuition-funded CME courses. He received research support from AACAP, Alcobra, Forest Research Institute, and Shire Pharmaceuticals Inc. Dr. Biederman received departmental royalties from a copyrighted rating scale used for ADHD diagnoses, paid by Ingenix, Prophase, Shire, Bracket Global, Sunovion, and Theravance; these royalties were paid to the Department of Psychiatry at MGH.

Kerstin Konrad received speaking fees from Medice, Lilly and Shire.

Josep-Antoni Ramos was on the speakers’ bureau and/or acted as consultant for Eli-Lilly, Janssen-Cilag, Novartis, Shire, Lundbeck, Almirall and Rubió in the last 3 years. He also received travel awards (air tickets + hotel) for taking part in psychiatric meetings from Janssen-Cilag, Rubió, Shire, and Eli-Lilly. The ADHD Program chaired by him received unrestricted educational and research support from the following pharmaceutical companies in the last 3 years: Eli-Lilly, Rovi, Ferrer, Lundbeck, Shire, and Rubió.

Pieter Hoekstra received a research grant from Shire and was part of the advisory board of Shire.

Jan Buitelaar has been in the past 3 years a consultant to/member of advisory board of/and/or speaker for Janssen Cilag BV, Eli Lilly, Medice, Shire, Roche, and Servier. He is not an employee of any of these companies, and not a stock shareholder of any of these companies. He has no other financial or material support, including expert testimony, patents, royalties.

Barbara Franke received educational speaking fees from Merz and Shire.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

AUTHORS CONTRIBUTIONS

Protocol design, quality testing, and analysis: Hoogman, Bralten, Hibar, Mennes, Zwiers, Schweren, Hulzen, Medland, Shumskaya, Jahanshad, Faraone, Thompson, Franke

Data collection, processing, analysis, and/or funding: Hoogman, Bralten, Hibar, Mennes, Zwiers, Schweren, van Hulzen, Medland, Shumskaya, Jahanshad, de Zeeuw, Szekely, Sudre, Wolfers, Onnink, Dammers, Mostert, Vives-Gilabert, Kohls, Oberwelland, Seitz, Schulte-Rüther, Ambrosino di Bruttopilo, Doyle, Høvik, Dramsdahl, Tamm, van Erp, Dale, Schork, Conzelmann, Zierhut, Baur, McCarthy, Yoncheva, Cubillo, Chantiluke, Metha, Paloyelis, Hohmann, Baumeister, Bramati, Mattos, Tovar-Moll, Douglas, Banaschewski, Brandeis, Kuntsi, Asherson, Rubia, Kelly, Di Martino, Milham, Castellanos, Frodl, Zentis, Lesch, Reif, Pauli, Jernigan, Haavik, Plessen Lundervold Hugdahl, Seidman, Biederman, Rommelse, Heslenfeld, Hartman, Hoekstra, Oosterlaan, von Polier, Konrad, Vilarroya, Ramos, Soliva, Durston, Buitelaar, Faraone, Shaw, Thompson, Franke

Manuscript preparation: Hoogman, Bralten, Mennes, Zwiers, Shumskaya, Shaw, Thompson, Faraone, Franke

All authors contributed edits and approved the content of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Polanczyk G, de Lima M, Horta B, Biederman J, Rohde L. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(6):942–8. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Faraone S, Biederman J, Mick E. The age-dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychol Med. 2006;36(2):159–65. doi: 10.1017/S003329170500471X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.(APA) APA. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 2000 4th text rev ed. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Valera EM, Faraone SV, Murray KE, Seidman LJ. Meta-analysis of structural imaging findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;61(12):1361–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Frodl T, Skokauskas N. Meta-analysis of structural MRI studies in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder indicates treatment effects. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2012;125(2):114–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01786.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Nakao T, Radua J, Rubia K, Mataix-Cols D. Gray matter volume abnormalities in ADHD: voxelbased meta-analysis exploring the effects of age and stimulant medication. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(11):1154–63. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ellison-Wright I, Ellison-Wright Z, Bullmore E. Structural brain change in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder identified by meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry. 2008;8:51. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-8-51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Norman LJ, Carlisi C, Lukito S, et al. Structural and Functional Brain Abnormalities in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: A Comparative Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73(8):815–25. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Castellanos FX, Lee PP, Sharp W, et al. Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA. 2002;288(14):1740–8. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.14.1740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hoogman M, Rijpkema M, Janss L, et al. Current self-reported symptoms of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder are associated with total brain volume in healthy adults. PLoS One. 2012;7(2):e31273. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Shaw P, Gilliam M, Liverpool M, et al. Cortical development in typically developing children with symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity: support for a dimensional view of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(2):143–51. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10030385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175–91. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Schmaal L, Veltman DJ, van Erp TG, et al. Subcortical brain alterations in major depressive disorder: findings from the ENIGMA Major Depressive Disorder working group. Mol Psychiatry. 2015 doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Halperin JM, Schulz KP. Revisiting the role of the prefrontal cortex in the pathophysiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychol Bull. 2006;132(4):560–81. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.4.560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.van Erp TG, Hibar DP, Rasmussen JM, et al. Subcortical brain volume abnormalities in 2028 individuals with schizophrenia and 2540 healthy controls via the ENIGMA consortium. Mol Psychiatry. 2015 doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analysis in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software. 2010;36(3):1–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Ruigrok AN, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Lai MC, et al. A meta-analysis of sex differences in human brain structure. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;39:34–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Sonuga-Barke E, Bitsakou P, Thompson M. Beyond the dual pathway model: evidence for the dissociation of timing, inhibitory, and delay-related impairments in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(4):345–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2009.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cha J, Fekete T, Siciliano F, et al. Neural Correlates of Aggression in Medication-Naive Children with ADHD: Multivariate Analysis of Morphometry and Tractography. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(7):1717–25. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Frodl T, Stauber J, Schaaff N, et al. Amygdala reduction in patients with ADHD compared with major depression and healthy volunteers. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2010;121(2):111–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01489.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Tajima-Pozo K, Yus M, Ruiz-Manrique G, Lewczuk A, Arrazola J, Montañes-Rada F. Amygdala Abnormalities in Adults With ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2016 doi: 10.1177/1087054716629213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Posner J, Siciliano F, Wang Z, Liu J, Sonuga-Barke E, Greenhill L. A multimodal MRI study of the hippocampus in medication-naive children with ADHD: what connects ADHD and depression? Psychiatry Res. 2014;224(2):112–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Almeida Montes L, Ricardo-Garcell J, Barajas De La Torre L, et al. Clinical correlations of grey matter reductions in the caudate nucleus of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2010;35(4):238–46. doi: 10.1503/jpn.090099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ahrendts J, Rüsch N, Wilke M, et al. Visual cortex abnormalities in adults with ADHD: a structural MRI study. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2011;12(4):260–70. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2010.518624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Guadalupe T, Zwiers MP, Teumer A, et al. Measurement and genetics of human subcortical and hippocampal asymmetries in large datasets. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(7):3277–89. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Aggleton JP. The contribution of the amygdala to normal and abnormal emotional states. Trends Neurosci. 1993;16(8):328–33. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(93)90110-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Viding E, Sebastian CL, Dadds MR, et al. Amygdala response to preattentive masked fear in children with conduct problems: the role of callous-unemotional traits. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169(10):1109–16. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12020191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Musser ED, Galloway-Long HS, Frick PJ, Nigg JT. Emotion regulation and heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(2):163–71e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.11.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Herpers PC, Rommelse NN, Bons DM, Buitelaar JK, Scheepers FE. Callous-unemotional traits as a cross-disorders construct. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2012;47(12):2045–64. doi: 10.1007/s00127-012-0513-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Shaw P, Stringaris A, Nigg J, Leibenluft E. Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171(3):276–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Conzelmann A, Mucha RF, Jacob CP, et al. Abnormal affective responsiveness in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: subtype differences. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65(7):578–85. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Burgess N, Maguire EA, O’Keefe J. The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron. 2002;35(4):625–41. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00830-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Shigemune Y, Abe N, Suzuki M, et al. Effects of emotion and reward motivation on neural correlates of episodic memory encoding: a PET study. Neurosci Res. 2010;67(1):72–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2010.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hibar DP, Westlye LT, van Erp TG, et al. Subcortical volumetric abnormalities in bipolar disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2016 doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(49):19649–54. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707741104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Bressler SL, Menon V. Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14(6):277–90. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Onnink AMH, Zwiers MP, Hoogman M, et al. Brain alterations in adult ADHD: Effects of gender, treatment and comorbid depression. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;24(3):397–409. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

supplement

RESOURCES