Abstract
PTSD is a devastating disorder, linked to profound mental, physical, occupational, and functional impairment. In addition, it is a highly complex disorder, characterized by symptom heterogeneity across multiple domains. Nevertheless, emotion dysregulation arising from exaggerated response to threat and/or inability to regulate negative emotional states plays a defining role in the pathophysiology of PTSD. In order to improve our understanding of how emotion dysregulation manifests in this illness, functional neuroimaging research over the past 20 years provides great insight into underlying neuroanatomy of each component of emotion dysregulation in the context of PTSD. While prior reviews exist on the topic of neuroimaging findings in PTSD literature, the present review synthesizes this work through the lens of emotion and its regulation. Studies that employed tasks of emotional responding and symptom provocation, implicit regulation (e.g., emotional stroop and interference), explicit regulation (e.g., cognitive reappraisal), and fear conditioning/extinction studies were reviewed. Findings demonstrate that emotion dysregulation in PTSD arises from complications within a large neurocircuitry involving the amygdala, insula, hippocampus, anterior cingulate cortex, and prefrontal cortex. Although exaggerated response in the amygdala and insula to negative emotional triggers is pervasive, PTSD is also marked by deficient appraisal, resolution, and management of negative emotional states sub-served by the ACC and PFC during regulation. Together, this further supports the importance of studying emotion regulation deficits in tandem to exaggerated symptom provocation in order to better understand the constellation of symptoms present in those with PTSD.
Keywords: PTSD, fMRI, neuroimaging, emotion, emotion regulation, review
1. Introduction
Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) is a devastating disorder, linked to profound mental, physical, occupational, and functional impairment.1 In addition, it is a highly complex disorder, characterized by symptom heterogeneity spanning the avoidance of trauma-related material, emotional blunting and distancing, hyper-vigilance, hyper-arousal, and persistent negative alterations in cognition and mood.2 Although symptoms suggest a constellation of disturbances, emotion dysregulation is considered a core component of the disorder.3–5 In particular, emotion dysregulation is thought to give rise to the presence of hypervigilance and attentional biases, enhanced startle response, hyper-arousal, emotional numbing, irritability, enhanced memories for traumatic events, difficulty in discriminating danger vs. safety, generalization of fear, and avoidance of emotional material or trauma reminders.4 Given its relation to a wide array of signature symptoms, the investigation into how emotion dysregulation manifests itself is integral to the study of PTSD.
Yet, emotion dysregulation as a symptom is decidedly complex and may be defined by exaggerated emotional reactivity based on atypical “bottom-up” detection or appraisal of emotional triggers.6 In contrast, emotion dysregulation may arise due to deficiency in “top-down” control of emotional response.6 Further, deficiency in regulation may occur either implicitly (e.g., unconsciously) or explicitly (e.g., consciously), with each form of regulation relying on distinct cognitive processes.7 For instance, deficits in implicit regulation result from impairment in the ability to unconsciously shift attention during an emotional experience that in turn modulates a response, while deficits in explicit regulation occur when trying to consciously change an emotional experience using cognitive strategies. While related, these sub-processes (e.g., emotional reactivity, implicit and explicit regulation attempt) are distinct, and emotion dysregulation may arise from complications within one or many of these processes. From the perspective of subjective experience or clinical observation, however, it is difficult to identify and isolate dysfunction specific to these domains.
Over the past 20 years, functional neuroimaging studies have provided the ability to examine brain functioning in vivo during tasks of emotion processing and implicit/explicit regulation. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has gained momentum over the last two decades as a neuroimaging technique as a way to non-invasively assess changes in metabolic activity in the brain in patients with PTSD by which to assess its clinical neuropathophysiology. Presently, a large number of fMRI studies have been published on the topic of emotion-specific brain dysfunction in those with PTSD and reviews of this literature are plentiful.3,4,8–32 However, despite substantial literature on this topic, findings are synthesized specific to symptom provocation or fear conditioning, largely ignoring the study of how neural substrates during emotion regulation may be impaired. Other reviews at the intersection of emotion and cognition exist, but do not make the study of brain changes during emotion regulation a focus of their respective examinations.33–38 Thus, there has yet to be a thorough review of fMRI findings that sufficiently explores ways in which emotion processing and its regulation are altered at the neural level in those with PTSD.
As such, the intent of this review was to survey fMRI studies through the dual lens of emotion and its regulation. Studies are summarized as they pertain to threat and emotion processing versus those that examine regulation of these processes at implicit and explicit levels. In addition, studies on fear conditioning and extinction are summarized as learning about and abolishing a fear response is highly relevant to the topic of controlling emotional states. Given the emphasis on the neural underpinnings of these processes, relevant neuroanatomy implicated within each study paradigm is also reviewed.
2. Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Emotion Processing
Most fMRI studies involving those with PTSD utilize tasks of passive emotion processing in the form of emotional faces that convey threat (e.g., angry, fearful), aversive imagery (e.g., mutilated bodies, images of violence), and trauma-specific cues, such as words, pictures, or autobiographical scripts. In healthy individuals, exposure to negative content activates the amygdala, insula, hippocampus, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC), predominantly.39 The amygdala, a dense set of nuclei within the medial and anterior portion of the temporal lobe40, is involved in processing motivationally-salient stimuli.41–43 While the amygdala is engaged in response to negative triggers and studied most commonly in this context, it is also activated in response to positive stimuli, such as happy faces44, and stimuli that are novel.45 In conjunction, the amygdala plays an important role in the consolidation of information from other brain regions during an emotional experience. These other regions include the insula, a portion of the cortex that is folded within lateral sulcus and responsible for the interoceptive awareness of an emotional state46,47, and the hippocampus, situated within the temporal cortices that is involved in contextual learning and memory. Although the hippocampus and the parahippocampal gyrus, a region that directly surrounds the hippocampus and which is also instrumental for learning and memory, are traditionally studied in their role of encoding spatial information, events, facts, and autobiographical information48, these regions are densely connected with the amygdala and send information regarding the probability that a stimulus is associated with danger based on prior experiences. The ACC, cortical tissue that surrounds the corpus callosum, is also active during appraisal of the emotional stimulus but is involved in inhibiting and controlling motor functioning associated with an emotional response.40 Finally, the VMPFC, defined as cortical tissue located below the genu of the corpus callosum along the midline, is involved in emotional appraisal as well as linking this experience with a physiological response.49
2.1 Amygdala
In comparison to non-traumatized and traumatized controls, individuals with PTSD exhibit greater activation within the amygdala.50–62 Although one study by Felmingham and colleagues did not find evidence of significant amygdala activation in response to negative images in a group of individuals with PTSD prior to treatment,63 no comparison group was used in this study, making the lack of a significant amygdala effect difficult to interpret.63 Although studies demonstrate that greater amygdala engagement occurs in PTSD compared to traumatized controls, individuals with a trauma history also exhibit elevated amygdala response in comparison to healthy, non-traumatized controls.58 Therefore, elevated amygdala response to negative triggers may be a result of trauma, or a pre-trauma feature. Much of the work that has found hyperactive amygdala response utilizes fearful or angry faces as probe to evoke threat53,57–60, although elevated amygdala response occurs when individuals with PTSD are shown aversive images or cues intended to evoke autobiographical memory of the trauma54,56,61 or when asked to recall a traumatic memory.62 In addition, greater amygdala response is evident in those with PTSD when masked stimuli probes are used, as in the case of masked fearful faces51,64–67 and stimuli that are inherently neutral (e.g., Chinese ideographs) but which were subconsciously primed with a masked negative face.68 Thus, response of the amygdala to threat in those with PTSD is a robust finding, and does not depend on stimuli type or input from other brain regions that provide conscious perception of an emotional trigger.69
Although elevated amygdala response to negative stimuli is a consistent feature of PTSD, the implications of this characteristic in terms of subjective and behavioral response (e.g., feeling more negative; producing a physiological response) is not as clear. For instance, the vast majority of studies do not link elevated amygdala response with a behavioral/physical output50–54,56,58–60,63, and while some studies found that individuals with PTSD rate images and faces as more negative55,57, other work fails to find group differences in self-reported feelings of physiological response (e.g., heart pounding).61 Therefore, elevated amygdala response in those with PTSD does not necessarily translate to increased experience of negative affect70, although more work is needed in this domain.
Finally, although studied to a lesser extent, individuals with PTSD also display greater amygdala response to neutral faces compared to non-traumatized controls71 and reduced amygdala response to happy faces compared to traumatized controls.72 This latter finding correlates with symptoms of emotional numbing and reduced engagement of the ventral striatum, a region instrumental for reward processing.72 These results suggest the possibility that PTSD is characterized by more pronounced deficits in salience detection that is not specific to negative content.
2.2 Insula
During exposure to negative faces53,58,59, trauma-specific imagery73 and anticipation of negative images74,75, individuals with PTSD exhibit greater activation in the insula compared to trauma-exposed and healthy controls. In addition, greater insula response occurs in those with PTSD in response to masked negative faces65 and neutral symbols that have been unconsciously paired with negative faces68, suggesting that regions involved in interoceptive awareness of emotional state are active without ability to consciously perceive. The insula is also over-engaged in those with PTSD during a relaxation period when individuals were previously told to re-imagine their trauma76, suggesting that individuals with PTSD may be incapable of disengaging from feelings spurred by their trauma when instructed to.76 The presence of exaggerated insula response during exposure to negative stimuli has been specifically tied to symptoms of re-experiencing73, hyper-arousal75 and the occurrence of flashbacks.77 Therefore, the extent to which individuals with PTSD re-experience physical reminders of their traumatic experience may be directly tied to over-active insula engagement during emotion processing.
2.3. Hippocampus
In terms of the state of hippocampal/parahippocampal gyrus findings in PTSD, several studies find evidence of enhanced engagement in PTSD during exposure to trauma-specific images.78–81 As individuals with PTSD experience trauma-specific images as more personally relevant36, over-engagement of regions responsible for encoding autobiographical information is not altogether surprising. However, the finding of enhanced hippocampal/parahippocampal activation in PTSD extends to studies utilizing stimuli that are not intended to serve as reminders of trauma, such as generally aversive images50,82, emotional faces83, and negative words.84 To note, over-engagement of the hippocampus in PTSD is not present across all emotional stimuli types, as there is less engagement of the hippocampus in response to positive images.85 Therefore, individuals with PTSD may over-generalize their personal relevance to negative stimuli in particular.
In several instances, greater hippocampal engagement during exposure to negative stimuli50,83 and trauma-specific cues61 correlates with greater amygdala engagement in those with PTSD. In some instances co-activation of the hippocampus and amygdala when individuals are exposed to negative images is related to accurately recalling this content at a later time.50 Therefore, salience detection encoded by the amygdala alongside memory formation may therefore help individuals remember emotional content. In contrast, other work finds that increased hippocampal engagement during the encoding of negative words is related to the occurrence of false positives (e.g., falsely stating that a subject has encountered a stimulus before84). Taken together, this suggests that greater hippocampal engagement during encoding of negative content may be related to better recall, but this phenomenon may be over-generalized. In addition, other work finds that greater hippocampal/parahippocampal engagement during the encoding of trauma-specific images is related to worse memory performance, qualified by inability to correctly recall seeing this content before.85 Discrepancy across studies in terms of whether enhanced hippocampal engagement boosts or hinders performance mirrors results from behavioral studies, which demonstrate that PTSD patients possess an advantage in remembering emotional content compared to healthy peers, but that memory distortions are prevalent.87–90 Therefore, memory disturbances in PTSD may be best qualified as enhanced, but over-generalized, alongside a failure to recall this material if related to the trauma. The clinical significance of enhanced hippocampal functioning and altered memory accuracy may be central to understanding the existence of the over-generalization of fear as a central feature of PTSD pathophysiology.91
2.4 Anterior Cingulate Cortex
Although diverse in its functioning, the ACC is involved broadly in the integration of information from multiple sources to assign value and resolve conflict, along with motoric response to inhibit or engage in a behavioral response.92 Specifically the ACC is involved in resolving the extent to which threat is attended to, based on the integration of external and internal clues.92,93 Further, the ACC helps modulate the extent to which the amygdala is engaged based on updating the likelihood of threat.92,94,95 The most widely-recognized sub-division of the ACC involves dichotomizing the region along a dorsal (dACC) and ventral (vACC) boundary.93 The vACC includes the subgenual ACC (sgACC) and rostral ACC (rACC) and is involved in detection of emotion30,96 based on its strong connections with the amygdala.97 The dACC, in contrast, is involved in conflict control, response selection, and error detection92,93 and forms little direct connection to the amygdala.97
During emotion processing, several studies find that individuals with PTSD under-engage the ACC57,58,60,73,78,79,98, although some of this work did not localize deficits to dorsal or ventral sub-regions. Nevertheless, under-engagement of the vACC57 and dACC58 or under-engagement of both regions in the same study are reported60,73,78,79,98, suggesting that deficits within the ACC are pervasive. As a consequence, both direct emotional appraisal (sub-served by vACC) and conflict resolution of emotional states (sub-served by dACC) are likely to be altered in PTSD. In addition, deficits in engaging the vACC and dACC is evident across studies of negative face processing57,58,60 and trauma-specific imagery73,78,79,98, suggesting that altered appraisal of negative emotion is not specific to any one type of negative stimulus.
2.5 Prefrontal Cortex
Unlike the amygdala, insula, hippocampus, and ACC – which suggest uniform direction of aberrant activation in PTSD (e.g., either over- or under-engaged) – there is discrepancy across studies regarding the PFC. A heterogeneous structure, the PFC contains functional sub-divisions based on dorsal/ventral and lateral/medial locations, partitioning the structure into dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) regions. In general, medial portions of the cortex are active in response to emotional triggers. For instance, activation in the DMPFC occurs during exposure to negative content99,100 and may be generally involved in evaluation of one’s own emotional experience.101,102 Individuals exhibit activation in DMPFC during self-focused regulation of emotion, theorized to play an important role in emotional self-monitoring as it serves emotion regulation goals.103 In contrast, the VMPFC forms heavy reciprocal connections with the amygdala and other sub-cortical structures as well as with the lateral cortex; therefore, function of the VMPFC is viewed as a relay-station for “bottom-up” information from limbic and sub-cortical structures signaling emotion detection, and lateral PFC signaling response selection and control.104 Owing to dense projections with the amygdala, the VMPFC is involved more so in the implicit, automatic regulation of emotion.30,96
During processing of fearful faces and script-driven imagery, several studies find that individuals with PTSD under-engage the DMPFC57,60,76 and VMPFC.57,62,66,98 Deficiency in recruiting the midline PFC suggests that emotional appraisal and automatic emotion regulation are both compromised in PTSD during passive viewing. Some studies have found that under-engagement of both regions is related to greater amygdala engagement57, suggesting that failure to engage these regions may be directly related to hyper-reactivity of the amygdala. In contrast, other work has found increased engagement of the VMPFC60, DMPFC105 and MPFC generally, without specification as to dorsal versus ventral boundaries53,59,73,106 and in response to negative faces and script-driven imagery. In addition, VMPFC activation in PTSD is positively related to amygdala reactivity in response to emotional faces and greater symptom severity.107 Therefore, over-engagement of the MPFC also appears linked to greater amygdala response in those with PTSD, making the role of the MPFC as it relates to PTSD illness unclear. As many studies involving those with PTSD do not qualify specific sub-regions that are altered (e.g., DMPFC v. VMPFC) and given the diversity of functions within this region, we conclude that more work is needed to fully qualify the nature of altered MPFC engagement in response to negative stimuli in this population.
3. Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Emotion Regulation
Exaggerated threat detection and negative emotional responding, as outlined above, may be borne from “bottom-up” aberrations specific to aberrant appraisal of emotion. Alternatively, they may accompany and/or result from altered “top-down” regulation of affect that occurs through attentional control and emotional coping strategies. As the neurocircuitry that underlies unconscious and conscious regulation of negative affect is distinct from appraisal of emotion, it deserves closer inspection as it relates to the pathophysiology of PTSD.
Unlike tasks of passive emotion processing, in which participants are shown emotional content without instruction to cognitively engage in this material, tasks of implicit emotion regulation test capacity for completing a cognitive task in light of emotional triggers. Individuals’ ability to accurately and/or rapidly complete the cognitive task in light of these emotional “distractors” that are irrelevant for cognitive performance, or changes in engagement of cortical regions in this process, provides clues as to the strength of emotion regulation capacity. Tasks of emotional interference most commonly include stroop tasks, in which participants are asked to carry out an information-processing task that contains both congruent and incongruent trials. For example, in a stroop task in which individuals are asked to identify the quantity of written numbers displayed on a screen, congruent trials are those without conflict between the numbers as written and quantity listed (e.g., two “2’s). On incongruent trials, a mismatch is present, such as three “4’s”. Emotional distractors are used throughout the task, just before and after trials in most cases. In another version of implicit emotion regulation, individuals are asked to make a judgment about a non-emotional stimulus, such as whether two houses are the same or different, while emotional distractors are presented before or after trials or within the background. Finally, some studies employ attention bias paradigms, which show participants a set of stimuli, one aversive and one neutral. Immediately following the display of these stimuli, one image is replaced by a dot-probe and the latency towards directing attention to this probe provides a measurement of attentional bias towards the preceding emotion.108 In most cases, negative faces are used as emotional triggers and greater attentional bias towards the dot-probe that replaces these faces indicates impaired regulation of negative affective states.
In contrast to studies of implicit emotion regulation are those of explicit emotion regulation, defined as when individual consciously and with intention attempt to alter their emotional reactions using cognitive control strategies. Multiple volitional strategies exist for regulating emotional experiences, such as distancing, distraction, and suppression. By far, the most widely-used technique to study explicit regulation is cognitive reappraisal, a strategy used to re-interpret the meaning of an emotional stimulus to alter one’s own affective experience.109 For example, cognitive reappraisal is used when individuals re-evaluate the scene of women crying outside a church as one that depicts tears of joy at a wedding, rather than sorrow at a funeral.
In healthy individuals during implicit regulation, numerous regions of the PFC are engaged including VMPFC, DMPFC, VLPFC, and DLPFC in addition to the ACC110,111, while explicit regulation relies on engagement of these regions alongside middle temporal, superior temporal, and parietal cortices.112–115 In particular, both tasks of implicit and explicit regulation rely on functioning of the DLPFC and VLPFC, which differ from medial portions of the PFC that are heavily involved in emotional appraisal. In contrast, the VLPFC is attributed to motor inhibition116, spatial attention and re-orienting attention to objects117–119, semantic processing120, categorization of objects118,119, and memory for semantic information.121–123 During emotion processing, the VLPFC is involved in the generation of inner speech124–126, which helps individuals categorize emotions for appraisal and reappraisal.113 Functionally, the DLPFC forms numerous connections with other brain regions, predominantly with sensory cortices and regions involved in the integration of sensory information, such as the inferior parietal lobe.127 The DLPFC does not form direct connections with sub-cortical regions involved in emotional response, such as the amygdala, and must receive this information through the involvement of intermediate brain regions, predominantly the VMPFC and ACC.128–130 The DLPFC is involved in executive functioning broadly-defined, including working memory131–133, decision making134, attentional control135, and response selection.136 Given these functions, DLPFC’s role may be described as the active generation of strategies in order to execute goal-directed behavior, including emotion regulation.118,119
3.1. Prefrontal Cortex
In individuals with PTSD undergoing tasks of implicit regulation, the direction of aberrations within the PFC are mixed and discrepancies across studies may depend on the location of aberration within the cortex, as well as the type of emotional distractor used. For instance, during a stroop task in which participants indicated whether the quantity of numbers displayed on a screen was congruent to the number as written (e.g., two “2’s”) while negative emotional images were shown prior to each trial, individuals with PTSD exhibited less engagement of the VLPFC in comparison to traumatized and healthy controls.137 During incongruent trials (e.g., when the quantity of numbers did not match the number displayed), individuals with PTSD displayed less engagement of the DLPFC in comparison to traumatized controls.137 As trauma-exposed individuals engaged the DLPFC to a greater extent compared to healthy controls, over-engagement of this region during difficult trials may reflect compensatory functioning that is not necessary in the absence of a trauma history.137 Less engagement of the VLPFC and greater engagement of the VMPFC also corresponds to greater PTSD symptom severity when participants are instructed to press a button in response to a salient stimulus while also viewing negative emotional images.138 Less VLPFC and greater VMPFC activation also corresponded to greater amygdala engagement in this task138, suggesting that exaggerated VMPFC response does not translate to successful regulation. Therefore, in those with PTSD there is less reliance on lateral PFC regions known to be involved in regulation, alongside greater engagement of VMPFC that is likely recruited for emotional appraisal.138
In contrast, other work finds that PTSD symptom severity positively relates to engagement of the VLPFC and VMPFC during an emotional interference task in which individuals were instructed to indicate via button-press the occurrence of a non-emotional ‘target’ stimulus amidst emotional distractors.139 As this study utilized emotional distractors that were trauma-specific, discrepancy in terms of whether the PFC is over- versus under-engaged during implicit regulation may depend stimulus type. Over-engagement in this case may signal the need for compensatory functioning when emotional distractors are personally relevant.
In terms of exploring altered engagement of the PFC during explicit regulation of negative affect in those with PFC, limited studies have been completed in this domain. First, New and colleagues demonstrated that individuals with PTSD were less effective at reappraisal in reducing negative affect (as evidenced by subjective ratings) and exhibited reduced engagement of the DMPFC and lateral PFC compared to healthy controls when using reappraisal to make negative images appear less negative, although this pattern was also observed in traumatized controls.140 Subsequently, Rabinak and colleagues found focal deficits in the DLPFC in those with PTSD, again when using reappraisal to make images appear less negative.141
3.2. Anterior Cingulate Cortex
During an implicit regulation same-different task in which individuals are instructed to make a judgment about a non-emotional scene when emotional distractors were used, and when individuals are participating in a stroop task in which participants viewed faces that matched a superimposed emotional word, participants with PTSD exhibit less activation in the vACC.142,143 Less engagement of the vACC is correlated with avoidance symptoms142 and re-experiencing symptoms143, suggesting that vACC deficiency during implicit regulation is linked to more than one symptom domain.
With regard to the dACC, greater symptom severity is related to more differential activation in the dACC during trials in which emotional versus non-emotional distractors were used during target-detection (e.g., colored shape indicating need for button press).138 In other words, engaging the dACC preferentially more during emotional distraction as opposed to non-emotional distraction suggests increased monitoring of potential conflict in those with more symptoms. PTSD may therefore be associated with exaggerated need for conflict monitoring during cognitive control when emotion is present. Other work has found that individuals with PTSD, compared to TECs, exhibit greater dACC activation during emotional counting stroop task when trauma-specific words were used144 and when emotional distractors were used in the form of images.145 However, there is also evidence that PTSD symptom severity is negatively associated with the dACC while positively related with the vACC during an attentional control task when distractors were trauma-specific.139 Less activation in the dACC is also associated with greater threat bias in those with PTSD146, suggesting that greater orienting to threat occurs when findings are reversed (e.g., less dACC/greater vACC). Therefore, greater dACC and less vACC responding during implicit regulation that is more evident in the PTSD literature may signal compensatory engagement necessary for the functional resolution of emotional conflict.
4. Studies of Fear Conditioning and Extinction
Related to the topic of symptom provocation and regulation is the process by which fear is conditioned and subsequently extinguished, studied using classical (Pavlovian) fear conditioning paradigms that allow for the study associating environmental cues with aversive events. Neural disruptions in both processes (e.g., conditioning and extinction) are pertinent to PTSD as this disorder is commonly associated with the over-learning of a fear response and difficulty in extinction, suggesting that fear is readily acquired and difficult to extinguish.147 During classical fear conditioning, a neutral stimulus (e.g., conditioned stimulus/CS, a light) is paired with an aversive stimulus (e.g., unconditioned stimulus/US, electrical shock) and a fear response based on associative learning develops to the neutral stimulus.148 This fear response, similar to passive emotion processing, is associated with engagement of the amygdala, insula, and ACC in humans.149 Related, fear generalization following conditioning occurs when stimuli other than the CS, but which share similarities with the CS, elicits a fear-related response.150 In healthy individuals, amygdala engagement is increased during fear conditioning and decreases over time during extinction, suggesting proper “learning” of a fear response and ability to implicitly regulate this fear response once the stimulus no longer signals threat.
Similar to implicit regulation, fear extinction occurs unconsciously, during which time a fear response is regulated as individuals re-learn to identify a CS as safe.148,151 Fear extinction recall is also used, in which individuals are brought back into the laboratory to test retention of extinction learning.152,153 This process studies the extent to which memory systems that encoded safety and accompanying control of fear are intact. Fear extinction and its retention are thought to be instantiated by a discrete group of regions including the hippocampus, amygdala, VMPFC, and vACC.152,154 In particular, the hippocampus is both necessary for activation of remembered fear based on exposure to a stimulus previously, and for re-learning contingencies about the potential for danger based on contextual encoding. In contrast, the VMPFC and vACC are essential for fear extinction and play an important role in retention of an extinguished response over time.148
The possibility of atypical neural functioning during fear conditioning and extinction in PTSD is likely given behavioral and physiological research. For instance, individuals with PTSD display elevated sweat response during threat of shock and this response remains elevated when individuals are shown the CS-, indicating safety.155 Enhanced sweat response also occurs during extinction, suggesting impaired implicit regulation of a learned fear response.155 In addition, there is evidence that individuals with PTSD self-report expecting to be shocked to a greater extent than controls (e.g., abnormal US expectancy in relation to the CS), indicating underlying deficits in differentiating threat from safety.156
4.1. Amygdala
Despite evidence of altered response to fear conditioning and extinction using peripheral and self-report measures, evidence of elevated amygdala response during this process is mixed.157–159 For instance, some work shows less engagement of the amygdala during conditioning when a negative image is treated as the CS in comparison to healthy and traumatized peers.160 Other work shows that amygdala activation does not differentiate between stimuli that have been conditioned to be aversive (CS+) and those that have been conditioned to signal safety (CS-), while amygdala activation does show differentiation in traumatized controls.105 Finally, others have found that individuals with PTSD exhibit greater engagement of the amygdala in response to the US (e.g., the shock) outside the conditioning process.161 Therefore, underlying aberrations in amygdala engagement in response to aversive stimuli may make it difficult to isolate elevated response in this region during the conditioning process. In addition, there may be differences in direction of effects (e.g., hyper versus hypo response of the amygdala) based on differences in aversive stimuli type (shock versus negative imagery), with greater engagement of the amygdala in response to shock, and less engagement of the amygdala in response to negative images.
During extinction, individuals with PTSD display greater amygdala engagement.158 Elevated amygdala response is also evident in PTSD during extinction recall when extinguished stimuli are presented 24-hours after conditioning.157 In contrast, other work has found no differences in amygdala response during extinction between PTSD and traumatized controls.159 Again, discrepant findings may be due to underlying differences in amygdala engagement that is changed in the conditioning process, as even more work demonstrates hypo-activation of the amygdala during fear renewal (e.g., when the CS+ is shown again after extinction) in those with PTSD.157 That is, PTSD is associated with atypical response of the amygdala across all phases of fear conditioning, extinction, and recall.
4.2. Hippocampus
Consistent with the finding that individuals with PTSD exhibit greater hippocampal activation during emotion processing, greater engagement of the hippocampus is also evident during exposure to the US.161 During extinction, however, individuals with PTSD do not exhibit significant engagement of the hippocampus, while trauma-exposed controls do, suggesting that individuals with PTSD failed to contextualize safety cues.158 In another study, individuals with PTSD exhibited less activation in the hippocampus during fear renewal.157 Less activation within the hippocampus coincided with less activation within the amygdala in this same study, suggesting deficiency in the detection of salient stimuli that may be driven by inability to recall that this stimuli was previously associated with danger.157
4.3. Anterior Cingulate Cortex
During fear conditioning and fear extinction, individuals with PTSD display elevated dACC engagement.158,159,161,162 Elevated recruitment of a region responsible for error detection and monitoring when forming a fear response suggests greater attention paid to conflict resolution in this process. Further, as elevated dACC is evident in both conditioning and extinction in those with PTSD, accurate dissociation between real versus imagined threat that occurs during conditioning may carry over and lead to deficiency in regulation during extinction.
Other work, however, finds decreased engagement of the ACC in response to trauma-specific images used as the US in individuals with PTSD during conditioning without specification as to location of this finding (e.g., dorsal versus ventral distinction).160 In this study, decreased engagement of the ACC in PTSD in comparison to healthy controls did not carry over into extinction, while patients in this sample possessed significant dissociation symptoms.160 Therefore, decreased functioning of the ACC during conditioning in response to trauma-specific content may relate to the presence of specific symptoms or be driven by the use of trauma-specific cues.
4.4. Prefrontal Cortex
During extinction, individuals with PTSD display less engagement of the VMPFC158,162 and DLPFC.160 In addition, reduced engagement in VMPFC during extinction is associated with greater fear-potentiated startle response in those with PTSD when conducting response inhibition outside the scanner in a Stop-Go task using stimuli that are not affective.163 That is, deficits during inhibition of affective stimuli may be related to inability to inhibit motoric responses to non-affective stimuli as well.
In contrast, other work has found greater activation in these same regions that was also associated with greater generalization of a CS.91 In this study, there was more activation within the VMPFC and DLPFC in individuals with PTSD as a novel stimulus dynamically changed to resemble the original CS+ in characteristics (e.g., shape and size) to a greater extent.91 Although more activation in the VMPFC and DLPFC was evident in trauma-exposed controls and patients with sub-threshold PTSD as well, the correlation between how similar each stimulus was rated and fMRI activation in the VMPFC and DLPFC regions was strongest for those with PTSD.91 Therefore, while most work suggests that individuals with PTSD possess deficient recruitment of the VMPFC and DLPFC during fear conditioning, these regions may be involved with identifying whether a stimulus predicts threat or safety based on stimulus composition. Under-engagement in the VMPFC and DLPFC during fear conditioning in those with PTSD may thus be driven by underlying deficits in capacity to correctly identify stimulus characteristics in order to accurately anticipate threat.
5. Conclusions
Emotion dysregulation is not a unitary construct, nor do all of its forms share the same neurocircuitry.6 Nevertheless, the importance of emotion dysregulation as an underlying mechanism for PTSD psychopathology is gaining traction.164–166 This is particularly salient and timely given that emotion regulatory function and dysfunction cuts across multiple disorders167 and may explain the heterogeneity of symptoms in PTSD (e.g., intrusive reminders, emotional numbing, avoidance, negative cognitions and mood, hyperarousal). As such, this review provided an overview of aberrant brain functioning in the contexts of negative emotional responding and its regulation. In surveying the available literature on these topics, we draw conclusions regarding the nature of underlying neural perturbations that may be responsible for the manifestation of emotion dysregulation as a central feature of PTSD.
First and in the case of emotion processing, findings from this review confirm the presence of a hyper-active amygdala and insula in response to stimuli that convey social threat (e.g., faces), aversive images, and cues that evoke personal trauma in those with PTSD. Without specificity in terms of which types of stimuli produce over-activation in these regions is telling, suggesting that those with PTSD possess uniform exaggerated salience detection and – as a consequence – greater interoceptive awareness of negative emotional states. Elevated amygdala activation in response to stimuli that are innocuous, such as neutral faces, suggests that the processing of motivational-salience is fundamentally altered in those with PTSD. Second, greater hippocampal engagement in response to general negative content implies that learned responses to threat may be over-generalized. In addition, under-engagement of the ACC during negative emotion processing suggests deficiency in error detection, perhaps as it relates to conflict resolution in terms of whether emotional states should be inhibited based on probability of true threat. Finally, altered engagement of the MPFC responsible for appraisal of this experience and control of arousal may explain the dynamic fluctuations and variability of fear and negative emotion symptoms observed in PTSD patients.
These conclusions are consistent with those from prior published reviews on the topic of the neurobiology of PTSD in the context of negative emotion processing4,11,14,20,29,36; however, we also offer new insights particular to brain functioning in the context of regulation. Primarily, this review provides a more in-depth review on the functioning of the PFC and ACC during tasks of implicit and explicit emotion regulation that prior syntheses of the literature have not made their primary interest. We conclude that that PTSD is also characterized by under-engagement of the VLPFC, DLPFC, DMPFC, and DLPFC across both implicit and explicit regulation. Deficiency in more than one region of the PFC suggests that multiple domains of cognitive control are compromised in this process, with deficits likely spanning attention, working memory, semantic processing, and organization and selection of strategies for regulation.117–120 Further, deficiency in recruiting overlapping cortical regions during tasks of implicit and explicit emotion regulation also suggests that aberrations are similar when regulation goals are made more or less obvious. In contrast, over-engagement of the VMPFC and vACC during implicit regulation suggests greater appraisal of emotional states when individuals are unconsciously trying to regulate, while aberrant engagement of the dACC suggests disturbances in filtering out task irrelevant emotional information and/or choosing which stimuli to respond to in light of competing cognitive demands and emotional triggers. Together, this provides significant evidence of deficient utilization of brain regions responsible for the complex act of resolving and managing emotional states in PTSD.
Evidence from studies of fear conditioning and extinction provide more evidence that PTSD is not wholly characterized by over-reactive response to fear, but rather that distinguishing threat versus safety is primarily impaired. Abnormal engagement of the amygdala is a consistent finding, but the direction of aberrations across the conditioning, extinction, and recall is mixed, suggesting confusion in deciphering which stimuli are motivationally relevant.150,168 Atypical engagement of the hippocampus along with over-engagement of the dACC during conditioning suggests deficient ability contextualizing a fear response, alongside greater monitoring and resolution of the potential for harm. Under-engagement of the PFC during extinction, a form of implicit regulation, is also evident and may be related to complications in emotion regulation stemming from inability to discriminate threat from safety.
To note, many nuances of PTSD as a complex disorder make understanding its associated neurobiology difficult. At this forefront is the fact that PTSD is characterized by heterogeneity of symptoms. Therefore, variability in findings between studies may signal that a “one-size-fits-all” approach may not be applicable to the study of altered neural networks in PTSD. Similarly, the impact of a single traumatic event versus constant trauma exposure on brain functioning is not fully clear, and the impact of multiple traumatic events over a lifetime and/or early life trauma may have differential consequences for brain functioning. Nevertheless, the corpus of studies reviewed here suggest that threat and negative emotion processing and emotion regulation deficits at the neural level accurately classify most cases of PTSD. Altered engagement within the amygdala, insula, hippocampus, ACC, and PFC demonstrate that emotion dysregulation in PTSD arises from complications across a large neurocircuitry. In addition, although PTSD is characterized by increased detection of negative emotion, we emphasize that this principle disturbance exists alongside deficiency in the appraisal, resolution, and management of negative emotional states. Future research should aim at delineating whether certain brain circuits may reflect particular symptom clusters of PTSD to help refine the nosology of the illness.169–172 Moreover, a more complete and precise understanding of the pathophysiology of PTSD is needed to test if existing and novel therapies can effectively target emotion dysregulation and its neural substrates.173–175
References
- 1.Kessler RC. Posttraumatic stress disorder: the burden to the individual and to society. J Clin Psychiatry. 2000;61(Suppl 5):4–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th. Arlington, VA: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Etkin A, Wager TD. Functional Neuroimaging of Anxiety: A Meta-Analysis of Emotional Processing in PTSD, Social Anxiety Disorder, and Specific Phobia. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(10):1476–1488. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Frewen PA. Toward a Psychobiology of Posttraumatic Self-Dysregulation: Reexperiencing, Hyperarousal, Dissociation, and Emotional Numbing. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1071(1):110–124. doi: 10.1196/annals.1364.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Seligowski AV, Lee DJ, Bardeen JR, Orcutt HK. Emotion Regulation and Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms: A Meta-Analysis. Cogn Behav Ther. 2015;44(2):87–102. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2014.980753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Etkin A, Büchel C, Gross JJ. The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(11):693–700. doi: 10.1038/nrn4044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gyurak A, Gross JJ, Etkin A. Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: A dual-process framework. Cogn Emot. 2011;25(3):400–412. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.544160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ahearn EP, Juergens T, Smith T, Krahn D, Kalin N. Fear, anxiety, and the neuroimaging of PTSD. Psychopharmacol Bull. 2007;40(3):88–103. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bremner JD. Neuroimaging in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Other Stress-Related Disorders. Neuroimaging Clin N Am. 2007;17(4):523–538. doi: 10.1016/j.nic.2007.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Fragkaki I, Thomaes K, Sijbrandij M. Posttraumatic stress disorder under ongoing threat: a review of neurobiological and neuroendocrine findings. Eur J Psychotraumatology. 2016;7(1):30915. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v7.30915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Francati V, Vermetten E, Bremner JD. Functional neuroimaging studies in posttraumatic stress disorder: review of current methods and findings. Depress Anxiety. 2007;24(3):202–218. doi: 10.1002/da.20208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Fredrikson M, Faria V. Neuroimaging in anxiety disorders. Mod Trends Pharmacopsychiatry. 2013;29:47–66. doi: 10.1159/000351938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Garfinkel SN, Liberzon I. Neurobiology of PTSD: A Review of Neuroimaging Findings. Psychiatr Ann. 2009;39(6):370–381. doi: 10.3928/00485713-20090527-01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hughes KC, Shin LM. Functional neuroimaging studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. Expert Rev Neurother. 2011;11(2):275–285. doi: 10.1586/ern.10.198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hull AM. Neuroimaging findings in post-traumatic stress disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2002;181(2):102–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Knight DC, Smith CN, Cheng DT, Stein EA, Helmstetter FJ. Amygdala and hippocampal activity during acquisition and extinction of human fear conditioning. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2004;4(3):317–325. doi: 10.3758/cabn.4.3.317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lanius RA, Bluhm R, Lanius U, Pain C. A review of neuroimaging studies in PTSD: Heterogeneity of response to symptom provocation. J Psychiatr Res. 2006;40(8):709–729. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Liberzon I, Abelson JL. Context Processing and the Neurobiology of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Neuron. 2016;92(1):14–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Liberzon I, Phan KL. Brain-imaging studies of posttraumatic stress disorder. CNS Spectr. 2003;8(9):641–650. doi: 10.1017/s109285290000883x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Liberzon I, Sripada CS. Progress in Brain Research. Vol. 167. Elsevier; 2007. The functional neuroanatomy of PTSD: a critical review; pp. 151–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Liberzon I. Neuroimaging Studies of Emotional Responses in PTSD. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1071(1):87–109. doi: 10.1196/annals.1364.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Nutt DJ, Malizia AL. Structural and functional brain changes in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2004;35(Suppl 1):11–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Pitman RK, Rasmusson AM, Koenen KC, et al. Biological studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(11):769–787. doi: 10.1038/nrn3339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ramage AE, Laird AR, Eickhoff SB, et al. A coordinate-based meta-analytic model of trauma processing in posttraumatic stress disorder: Meta-Analytic Model of Trauma Processing. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34(12):3392–3399. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Rauch SL, Shin LM, Phelps EA. Neurocircuitry Models of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Extinction: Human Neuroimaging Research—Past, Present, and Future. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(4):376–382. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sartory G, Cwik J, Knuppertz H, et al. In Search of the Trauma Memory: A Meta-Analysis of Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Symptom Provocation in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) In: Soriano-Mas C, editor. PLoS ONE. 3. Vol. 8. 2013. p. e58150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Shin LM, Liberzon I. The neurocircuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):169. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Shin LM. Amygdala, Medial Prefrontal Cortex, and Hippocampal Function in PTSD. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1071(1):67–79. doi: 10.1196/annals.1364.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Shvil E, Rusch HL, Sullivan GM, Neria Y. Neural, Psychophysiological, and Behavioral Markers of Fear Processing in PTSD: A Review of the Literature. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2013;15(5) doi: 10.1007/s11920-013-0358-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.VanElzakker MB, Kathryn Dahlgren M, Caroline Davis F, Dubois S, Shin LM. From Pavlov to PTSD: The extinction of conditioned fear in rodents, humans, and anxiety disorders. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;113:3–18. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.11.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Lissek S, Powers AS, McClure EB, et al. Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Behav Res Ther. 2005;43(11):1391–1424. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Jovanovic T, Norrholm SD. Neural Mechanisms of Impaired Fear Inhibition in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Front Behav Neurosci. 2011;5 doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Brown VM, Morey RA. Neural Systems for Cognitive and Emotional Processing in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Front Psychol. 2012;3 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Delgado MR, Nearing KI, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA. Neural Circuitry Underlying the Regulation of Conditioned Fear and Its Relation to Extinction. Neuron. 2008;59(5):829–838. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Hayes JP, Hayes SM, Mikedis AM. Quantitative meta-analysis of neural activity in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2012;2(1):9. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-2-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hayes JP, VanElzakker MB, Shin LM. Emotion and cognition interactions in PTSD: a review of neurocognitive and neuroimaging studies. Front Integr Neurosci. 2012;6 doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Koenigs M, Grafman J. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: The Role of Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Amygdala. The Neuroscientist. 2009;15(5):540–548. doi: 10.1177/1073858409333072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Patel R, Spreng RN, Shin LM, Girard TA. Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and beyond: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36(9):2130–2142. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional Neuroanatomy of Emotion: A Meta-Analysis of Emotion Activation Studies in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage. 2002;16(2):331–348. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Lévêque M. Psychosurgery. New York, NY: Springer; 2014. The Neuroanatomy of Emotions; pp. 49–106. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Herman JP, Ostrander MM, Mueller NK, Figueiredo H. Limbic system mechanisms of stress regulation: Hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2005;29(8):1201–1213. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Urry HL. Amygdala and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Are Inversely Coupled during Regulation of Negative Affect and Predict the Diurnal Pattern of Cortisol Secretion among Older Adults. J Neurosci. 2006;26(16):4415–4425. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3215-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Liddell BJ, Brown KJ, Kemp AH, et al. A direct brainstem–amygdala–cortical ‘alarm’ system for subliminal signals of fear. NeuroImage. 2005;24(1):235–243. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Costafreda SG, Brammer MJ, David AS, Fu CHY. Predictors of amygdala activation during the processing of emotional stimuli: A meta-analysis of 385 PET and fMRI studies. Brain Res Rev. 2008;58(1):57–70. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Blackford JU, Buckholtz JW, Avery SN, Zald DH. A unique role for the human amygdala in novelty detection. NeuroImage. 2010;50(3):1188–1193. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Craig AD. How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(1):59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Grupe DW, Nitschke JB. Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: an integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14(7):488–501. doi: 10.1038/nrn3524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Opitz B. Memory Function and the Hippocampus. In: Szabo K, Hennerici MG, editors. Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience. Vol. 34. Basel: S. KARGER AG; 2014. pp. 51–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Damasio AR. The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;351(1346):1413–1420. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1996.0125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Brohawn KH, Offringa R, Pfaff DL, Hughes KC, Shin LM. The Neural Correlates of Emotional Memory in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68(11):1023–1030. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Bryant RA, Kemp AH, Felmingham KL, et al. Enhanced amygdala and medial prefrontal activation during nonconscious processing of fear in posttraumatic stress disorder: An fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;29(5):517–523. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.El Khoury-Malhame M, Reynaud E, Soriano A, et al. Amygdala activity correlates with attentional bias in PTSD. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(7):1969–1973. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Fonzo GA, Simmons AN, Thorp SR, Norman SB, Paulus MP, Stein MB. Exaggerated and Disconnected Insular-Amygdalar Blood Oxygenation Level-Dependent Response to Threat-Related Emotional Faces in Women with Intimate-Partner Violence Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68(5):433–441. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Hendler T, Rotshtein P, Yeshurun Y, et al. Sensing the invisible: differential sensitivity of visual cortex and amygdala to traumatic context. NeuroImage. 2003;19(3):587–600. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00141-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Patel R, Girard TA, Pukay-Martin N, Monson C. Preferential recruitment of the basolateral amygdala during memory encoding of negative scenes in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2016;130:170–176. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Protopopescu X, Pan H, Tuescher O, et al. Differential time courses and specificity of amygdala activity in posttraumatic stress disorder subjects and normal control subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(5):464–473. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Shin LM, Wright C, Cannistraro PA, et al. A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Amygdala and Medial Prefrontal Cortex Responses to Overtly Presented Fearful Faces in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(3):273–281. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.3.273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Simmons AN, Matthews SC, Strigo IA, et al. Altered amygdala activation during face processing in Iraqi and Afghanistani war veterans. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2011;1(1):6. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-1-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Stevens JS, Jovanovic T, Fani N, et al. Disrupted amygdala-prefrontal functional connectivity in civilian women with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47(10):1469–1478. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.05.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Williams LM, Kemp AH, Felmingham K, et al. Trauma modulates amygdala and medial prefrontal responses to consciously attended fear. NeuroImage. 2006;29(2):347–357. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.St Jacques PL, Botzung A, Miles A, Rubin DC. Functional neuroimaging of emotionally intense autobiographical memories in post-traumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2011;45(5):630–637. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Peres JFP, Foerster B, Santana LG, et al. Police officers under attack: Resilience implications of an fMRI study. J Psychiatr Res. 2011;45(6):727–734. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Felmingham K, Kemp A, Williams L, et al. Changes in anterior cingulate and amygdala after cognitive behavior therapy of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol Sci. 2007;18(2):127–129. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01860.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Armony JL, Corbo V, Clément M-H, Brunet A. Amygdala response in patients with acute PTSD to masked and unmasked emotional facial expressions. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(10):1961–1963. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Felmingham K, Williams LM, Kemp AH, et al. Neural responses to masked fear faces: Sex differences and trauma exposure in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010;119(1):241–247. doi: 10.1037/a0017551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Killgore WDS, Britton JC, Schwab ZJ, et al. CORTICO-LIMBIC RESPONSES TO MASKED AFFECTIVE FACES ACROSS PTSD, PANIC DISORDER, AND SPECIFIC PHOBIA: Research Article: Neurocircuitry of Anxiety Disorders. Depress Anxiety. 2014;31(2):150–159. doi: 10.1002/da.22156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Rauch SL, Whalen PJ, Shin LM, et al. Exaggerated Amygdala Response to Masked Facial Stimuli in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A Functional MRI Study. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47(9):769–776. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00828-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Mazza M, Catalucci A, Mariano M, et al. Neural correlates of automatic perceptual sensitivity to facial affect in posttraumatic stress disorder subjects who survived L’Aquila eartquake of April 6, 2009. Brain Imaging Behav. 2012;6(3):374–386. doi: 10.1007/s11682-012-9151-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Diano M, Celeghin A, Bagnis A, Tamietto M. Amygdala Response to Emotional Stimuli without Awareness: Facts and Interpretations. Front Psychol. 2017;7 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Suvak MK, Barrett LF. Considering PTSD from the perspective of brain processes: A psychological construction approach. J Trauma Stress. 2011;24(1):3–24. doi: 10.1002/jts.20618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Brunetti M, Sepede G, Mingoia G, et al. Elevated response of human amygdala to neutral stimuli in mild post traumatic stress disorder: neural correlates of generalized emotional response. Neuroscience. 2010;168(3):670–679. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.04.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Felmingham KL, Falconer EM, Williams L, et al. Reduced Amygdala and Ventral Striatal Activity to Happy Faces in PTSD Is Associated with Emotional Numbing. In: Hampson M, editor. PLoS ONE. 9. Vol. 9. 2014. p. e103653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Hopper JW, Frewen PA, van der Kolk BA, Lanius RA. Neural correlates of reexperiencing, avoidance, and dissociation in PTSD: Symptom dimensions and emotion dysregulation in responses to script-driven trauma imagery. J Trauma Stress. 2007;20(5):713–725. doi: 10.1002/jts.20284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Aupperle RL, Allard CB, Simmons AN, et al. Neural responses during emotional processing before and after cognitive trauma therapy for battered women. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2013;214(1):48–55. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Simmons AN, Paulus MP, Thorp SR, Matthews SC, Norman SB, Stein MB. Functional Activation and Neural Networks in Women with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Related to Intimate Partner Violence. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(8):681–690. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Frewen PA, Dozois DJA, Neufeld RWJ, Densmore M, Stevens TK, Lanius RA. Social emotions and emotional valence during imagery in women with PTSD: Affective and neural correlates. Psychol Trauma Theory Res Pract Policy. 2010;2(2):145–157. doi: 10.1037/a0019154. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Osuch EA, Benson B, Geraci M, et al. Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Correlated with Flashback Intensity in Patients with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50(4):246–253. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01107-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Yang P, Wu M-T, Hsu C-C, Ker J-H. Evidence of early neurobiological alternations in adolescents with posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Neurosci Lett. 2004;370(1):13–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.07.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Hou C, Liu J, Wang K, et al. Brain responses to symptom provocation and trauma-related short-term memory recall in coal mining accident survivors with acute severe PTSD. Brain Res. 2007;1144:165–174. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Nilsen AS, Blix I, Leknes S, et al. Brain Activity in Response to Trauma-specific, Negative, and Neutral Stimuli. A fMRI Study of Recent Road Traffic Accident Survivors. Front Psychol. 2016;7 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Sakamoto H, Fukuda R, Okuaki T, et al. Parahippocampal activation evoked by masked traumatic images in posttraumatic stress disorder: A functional MRI study. NeuroImage. 2005;26(3):813–821. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Whalley MG, Rugg MD, Smith APR, Dolan RJ, Brewin CR. Incidental retrieval of emotional contexts in post-traumatic stress disorder and depression: An fMRI study. Brain Cogn. 2009;69(1):98–107. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Werner NS, Meindl T, Engel RR, et al. Hippocampal function during associative learning in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2009;43(3):309–318. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Thomaes K, Dorrepaal E, Draijer NPJ, et al. Increased activation of the left hippocampus region in Complex PTSD during encoding and recognition of emotional words: A pilot study. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2009;171(1):44–53. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Jatzko A, Schmitt A, Demirakca T, Weimer E, Braus DF. Disturbance in the neural circuitry underlying positive emotional processing in post–traumatic stress disorder (PTSD): An fMRI study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;256(2):112–114. doi: 10.1007/s00406-005-0617-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Hayes JP, LaBar KS, McCarthy G, et al. Reduced hippocampal and amygdala activity predicts memory distortions for trauma reminders in combat-related PTSD. J Psychiatr Res. 2011;45(5):660–669. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.10.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Vrana SR, Roodman A, Beckham JC. Selective processing of trauma-relevant words in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 1995;9(6):515–530. [Google Scholar]
- 88.McNally RJ, Lasko NB, Macklin ML, Pitman RK. Autobiographical memory disturbance in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav Res Ther. 1995;33(6):619–630. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(95)00007-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Golier JA, Yehuda R, Lupien SJ, Harvey PD, Grossman R, Elkin A. Memory performance in Holocaust survivors with posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(10):1682–1688. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Paunovic N, Lundh L-G, Öst L-G. Attentional and memory bias for emotional information in crime victims with acute posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) J Anxiety Disord. 2002;16(6):675–692. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6185(02)00136-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Kaczkurkin AN, Burton PC, Chazin SM, et al. Neural Substrates of Overgeneralized Conditioned Fear in PTSD. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174(2):125–134. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15121549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(2):85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4(6):215–222. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01483-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Kanske P, Kotz SA. Emotion Speeds up Conflict Resolution: A New Role for the Ventral Anterior Cingulate Cortex? Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(4):911–919. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Diekhof EK, Geier K, Falkai P, Gruber O. Fear is only as deep as the mind allows. NeuroImage. 2011;58(1):275–285. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Quirk GJ, Gehlert DR. Inhibition of the amygdala: key to pathological states? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;985:263–272. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07087.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Beckmann M, Johansen-Berg H, Rushworth MFS. Connectivity-Based Parcellation of Human Cingulate Cortex and Its Relation to Functional Specialization. J Neurosci. 2009;29(4):1175–1190. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3328-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Lanius RA, Frewen PA, Girotti M, Neufeld RWJ, Stevens TK, Densmore M. Neural correlates of trauma script-imagery in posttraumatic stress disorder with and without comorbid major depression: A functional MRI investigation. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2007;155(1):45–56. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Britton JC, Taylor SF, Sudheimer KD, Liberzon I. Facial expressions and complex IAPS pictures: Common and differential networks. NeuroImage. 2006;31(2):906–919. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Fera F, Weinberger DR. Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful stimuli. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53(6):494–501. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01786-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Lane RD, Reiman EM, Ahern GL, Schwartz GE, Davidson RJ. Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(7):926–933. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.7.926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Paradiso S, Johnson DL, Andreasen NC, et al. Cerebral blood flow changes associated with attribution of emotional valence to pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral visual stimuli in a PET study of normal subjects. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(10):1618–1629. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.10.1618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Ochsner KN, Ray RD, Cooper JC, et al. For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. NeuroImage. 2004;23(2):483–499. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Banks SJ, Eddy KT, Angstadt M, Nathan PJ, Phan KL. Amygdala–frontal connectivity during emotion regulation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2(4):303–312. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Brunetti M, Sepede G, Ferretti A, Mingoia G, Romani GL, Babiloni C. Response inhibition failure to visual stimuli paired with a “single-type” stressor in PTSD patients: An fMRI pilot study. Brain Res Bull. 2015;114:20–30. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2015.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Lanius RA, Williamson PC, Densmore M, et al. Neural correlates of traumatic memories in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI investigation. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(11):1920–1922. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Dickie EW, Brunet A, Akerib V, Armony JL. Neural correlates of recovery from post-traumatic stress disorder: A longitudinal fMRI investigation of memory encoding. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(7):1771–1778. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Mogg K, Bradley BP. Orienting of Attention to Threatening Facial Expressions Presented under Conditions of Restricted Awareness. Cogn Emot. 1999;13(6):713–740. doi: 10.1080/026999399379050. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Gross JJ. Antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74(1):224. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.1.224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Bishop SJ. Neural Mechanisms Underlying Selective Attention to Threat. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129(1):141–152. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Pessoa L, Kastner S, Ungerleider LG. Attentional control of the processing of neural and emotional stimuli. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2002;15(1):31–45. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00214-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, et al. Cognitive Reappraisal of Emotion: A Meta-Analysis of Human Neuroimaging Studies. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24(11):2981–2990. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Kohn N, Eickhoff SB, Scheller M, Laird AR, Fox PT, Habel U. Neural network of cognitive emotion regulation — An ALE meta-analysis and MACM analysis. NeuroImage. 2014;87:345–355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Messina I, Bianco S, Sambin M, Viviani R. Executive and semantic processes in reappraisal of negative stimuli: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Front Psychol. 2015;6 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Ochsner KN, Silvers JA, Buhle JT. Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: a synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion: Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012;1251(1):E1–E24. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06751.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(4):170–177. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Badre D, Wagner AD. Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(13):2883–2901. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL. The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron. 2008;58(3):306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Corbetta M, Shulman GL. CONTROL OF GOAL-DIRECTED AND STIMULUS-DRIVEN ATTENTION IN THE BRAIN. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(3):215–229. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Marumo K, Takizawa R, Kinou M, et al. Functional abnormalities in the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex during a semantic fluency task, and their association with thought disorder in patients with schizophrenia. NeuroImage. 2014;85:518–526. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Machizawa MG, Kalla R, Walsh V, Otten LJ. The Time Course of Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Memory Formation. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103(3):1569–1579. doi: 10.1152/jn.90937.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Novick JM, Kan IP, Trueswell JC, Thompson-Schill SL. A case for conflict across multiple domains: Memory and language impairments following damage to ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2009;26(6):527–567. doi: 10.1080/02643290903519367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Snyder HR, Banich MT, Munakata Y. Choosing Our Words: Retrieval and Selection Processes Recruit Shared Neural Substrates in Left Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23(11):3470–3482. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Geva S, Jones PS, Crinion JT, Price CJ, Baron J-C, Warburton EA. The neural correlates of inner speech defined by voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Brain. 2011;134(10):3071–3082. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Jones SR, Fernyhough C. Neural correlates of inner speech and auditory verbal hallucinations: A critical review and theoretical integration. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007;27(2):140–154. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Morin A, Hamper B. Self-reflection and the inner voice: activation of the left inferior frontal gyrus during perceptual and conceptual self-referential thinking. Open Neuroimaging J. 2012;6(1) doi: 10.2174/1874440001206010078. http://benthamopen.com/FULLTEXT/TONIJ-6-78. Accessed January 6, 2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Hoshi E. Functional specialization within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: A review of anatomical and physiological studies of non-human primates. Neurosci Res. 2006;54(2):73–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2005.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Barbas H. Connections underlying the synthesis of cognition, memory, and emotion in primate prefrontal cortices. Brain Res Bull. 2000;52(5):319–330. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(99)00245-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Ghashghaeia HT, Hilgetag CC, Barbas H. Sequence of information processing for emotions based on the anatomic dialogue between prefrontal cortex and amygdala. NeuroImage. 2007;34(3):905–923. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Stefanacci L, Amaral DG. Some observations on cortical inputs to the macaque monkey amygdala: An anterograde tracing study. J Comp Neurol. 2002;451(4):301–323. doi: 10.1002/cne.10339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Arnsten AFT, Jin LE. Progress in Molecular Biology and Translational Science. Vol. 122. Elsevier; 2014. Molecular Influences on Working Memory Circuits in Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex; pp. 211–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Barbey AK, Koenigs M, Grafman J. Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex. 2013;49(5):1195–1205. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Edin F, Klingberg T, Johansson P, McNab F, Tegnér J, Compte A. Mechanism for top-down control of working memory capacity. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2009;106(16):6802–6807. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901894106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Lee D, Seo H. Mechanisms of Reinforcement Learning and Decision Making in the Primate Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1104(1):108–122. doi: 10.1196/annals.1390.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Brosnan MB, Wiegand I. The Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex, a Dynamic Cortical Area to Enhance Top-Down Attentional Control. J Neurosci. 2017;37(13):3445–3446. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0136-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Yamagishi T, Takagishi H, Fermin AdeSR, Kanai R, Li Y, Matsumoto Y. Cortical thickness of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex predicts strategic choices in economic games. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016;113(20):5582–5587. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1523940113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Blair KS, Vythilingam M, Crowe SL, et al. Cognitive control of attention is differentially affected in trauma-exposed individuals with and without post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med. 2013;43(01):85–95. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712000840. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Pannu Hayes J, LaBar KS, Petty CM, McCarthy G, Morey RA. Alterations in the neural circuitry for emotion and attention associated with posttraumatic stress symptomatology. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2009;172(1):7–15. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Morey RA, Petty CM, Cooper DA, LaBar KS, McCarthy G. Neural systems for executive and emotional processing are modulated by symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder in Iraq War veterans. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2008;162(1):59–72. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.New AS, Fan J, Murrough JW, et al. A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Deliberate Emotion Regulation in Resilience and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(7):656–664. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Rabinak CA, MacNamara A, Kennedy AE, et al. FOCAL AND ABERRANT PREFRONTAL ENGAGEMENT DURING EMOTION REGULATION IN VETERANS WITH POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER: Research Article: Prefrontal Regulation of Affect in PTSD. Depress Anxiety. 2014;31(10):851–861. doi: 10.1002/da.22243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Kim MJ, Chey J, Chung A, et al. Diminished rostral anterior cingulate activity in response to threat-related events in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2008;42(4):268–277. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Offringa R, Brohawn KH, Staples LK, et al. Diminished rostral anterior cingulate cortex activation during trauma-unrelated emotional interference in PTSD. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2013;3(1):10. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-3-10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Shin LM, Whalen PJ, Pitman RK, et al. An fMRI Study of Anterior Cingulate Function in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50(12):932–942. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01215-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.White SF, Costanzo ME, Blair JR, Roy MJ. PTSD symptom severity is associated with increased recruitment of top-down attentional control in a trauma-exposed sample. NeuroImage Clin. 2015;7:19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Fani N, Jovanovic T, Ely TD, et al. Neural correlates of attention bias to threat in post-traumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychol. 2012;90(2):134–142. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Mahan AL, Ressler KJ. Fear conditioning, synaptic plasticity and the amygdala: implications for posttraumatic stress disorder. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35(1):24–35. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Milad MR, Quirk GJ. Fear Extinction as a Model for Translational Neuroscience: Ten Years of Progress. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63(1):129–151. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Fullana MA, Harrison BJ, Soriano-Mas C, et al. Neural signatures of human fear conditioning: an updated and extended meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21(4):500–508. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Lissek S, Biggs AL, Rabin SJ, et al. Generalization of conditioned fear-potentiated startle in humans: experimental validation and clinical relevance. Behav Res Ther. 2008;46(5):678–687. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Graham BM, Milad MR. The Study of Fear Extinction: Implications for Anxiety Disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(12):1255–1265. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11040557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Quirk GJ, Mueller D. Neural Mechanisms of Extinction Learning and Retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(1):56–72. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Hermans D, Craske MG, Mineka S, Lovibond PF. Extinction in Human Fear Conditioning. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(4):361–368. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Greco JA, Liberzon I. Neuroimaging of Fear-Associated Learning. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41(1):320–334. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Grillon C, Morgan CA, 3rd, Davis MT, Southwick SM. Effects of experimental context and explicit threat cues on acoustic startle in Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44(10):1027–1036. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00034-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Rabinak CA, Mori S, Lyons M, Milad MR, Phan KL. Acquisition of CS-US contingencies during Pavlovian fear conditioning and extinction in social anxiety disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder. J Affect Disord. 2017;207:76–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.09.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Garfinkel SN, Abelson JL, King AP, et al. Impaired Contextual Modulation of Memories in PTSD: An fMRI and Psychophysiological Study of Extinction Retention and Fear Renewal. J Neurosci. 2014;34(40):13435–13443. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4287-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Milad MR, Pitman RK, Ellis CB, et al. Neurobiological Basis of Failure to Recall Extinction Memory in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(12):1075–1082. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Shvil E, Sullivan GM, Schafer S, et al. Sex differences in extinction recall in posttraumatic stress disorder: A pilot fMRI study. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;113:101–108. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Diener SJ, Nees F, Wessa M, et al. Reduced amygdala responsivity during conditioning to trauma-related stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: Conditioning to trauma-related stimuli in PTSD. Psychophysiology. 2016;53(10):1460–1471. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Linnman C, Zeffiro TA, Pitman RK, Milad MR. An fMRI study of unconditioned responses in post-traumatic stress disorder. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2011;1(1):8. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-1-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Rougemont-Bücking A, Linnman C, Zeffiro TA, et al. Altered Processing of Contextual Information during Fear Extinction in PTSD: An fMRI Study: Altered Processing of Contextual Information during Fear Extinction in PTSD. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2011;17(4):227–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2010.00152.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Jovanovic T, Ely T, Fani N, et al. Reduced neural activation during an inhibition task is associated with impaired fear inhibition in a traumatized civilian sample. Cortex. 2013;49(7):1884–1891. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Sheppes G, Suri G, Gross JJ. Emotion Regulation and Psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2015;11(1):379–405. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Amstadter A. Emotion regulation and anxiety disorders. J Anxiety Disord. 2008;22(2):211–221. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Neural correlates of emotion regulation in psychopathology. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(10):413–418. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Fernandez KC, Jazaieri H, Gross JJ. Emotion Regulation: A Transdiagnostic Perspective on a New RDoC Domain. Cogn Ther Res. 2016;40(3):426–440. doi: 10.1007/s10608-016-9772-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Onat S, Büchel C. The neuronal basis of fear generalization in humans. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(12):1811–1818. doi: 10.1038/nn.4166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Nicholson AA, Friston KJ, Zeidman P, et al. Dynamic causal modeling in PTSD and its dissociative subtype: Bottom-up versus top-down processing within fear and emotion regulation circuitry: DCM in PTSD and Its Dissociative Subtype. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017 Aug; doi: 10.1002/hbm.23748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Kemp AH, Felmingham KL, Falconer E, Liddell BJ, Bryant RA, Williams LM. Heterogeneity of non-conscious fear perception in posttraumatic stress disorder as a function of physiological arousal: An fMRI study. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2009;174(2):158–161. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Lieberman L, Gorka SM, DiGangi JA, Frederick A, Phan KL. Impact of posttraumatic stress symptom dimensions on amygdala reactivity to emotional faces. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2017;79:401–407. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.07.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Grupe DW, Wielgosz J, Davidson RJ, Nitschke JB. Neurobiological correlates of distinct post-traumatic stress disorder symptom profiles during threat anticipation in combat veterans. Psychol Med. 2016;46(09):1885–1895. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716000374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.MacNamara A, Rabinak CA, Kennedy AE, et al. Emotion regulatory brain function and SSRI treatment in PTSD: neural correlates and predictors of change. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41(2):611–618. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Nicholson AA, Rabellino D, Densmore M, et al. The neurobiology of emotion regulation in posttraumatic stress disorder: Amygdala downregulation via real-time fMRI neurofeedback: Amygdala Down-Regulation in PTSD. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017;38(1):541–560. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Fonzo GA, Goodkind MS, Oathes DJ, et al. Selective effects of psychotherapy on frontopolar cortical function in PTSD. Am J Psychiatry. 2017 doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.16091073. appi-ajp. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]