Skip to main content
. 2018 May 11;9:709. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00709

Table 3A.

Summary of compensatory and restorative studies (older adults and populations with health conditions).

Article Sample (completed) Device Active Control Near transfer test Far transfer* test Retention test Independent of company Cognitive outcomes Findings
Ackerman et al., 2010 78 healthly adults (mean 60.7 yrs) Wii Big Brain Academy Reading exercises Yes No No Yes Fluid and crystalline intelligence, processing speed There was an effect of training on measures of processing speed (ps < 0.01) and fluid intelligence (verbal tests) (ps < 0.01), but there was no benefit for the TG compared to the control group for any measures.
Åkerlund et al., 2013 47 participants with impaired WM following traumatic brain injury (47.7 ± 11.3 yrs) Cogmed No Yes Yes (SR) No Yes STM, WM Both TG and control improved on digit span (STM). The TG showed a significantly greater improvement in digit span (p = 0.045). TG did not show greater improvement on other measures of WM (spatial span, sequence memory). There was no self-reported change in executive function or psychological health for either group.
Ballesteros et al., 2014 30 healthy older adults (57–80 yrs) Lumosity No Yes Yes (SR) No Yes Processing speed, selective attention, EF, spatial working memory, episodic memory TG showed greater improvement than controls in oddball task performance (selective attention), speed of processing and Wechsler memory (ps < 0.05), but not EF, spatial WM or self-reported wellbeing.
Björkdahl et al., 2013 45 adults with WM deficits following brain injury (51.0 ± 11 yrs) Cogmed No Yes Yes Yes Yes STM TG showed significant improvement in STM (digit span) following training (p = 0.003), with no change in the control group. Both groups improved on therapist ratings of motor skill.
Brehmer et al., 2011 23 healthy older adults (mean 63.7 yrs) Cogmed Non-adaptive version of training task Yes No No No WM, sustained attention, inhibition, STM reasoning Interaction effects indicated greater gains in divided attention and WM (span tasks) for the TG (p < 0.05). This was not found for inhibition, STM and reasoning. Training gains were related to changes in neural activation.
Charvet et al., 2015 20 participants with cognitive impairment due to MS. (39.8 ± 11.5 yrs) Lumosity Computer games Yes Yes No Yes Memory, processing speed There was a significant difference between TG and active controls post-training in composite cognitive score (p = 0.02) but not for individual tests (e.g., WAIS, Corsi blocks). TG also performed better on a motor function task (p = 0.01).
Edwards et al., 2013a 74 adults (>40 yrs) with Parkinson's disease (68.9 ± 8.1 yrs) InSight, Posit Science No Yes Yes (SR) No No Visual speed of processing TG showed significantly greater improvements in visual processing speed (useful field of view) (p = 0.032). TG did not differ from controls in self-reported cognitive performance or depressive symptoms.
Edwards et al., 2013b 67 healthy older adults (74.0 ± 7.5 yrs) InSight, Post Science No Yes Yes (SR) No No Visual speed of processing TG showed significantly greater improvements in visual processing speed (useful field of view) (p = 0.043) than wait-list controls. There was no effect of training on self-reported social or cognitive function.
Finn and McDonald, 2011 16 older adults with mild cognitive impairment (72.7 ± 7.1 yrs) Lumosity No Yes Yes (SR) No Yes Sustained attention, WM, set shifting, visual memory TG showed greater improvement on sustained attention, but not WM, memory (pattern recognition) or shifting (set shifting). Also no effect on subsequent training of waitlist controls. No effect on self-report of mood.
Gropper et al., 2014 62 university students with ADHD or learning disabilities (28.0 ± 7.2 yrs) Cogmed No Yes Yes (SR) Yes Yes WM, sustained attention, selective attention, reading and mathematics comprehension There was no effect of training on WM (digit span), sustained or selective attention or mathematics and reading comprehension (ps > 0.05). There were similarly no group differences at 2-month follow-up. There was a reduction in self-reported ADHD symptoms.
Haimov and Shatil, 2013 51 older adults with insomnia (65–85 yrs) Cognifit Simple computer tasks Yes Yes No No Range of tests including: memory, divided attention, inhibition, shifting, WM, processing speed TG showed improvements in several functions, including memory (p < 0.001), divided attention (p < 0.05), processing speed (p < 0.01), visual WM (p < 0.001). The TG showed greater improvements than the active control in memory (p < 0.001), visual (p < 0.001), and auditory (p < 0.001) WM. TG also showed improvements in sleep quality.
Hellgren et al., 2015 48 adults with acquired brain injury (mean 43.7 yrs) Cogmed No Yes No No Yes WM, processing speed, sustained attention, divided attention The TG improved on all tests of WM and attention (ps < 0.001). TG reported increased quality of life (p < 0.001). No control group comparison.
Hyer et al., 2016 68 older adults (>65 yrs) with memory impairment Cogmed Non-adaptive version of training task Yes No Yes Yes WM, executive function The TG showed greater improvements than active controls on one of two WM span tests (p = 0.01), but not on an executive function test.
Kesler et al., 2013 41 women with history of breast cancer (56.0 ± 7.0 years). Lumosity No Yes No No Yes Executive function, WM, processing speed The TG showed significantly greater improvement than controls in EF (WCST) (p = 0.008) and processing speed (symbol search) (p = 0.009) but not WM (digit span) (p = 0.57).
Klavora et al., 1995 10 participants (45–80 yrs) unsafe to drive following stroke Dynavision No Yes Yes Yes Yes Processing speed There was a significant improvement in performance on the trained task (ps < 0.05) and an increase in the proportion of participants rated as safe to drive.
Legault and Faubert, 2012 41 healthy older adults (64–73 yrs, mean 66.3) Neurotracker Perceptual task (contrast detection) No Yes No No None At a distance of 4 m the TG showed significantly better perception of partially masked human motion than active controls (p = 0.040). There was no difference at 16 m.
Leung et al., 2015 209 healthy older adults (70.1 ± 6.4 yrs) Brain Fitness, Posit Science Educational programme Yes No No Yes Sustained attention, WM, verbal STM, The TG showed greater improvement on one of two sustained attention tests (p = 0.026) and on working memory (p = 0.012) but not STM.
Liu et al., 2016 (study 2) 102 adults with ADHD (18–35 yrs) Cogmed No Yes No No Yes WM, general intelligence No effect of training group on changes in WM (delayed match to sample test) (ps > 0.05).
Liu et al., 2017 88 young adults with ADHD (23.7 ± 3.3 yrs) Cogmed No Yes No No Yes WM, fluid intelligence No transfer to response control in Go/NoGo task.
Lundqvist et al., 2010 21 adults with acquired brain injury (43.3 ± 9.8 yrs) Cogmed No Yes Yes (SR) Yes Yes Divided attention, inhibition, switching, WM. TG showed significant improvements in measures of WM, inhibition, switching, and divided attention immediately post-training (p < 0.001 to p = 0.002) and at 20-week follow-up (p < 0.001 to p = 0.002). There was no change in passive control group. TG also improved self-ratings of occupational performance.
Mawjee et al., 2015 97 young adults (18–35 yrs) with ADHD (23.9 ± 3.4 yrs) Cogmed No Yes Yes (SR) No Yes WM, STM, processing speed There were no differences between TG and controls following training. There was also no difference in self-reported ADHD symptoms and cognitive failures.
Mayas et al., 2014 27 healthy adults (57–77) Lumosity No Yes No No Yes Alertness and distractibility There was no effect of group (TG v control) on digit categorization performance in the oddball task. The TG significantly improved from pre to post in distractibility (p = 0.05) and alertness (p = 0.04).
McDougall and House, 2012 41 healthy older adults (74.6 ± 8.5) Nintendo Brain Age No Yes No No Yes Intelligence Sub-tests of the WAIS only showed a benefit for backward digit span (p < .05). There was no effect for vocabulary, block design, arithmetic and forward digit span. There was no effect of more frequent use.
Nouchi et al. (2012) 28 healthy older adults (69.1 ± 2.4 yrs) Nintendo Brain Age Video game Yes No No Yes Executive function, WM, processing speed Following training, the TG showed significantly greater improvements in EF (ps = 0.001–0.006) and processing speed (ps = 0.005–0.014). There was no difference in WM (digit span) improvement between groups (p > 0.05)
Peretz et al., 2011 155 healthy older adults (68 ± 7 yrs) Cognifit Video games Yes No No No Overall cognitive performance There was a significant improvement in overall cognitive score in the TG (p < 0.05) and the active control (p < 0.05). There was no difference in improvement between groups.
Preiss et al., 2013 31 participants with unipolar and bipolar depression (44.2 ± 14.2 yrs) Cognifit No Yes Yes (SR) No No WM, shifting, inhibition, divided attention, STM, executive function There was no difference between TG and controls for WM, shifting, inhibition, divided attention, STM, or executive function (Stroop, WCST). Improvements in self-report of depressive symptoms.
Rass et al., 2015 56 methadone maintenance patients (43.4 ± 8.0 yrs) Cogmed Non-adaptive version of training task Yes Yes (SR) No Yes WM, STM, processing speed, reasoning, inhibition Greater WM (digit span, OSPAN) improvements in the TG than controls (p = 0.003). No group differences in improvement in processing speed (trail making), inhibition or reasoning. TG reported less drug use post-training than active controls (p = 0.045).
Siberski et al., 2015 32 adults with intellectual and developmental disabilities (40.5 ± 11.0 yrs) Cognifit Video games Yes No No No Divided attention, inhibition, shifting, processing speed, WM The TG improved in measures of monitoring (p = 0.017), visual WM (p = 0.003), and processing speed (p = 0.038) but not divided attention, shifting (WCST), or inhibition (Stroop). There were no group differences for any measure post-intervention.
Smith et al., 2009 487 healthy older adults (>65 yrs) Brain Fitness, Posit Science Educational training Yes No No No Cognitive assessment battery (inc. attention and memory), WM The TG showed greater improvement than controls in the cognitive battery (p = 0.02) and WM task (p = 0.006).
Strenziok et al., 2014 42 healthy older adults Brain Fitness, Posit Science Video games Yes No No Yes Reasoning, WM, STM. The TG showed a significant improvement in reasoning (WAIS matrix) scores (p < 0.05) but no improvement in WM (letter number sequencing).
Von Ah et al., 2012 82 breast cancer survivors (56.5 ± 8.5 yrs) InSight, Posit Science Memory training Yes Yes (SR) Yes No Memory, speed of processing TG showed enhanced processing speed (useful field of view test) in comparison to passive controls post-training (p = 0.040) and at 2-month follow-up (p = 0.016). Also improved memory post-training (p = 0.0004) and at follow-up (p = 0.010). TG improved self-reported cognitive functioning (p = 0.042).
Wentink et al., 2016 107 adults (45–75 years) recovering from stroke Lumosity No Yes Yes (SR) Yes Yes WM, inhibition, fluid intelligence TG outperformed controls in one of four WM tests (p = 0.02) and an inhibition test (p < 0.001) post-training. At 16-week follow-up there were no group differences in WM, inhibition, attention, and fluid intelligence tests. Also no differences in self-reported cognitive failures or quality of life.
*

Transfer to tasks other than laboratory cognitive tests.

SR, self-report outcome; WM, working memory; STM, short-term memory; EF, executive function; TG, treatment group; OSPAN, Operation Span task; WCST, Wisconsin card Sorting Task; WAIS, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; ADHD, Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; MS, Multiple Sclerosis; ps, multiple p-values.

Quality assessment color key: Strong (80+%)Fairly strong (7079%)Moderate (6069%)Weak  (5059-%)Very weak (46049%)