Abstract
Alcohol use is associated with a higher rate of violent offending among males and a higher rate of violent victimization among females, especially for Intimate Partner Violence (IPV). Using comparable self-reported data from the GENACIS Project, the present study examines between the United States (n=2,363) and Japan (n=1,660) whether the expected gender difference in alcohol-related IPV is explained by alcohol-aggression expectancy. The results indicate that though males are more likely than females to expect that alcohol would make them more aggressive, alcohol-aggression expectancy has a very little to do with the gender difference in alcohol-related IPV. In both countries, overall, alcohol use of males, irrespective of their alcohol-aggression expectancy, most strongly and directly accounted for the gender difference in alcohol-related IPV.
Keywords: Alcohol and IPV, gender and alcohol, alcohol-aggression expectancy, cross-national study
Empirical research reports a strong association between alcohol use and violence, especially within the criminal justice system (Berthelot et al. 2015; Fagan 1990; Felson et al. 2008a; 2008b; Mustaine and Tewksbury 2009; Parker and Rebhun 1995; Sherley 2005). About 36% of victims of violent crime perceived that their perpetrators were under the influence of alcohol (Rand et al. 2010; see Greenfeld 1998 for an earlier report); more than 35% of state inmates and 20% of federal inmates who are serving time for violent crimes were under the influence of alcohol at the time of the offense (Rand et al. 2010; see Greenfeld 1998 for an earlier report); about 40% of convicted homicide and assault offenders were drinking at the time of the offense (Greenfeld 1998); and almost half of all victims of homicide (Kuhns et al. 2011) and 37% of sexual assault victims (Hindmarch and Brinkmann 1999) tested positive for alcohol. Moreover, alcohol use is associated with an escalation of violence and an exacerbation of the risk and the seriousness of injuries resulting from violent interactions (Greenfeld 1998; Martin and Backman 1997; Rand et al. 2010).
ALCOHOL, GENDER, AND VIOLENCE
Though alcohol use is associated with an increased confrontation with violence for both genders (Bachman and Peralta 2002; Hoaken and Pihl 2000), the strength of this association varies by gender, such that alcohol use is more strongly associated with violence for males compared to females (Giancola and Zeichner 1995; Nolen-Hoeksema 2004; Scott et al. 1999). Empirical research, for instance, reports that 35% of male inmates (compared to 22% of female inmates) in jail were under the influence of alcohol at the time of their offense (James 2004); alcohol was present in more than 50% of male-perpetrated (but only 27% of female-perpetrated) violence (Pernanen 1991); and alcohol was present in 53% of male (while only 38% of female) victims of homicide (Sheehan et al. 2012). Males are overall more likely than females to state that alcohol use was a reason for causing an injury to another person or damage to property (Perkins 1992) or being aggressive in general (Crawford 1984), and more male than female arrestees report heavy and frequent alcohol use prior to their arrest (Nunes-Dinis and Weisner 1997).
Moreover, there is a gender difference in the way alcohol use is associated with violence, with empirical research reporting overall a higher rate of violent offending among males (Collins and Messerschmidt 1993; Giancola and Zeichner 1995) and a higher rate of violent victimization among females (Kuhns et al. 2011; Nolen-Hoeksema 2004; Parks and Fals-Stewart 2004) upon an increased alcohol use. The National Incident-Based Reporting System (NIBRS) reports that of all violent incidents involving alcohol, 80% have been perpetrated by male offenders whereas only 40% of all such incidents involved male victims (Rand et al. 2010). It is not surprising, therefore, that much of the empirical research on alcohol and violence has focused on alcohol’s role in violent offending among males and violent victimization among females, especially with interpersonal violence such as intimate partner violence.
ALCOHOL AND INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE
Intimate Partner Violence (IPV) can be defined as “any behaviour within an intimate relationship that causes physical, psychological or sexual harm to those in the relationship” (Krug et al. 2002: 89). Gender differences are more evident in IPV than in other interpersonal violence, except for sexual assault (Testa et al. 2011), with violence perpetrated by males on their female partners being more common and injurious compared to violence perpetrated by females on their male partners (Kantor and Jasinski 1998). According to U.S. Department of Justice estimates, about 80% of all homicide and manslaughter that occurs between intimate partners involved female victims (Greenfeld et al. 1998).
Moreover, alcohol is highly prevalent in IPV (Fals-Stewart 2003; Graham et al. 2004) with more than half of all violent episodes occurring in a residence or home where alcohol is involved (Rand et al. 2010). Unlike most other interpersonal violence, however, where both offender and victim tend to be intoxicated, IPV tends to involve only one, usually a male perpetrator, who has been drinking at the time of the incident (Martin and Bachman 1997; Pernanen 1991). Moreover, Greenfield et al. (2009) note a gender difference in the type of alcohol-involved victimization, such that females are more likely to be victims of IPV as a result of others’ drinking whereas males are more likely to be victims of alcohol-related physical assaults by other males. Like research on alcohol and violence more generally, much empirical research on alcohol use and IPV, therefore, has also focused on alcohol-related IPV perpetrated by males on their female partners, finding an increasing risk of IPV when male perpetrators have been drinking alcohol (see for a review, Graham et al. 2011). Other empirical studies have found a significant positive association between alcohol use and victimization among females (see for a review, Devries et al. 2013).
Both experimental and correlational studies have consistently found an association between alcohol use and violence (for a review, see Exum 2006) and the gender difference in this association. Explanations that focus solely on pharmacological effects of alcohol on aggression, however, cannot fully account for the gender differences in how alcohol use is associated with IPV, or more generally violence, unless alcohol’s pharmacological effect on aggression varies by gender. Needed also are socio-psychological explanations that focus on social and cultural factors, such as alcohol-aggression expectancy, that might account for the gender difference in how alcohol use is associated with violence.
GENDER, CULTURE, AND ALCOHOL-AGGRESSION EXPECTANCY
The significant effect of our “response expectancy” on all facets of our experience, including mood, reaction to medications, and behaviors, has been noted for a long time (Kirsch 1999: 3). Specifically related to alcohol, alcohol expectancy refers to our expectation of the pharmacological effect of alcohol, for instance, on aggression (see for a review, Goldman et al. 1999; Nicolai et al. 2010; Powers et al. 2015). The learned disinhibition hypothesis states that alcohol’s impairing effect on our ability to inhibit deviance results not from its pharmacological effect but from our expectation of its pharmacological effect (Collins 1983). Numerous studies have shown a strong effect of alcohol expectancy on behaviors and moods, including the expectation of alcohol’s effect on aggression influencing aggressive behaviors while being intoxicated (e.g., Beck and Heinz 2013).
Empirical research consistently finds that males are more likely than females to expect that alcohol would make them more aggressive (Barnwell et al. 2006; Borders et al. 2007). This gender difference could be explained by gender norms that shape our beliefs concerning what behaviors are accepted in any given situation, including those associated with alcohol use and violence. Because gender norms are in general more strongly restricting of female behaviors and interactions compared to those of males (Blume 1997; Eagly and Wood 1991; Eron and Huesmann 1989; Kunkel and Nielsen 1998; Tibbetts and Herz 1996) and alcohol use and violence have historically been more strongly condemned among females compared to males, gender norms could explain why males are more likely than females to expect alcohol would make them more aggressive. The present study, therefore, examines whether or not alcohol-aggression expectancy accounts for the expected gender difference in alcohol-related IPV, or more specifically, why alcohol use is more likely to be related to violent offending among males and violent victimization among females.
Similarly, empirical research indicates that the association between alcohol intoxication and violence varies across societies and cultural drinking norms (MacAndrew and Edgerton 1969; Parker 1993). Like gender norms, cultural norms surrounding drinking affect how people drink and the beliefs concerning behaviors associated with alcohol use that are considered acceptable or problematic, such as excessive drinking, public display of intoxication, and alcohol-related violence (Greenfield and Room 1997; Room and Mäkelä 2000). Most societies, moreover, apply a unique set of behavioral norms for drinking, and behaviors that are considered problematic under normal circumstances, such as rowdiness, are often tolerated while under the influence of alcohol (MacAndrew and Edgerton 1969; Room 2001). MacAndrew and Edgerton (1969) go so far to state that behaviors associated with alcohol intoxication are culturally induced, rather than pharmacologically induced, thereby explaining why aggression and violence are more strongly associated with alcohol intoxication in some groups of people and societies than in others that associate it with joyousness and happiness (see a review, Room 2001). Some empirical studies, moreover, indicate that aggressive behaviors vary more widely across cultures than between genders (Rohner 1976; see for a more recent example, Bernards and Graham 2013), suggesting the importance of considering cultural and structural factors, in addition to pharmacological factors, in explaining alcohol-related violence.
As in much of social science, a lack of empirical research conducted outside North America, especially cross-cultural research that compares two or more distinct cultures, is a major limitation of research on alcohol and aggression/violence (Room 2001). Even rarer is cross-cultural research that tests theories of alcohol and violence, such as alcohol expectancy and the learned disinhibition hypothesis. Room (2001) indeed notes only four empirical studies that compared alcohol expectancy across countries. The present study, therefore, attempts to fill this critical gap in the literature by examining cross-nationally between the U.S. and Japan alcohol-aggression expectancy as an explanation for the gender difference in alcohol-related IPV.
In cross-national research, Japan offers an interesting contrast to Western industrialized societies because of their similarities in terms of modernization and subsequent economic, political, and structural change and their differences particularly in terms of culture (Nakane 1970; Reischauer 1988). Compared to the U.S., a society that is relatively more egalitarian in terms of gender, Japan exhibits greater gender inequality in many aspects of social life, including division of labor (Ochiai 2014), attitude toward gender roles (Treas and Widmer 2000), and participation in higher education (OECD 2004) and the labor market (United Nations Statistics Division 2007). Empirical research indicates that societies characterized by greater gender inequality more strongly prescribe gender norms concerning all aspects of behaviors and social interactions (Kashima et al. 1995; Nayak et al. 2003), including alcohol use and violence, thus creating greater gender differences in these behaviors (including deviance indicated by Kobayashi et al. 2008). Cross-cultural research, for instance, indicates a greater gender difference in alcohol use in societies that adhere to more traditional gender roles compared to egalitarian societies (Gefou-Madianou 1992; Wilsnack et al. 2000). Empirical studies also find a greater occurrence of IPV perpetrated by males on their female partners, and violence against women in general, in societies with higher levels of gender inequality (Archer 2006). The gender differences in alcohol use, IPV, alcohol-aggression expectancy, and the association between alcohol use and IPV are, therefore, all hypothesized to be greater in Japan compared to the U.S.
HYPOTHESES
Based on the review of literature, the following five hypotheses were generated and tested in the present study:
There is a gender difference in the association between alcohol use and IPV, such that alcohol use is more strongly associated with IPV offending for males and with IPV victimization for females.
There is a gender difference in alcohol-aggression expectancy, such that males are more likely than females to expect that alcohol would make them more aggressive.
The association between alcohol use and IPV offending varies by alcohol-aggression expectancy, such that drinking and IPV offending are more strongly associated among those who expect that alcohol would make them more aggressive than among those who do not expect that alcohol would make them more aggressive, while the association between drinking and IPV victimization does not vary by alcohol-aggression expectancy.
The gender difference in alcohol-aggression expectancy explains the gender difference in the association between alcohol use and IPV.
All hypothesized gender differences in the associations among alcohol use, IPV, and alcohol-aggression expectancy are greater in Japan than in the U.S.
DATA AND MEASURES
The U.S. and Japanese data used to test the five hypotheses are from the GENACIS (Gender, Alcohol, and Culture: An International Study) project. GENACIS is an on-going international project involving 35 countries that focuses on country and gender differences in the extent of alcohol consumption and its negative consequences, including IPV (see for more information, Wilsnack 2012). A nationally-representative sample was selected in both countries and given a comparable survey on the use of alcohol and alcohol-related problems. In addition to the language in which survey was administered, the wording of the survey questions used in two countries also differed slightly. The Japanese survey used a standard GENACIS questionnaire, adopted in most of the GENACIS surveys. The U.S. survey, on the other hand, used a questionnaire developed independently, but similar to the standard GENACIS questionnaire, and included the same measures of alcohol consumption, alcohol problems, and demographics, but asked slightly different questions on IPV and alcohol-aggression expectancy (see below).
Because the primary dependent variable is IPV, respondents who were not married or did not live with a romantic partner were excluded (n=1,951 or 45.20% of U.S. respondents and n=555 or 24.63% of Japanese respondents). Additionally, those who had a missing value on the majority of the items used to create a problem drinking scale (n=3 U.S. respondents and n=117 Japanese respondents), either of IPV items (n=23 Japanese respondents), and an alcohol-aggression expectancy item (n=17 U.S. respondents and n=24 Japanese respondents) were excluded1. Finally, after excluding U.S. respondents who were outside the age range of the Japanese sample (see Footnote 2), the analyses were conducted with 2,343 U.S. and 1,532 Japanese respondents. Abstainers who do not drink alcohol were not excluded because their exclusion would result in a small variance on the IPV measure. All variables were created separately by country with missing values of each variable imputed with either the mode (for nominal and ordinal variables) or the mean (for interval-ratio variables) of that variable in the respective country sample.
Demographic Variables
Besides dummy variables measuring country (U.S.=1) and gender (males=1), five variables that capture basic socio-demographic variations across two samples were controlled in all multivariate analyses. Age is an interval-ratio variable2. Underage child is a dummy variable that measures whether or not the respondent lived with at least one underage child (= 1). Educational attainment is a dummy variable (some college degree or higher=1). Household income is a dummy variable (greater than the sample median category3=1). Employment status is a dummy variable (employed full time=1). Descriptive statistics of five control variables and gender calculated separately by country are shown in Table 1. As compared to the Japanese sample, the U.S. sample was significantly younger and had significantly higher proportions of males and those with some college or a higher education level, as well as those who live with at least one underage child and those who were not employed full time.
Table 1.
Descriptive Statistics of Control and Theoretical Variables and Frequency and Percentage Distributions of IPV
U.S. (n=2,343) | Japan (n=1,532) | Significance test 1 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Control variables | Gender | 54.55% males | 50.65% males | χ2=5.635* | |||
Mean age (S.D. ) | 40.71 (12.70) | 48.25 (11.84) | t =−18.547*** | ||||
Underage child | 62.01% living with underage child | 47.85% living with underage child | χ2=75.627*** | ||||
Educational attainment | 37.77% with some college or higher | 31.33% with some college or higher | χ2=16.840*** | ||||
Household income | 48.49% earned more than the median category | 51.76% earned more than the median category | χ2=3.980* | ||||
Employment status | 60.22% employed full time | 72.85% employed full time | χ2=65.073*** | ||||
| |||||||
U.S. (n=2,343) | Japan (n=1,532) | ||||||
| |||||||
Males (n=1,278) | Females (n=1,065) | Significance test3 | Males (n=776) | Females (n=756) | Significance test4 | ||
| |||||||
Theoretical variables | Mean problem drinking (S.D.) | 1.555 (1.623) | 0.8019 (1.213) | t =12.501*** | 1.0845 (1.408) | 0.325 (0.852) | t =12.727*** |
Alcohol-aggression expectancy2 | 16.28% | 12.30% | χ2=7.417** | 25.77% | 9.92% | χ2=65.339*** | |
| |||||||
IPV | Victimization only | 63 (4.93%) | 27 (2.54%) | 20 (2.58%) | 32 (4.23%) | ||
Offending only | 45 (3.52%) | 90 (8.45%) | 48 (6.19%) | 27 (3.57%) | |||
Both victimization and offending | 121 (9.47%) | 127 (11.92%) | 45 (5.80%) | 50 (6.61%) | |||
Neither victimization nor offending | 1,049 (82.08%) | 821 (77.2%) | 663 (85.44%) | 643 (85.05%) |
Significance test of difference between two countries.
The percentage of respondents who expect that alcohol would makes them more aggressive.
Significance test of difference between males and females.
All significance tests are two-tailed tests with
p <.05,
p <.01,
p <.001.
IPV
A variable measuring IPV was created based on respondents’ answers to two questions concerning their experiences with IPV offending and victimization. The Japanese and U.S. surveys differ slightly in terms of the indicator wording4, the specific behaviors used to measure IPV5, and the reference period in the two questions. The U.S. survey was more restrictive in terms of the behaviors (e.g., the Japanese survey includes “other,” an all-encompassing IPV-behavior category) and the reference period (i.e., past year for the U.S. survey and last two years for Japanese survey). Therefore, a caution is in order when comparing the results on IPV across these two countries. However, the primarily focus of the present study is the comparison of the gender differences in the associations among problem drinking, alcohol-aggression expectancy, and IPV, rather than the comparison of the country differences in the estimated values of these variables. The IPV questions in the two surveys are comparable enough to allow us to combine answers in each survey into a variable, called IPV, with the following four categories: “victimization only,” “offending only,” “both victimization and offending,” and “neither victimization nor offending.” Frequency and percentage distributions across these four categories of IPV are shown in Table 1, which indicate overall that while the experience of IPV was equally prevalent across gender in the Japanese sample (15%), it was more prevalent among females (23%) compared to males (18%) in the U.S. sample.
Problem Drinking
Although there are different measures of drinking examined with the GENACIS project, a scale measuring problem drinking was created because the items used to create this scale were the most comparable drinking measures available for two samples. Each of the seven items used for the problem drinking scale measured respondents’ experience in the past 12 months with alcohol-related consequences indicative of heavy and dependent drinking. All seven items were measured as “yes” or “no” of having in the past 12 months: “drunk enough to feel the effects of the alcohol,” “felt sick as a result of drinking,” “taken drink to get over any of the bad aftereffects of drinking,” “found that you were not able to stop drinking once you had started,” “needed a first drink in the morning to get yourself going after a heavy drinking session,” “had a feeling of guilt,” and “been unable to remember what happened the night before because you had been drinking.” A problem drinking scale was created by summing the number of “yes” answers on the seven items (α=0.71 and the mean of 1.21 with the S.D. of 1.50 for the U.S. sample and α=0.76 and the mean of 0.72 with the S.D. of 1.23 for the Japanese sample). Descriptive statistics of problem drinking calculated separately by country and gender are shown in Table 1. As expected, males in both countries engaged in a significantly higher level of problem drinking than females.
Alcohol-Aggression Expectancy
A variable alcohol-aggression expectancy was created based on respondents’ answers to a question regarding their expectation of alcohol’s effect on their own aggression. The wording of the question used to create this variable differed slightly across the two countries6. Alcohol-aggression expectancy is a dummy variable coded one for those who expect that alcohol would make them more aggressive. The percentage of respondents who expected that alcohol would make them more aggressive calculated separately by gender and country is shown in Table 1. As expected, males in both countries were significantly more likely than females to expect that alcohol would make them more aggressive.
ANALYSIS RESULTS
Because IPV is a (multi)nominal variable with four categories, Multinomial Logistic Regression analysis with “neither victimization nor offending” as the reference category was used as a primary analytical technique. Three types of analysis results produced by Multinomial Logistic Regression analysis are reported for most models. First, the likelihood ratio tests of the main effects of independent variables on the dependent variable as a whole were examined to compare across gender and country the significance of the associations between problem drinking and overall IPV and between alcohol-aggression expectancy and overall IPV. Second, the parameter estimates of the individual effects of independent variables on each category of the dependent variable were examined to compare across gender and country the significant associations between problem drinking and each of the specific categories of IPV and between alcohol-aggression expectancy and each of the specific categories of IPV. Finally, the likelihood ratio tests of overall models were examined to compare the model fit across gender and country. All multivariate models were assessed for assumptions and multicollinearity and were significant at the 0.05 significance level. The following analysis results compared across country on the gender differences related to the five hypotheses are reported: (1) IPV, (2) the association between problem drinking and IPV, (3) alcohol-aggression expectancy, and (4) the association between problem drinking and IPV, controlling for alcohol-aggression expectancy.
IPV
In order to establish a baseline model of the country and gender differences in IPV, IPV was regressed on gender separately by country. The likelihood ratio tests (see top of Model 1, Table 2) show that gender was significantly associated with IPV in both countries (χ2=29.01, p<.001 for the U.S. sample and χ2=14.15, p=.003 for the Japanese sample).
Table 2.
U.S. (n=2,343)
|
Japan (n=1,532)
|
||||||||||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | ||||||||||||||||||||
| |||||||||||||||||||||||||
Likelihood ratio tests of the main effects if IPV | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | |||||||
|
|
||||||||||||||||||||||||
IPV | Country (U.S.=1) | ||||||||||||||||||||||||
Gender (males=1) | 29.014 | 3 | 0 | 37.201 | 3 | 0.000 | 37.278 | 3 | 0.000 | 14.153 | 3 | 0.003 | 9.254 | 3 | 0.026 | 9.374 | 3 | 0.025 | |||||||
Problem drinking | 84.686 | 3 | 0.000 | 58.516 | 3 | 0.000 | 20.808 | 3 | 0.000 | 12.991 | 3 | 0.005 | |||||||||||||
Alcohol-aggression expectancy | 21.290 | 3 | 0.000 | 8.574 | 3 | 0.036 | |||||||||||||||||||
|
|
||||||||||||||||||||||||
Parameter estimates of individual effects of IPV | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | |
|
|
||||||||||||||||||||||||
Victimization only | Intercept | −0.120 | 0.458 | 0.793 | −0.787 | 0.498 | 0.114 | −0.558 | 0.567 | 0.325 | −1.554 | 0.703 | 0.027 | −1.425 | 0.753 | 0.058 | −1.609 | 0.834 | 0.054 | ||||||
Country (U.S.=1) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
Gender (males=1) | 0.736 | 0.253 | 2.088 | 0.004 | 0.503 | 0.262 | 1.653 | 0.055 | 0.505 | 0.262 | 1.658 | 0.054 | −0.830 | 0.309 | 0.436 | 0.007 | −0.781 | 0.324 | 0.458 | 0.016 | −0.763 | 0.326 | 0.466 | 0.019 | |
Problem drinking | 0.266 | 0.066 | 1.305 | 0.000 | 0.253 | 0.069 | 1.287 | 0.000 | −0.061 | 0.138 | 0.941 | 0.658 | −0.039 | 0.144 | 0.962 | 0.785 | |||||||||
Alcohol-aggression expectancy | 0.244 | 0.289 | 1.277 | 0.399 | −0.763 | 0.326 | 0.790 | 0.595 | |||||||||||||||||
|
|
||||||||||||||||||||||||
Offending only | Intercept | −1.565 | 0.384 | 0.000 | −2.156 | 0.423 | 0.000 | −1.368 | 0.472 | 0.004 | −1.024 | 0.554 | 0.064 | −1.852 | 0.616 | 0.003 | −1.712 | 0.661 | 0.010 | ||||||
Country (U.S.=1) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
Gender (males=1) | −0.832 | 0.201 | 0.435 | 0.000 | −1.032 | 0.211 | 0.356 | 0.000 | −1.03 | 0.211 | 0.357 | 0.000 | 0.688 | 0.288 | 1.992 | 0.017 | 0.417 | 0.302 | 1.517 | 0.167 | 0.399 | 0.303 | 1.49 | 0.187 | |
Problem drinking | 0.231 | 0.061 | 1.260 | 0.000 | 0.170 | 0.065 | 1.185 | 0.009 | 0.306 | 0.081 | 1.357 | 0.000 | 0.288 | 0.086 | 1.334 | 0.001 | |||||||||
Alcohol-aggression expectancy | 0.840 | 0.231 | 2.315 | 0.000 | 0.177 | 0.299 | 1.193 | 0.553 | |||||||||||||||||
|
|
||||||||||||||||||||||||
Both victimization and offending | Intercept | 0.312 | 0.307 | 0.309 | −0.686 | 0.340 | 0.043 | −0.047 | 0.383 | 0.903 | 0.473 | 0.478 | 0.322 | −0.147 | 0.526 | 0.780 | 0.438 | 0.565 | 0.438 | ||||||
Country (U.S.=1) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
Gender (males=1) | −0.103 | 0.151 | 0.903 | 0.498 | −0.48 | 0.162 | 0.618 | 0.003 | −0.488 | 0.163 | 0.613 | 0.003 | −0.053 | 0.25 | 0.949 | 0.833 | −0.278 | 0.264 | 0.757 | 0.292 | −0.356 | 0.267 | 0.7 | 0.182 | |
Problem drinking | 0.380 | 0.044 | 1.463 | 0.000 | 0.332 | 0.047 | 1.394 | 0.000 | 0.248 | 0.078 | 1.282 | 0.001 | 0.164 | 0.084 | 1.178 | 0.050 | |||||||||
Alcohol-aggression expectancy | 0.666 | 0.182 | 1.946 | 0.000 | 0.760 | 0.264 | 2.137 | 0.004 |
The category, neither victimization nor offending, is set as the reference category.
Control variables are age, underage child, employment status, household income, and educational attainment.
Parameter estimates of individual effects of IPV are examined using Two-Tailed Significance Tests.
The parameter estimates (see bottom of Model 1, Table 2), however, indicate that gender was differentially associated with categories of the IPV in the two countries. Consistent with the expectation, females were significantly more likely than males to experience “victimization only” and males were significantly more likely than females to experience “offending only” in the Japanese sample. The gender difference in IPV was unexpectedly reversed in the U.S. sample, on the other hand, where males were significantly more likely than females to experience “victimization only” and females were significantly more likely than males to experience “offending only.” The findings in the U.S. sample are, however, consistent with some studies (e.g., Archer 2006) that found a higher rate of aggression among females toward their male partners, though not necessarily a higher degree of aggression that results in greater injuries. Indeed, behaviors used to create IPV varied greatly in the degree of seriousness and aggression from “pushing or grabbing” to “used a knife or gun,” and thus, it is possible that even though the rate of offending is higher among females compared to males in the U.S. sample, the degree of seriousness and aggression is higher among males compared to females. When the analysis was run with two countries combined (see the combined results in Table 2), the parameter estimate for the interaction term between country and gender was significant for “victimization only” and “offending only,” confirming the inverse results of the association between gender and IPV found across the two countries. In both countries, there was no significant gender difference in the likelihood of experiencing “both offending and victimization.”
Problem Drinking and Its association with IPV
The inverse results found across countries on the gender difference in IPV, albeit unexpected results for the U.S., provide an interesting cross-national context to examine gender differences in associations among problem drinking, alcohol-aggression expectancy, and IPV. In the next set of analyses, country and gender differences in problem drinking were examined first, and then the gender difference in the association between problem drinking and IPV was examined across country.
First, country and gender differences in the level of problem drinking were examined by regressing problem drinking on country and gender using Ordinary Least Squares analysis (see the combined model in Table 3). The results indicate that the U.S. respondents engaged in a significantly higher level of problem drinking (β=.106, p<.001) than Japanese respondents. The analysis run separately by country (see the separate models in Table 3) show that, as expected, males in both countries engaged in a significantly higher level of problem drinking than females (β=.258, p<.001 in the U.S. sample and β=.307, p<.001 in the Japanese sample). The gender difference, however, did not differ significantly across two countries.
Table 3.
Multiple Regression Analyses1 with Problem Drinking and Alcohol-Aggression Expectancy as Dependent Variables, Separated by Country and for a Combined Sample
U.S. (n=2,343) | Japan (n=1,532) | Combined (n=3,875) | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|
|
|||||||||||
b | S.E. | β | p | b | S.E. | β | p | b | S.E. | β | p | ||
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Problem drinking2 | Intercept | 1.587 | 0.145 | 0.000 | 1.592 | 0.187 | 0.000 | 1.326 | 0.125 | 0.000 | |||
Country (U.S.=1) | 0.308 | 0.046 | 0.106 | 0.000 | |||||||||
Gender (males=1) | 0.776 | 0.062 | 0.258 | 0.000 | 0.753 | 0.064 | 0.307 | 0.000 | 0.765 | 0.045 | 0.269 | 0.000 | |
R2 | 0.107 | 0.157 | 0.145 | ||||||||||
| |||||||||||||
Alcohol-aggression expectancy3 | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | |
|
|
|
|||||||||||
Intercept | −1.308 | 0.291 | 0.000 | −0.888 | 0.426 | 0.037 | −1.238 | 0.274 | 0.000 | ||||
Country (U.S.=1) | 0.068 | 0.158 | 1.070 | 0.669 | |||||||||
Gender (males=1) | 0.301 | 0.128 | 1.351 | 0.019 | 1.266 | 0.169 | 3.546 | 0.000 | 1.173 | 0.154 | 3.232 | 0.000 | |
Country×Gender | −0.831 | 0.191 | 0.435 | 0.000 | |||||||||
Nagelkerke R2 | 0.027 | 0.099 | 0.058 |
Control variables are age, underage child, employment status, household income, and educational attainment.
Ordinary least squares regression results with two-tailed tests.
Binary logistic regression results with two-tailed tests.
Second, the gender difference in the association between problem drinking and IPV was examined. The likelihood ratio tests separated by country and gender (see the top of Model 1, Table 4) show that problem drinking was significantly associated with IPV among all groups, except Japanese females. The parameter estimates separated by country and gender (see the bottom of Model 1, Table 4) show gender differences in both countries in how problem drinking and IPV are associated. While problem drinking had significant positive associations with all three categories of IPV among U.S. males, it had significant positive associations with “offending only” and “both victimization and offending” among U.S. females and Japanese males. Among Japanese females, problem drinking had a significant positive association with “offending only”7. Our results, therefore, indicate some gender differences in the alcohol-related IPV in both countries.
Table 4.
Multinomial Logistic Regression Analysis1 2 3 Regressing IPV on Problem Drinking and Alcohol-aggression expectancy Separated by Country and Gender
U.S. (n
=2,343) |
Japan
(n=1,532) |
||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Males (n =1,278) | Females (n =1,065) | Males (n =776) | Females (n =756) | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 1 | Model 2 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Likelihood ratio tests main effects of IPV | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | |||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||||||||||||
IPV | Problem drinking | 51.9 | 3 | 0 | 34.569 | 3 | 0.000 | 34.1 | 3 | 0 | 26.233 | 3 | 0.000 | 18.2 | 3 | 0 | 12.774 | 3 | 0.005 | 6.43 | 3 | 0.09 | 4.436 | 3 | 0.218 | ||||||||
Alcohol-aggression expectancy | 13.032 | 3 | 0.004 | 9.355 | 3 | 0.025 | 4.628 | 3 | 0.201 | 3.196 | 3 | 0.362 | |||||||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||
Parameter estimates of individual effects of IPV | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | b | S.E. | OR | p | |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||
Victimization only | Intercept | −0.422 | 0.611 | 0.490 | −0.151 | 0.682 | 0.824 | −1.759 | 0.824 | 0.033 | −1.851 | 1.033 | 0.073 | −0.768 | 1.188 | 0.518 | −0.904 | 1.280 | 0.480 | −0.967 | 0.907 | 0.286 | −1.209 | 1.124 | 0.282 | ||||||||
Problem drinking | 0.282 | 0.075 | 1.325 | 0.000 | 0.265 | 0.078 | 1.303 | 0.001 | 0.174 | 0.152 | 1.190 | 0.252 | 0.178 | 0.156 | 1.195 | 0.252 | 0.069 | 0.157 | 1.071 | 0.663 | 0.082 | 0.164 | 1.086 | 0.617 | −0.425 | 0.328 | 0.654 | 0.194 | −0.408 | 0.336 | 0.665 | 0.224 | |
Alcohol-aggression expectancy | 0.296 | 0.331 | 1.344 | 0.371 | −0.087 | 0.646 | 0.917 | 0.893 | −0.149 | 0.558 | 0.861 | 0.789 | −0.298 | 0.770 | 0.742 | 0.699 | |||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||
Offending only | Intercept | −2.960 | 0.694 | 0.000 | −2.268 | 0.757 | 0.003 | −0.780 | 0.496 | 0.116 | 0.094 | 0.578 | 0.871 | −1.957 | 0.796 | 0.014 | −1.853 | 0.840 | 0.027 | −1.709 | 0.923 | 0.064 | −1.424 | 1.068 | 0.183 | ||||||||
Problem drinking | 0.275 | 0.086 | 1.317 | 0.001 | 0.222 | 0.090 | 1.248 | 0.014 | 0.197 | 0.091 | 1.217 | 0.031 | 0.124 | 0.096 | 1.132 | 0.197 | 0.322 | 0.095 | 1.380 | 0.001 | 0.310 | 0.100 | 1.363 | 0.002 | 0.321 | 0.154 | 1.379 | 0.037 | 0.279 | 0.176 | 1.322 | 0.113 | |
Alcohol-aggression expectancy | 0.766 | 0.358 | 2.151 | 0.032 | 0.934 | 0.310 | 2.545 | 0.003 | 0.136 | 0.342 | 1.145 | 0.691 | 0.316 | 0.602 | 1.372 | 0.409 | |||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||
Both victimization and offending | Intercept | −0.770 | 0.440 | 0.083 | −0.078 | 0.493 | 0.874 | 0.106 | 0.483 | 0.827 | 0.548 | 0.565 | 0.332 | −0.572 | 0.791 | 0.470 | −0.066 | 0.841 | 0.937 | 0.406 | 0.659 | 0.537 | 1.064 | 0.756 | 0.159 | ||||||||
Problem drinking | 0.350 | 0.056 | 1.420 | 0.000 | 0.295 | 0.059 | 1.344 | 0.000 | 0.427 | 0.073 | 1.532 | 0.000 | 0.396 | 0.076 | 1.485 | 0.000 | 0.307 | 0.096 | 1.360 | 0.001 | 0.239 | 0.103 | 1.270 | 0.020 | 0.090 | 0.153 | 1.094 | 0.555 | −0.014 | 0.164 | 0.986 | 0.930 | |
Alcohol-aggression expectancy | 0.770 | 0.236 | 2.160 | 0.001 | 0.458 | 0.295 | 1.582 | 0.120 | 0.736 | 0.347 | 2.088 | 0.034 | 0.741 | 0.425 | 2.096 | 0.082 |
The category, neither victimization nor offending, is set as the reference category.
Control variables are age, underage child, employment status, household income, and educational attainment.
Parameter estimates of individual effects of IPV are examined using Two-Tailed Significance Tests.
Additionally, the analysis run separated by country (see Model 2, Table 2) shows that problem drinking explained some of the gender difference in IPV. There were, however, country variations that reflect the inverse association between gender and IPV across countries. For U.S. respondents, problem drinking explained away the gender difference in “victimization only” or why males were more likely than females to experience “victimization only” but failed to explain the gender difference in “offending only” or why females were more likely than males to experience “offending only.” For U.S. respondents, moreover, the inclusion of problem drinking strengthened the gender difference in “both offending and victimization,” indicating that females were more likely than males to experience “both offending and victimization” after controlling for problem drinking. The change in the significance of problem drinking is explained by the stronger association between problem drinking and “both offending and victimization” for females compared to males in the U.S. sample (see Model 1, Table 4). For Japanese respondents, on the other hand, problem drinking explained away the gender difference in “offending only” or why males were more likely than females to experience “offending only” but failed to explain the gender difference in “victimization only” or why females were more likely than males to experience “victimization only.” The latter finding among Japanese respondents is understandable given that problem drinking overall did not have a significant association with “victimization only” among either gender in Japan (see Model 1, Table 4). Indeed, problem drinking was associated with “victimization only” only among U.S. males, consistent with how problem drinking explained away the gender difference in “victimization only” among U.S. respondents. Among Japanese respondents, problem drinking was significantly associated with the experience with “both offending and victimization,” but this significance came mainly from the significant association between these two variables among males in the Japanese sample.
Our results, overall, indicate some gender differences in the association between problem drinking and IPV in both countries. Contrary to the hypothesis, however, there is no clear gender difference in the type of IPV (i.e., victimization or offending) associated with problem drinking. Instead, our findings suggest that problem drinking is more strongly associated with IPV in general (be it victimization or offending) among males compared to females in both countries. This gender difference might explain why gender difference in “victimization only” among U.S. respondents and in “offending only” among Japanese respondents became insignificant once problem drinking was controlled. Not only did males in both countries engage in a higher level of problem drinking than females, but males were also more likely than females to experience “victimization only” in the U.S. sample and “offending only” in the Japanese sample.
Finally, to compare across gender and country the increase in the proportion of the variance of IPV explained from the baseline model with only control variables to the model with problem drinking, the likelihood ratio tests for the significance of two models were calculated separately by country and gender (first two columns of Table 5). The results indicate that both models were significant for all four groups. The improvement in models resulting from adding problem drinking was calculated by subtracting chi-square values of the two models (fourth column in Table 5), which once again indicated that problem drinking was significant in explaining IPV in every group, except Japanese females. Overall, confirming the earlier results, the model fit compared across four groups indicates that problem drinking was more strongly associated with IPV among males than among females in both countries. The results, therefore, suggest that to the extent there is an association between problem drinking and IPV, it is the problem drinking of males in both countries that is associated with IPV (whether for victimization or offending). Additionally, the model fit compared across four groups indicates that problem drinking was more strongly associated with IPV among U.S. respondents than among Japanese respondents, and indeed, the association was even stronger for U.S. females compared to Japanese males, while it was nonexistent among Japanese females.
Table 5.
Likelihood Ratio Tests for Significance of the Multinomial Logistic Regression Models with IPV as the Dependent Variable by Country and Gender
Country/gender subgroups | (1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Control variables |
Control variables and problem
drinking |
Control variables, problem
drinking, and alcohol-aggression expectancy |
Improvements from (1) to (2)
|
Improvements from (2) to (3)
|
Improvements from (1) to (3)
|
|||||||||||||
χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | χ2 | df | p | |
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
U.S. males (n =1,278) | 120.774 | 15 | 0.000 | 172.67 | 18 | 0.000 | 185.702 | 21 | 0.000 | 51.896 | 3 | 0.000 | 13.032 | 3 | 0.005 | 64.928 | 6 | 0.000 |
U.S. females (n =1,065) | 148.985 | 15 | 0.000 | 183.070 | 18 | 0.000 | 192.426 | 21 | 0.000 | 34.085 | 3 | 0.000 | 9.356 | 3 | 0.025 | 43.441 | 6 | 0.000 |
Japan males (n =776) | 55.086 | 15 | 0.000 | 73.250 | 18 | 0.000 | 77.877 | 21 | 0.000 | 18.164 | 3 | 0.000 | 4.627 | 3 | 0.201 | 22.791 | 6 | 0.000 |
Japan females (n =756) | 51.285 | 15 | 0.000 | 57.714 | 18 | 0.000 | 60.91 | 21 | 0.000 | 6.429 | 3 | 0.093 | 3.196 | 3 | 0.362 | 9.625 | 6 | 0.141 |
Alcohol-Aggression Expectancy and Problem Drinking’s Association with IPV
There was a gender difference in the association between problem drinking and IPV in both countries, though not in the ways hypothesized among the U.S. respondents. In the final set of analyses, country and gender differences in alcohol-aggression expectancy were examined first, and country differences in how alcohol-aggression expectancy explains the gender difference in the association between problem drinking and IPV were examined second by adding alcohol-aggression expectancy to the model.
First, country and gender differences in the expectation of aggression associated with alcohol use were examined by regressing alcohol-aggression expectancy on country and gender using Logistic Regression analysis (see the combined model in Table 3). The results indicate that, consistent with the hypothesis, the gender difference in alcohol-aggression expectancy was significantly greater in the Japanese sample compared to the U.S. sample. The analysis run separately by country (see the separate models in Table 3) indicates that Japanese males had 3.55 times higher odds (p<.001) than Japanese females and U.S. males had 1.35 times higher odds (p=.019) than U.S. females of expecting that alcohol would make them more aggressive.
Second, how alcohol-aggression expectancy explains the gender difference in the association between problem drinking and IPV was examined across country by regressing IPV on problem drinking, while controlling for alcohol-aggression expectancy. The likelihood ratio tests run separately by country and gender (see top of Model 2, Table 4) indicate that alcohol-aggression expectancy had a significant association with IPV only among U.S. respondents. The parameter estimates (see bottom of Model 2, Table 4) show in more detail the country and gender differences in the alcohol-aggression expectancy’s association with IPV and suggest five major findings.
First, alcohol-aggression expectancy was not associated with the likelihood of experiencing “victimization only” among any of the four groups, and thus, gender remained significantly associated with this category of IPV among Japanese respondents (see Model 3 of Table 2). In other words, neither problem drinking nor alcohol-aggression expectancy could explain why females were significantly more likely than males to experience “victimization only” in the Japanese sample. This finding is, however, expected, because alcohol-aggression expectancy concerns with alcohol’s effect on aggression and violent offending, and thus, is less likely to be associated with the likelihood of experiencing violent victimization.
Second, all of the significant associations between problem drinking and IPV remained significant after alcohol-aggression expectancy was included in the model, except for the association between problem drinking and “offending only” among females in both countries. The finding indicates that alcohol-aggression expectancy explains the association between problem drinking and the experience with “offending only” among females in both countries. Among males in both countries, on the other hand, problem drinking had a direct association with IPV after controlling for alcohol-aggression expectancy. Some of the gender differences in problem drinking’s association with IPV, therefore, can be explained by the gender difference in alcohol-aggression expectancy.
Thirdly, although Japanese males were significantly more likely than any other groups to expect that alcohol would make them more aggressive, alcohol-aggression expectancy had no significant association with “offending only” among them. The only category of IPV associated with alcohol-aggression expectancy among Japanese males was “both offending and victimization.” Among Japanese females, neither problem drinking nor alcohol-aggression expectancy had any significant association with any of the categories of IPV. Among Japanese respondents, therefore, problem drinking, and more specifically the problem drinking of males, but not their alcohol-aggression expectancy, explained the gender difference in the likelihood of experiencing “offending only,” while neither the problem drinking nor the alcohol-aggression expectancy of females explain the gender difference in the likelihood of experiencing “victimization only.” Among U.S. respondents, on the other hand, although alcohol-aggression expectancy had a significant association with the likelihood of experiencing “offending only” among both males and females, it cannot explain why females were more likely than males to experience “offending only,” as gender remained significant in the combined model (see Model 3, Table 2). Among U.S. respondents, like Japanese respondents, there was a significant association between alcohol-aggression expectancy and “both offending victimization” among males but not among females.
Fourthly, in order to test Hypothesis 3, which stated that the association between alcohol use and IPV offending varies by alcohol-aggression expectancy, such that alcohol use and IPV offending are more strongly associated among those who expect that alcohol would make them more aggressive, while the association between alcohol use and IPV victimization does not vary by alcohol-aggression expectancy, an interaction term between alcohol-aggression expectancy and problem drinking was examined in the model separated by country and gender. Contrary to the hypothesis, the interaction term was significant only among Japanese males on “both offending and victimization” (results not shown). Consistent with the expectation, the positive association between problem drinking and this category of IPV was experienced more strongly among those Japanese males who expected that alcohol would make them more aggressive than those who did not expect that alcohol would make them more aggressive.
Finally, in order to compare the increase in the proportion of the variance of IPV explained from the model with problem drinking to the model with both problem drinking and alcohol-aggression expectancy, the likelihood ratio tests for the significance of the models were calculated separately by country and gender (see columns 2 and 3 of Table 5). The improvement in the two models resulting from adding alcohol-aggression expectancy was calculated for each of the four groups by subtracting the chi-square values of the two models (see column 5 in Table 5). The results once again indicate that alcohol-aggression expectancy was not significant in explaining IPV among either genders of Japanese respondents, while it was significant in explaining IPV among both genders of U.S. respondents.
DISCUSSION
Our results overall provide some support for the five hypotheses and, based on the review of literature, generate four major implications and offer suggestions for future research.
First, there was a significant gender difference in the level of problem drinking in both countries. Consistent with the expectation, males in both countries engaged in a significantly higher level of problem drinking than females. Contrary to Hypothesis 5, however, this gender difference did not vary significantly across the two countries.
Second, consistent with Hypothesis 1, the association between problem drinking and IPV varied by gender in both countries, but in an unexpected way among U.S. respondents. Among Japanese respondents, while problem drinking was positively associated with IPV offending among males, it was not associated with any of the categories of IPV, including IPV victimization, among females. Among Japanese respondents, moreover, problem drinking explained away the gender difference in the likelihood of experiencing IPV offending but not of IPV victimization. Among Japanese respondents, overall, problem drinking was associated with IPV only among males, and more specifically male perpetrators of IPV, but not among females on any categories of IPV. Unlike Japanese respondents, among U.S. respondents, problem drinking of IPV victims, and specifically male victims, was important in explaining their own IPV victimization, as the problem drinking explained away the gender difference in the likelihood of experiencing IPV victimization. Additionally, in contrast to Japanese respondents, the problem drinking of females and not just of males was important in explaining overall IPV among U.S. respondents. The model fit compared across four groups indicates that problem drinking was more strongly associated with IPV among males compared to females in both countries, and problem drinking was more strongly associated with IPV among U.S. respondents compared to Japanese respondents.
Third, consistent with Hypothesis 2, males in both countries were significantly more likely than females to expect that alcohol would make them more aggressive. Consistent with Hypothesis 5, moreover, the gender difference in alcohol-aggression expectancy was significantly greater among Japanese respondents than U.S. respondents. Indeed, while Japanese males had almost four times higher odds than Japanese females of expecting that alcohol would make them more aggressive, the gender difference in alcohol-aggression expectancy found among U.S. respondents was relatively small.
Fourth, contrary to Hypothesis 4, alcohol-aggression expectancy overall accounted for only some of the gender differences in the association between problem drinking and IPV in both countries. The inclusion of alcohol-aggression expectancy in the model produced no significant difference in the association between problem drinking and IPV, except among females on IPV offending in both countries, suggesting that alcohol-aggression expectancy explains the association between problem drinking and the experience with IPV offending only among females. Alcohol-aggression expectancy overall had no significant association with the likelihood of experiencing IPV victimization in either country. Indeed, problem drinking among U.S. males was the only independent variable significantly associated with the likelihood of experiencing IPV victimization. The failure of alcohol-aggression expectancy in explaining the gender difference in alcohol-related IPV results from weak associations between alcohol-aggression expectancy and IPV offending among Japanese males and between alcohol-aggression expectancy and IPV victimization among U.S. males. Additionally, neither problem drinking nor alcohol-aggression expectancy explained the higher likelihood of experiencing IPV victimization among Japanese females, compared to Japanese males, nor among U.S. males, compared to U.S. females. Indeed, neither problem drinking nor alcohol-aggression expectancy had a significant association with IPV among Japanese females, suggesting that the problem drinking of their male perpetrators might more strongly associate with Japanese females’ experience with IPV. The same cannot be said about U.S. respondents, however, as there was no significant association between problem drinking and offending among U.S. females. For U.S. males, their own problem drinking, and not the problem drinking of female perpetrators, seems to be more strongly associated with their higher likelihood of experiencing IPV victimization compared to U.S. females. In addition, although alcohol-aggression expectancy was significantly associated with IPV offending among both genders in the U.S., neither problem drinking nor alcohol-aggression expectancy explained the higher likelihood of experiencing IPV offending among females compared to males. Both problem drinking and alcohol-aggression expectancy were overall significantly more likely to explain IPV of males, compared to females, and of U.S. respondents, compared to Japanese respondents.
The present study contributes to the cross-national research on alcohol-related IPV by examining alcohol-aggression expectancy as an explanation for the gender difference in the association between problem drinking and IPV in the U.S. and Japan. Gender and country differences in problem drinking’s association with IPV confirm the importance of taking into account factors besides pharmacological effects of alcohol on violence. When only a single country is examined, as in most empirical studies of alcohol and violence, and a gender difference is found, it is difficult to explicate why such gender difference exists because gender is usually treated as a proxy for a multitude of different factors, including biological sex differences and gender norms. The present study is unique in that country differences can be attributed not to biological differences but rather to structural and cultural differences, assuming that alcohol has a similar pharmacological effect on aggression when two populations are compared. Japan also provides an interesting contrast to the U.S. because these two countries differ in terms of their levels of gender inequality and how strongly gender norms are prescribed.
The present study has several limitations that suggest directions for future studies. First, the data collection method differed between the U.S. and Japanese data. Some empirical research suggests that the method of data collection (e.g., face-to-face interviews vs. telephone or postal surveys) can affect the validity of data, especially when sensitive personal questions are asked; other research, however, does not find such differences (see Greenfield and Kerr 2008). Second, the GENACIS data also varied in the wording of some questions in the U.S. and Japan surveys. The most important concern may be how the questions about IPV were asked across the two samples, and though the questions were similar enough to allow us to create a comparable IPV variable, the U.S. survey used more restricting questions about IPV than the Japanese survey. Thus country differences found in the present study need to be interpreted with a caution. Given the higher prevalence of IPV among U.S. respondents compared to Japanese respondents found in the present study, it is possible that the country differences would be even greater had the U.S. survey used a less restrictive, more identical measure of IPV.
A third limitation is the inherent difficulty of interpreting self-reports of IPV, particularly of IPV offending. Although many early studies of IPV asked (or reported) only about males’ offending and females’ victimization, more recent surveys have increasingly asked both males and females about both offending and victimization. Such surveys, especially in higher-income countries, often find that women report similar or even somewhat higher rates of IPV offending, as we found among U.S. respondents in the present study, and a high prevalence of “mutual competency” where both parties are involved in the escalation of IPV (Cochran et al. 2011). Such findings are difficult to interpret due to the lack in most IPV measures of questions about severity and questions that allow proactive or unprovoked acts of aggression to be distinguished from reactive or self-defensive aggressive behaviors (see Graham et al. 2008). Future multinational studies of IPV would be strengthened by the use of more nuanced, context-specific IPV measures that allow inferences about the severity and the proactive vs. reactive nature of intimate aggressive behaviors. The unexpected findings among U.S. respondents may indeed reflect a greater tendency of IPV episodes in the U.S. to involve aggression and victimization by both partners as an escalation of conflict from initially verbal conflict, and that such verbal conflict may not seem like aggression to men (more than to women), resulting in more men claiming they were victims and not aggressors (see for a review on the gender difference in the perception of IPV, Russell and Kraus 2016).
Fourth, although the present study used an empirically-derived scale of problem drinking, more general and objective measures of drinking, especially the frequency and typical quantity of drinking, could more directly test the pharmacological disinhibition hypothesis against the learned disinhibition hypothesis. Fifth, our problem drinking scale does not measure the engagement in drinking immediately preceding IPV involvement. Thus, even if a respondent engages in a higher level of problem drinking, this does not mean that he/she was drinking at the time of IPV involvement. Finally, our cross-sectional data pose a limitation in explicating the causal order of the variables. It is possible that our dependent variable, IPV, could affect problem drinking or alcohol-aggression expectancy, rather than the other way around. It is also possible that problem drinking and IPV are both manifestations of an underlying problem in one’s life (e.g., marital problem) that shares a common cause (Costello et al. 2014), and thus they might have no causal association with each other. In order to explicate the causal sequence among the theoretical variables and IPV, it is important to utilize longitudinal data that include measures of respondents’ drinking and alcohol-aggression expectancy that temporally precede their IPV involvement.
CONCLUSIONS
As expected from previous studies, the present study found gender differences in the association between problem drinking and IPV in both the U.S. and Japan. Overall, problem drinking had a stronger association with IPV among males compared to females in both countries. Contrary to our hypothesis, however, while problem drinking had significant positive associations with all three categories of IPV among U.S. males, it had significant positive associations with “offending only” and “both victimization and offending” among U.S. females and Japanese males. Among Japanese females, problem drinking had no significant association with IPV. Additionally, although males were more likely than females in both countries to expect that alcohol would make them more aggressive, alcohol-aggression expectancy cannot explain the gender difference in the association between problem drinking and IPV in either the U.S. and Japan. The failure to find expected patterns among Japanese respondents could be attributed to two factors. First, neither problem drinking nor alcohol-aggression expectancy was associated with IPV victimization among Japanese females, thus these variables cannot explain why Japanese females were more likely than Japanese males to experience IPV victimization. Second, while problem drinking was significantly associated with IPV offending among Japanese males and even though they were the most likely of all groups to expect that alcohol would make them more aggressive, alcohol-aggression expectancy was not associated with IPV offending among Japanese males. Thus, problem drinking had a direct association with IPV offending among Japanese males and explained why they were more likely than Japanese females to experience IPV offending. Among Japanese respondents, therefore, the problem drinking of males explains the gender difference in IPV. Similarly, among U.S. respondents, the failure to find expected patterns appears to be due to the failure of alcohol-aggression expectancy to explain IPV among males. Although problem drinking had positive associations with both IPV offending and IPV victimization among U.S. males, alcohol-aggression expectancy did little to explain their overall IPV. In both countries, therefore, it is the males’ drinking that explained the gender difference in IPV, whether for victimization or offending, and contrary to the learned-disinhibition hypothesis, alcohol-aggression expectancy could not explain the gender difference in the association between problem drinking and IPV.
Figure 1.
Linear Regression Equations for Age of Onset of Drinking and Experiences of Problem Drinking (Left) and Age of Onset of Drinking and Harmful Effects of Drinking (Right) by Country and Gender.
Note: All models control for age, marital status, underage child, income, education, and employment status.
Figure 2.
Results of the SEM Analysis for the U.S. Sample (Top) and the Japanese Sample (Bottom) with Standardized Coefficients.
Note: Standardized coefficients are shown as males/females. All models control for age, marital status, underage child, income, education, and employment status. All significance tests are two-tailed tests with * <.05, ** <.01, and *** <.001.
Figure 3.
Results of the SEM Analysis for the U.S. Sample (Top) and the Japanese Sample (bottom) with Standardized Coefficients.
Note: Standardized coefficients are shown as males/females. All models control for age, marital status, underage child, income, education, and employment status. All significance tests are two-tailed tests with * <.05, ** <.01, and *** <.001.
Biographies
Miyuki Fukushima Tedor is an associate professor in the Department of Criminology, Anthropology, and Sociology at Cleveland State University. She received BAs in philosophy, psychology, and sociology, a MA in sociology, and a PhD in sociology, all from the University of Oklahoma. Her research interests include cross-national examination, between the United States and Japan, of theories of crime; gender and crime; juvenile delinquency; and drugs and alcohol use and crime.
Linda M. Quinn is a Senior College Lecturer and Statistical Consultant in the Mathematics department at Cleveland State University since 2009. She has been involved in statistical applications and analyses of research studies since 1989. She has been a lead statistician at such institutions as the Cleveland Research Institute, Case Western Reserve University Department of Medicine, the program in Health Care Research at the Cleveland Veterans Affairs Medical Center, AcroMed Corporation, and American Greetings. She was an independent statistical consultant for over 20 years applying statistics across many disciplines. Her graduate studies were at Cleveland State University, Bowling Green State University, and Case Western Reserve University. She has MS degrees in statistics, computer science, and operations research and a doctorate in educational leadership and lifelong learning.
Sharon Carlson Wilsnack received her B.A. from Kansas State University, her M.A. and Ph.D. in clinical psychology from Harvard University, and studied as a Fulbright Fellow at the University of Freiburg, Federal Republic of Germany. She is presently Chester Fritz Distinguished Professor in the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, University of North Dakota School of Medicine and Health Sciences. Dr. Wilsnack’s background includes experience as a substance abuse therapist and treatment program director as well as in research and medical education. Sharon Wilsnack and Richard Wilsnack direct a 20-year longitudinal study of drinking behavior in U.S. women, and coordinate an international collaborative research project on gender and alcohol which involves researchers from more than 40 countries. Sharon Wilsnack is a Fellow of the American Psychological Association. She served as a member of the Institute of Medicine’s Committee to Study Fetal Alcohol Syndrome, as a member of the National Advisory Council on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism/National Institutes of Health, and on numerous other boards and advisory groups concerned with alcohol abuse and women’s health. She was a member and panel chair of the NIAAA Task Force on College Drinking and a member of the Center for Substance Abuse Treatment’s Treatment Improvement Protocol (TIP) Consensus Panel on Special Needs of Women in Substance Abuse Treatment.
Richard W. Wilsnack, Ph.D., is Professor Emeritus in the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, University of North Dakota School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Grand Forks, North Dakota, USA. He received a B.A. from Pomona College (Claremont, California), an M.Sc. (Econ.) from the London School of Economics, and a Ph.D. in sociology from Harvard University. His research interests include gender influences on alcohol consumption and related problems; cross-cultural patterns of alcohol consumption and alcohol-related harms; and longitudinal and retrospective research on drinking histories. Sharon Wilsnack and Richard Wilsnack have directed a 20-year national longitudinal study of alcohol use and abuse in U.S. women, and have coordinated the multinational GENACIS project, which includes comparable general population surveys of men’s and women’s alcohol use and alcohol-related problems in 38 countries on 5 continents. They are currently engaged in multinational research on alcohol-related harms to individuals other than the drinkers.
Thomas K. Greenfield, PhD directed the NIAAA-supported National Alcohol Research Center from 1999 to 2015, and continues as the scientific director of the Alcohol Research Group, of the Public Health Institute, in Emeryville, California. He is also core training faculty for the Clifford Attkisson Clinical Services Research Training Program in the Department of Psychiatry at UCSF. He currently leads, with Kate Karriker-Jaffe, an R01 grant on alcohol’s harms to others (AHTO) among US adults, and with Sharon Wilsnack and Kim Bloomfield is beginning a new international AHTO grant that will examine how national policies are associated with these harms in high, low and middle income countries. He now serves on the NIH Community Influences on Health Behavior study section.
Footnotes
The American and Japanese data used in this study are from the project, Gender, Alcohol and Culture: An International Study (GENACIS). GENACIS is a collaborative international project affiliated with the Kettil Bruun Society for Social and Epidemiological Research on Alcohol and coordinated by GENACIS partners from the University of North Dakota, Aarhus University, the Alcohol Research Group/Public Health Institute, the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, the University of Melbourne, and the Research Institute of Addiction Switzerland. Support for aspects of the project comes from the World Health Organization, the Quality of Life and Management of Living Resources Programme of the European Commission (Concerted Action QLG4-CT-2001-0196), the U.S. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism/National Institutes of Health (Grants R21 AA012941 and R01 AA015775), the German Federal Ministry of Health, the Pan American Health Organization, and Swiss national funds. Support for individual country surveys was provided by government agencies and other national sources. The study leaders and funding sources for data sets used in this report are: Japan: Shinji Shimizu, Ph.D., Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (Grant 13410072); USA: Thomas K. Greenfield, Ph.D., U.S. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism/National Institutes of Health (Grants R01AA04610, R21AA12941, and P50 AA05595).
Because these three measures (problem drinking, IPV, and alcohol-aggression expectancy) are dependent and theoretical variables that are critical for the present study, cases with a missing value on these measures were excluded. The analyses done with missing values imputed instead of excluded, however, produced results that are identical to the results reported here.
The Japanese sample was limited to persons who were between the ages of 20 and 70 years old. U.S. respondents who were younger than 20 or older than 70 were, therefore, excluded to match the age range of the two samples.
Household income was originally measured as an ordinal variable in both countries. The median income category for the present study used for the U.S. sample was $30,000 and for the Japanese sample was ¥7 million, calculated separately for each country.
The U.S. questions asked, “In the past year, has your (husband/wife/partner) ever done one of the following things?” (victimization) and “In the past year, have you ever done one of the following things to your (husband/wife/partner)?” (offending). The Japanese questions asked (in Japanese), “People can be physically aggressive in many ways, for example, punching, or in some other way. What is the most physically aggressive thing your spouse/partner has done to you during the last 2 years?” (victimization) and “What is the most physically aggressive thing you have done during the last 2 years to your spouse/partner?” (offending).
Behaviors referred to in the two IPV questions in the U.S. questionnaire were: “thrown something at you,” “pushed, grabbed, shoved,” “slapped, hit, bit, kicked or tried to hit,” “beat up, choked, burned or scalded,” “threatened with a knife or gun,” and “used knife or gun.” Behaviors referred to in the two IPV questions in the Japanese questionnaire were: “push, shove, grab, slap, punch, kick, beat up, throw something at you, hit you with an object, threaten you, threaten you with a weapon, cut you with a knife, and other.”
The U.S. question asked “If you were to drink enough alcohol to feel the effects, what are the chances that you would become aggressive?” with answer choices: “a very strong chance,” “a strong chance,” “some chance (50/50),” not much chance,” and “no chance at all.” The question in the Japanese survey asked “When you drink, how true would you say it is that you become more aggressive toward other people?” with answer choices: “usually true,” “sometimes true,” and “never true.”
This may be the case of Simpson’s paradox (Pearl 2014) in which a significance of a relationship between two variables may change once another variable is introduced.
Contributor Information
Miyuki Fukushima Tedor, Department of Criminology, Anthropology, and Sociology, Cleveland State University.
Linda M. Quinn, Department of Mathematics, Cleveland State University
Sharon C. Wilsnack, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, School of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of North Dakota
Richard W. Wilsnack, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, School of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of North Dakota
Thomas K. Greenfield, Alcohol Research Group, Public Health Institute. Emeryville, CA.
References
- Archer John. Cross-Cultural Differences in Physical Aggression Between Partners: A Social-Role Analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2006;10(2):133–53. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1002_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Backman Ronet, Peralta Robert. The Relationship Between Drinking and Violence in an Adolescent Population: Does Gender Matter? Deviant Behavior. 2002;23:1–19. [Google Scholar]
- Barnwell Sara Smucker, Borders Ashley, Earleywine Mitch. Alcohol-Aggression Expectancies and Dispositional Aggression Moderate the Relationship Between Alcohol Consumption and Alcohol-Related Violence. Aggressive Behavior. 2006;32(6):517–27. doi: 10.1002/ab.20187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck Anne, Heinz Andreas. Alcohol-Related Aggression—Social and Neurobiological Factors. Deutsches Ärzteblatt International. 2013;110(42):711–5. doi: 10.3238/arztebl.2013.0711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernards Sharon, Graham Kathryn. The Cross-Cultural Association Between Marital Status and Physical Aggression Between Intimate Partners. Journal of Family Violence. 2013;28:403–18. doi: 10.1007/s10896-013-9505-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berthelot Emily R, Brown Timothy C, Drawve Grant, Burgason Kyle A. The Southern Pub Crawl and Brawl; An Examination of the Alcohol-Violence Nexus in a Southern City. Deviant Behavior. 36(8):605–24. [Google Scholar]
- Blume Sheila B. Women and Alcohol: Issues in Social Policy. In: Wilsnack RW, Wilsnack RD, editors. Gender and Alcohol: Individual and Social Perspectives. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies; 1997. pp. 462–89. [Google Scholar]
- Borders Ashley, Barnwell Sara Smucker, Earleywine Mitch. Alcohol-Aggression Expectancies and Dispositional Rumination Moderate the Effect of Alcohol Consumption on Alcohol-Related Aggression and Hostility. Aggressive Behavior. 2007;33:327–38. doi: 10.1002/ab.20187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cochran John K, Sellers Christine S, Wiesbrock Valerie, Palacios Wilson R. Repetitive Intimate Partner Victimization: An Explanatory Application of Social Learning Theory. Deviant Behavior. 32(9):790–817. [Google Scholar]
- Collins James J. Alcohol Use and Expressive Interpersonal Violence: A Proposed Explanatory Model. In: Gottheil E, Druley KA, Skoloda TE, Waxman HM, editors. Alcohol, Drug Abuse and Aggression. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas; 1983. pp. 5–25. [Google Scholar]
- Collins James J, Messerschmidt Pamela M. Epidemiology of Alcohol-Related Violence. Alcohol Health and Research World. 1993;17(2):93–100. [Google Scholar]
- Costello Barbara J, Anderson Bradley J, Stein Michael D. Self-Control and Adverse ‘Drinking’ Consequences. Deviant Behavior. 2014;35(12):973–92. [Google Scholar]
- Crawford Alex. Alcohol and Expectancy - I. Perceived Sex Differences in the Effects of Drinking. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 1984;19(1):63–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deries Karen M, Child Jennifer C, Bacchus Loraine J, Mak Joelle, Falder Gail, Graham Kathryn, Watts Charlotte, Heise Lori. Intimate Partner Violence Victimization and Alcohol Consumption in Women: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Addiction. 2013;109:379–91. doi: 10.1111/add.12393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eagly Alice H, Wood Wendy. Explaining Sex Differences in Social Behavior: A Meta-Analytic Perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1991;17(3):306–15. [Google Scholar]
- Eron Leonard D, Rowell Huesmann L. The Genesis of Gender Differences in Aggression. In: Luzzcz M, Niemela T, editors. Psychological development: Perspective across the Life-Span. New York, NY: Elsevier Science; 1989. pp. 55–67. [Google Scholar]
- Exum M Lyn. Alcohol and Aggression: An Integration of Findings From Experimental Studies. Journal of Criminal Justice. 2006;34:131–45. [Google Scholar]
- Fagan Jeffrey. Intoxication and Aggression. Crime and Justice: A Review of Research. 1990;13:241–320. [Google Scholar]
- Fals-Stewart William. The Occurrence of Partner Physical Aggression on Days of Alcohol Consumption: A Longitudinal Diary Study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2003;71(1):41–52. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.71.1.41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Felson Richard B, Savolainen Jukka, Aaltonen Mikko, Moustgaard Heta. Is the Association Between Alcohol Use and Delinquency Causal or Spurious? Criminology. 2008a;46(3):785–808. [Google Scholar]
- Felson Richard B, Teasdale Brent, Burchfield Keri B. The Influence of Being Under the Influence: Alcohol Effects on Adolescent Violence. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 2008b;45(2):119–41. [Google Scholar]
- Gefou-Madianou Dimitra., editor. Alcohol, Gender and Culture. New York, NY: Routledge; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Giancola Peter Richard, Zeichner Amos. An Investigation of Gender Differences in Alcohol-Related Aggression. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1995;56:573–9. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1995.56.573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman Mark S, Darkes Jack, Del Boca Frances K. Expectancy Mediation of Biospcyhosocial Risk for Alcohol Use and Alcoholism. In: Kirsch I, editor. How Expectancies Shape Experience. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1999. pp. 233–62. [Google Scholar]
- Graham Kathryn, Bernards Sharon, Wilsnack Sharon C, Gmel Gerhard. Alcohol May Not Cause Partner Violence but It Seems to Make It Worse: A Cross National Comparison of the Relationship Between Alcohol and Severity of Partner Violence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2011;26(8):1503–23. doi: 10.1177/0886260510370596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graham Kathryn, Bernards Sharon, Munné Myriam, Wilsnack Sharon C., editors. Unhappy Hours: Alcohol and Partner Aggression in the Americas. Washington, DC: Pan American Health Organization; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Graham Kathryn, Plant Moira, Plant Martin. Alcohol, Gender and Partner Aggression: A General Population Study of British Adults. Addiction Research and Theory. 2004;12(4):385–401. [Google Scholar]
- Greenfeld Lawrence A. Prepared for the Assistant Attorney General’s National Symposium on Alcohol Abuse and Crime. Washington D.C; 1998. Apr, Alcohol and Crime: An Analysis of National Data on the Prevalence of Alcohol Involvement in Crime; p. 507. [Google Scholar]
- Greenfeld Lawrence A, Rand Michael R, Craven Diane, Klaus Patsy A, Perkins Craig A, Ringel Cheryl, Warchol Greg, Maston Cathy, Fox James Alan. Violence by Intimates: Analysis of Data on crimes by Current or Former Spouses, Boyfriends, and Girlfriends. Bureau of Justice Statistics Factbook. U.S. Department of Justice; Washington D.C: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Greenfield Thomas K, Kerr William C. Alcohol Measurement Methodology in Epidemiology: Recent Advances and Opportunities. Addiction. 2008;103(7):1082–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02197.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenfield Thomas K, Room Robin. Situational Norms for Drinking and Drunkenness: Trends in the U.S. Adult Population, 1979–1990. Addiction. 1997;92(1):33–47. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenfield Thomas K, Ye Yu, Kerr William C, Bond Jason, Rehm Jűrgen, Giesbrecht Norman. Externalities from Alcohol Consumption in the 2005 US National Alcohol Survey: Implications for Policy. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2009;6(12):3205–24. doi: 10.3390/ijerph6123205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hindmarch Ian, Brinkmann Rűdiger. Trends in the Use of Alcohol and Other Drugs in Cases of Sexual Assault. Human Psychopharmacology. 1999;14:225–31. [Google Scholar]
- Hoaken Peter NS, Pihl Robert O. The Effects of Alcohol Intoxication on Aggressive Responses in Men and Women. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 2000;35(5):471–7. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/35.5.471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James Doris J. Prior Alcohol Use of Jail Inmates, 2002 and 1996. Profile of Jail Inmates, 2002. Washington D.C: Washington: Bureau of Justice Statistics; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Kantor Glenda Kaufman, Jasinski Jana L. Dynamics and Risk Factors in Partner Violence. In: Jasinski JL, Williams LM, editors. Partner Violence: A Comprehensive Review of 20 Years of Research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1998. pp. 1–43. [Google Scholar]
- Kashima Yoshihisa, Yamaguchi Susumu, Kim Uichol, Choi Sang-Chin, Gelfand Michele J, Yuki Masaki. Culture, Gender, and Self: A Perspective from Individualism-Collectivism Research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;69(5):925–37. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirsch Irving. Response Expectancy: An Introduction. In: Kirsch I, editor. How Expectancies Shape Experience. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1999. pp. 3–13. [Google Scholar]
- Kobayashi Emiko, Sharp Susan F, Grasmick Harold G. Gender and Deviance: A Comparison of College Students in Japan and the U.S. Deviant Behavior. 2008;29(5):413–39. [Google Scholar]
- Krug Etienne G, Dahlberg Linda L, Mercy James A, Zwi Anthony B, Lozano Rafael., editors. World Report on Violence and Health. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Kuhns Joseph B, Wilson David B, Clodfelter Tammatha A, Maguire Edward R, Ainsworth Stephanie A. A Meta-Analysis of Alcohol Toxicology Study Findings Among Homicide Victims. Addiction. 2011;106(1):62–72. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03153.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- KunKel Charlotte, Nielsen Joyce Mccari. Gender, Residual Deviance, and Social Control. Deviant Behavior. 19(4):339–60. [Google Scholar]
- MacAndrew Craig, Edgerton Robert B. Drunken Comportment: A Social Explanation. London: Nelson; 1969. [Google Scholar]
- Martin Susan Ehrlich, Bachman Ronet. The Relationship of Alcohol to Injury in Assault Cases. In: Galanter M, editor. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Volume 13: Alcoholism and Violence. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1997. pp. 41–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mustaine Elizabeth Ehrhardt, Tewksbury Richard. Transforming Potential Offenders into Motivated Ones: Are Sex Offenders Tempted by Alcohol and Pornography? Deviant Behavior. 2009;30(7):561–588. [Google Scholar]
- Nakane Chie. Japanese Society. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press; 1970. [Google Scholar]
- Nayak Madhabika B, Byrne Christina A, Martin Mutsumi K, Abraham Anna George. Attitudes Toward Violence Against Women: A Cross-National Study. Sex Roles. 2003;49(7/8):333–42. [Google Scholar]
- Nicolai Jennifer, Demmel Ralf, Moshagen Morten. The Comprehensive Alcohol-Aggression Expectancy Questionnaire: Confirmatory Factor Analysis, Scale Refinement, and Further Validation. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2010;92(5):400–9. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2010.497396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema Susan. Gender Differences in Risk Factors and Consequences for Alcohol Use and Problems. Clinical Psychology Review. 2004;24:981–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nunes-Dinis Maria C, Weisner Constance. Gender Differences in the Relationship of Alcohol and Drug Use to Criminal Behavior in a Sample of Arrestees. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 1997;23(1):129–41. doi: 10.3109/00952999709001692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ochiai Emiko. Leaving the West, Rejoining the East? Gender and Family in Japan’s Semi-Compressed Modernity. International Sociology. 2014;29(3):209–28. [Google Scholar]
- OECD. Education at a Glance: OECD Indicators. Paris, France: Organization for Economic CoOperation and Development; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Parker Robert Nash. Alcohol and Theories of Homicide. In: Adler F, Laufer W, editors. Advances in Criminological Theory. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction; 1993. pp. 113–142. [Google Scholar]
- Parker Robert Nash, Rebhun Linda-Anne. Alcohol and Homicide: A Deadly Combination of Two American Traditions. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Parks Kathleen A, Fals-Stewart William. The Temporal Relationship Between College Women’s Alcohol Consumption and Victimization Experiences. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2004;28(4):625–9. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000122105.56109.70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearl Judea. Comment: Understanding Simpson’s Paradox. The American Statistician. 2014;68(1):8–13. [Google Scholar]
- Perkins H Wesley. Gender Patterns in Consequences of Collegiate Alcohol Abuse: A 10-Year Study of Trends in an Undergraduate Population. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1992;53:458–62. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1992.53.458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pernanen Kai. Alcohol in Human Violence. New York: Guliford; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Powers Ráchael A, Leili Jennifer, Hagman Brett, Cohn Amy. The Impact of College Education on Rape Myth Acceptance, Alcohol Expectancies, and Bystander Attitudes. Deviant Behavior. 2015;36(12):956–73. [Google Scholar]
- Rand Michael R, Sabol William J, Sinclair Michael, Snyder Howard. Alcohol and Crime: Data from 2002 to 2008. Washington D.C: Bureau of Justice Statistics; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Reischauer Edwin O. The Japanese. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Rohner Ronald P. Sex Differences in Aggression: Phylogenetic and Exculturation Perspectives. Ethos. 1976;4:57–72. [Google Scholar]
- Room Robin. Intoxication and Bad Behaviour: Understanding Cultural Differences in the Link. Social Science and Medicine. 2001;53:189–98. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00330-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robin Room, Mäkelä Klaus. Typologies of the Cultural Position of Drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2000;61:475–83. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2000.61.475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell Brenda, Kraus Shane. Perceptions of Partner Violence: How Aggressor Gender, Musculinity/Femininity, and Victim Gender Influence Criminal Justice Decisions. Deviant Behavior. 2016;37(6):679–91. [Google Scholar]
- Scott KD, Schafer J, Greenfield Thomas G. The Role of Alcohol in Physical Assault Perpetration and Victimization. Journal of Studies in Alcohol. 1999;60(4):528–36. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1999.60.528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan Connor M, Rogers Richard G, Williams George W, IV, Boardman Jason D. Gender Differences in the Presence of Drugs in Violent Deaths. Addiction. 2012;108:547–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.04098.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherley Alison J. Contextualizing the Sexual Assault Event: Images from Police Files. Deviant Behavior. 2005;26(2):87–108. [Google Scholar]
- Testa Maria, Kubiak Audrey, Quigley Brian M, Houston Rebecca J, Derrick Jaye L, Levitt Ash, Homish Gregory G, Leonard Kenneth E. Husband and Wife Alcohol Use as Independent or Interactive Predictors of Intimate Partner Violence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2011;73(2):268–76. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tibbetts Stephen G, Herz Denise C. Gender Differences in Factors of Social Control and Rational Choice. Deviant Behavior. 1996;17(2):183–208. [Google Scholar]
- Tres Judith, Widmer Eric D. Married Women’s Employment Over the Life Course: Attitudes in Cross-National Perspective. Social Forces. 2000;78(4):1409–36. [Google Scholar]
- United Nations Statistics Division. Statistics and indicators on women and men Women Legislators and managers. New York: United Nations; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Wilsnack Sharon C. The GENACIS Project: A Review of Findings and Some Implications for Global Needs in Women-Focused Substance Abuse Prevention and Intervention. Substance Abuse and Rehabilitation. 2012;3(1):5–15. doi: 10.2147/SAR.S21343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilsnack Richard W, Vogeltanz Nancy D, Wilsnack Sharon C, Robert Harris T. Gender Differences in Alcohol Consumption and Adverse Drinking Consequences: Cross-Cultural Patterns. Addiction. 2000;95(2):251–65. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.95225112.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]