Skip to main content
Neural Plasticity logoLink to Neural Plasticity
. 2018 May 27;2018:9726950. doi: 10.1155/2018/9726950

Heterozygous CDKL5 Knockout Female Mice Are a Valuable Animal Model for CDKL5 Disorder

Claudia Fuchs 1, Laura Gennaccaro 1, Stefania Trazzi 1, Stefano Bastianini 1, Simone Bettini 1, Viviana Lo Martire 1, Elisa Ren 1, Giorgio Medici 1, Giovanna Zoccoli 1, Roberto Rimondini 2, Elisabetta Ciani 1,
PMCID: PMC5994305  PMID: 29977282

Abstract

CDKL5 disorder is a severe neurodevelopmental disorder caused by mutations in the X-linked CDKL5 (cyclin-dependent kinase-like five) gene. CDKL5 disorder primarily affects girls and is characterized by early-onset epileptic seizures, gross motor impairment, intellectual disability, and autistic features. Although all CDKL5 female patients are heterozygous, the most valid disease-related model, the heterozygous female Cdkl5 knockout (Cdkl5 +/−) mouse, has been little characterized. The lack of detailed behavioral profiling of this model remains a crucial gap that must be addressed in order to advance preclinical studies. Here, we provide a behavioral and molecular characterization of heterozygous Cdkl5 +/− mice. We found that Cdkl5 +/− mice reliably recapitulate several aspects of CDKL5 disorder, including autistic-like behaviors, defects in motor coordination and memory performance, and breathing abnormalities. These defects are associated with neuroanatomical alterations, such as reduced dendritic arborization and spine density of hippocampal neurons. Interestingly, Cdkl5 +/− mice show age-related alterations in protein kinase B (AKT) and extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK) signaling, two crucial signaling pathways involved in many neurodevelopmental processes. In conclusion, our study provides a comprehensive overview of neurobehavioral phenotypes of heterozygous female Cdkl5 +/− mice and demonstrates that the heterozygous female might be a valuable animal model in preclinical studies on CDKL5 disorder.

1. Introduction

Cyclin-dependent kinase-like 5 (CDKL5) disorder (OMIM no. 300203) is a severe neurodevelopmental disorder caused by mutations in the X-linked CDKL5 gene. Primary clinical features include early-onset intractable epileptic seizures, gross motor impairment, severe intellectual disability, and autistic-like features [14]. The majority of CDKL5 patients are heterozygous females carrying missense, nonsense, splice, or frameshift CDKL5 gene mutations or a genomic deletion [5]. Due to the different CDKL5 mutations and the variable X-chromosome random inactivation (XCI) in females, the phenotypic spectrum of the disease spans from milder forms—which include the possibility of autonomous walking and less severe epilepsy that is amenable to control—to severe forms featuring intractable seizures, more severe microcephaly and the absence of motor milestones. Boys carrying mutations in CDKL5 are much rarer and show more severe epileptic encephalopathy than girls [6, 7], probably due to the more severe consequences of dominant X-linked mutations in males than in females.

CDKL5 encodes a ubiquitously expressed serine/threonine kinase whose catalytic domains share homology with members of the cyclin-dependent kinase family and mitogen-activated protein kinases [8]. CDKL5 is expressed at high levels in the brain, reaching a peak during postnatal development, when crucial events, such as neuronal maturation and synaptogenesis, occur [9, 10]. CDKL5 is mainly expressed in neurons and is distributed in the cell nucleus and cytoplasm, as it shuttles between these cellular compartments [9]. The identification of the molecular targets of CDKL5 has started to shed light on the role of CDKL5 in neurodevelopment. The CDKL5 substrates that have, so far, been identified are MeCP2 [11, 12], DNA methyltransferase 1 (DNMT1) [13], Rac1 [14], amphiphysin 1 [15], PSD-95 [16], NGL-1 [17], Mind Bomb 1 (Mib1) [18], Shootin1 [19], HDAC4 [20], and IQGAP1 [21]. Furthermore, different signaling networks are affected by the absence of CDKL5 [22]. Interestingly, loss of CDKL5 alters Akt-mTOR and Akt-GSK3-β signaling pathways [2225]. Notably, mutations and dysfunction of components of these pathways have been implicated in the etiology of several neurodevelopmental disorders, such as autism, Rett syndrome, Fragile X syndrome, and tuberous sclerosis [2628].

Several Cdkl5 knockout (KO) mouse models have been generated to understand how CDKL5 dysfunction leads to neurological defects in CDKL5 disorder [22, 23, 29, 30]. Despite the lack of overt epilepsy, Cdkl5 KO mice exhibit numerous behavioral deficits across motor, sensory, cognitive, and socioemotional domains that are reminiscent of human symptomatology [2225, 29, 31, 32]. These behavioral defects are associated with morphological alterations, such as reduced dendritic arborization of hippocampal and cortical neurons, deficits in dendritic spine density and organization, and altered connectivity [20, 2325, 3335]. However, the majority of work involving Cdkl5 KO mouse models has focused on the loss of Cdkl5 in male mice and only a few studies have included an initial characterization of heterozygous female Cdkl5 KO mice [23, 25, 36]. An extensive behavioral characterization of heterozygous Cdkl5 KO females is still missing, and this represents a critical gap, since heterozygous Cdkl5 KO female mice better recapitulate the mosaic distribution of mutant cells present in the majority of CDKL5 patients. Given the clinical relevance of CDKL5 disorder in females, the aim of our current study was to evaluate the behavioral abnormalities in heterozygous Cdkl5 KO female mice (Cdkl5 +/−) compared to homozygous Cdkl5 KO (Cdkl5 −/−) and wild-type (Cdkl5 +/+) female mice, focusing on cognitive (learning and memory) behaviors, motor activity and coordination, social behaviors, and breathing pattern, features that are particularly compromised in CDKL5 patients. Since behavioral abnormalities are the main biomarkers of CDKL5 disorder, our study may crucially define the validity of the heterozygous Cdkl5 KO murine models for this disorder.

2. Experimental Procedures

2.1. Animal Husbandry

The mice used in this work derive from the Cdkl5 null strain in the C57BL/6N background developed in [23] and backcrossed in C57BL/6J for three generations. Heterozygous Cdkl5 KO (+/−) and homozygous Cdkl5 KO (−/−) females were produced and karyotyped as previously described [23]. Age-matched controls (wild-type females (+/+), when possible littermates) were used for all experiments. Mice were housed three to five animals per cage and maintained on a 12 h light: 12 h dark cycle in a temperature- (23°C) and humidity-controlled environment with standard mouse chow and water ad libitum. The animals' health and comfort were controlled by the veterinary service. All research and animal care procedures were performed in accordance with the Italian and European Community law for the use of experimental animals and were approved by Bologna University Bioethical Committee. In this study, all efforts were made to minimize animal suffering and to keep the number of animals used to a minimum.

2.2. Western Blotting

Western blot analysis was conducted on independent cohorts of 3-week-, 8-week-, and 12–14-week-old mice (n = 3–8 for each genotype) that had not undergone behavioral assessment. Animals were euthanized with isoflurane (2% in pure oxygen) and sacrificed by cervical dislocation. The brain was quickly removed, and the somatosensory cortex, the hippocampal formation, and the cerebellum were homogenized in ice-cold RIPA buffer (50 mM Tris-HCl, pH 7.4, 150 mM NaCl, 1% Triton-X100, 0.5% sodium deoxycholate, and 0.1% SDS) supplemented with 1 mM PMSF and 1% protease and phosphatase inhibitor cocktail (Sigma-Aldrich). Extracts were immediately processed by Western blot or kept frozen (−80°C) until assayed. Protein concentration was determined by the Lowry method [37]. Equivalent amounts (10 μg) of protein were subjected to electrophoresis on a BOLT Bis-Tris Plus gel (Thermo Fisher Scientific) and transferred to a Hybond ECL nitrocellulose membrane (Amersham Life Science). The following primary antibodies were used: sheep polyclonal anti-CDKL5 (1 : 1000, MRC Protein Phosphorylation and Ubiquitylation Unit—University of Dundee, UK), rabbit polyclonal anti-GAPDH (1 : 5000, Sigma-Aldrich), mouse monoclonal β-catenin (1 : 1000, BD Transduction Laboratories), rabbit polyclonal anti-phospho-AKT (Ser473) (1 : 1000), rabbit polyclonal anti-AKT (1 : 1000), rabbit polyclonal anti-phospho-ERK1/2 (1 : 1000), mouse monoclonal anti-ERK1/2 (1 : 1000), and rabbit polyclonal anti-phospho-MSK1 (Thr 581) (1 : 1000, Cell Signaling Technology). The following secondary antibodies were used: HRP-conjugated goat anti-sheep IgG, HRP-conjugated goat anti-mouse IgG, and HRP-conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG (1 : 5000, Jackson ImmunoResearch) antibodies. Densitometric analysis of digitized images was performed using Chemidoc XRS Imaging Systems and Image Lab™ Software (Bio-Rad).

2.3. Histological and Immunohistochemistry Procedures

Histological analysis was conducted on independent cohorts of 14-week-old mice (n = 3 − 4 for each genotype) that had not undergone behavioral assessment. Animals were euthanized with isoflurane (2% in pure oxygen) and sacrificed by cervical dislocation. Brains were quickly removed and cut along the midline. Right hemispheres were processed for immunohistochemistry procedure, while left hemispheres were Golgi-stained as described below. All steps of sectioning, imaging, and data analysis were conducted blindly. PSD-95 immunohistochemistry—right hemispheres were fixed by immersion in 4% paraformaldehyde in 100 mM phosphate buffer, pH 7.4, stored in fixative for 48 h, kept in 20% sucrose for an additional 24 h, and then frozen with cold ice. Right hemispheres were cut with a freezing microtome into 30 μm thick coronal sections that were serially collected. One out of 12 free-floating sections from the hippocampal formation were incubated for 24 h at 4°C with a rabbit polyclonal anti-PSD-95 antibody (1 : 1000; Abcam). Sections were then incubated at room temperature for 2 h with a CY3-conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG (1 : 200; Jackson ImmunoResearch). For quantification of PSD-95 immunoreactive puncta, images from the molecular layer of the hippocampal dentate gyrus were acquired using a Leica TCS SL confocal microscope (Leica Mycrosytems; 63x oil immersion objective, NA 1.32; zoom factor = 8). Three to four sections per animal were analyzed. Counting was carried out using Image Pro Plus software (Media Cybernetics), and the number of PSD-95 immunoreactive puncta was expressed per μm2. Golgi staining—left hemispheres were Golgi-stained using the FD Rapid GolgiStain TM Kit (FD NeuroTechnologies). Briefly, hemispheres were immersed in the impregnation solution containing mercuric chloride, potassium dichromate, and potassium chromate and stored at room temperature in darkness for 2–3 weeks. Hemispheres were cut with a microtome in 100 μm thick coronal sections that were directly mounted on gelatin-coated slides and were air-dried at room temperature in the dark for an additional 2–3 days. After drying, sections were rinsed with distilled water and subsequently stained in the developing solution of the kit. A light microscope (Leica Mycrosystems) equipped with a motorized stage and focus control system and a color digital camera (CoolSNAP-Pro; Media Cybernetics) were used for neuronal tracing and to take bright-field images. Measurements were carried out using Image Pro Plus software (Media Cybernetics). Neuronal tracing—a series of sections across the whole rostrocaudal extent of the hippocampal dentate gyrus were used for reconstruction of Golgi-stained neurons. Golgi-stained neurons (15–20 per animal) were sampled from the outer part of the granule cell layer and traced with a dedicated software custom-designed for dendritic reconstruction (Immagini Computer, Milan, Italy), interfaced with Image Pro Plus (Media Cybernetics). The dendritic tree was traced live, at a final magnification of 500x, by focusing into the depth of the section. The operator starts with branches emerging from the cell soma and after having drawn the first parent branch goes on with all daughter branches of the next order in a centrifugal direction. At the end of tracing, the program reconstructs the total dendritic length, the mean length of branches, and the number of branches. Spine density and morphology—in Golgi-stained sections, spines of granule neurons were counted using a 100x oil immersion objective lens. Dendritic spine density was measured by manually counting the number of dendritic spines on dendritic segments in the molecular layer and expressed as the number of dendritic spines per μm. Based on their morphology, dendritic spines can be divided into five different classes (immature spines: filopodium-like, thin-shaped, and stubby-shaped; mature spines: mushroom- and cup-shaped), which also reflect their state of maturation. The total number of spines was expressed per μm, and the number of spines belonging to each class was counted and expressed as a percentage. About 200–250 spines from 25 to 30 dendrites derived from 12 to 15 neurons were analyzed per condition.

2.4. Behavioral Assays

All animal behavioral studies and analysis were performed blinded to genotype. Mice were allowed to habituate to the testing room for at least 1 h before the test, and testing was performed at the same time of day. All animal behaviors were tested at 12–16 weeks of age.

2.5. Test Cohorts

A total of 124 animals separated in six independent test cohorts were used for neurobehavioral studies. The sequence of the tests was arranged to minimize the effect of one test influencing subsequent evaluation of the next test, and mice were allowed to recover several days between different tests. The first test cohort consisted of 26 animals (+/+ n = 7, +/− n = 9, and −/− n = 10) that were tested with the following assays at indicated time points: buried food test, 12 weeks; accelerating rotarod assay, 13 weeks; open field, 14 weeks; and Morris water maze, 14–15 weeks. The second cohort consisted of 11 animals (+/+ n = 4, +/− n = 2, and −/− n = 5) that were tested with the following assays at indicated time points: buried food test, 12 weeks; open field, 14 weeks; and Morris water maze, 14–15 weeks. The third cohort consisted of 19 animals (+/+ n = 5, +/− n = 8, and −/− n = 7) that were tested with the following assays at the indicated time points: nesting, 12 weeks; accelerating rotarod assay, 12 weeks; marble burying, 13 weeks; open field, 14 weeks; Morris water maze, 14–15 weeks; and passive avoidance, 16 weeks. The fourth test cohort consisted of 19 animals (+/+ n = 8, +/− n = 4, and −/− n = 7) that were tested with the following assays at the indicated time points: accelerating rotarod assay, 12 weeks; marble burying, 13 weeks; Morris water maze, 14–15 weeks; and passive avoidance, 16 weeks. The fifth test cohort consisted of 23 animals (+/+ n = 5, +/− n = 8, and −/− n = 10) that were tested for nesting at 12 weeks of age or with the buried food test or the marble burying test at 13 weeks of age. The sixth test of cohort consisted of 26 animals (+/+ n = 9, +/− n = 11, and −/− n = 8) that were tested for breathing abnormalities at 14 weeks and body weight at 8 and 14 weeks.

2.6. Buried Food Test

The buried food test relies on the animal's ability to smell volatile odors and its natural tendency to use olfactory cues for foraging. The testing protocol of 3 days consists of an odor familiarization exercise on day 1, food deprivation on day 2, and testing on day 3. On day 1, mice were placed in a clean cage and a piece of cookie (exactly the same kind as in the test) was placed in each cage and left overnight. Cages were inspected on day 2 to verify that the cookies had been consumed, thus verifying that the bait was highly palatable. On day 2, mice were deprived of food for 24 h, and the test was performed on day 3. Mice were individually introduced into a clean cage containing 4 cm of deep clean bedding and allowed to acclimate to the cage for 5 min to reduce the interference of novel environment exploration during the test. A cookie was buried beneath 1 cm of bedding in a random corner of the cage, and the mouse was introduced into the cage. The time necessary for the animal to retrieve the cookie with its front paws was measured in seconds (latency) up to a maximum of 15 min (900 s).

2.7. Nesting

Nest building ability was evaluated as proposed by Deacon [38]. Animals were placed in individual cages with standard bedding, and a standard piece of paper towel (23 cm × 23 cm) was provided. The nests were independently assessed at 3 and 22 h by two operators using the following scoring system: 0—no nest, 1—primitive flat nest (pad-shaped, consisting of a flat paper tissue which slightly elevates a mouse above the bedding), 2—more complex nest (including warping and biting the paper towel), 3—complex accurate cup-shaped nests (with shredded paper interwoven to form the walls of the cup), and 4—complex hooded nest, with walls forming a ceiling so the nest becomes a hollow sphere with one opening.

2.8. Marble Burying

The marble burying test was performed by placing animals individually in a home-cage-like environment with 5 cm of unscented standard bedding material and 20 marbles (14.3 mm in diameter) arranged in a 4 × 5 matrix and left undisturbed for 30 min. The number of marbles that were at least two-thirds buried at the end of the trial was counted.

2.9. Accelerating Rotarod Assay

Before the first test session, animals were briefly trained at a constant speed of 5 rpm on the rotarod apparatus (Ugo Basile) for 30 s. Thirty minutes later, testing was performed at an accelerating linear speed (5–35 rpm within 270 s + 30 s max speed). Four testing trials with an intertrial interval of 1 h were performed. The latency to fall from the rotating rod and the number of passive rotations (rotation in which the mouse does not perform any coordinated movement but is passively transported by the rotating apparatus) were recorded for each trial.

2.10. Open Field

In order to assess locomotion, animals were placed in the center of a square arena (50 × 50 cm) and their behavior was monitored for 20 min using a video camera placed above the center of the arena. Distinct features of locomotor activity, including total distance traveled, average locomotion velocity, and the time spent at the border, by the walls, and in the center, were scored by EthoVision10XT software (Noldus Information Technology B.V., The Netherlands). The numbers of stereotypical jumps (repetitive beam breaks < 1 s) in the corners of the arena were manually counted by a trained observer who was blind to the genotype. The test chambers were cleaned with 70% ethanol between test subjects. To evaluate whether Cdkl5 KO female mice show stereotypic behaviors also in the home-cage environment, animals were single-caged for 48 h and stereotypic jumps were evaluated in a time window of 20 min.

2.11. Morris Water Maze

Hippocampal-dependent spatial learning and memory was assessed using the Morris water maze (MWM). Mice were trained to locate a hidden escape platform in a circular pool. The apparatus consisted of a circular water tank (1 m in diameter, 50 cm high) with a transparent round escape platform (10 cm2) placed in a fixed position. The tank was filled with tap water at a temperature of 22°C up to 0.5 cm above the top of the platform, and the water was made opaque with milk. In the experimental room, intramaze and extramaze visual cues were placed to enable spatial orientation. Mouse behavior was automatically videotracked (EthoVision 3.1; Noldus Information Technology B.V.). During training, each mouse was subjected to either 1 swimming session of 4 trials (day 1) or 2 sessions of 4 trials per day (days 2–5), with an intersession interval of 1 h (acquisition phase). Mice were allowed to search for the platform for up to 60 s. If a mouse did not find the platform, it was gently guided to it and allowed to remain there for 15 s. During the intertrial-interval (15 s), mice were placed in an empty cage. The latency to find the hidden platform was used as a measure of learning. Twenty-four h after the last acquisition trial, on day 6, the platform was removed and a probe test was run. Animals were allowed to search for the platform for up to 60 s. The latency of the first entrance into the former platform area, the frequency of entrances and the time spent in the quadrant in which the platform had been located during training, and the proximity to the former platform quadrant (Gallagher's test) were employed as measures of retention of acquired spatial preference. During the learning phase, the average and maximum swim speeds were also analyzed. The percentage of floating was defined as the percentage of time swimming at a speed slower than 4 cm/s. Animals that failed the visual clue test were excluded from the analysis (+/− n = 1 and −/− n = 3).

2.12. Passive Avoidance

For the passive avoidance task, a memory task that involves contributions from both the hippocampus and amygdala, we used a tilting-floor box (47 × 18 × 26 cm) divided into 2 compartments (lit and dark) by a sliding door and a control unit incorporating a shocker (Ugo Basile). This classic instrument for Pavlovian conditioning exploits the tendency in mice to escape from an illuminated area into a dark one (step-through method). In general, mice step quickly through the gate and enter the dark compartment (innate preference for the dark compartment). Upon entering the dark compartment, mice received a brief mild foot shock (0.4 mA for 3 s) and were removed from the chamber after a 15 s delay. The chambers were cleaned with 70% ethanol between testing one subject and another. After a 24 h retention period, mice were placed back into the illuminated compartment and the latency to reenter the dark chamber was measured up to 358 s.

2.13. Noninvasive Assessment of Sleep and Breathing Pattern

Hypnic and respiratory phenotypes of mice were assessed noninvasively with a validated technique based on whole-body plethysmography (WBP) [39, 40]. Briefly, each mouse was placed inside a WBP chamber flushed with air at 1.5 l/h for the first 8 h of the light period. The respiratory (WBP chamber pressure) signal was continuously recorded together with chamber humidity and temperature, digitized, and stored at 128 Hz, 4 Hz, and 4 Hz, respectively. The system was calibrated with a 100 μl micro-syringe (Hamilton, Reno, USA) at the end of each recording. The states of wakefulness, nonrapid eye movement sleep (NREMS), and rapid eye movement sleep (REMS) were scored based on inspection of the raw WBP signal with the investigators blinded to the animal's genotype. Quantitative analysis of breathing was restricted to stable sleep episodes ≥ 12 s because of the frequent occurrence of movement artefacts during wakefulness. Apneas were automatically detected as breaths with instantaneous total breath duration (TTOT) > 3 times; the average TTOT for each mouse and sleep state and detection accuracy were checked on raw recordings.

2.14. Statistical Analysis

Data from single animals represented the unity of analysis. Results are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean (± SE). Statistical analysis was performed with SPSS (version 23) or GraphPad Prism (version 6). All datasets were analyzed using the ROUT method (Q = 1%) to identify significant outliers and the Shapiro-Wilk test for normality. Datasets with normal distribution were analyzed for significance using a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with genotype as a factor. Post hoc multiple comparisons were carried out using the Fisher least significant difference (Fisher's LSD) test. Datasets with nonparametric distribution were analyzed using the Kruskal-Wallis test. Post hoc multiple comparisons were carried out using Dunn's multiple comparison test or the Mann–Whitney test. For the rotarod assay and the learning phase of the MWM test, statistical analysis was performed using a repeated ANOVA. For categorical data, that is, percentages of spines, we used a chi-squared test. A probability level of p < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant. Data from several neurobehavioral assays were analyzed with the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) functions and the relative area under the curve (AUC), and correspondent p values are reported in Supplementary Table 1. This analysis enables the assessment of the ability of a test to discriminate between control and diseased animals. The area under the curve (AUC) reflects the specificity and sensitivity of a test and ranges from totally noninformative (AUC = 0.5) to perfect test (AUC = 1) [41, 42].

3. Results

3.1. Cdkl5 Expression Levels in the Brain of Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− Female Mice

To determine the degree of Cdkl5 levels in the brain of female Cdkl5 KO mice, we performed Western blot assays on total protein extracts from various brain regions from adult (3–4 months of age) wild-type (Cdkl5 +/+), heterozygous (Cdkl5 +/−), and homozygous (Cdkl5 −/−) Cdkl5 KO female mice. The Cdkl5 signal was normalized to GAPDH (glyceraldehyde 3-phosphate dehydrogenase) levels, and the relative amounts are illustrated in Figure 1(a). Western blot analyses showed that Cdkl5 expression levels were not uniform among the cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum (Figure 1(a)). In fact, Cdkl5 levels were higher in the cortex, compared with the hippocampus and cerebellum (Figure 1(a)), while the cerebellum was the brain region that showed the lowest Cdkl5 expression levels (Figure 1(a)). Regardless of the relative amounts, in all analyzed brain regions Cdkl5 expression levels were reduced to about 35–46% in Cdkl5 +/− mice in comparison to wild-type mice (Mann–Whitney test: Cx: U = 0, p = 0.0095; Hp: U = 0, p = 0.0159; Cb: U = 0, p = 0.0043) and were absent in Cdkl5 −/− mice (Figure 1(b)).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Intermediate Cdkl5 expression levels in the brain of Cdkl5 +/− female mice. (a, b) Western blot analysis of Cdkl5 levels normalized to GAPDH (glyceraldehyde 3-phosphate dehydrogenase) levels in the cortex (Cx), hippocampus (Hp), and cerebellum (Cb) of wild-type (Cdkl5 +/+ n = 5), heterozygous (Cdkl5 +/− n = 6), and homozygous (Cdkl5 −/− n = 5) Cdkl5 KO female mice aged 3–4 months. Immunoblots in (a) are examples from one animal of each experimental group, and the graph shows the relative amounts of Cdkl5 expression in the different brain structures of Cdkl5 +/+ mice. Regardless of the relative amounts, Cdkl5 expression levels were reduced to about 35–46% in Cdkl5 +/− mice in comparison to wild-type mice and absent in Cdkl5 −/− mice in all analyzed brain regions (b). Data in (b) are expressed as a percentage of the values of Cdkl5 +/+ mice. Values are represented as means ± SE. p < 0.05 and ∗∗ p < 0.001 (Mann–Whitney test after unpaired t-test).

3.2. Reduced Body Weight and Olfactory Performance in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− Female Mice

We first evaluated whether mutant Cdkl5 female mice showed a significant difference in body weight. As previously reported [23], no significant weight differences were observed in young adult female Cdkl5 KO mice [F (2, 25) = 0.9982, p = 0.3828] (8 weeks old; Figure 2(a)). However, at 14 weeks of age, the average weight of Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− female mice was lower compared with wild-type (Cdkl5 +/+) females [F (2, 25) = 31.66, p < 0.0001; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0065 and p < 0.0001, resp.]. Cdkl5 +/− females achieved an intermediate body weight between that of Cdkl5 +/+ and Cdkl5 −/− female mice.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Reduced body weight, impaired olfaction, and autistic-like features in Cdkl5 +/− female mice. (a) Body weight at 8 and 14 weeks of age in Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 9), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 8 and n = 11, resp.), and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 11 and n = 8, resp.) mice. Only at 14 weeks of age Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice showed reduced body weight compared to Cdkl5 +/+ mice. (b) Olfactory ability evaluated using the buried food test. Cdkl5 +/− (n = 15) and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 18) mice showed an increased latency to find the buried cookie compared to Cdkl5 +/+ mice (n = 14), indicating olfactory impairment in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice. (c) Nest building ability evaluated at 3 and 22 h. Cdkl5 +/− (n = 9) and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 10) mice showed impaired nesting behavior compared to Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 8) mice. (d) Marble burying test. Cdkl5 +/− (n = 19) and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 15) mice buried fewer marbles compared to Cdkl5 +/+ mice (n = 15). Values represent mean ± SEM. p < 0.05, ∗∗ p < 0.01, and ∗∗∗ p < 0.001 (datasets in (a), Fisher's LSD test after ANOVA; datasets in (b–d), Dunn's test after Kruskal-Wallis).

Among the many sensory stimuli that influence behavioral decisions regarding food choice, olfactory inputs are likely to contribute to food uptake and the regulation of energy homeostasis [43]. Using the buried food test, we evaluated the ability of Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− female mice to smell volatile odors. We found that 3 month old Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− female mice showed a reduced olfactory performance compared with wild-type (Cdkl5 +/+) females [Kruskal-Wallis, p = 0.0019; Dunn's test: p = 0.0387 and p = 0.0016, resp.] (Figure 2(b)), suggesting an olfactory impairment in the Cdkl5 KO mice.

All the behavioral experiments described below were performed on female Cdkl5 KO mice at 3–4 months of age.

3.3. Autistic-Like Features in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− Female Mice

To investigate whether loss of Cdkl5 function in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− female mice is associated with autistic-like (ASD-like) phenotypes, we first analyzed home-cage social behaviors (nest building ability and marble burying). Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− displayed impaired nesting compared to control mice. After 3 h, Cdkl5 +/+ female mice showed initial structured nests with a median score of 1, while Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice showed a significantly lower median nest quality with most of the mice leaving the material untouched [Kruskal-Wallis, p = 0.0133; Dunn's test: p = 0.0461 and p = 0.0202, resp.] (Figure 2(c)). Even after 22 h, the majority of Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice failed to build complex structured nests with a median nest quality of 1.4 and 0.8, respectively, in contrast to Cdkl5 +/+ mice who had a median nest quality of 3.5 [Kruskal-Wallis, p < 0.0001; Dunn's test: p = 0.0131 and p < 0.0001, resp.] (Figure 2(c)).

The marble burying test was used to evaluate exploration and environmental interest. Since marble burying may depend not only on interest in the environment but also on stress novelty, animals were tested after 5 min of habituation. Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice buried a significantly lower number of marbles compared to Cdkl5 +/+ mice [Kruskal-Wallis, p < 0.0001; Dunn's test: p = 0.0110 and p < 0.0001, resp.] (Figure 2(d)), suggesting that Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− female mice had a decreased interest in the environment.

3.4. Impaired Motor Function in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− Female Mice

To assess whether motor impairment is present in female Cdkl5 KO mice, we assessed motor function of Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− female mice on an accelerating rotarod assay and in the open-field test. Mice were tested on a rotating rod for 4 trials with an intertrial interval of 1 h, and the latency to fall from the rotating rod was evaluated. Cdkl5 −/− exhibited significantly reduced falling latency compared to Cdkl5 +/+ mice in the third and fourth trials [F (2, 58) = 3.591, p = 0.0339; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0013 and p = 0.0274, resp.] (Figure 3(a)). Similarly, in Cdkl5 +/− mice the latency to fall was reduced in the last two trials (Figure 3(a)), although the difference was statistically significant only in the fourth trial [Fisher's LSD: trial 3: p = 0.053, trial 4: p = 0.0185], suggesting that loss of CDKL5 impairs not only motor coordination but also motor learning skills. We further evaluated the frequency of passive rotations (number of passive rotations/sec), rotations in which the mouse does not perform any coordinated movement and is passively transported from the rotating apparatus. Rotarod performance notably differed among groups, with Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice showing significantly more passive rotations during the four trials [F (2, 58) = 11.94, p < 0.0001; Fisher's LSD: trial 1: p = 0.0006 and p = 0.0013; trial 2: p = 0.0291 and p = 0.0234; trial 3: p = 0.0225 and p = 0.0102; trial 4: p = 0.0262 and p = 0.0139, resp.] (Figure 3(b)), confirming that both Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− female mice exhibit impaired motor coordination.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Impaired motor coordination, hyperactivity, and stereotypes in Cdkl5 +/− female mice. (a, b) Rotarod assay, measuring latency to fall (a) and frequency of passive rotations (b; rotations in which the mouse does not perform any coordinated movement but is passively transported from the rotating apparatus) on the accelerating rotating rod. Testing was performed in 4 trials with an intertrial interval of 1 h. Cdkl5 +/− (n = 21) and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 23) mice showed a decreased latency to fall (a) and an increased frequency of passive rotations compared to Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 17) mice, indicating impaired motor coordination in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice. (c, d) Locomotor activity measured as total distance traveled (c) and average locomotion velocity (d) during a 20 min open-field test. Cdkl5 +/− (n = 19) and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 22) mice exhibited increased locomotor activity with a longer total distance traveled (c) at a greater average speed (d) compared to Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 16) mice. (e) Number of stereotypic jumps (repetitive beam breaks < 1 s) in the corners of the open-field arena during the 20 min trial. Cdkl5 +/− (n = 19) and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 21) mice showed an increased number of repetitive stereotyped jumps compared to Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 15) mice. (f) Time (cumulative duration) spent by the border, near the walls, and in the center of the open-field arena. Cdkl5 +/+, Cdkl5 +/−, and Cdkl5 −/− mice spent a comparable time at the border, near the walls, and in the center compared to controls, suggesting that hyperactivity was not due to increased anxiety. Values represent mean ± SEM. p < 0.05, ∗∗ p < 0.01, and ∗∗∗ p < 0.001 (datasets in (a–d, f), Fisher's LSD test after ANOVA; datasets in (e) Dunn's test after Kruskall-Wallis).

To further evaluate locomotor activity, we performed an open-field test. Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice traveled a significantly longer distance [F (2, 54) = 11.90, p < 0.0001; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0116 and p < 0.0001, resp.] (Figure 3(c)) and moved at a higher average speed [F (2, 54) = 11.89, p < 0.0001; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0116 and p < 0.0001, resp.] (Figure 3(d)) compared to their controls (Cdkl5 +/+), indicating elevated locomotor activity in Cdkl5 mutants. To determine whether this hyperactivity was caused by increased anxiety, we compared the time spent in the border area, near the walls, and in the center of the arena (Figure 3(f)). Cdkl5 +/+, Cdkl5 +/−, and Cdkl5 −/− spent a comparable time at the border [F (2, 54) = 1.135, p = 0.3289], near the walls [F (2, 54) = 0.6457, p = 0.5285], and in the center [F (2, 54) = 0.4563, p = 0.6361] (Figure 3(f)), suggesting that the increased locomotor activity is not due to increased anxiety.

In addition to motor dysfunction, stereotypic movements frequently cooccur in CDKL5 patients [3]. Therefore, we evaluated stereotypic behavior in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice by counting the number of repetitive jumps in the corners of the open-field arena. Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− showed an increased number of stereotypical jumps compared to wild-type females [Kruskal-Wallis, p < 0.0001; Dunn's test: p = 0.0362 and p < 0.0001, resp.] (Figure 3(e)). An increased number in vertical jumps were also observed in Cdkl5 KO female mice in the home-cage environment (Cdkl5 +/+ 0 ± 0, Cdkl5 +/− 2.2 ± 1.9, and Cdkl5 −/− 1.8 ± 1.8), suggesting that the stereotypic behavior was not caused by the stressful situation (the new environment).

3.5. Impaired Learning and Memory Performance in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− Female Mice

To examine whether Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− female mice show cognitive impairment, hippocampus-dependent learning and memory were evaluated using the Morris water maze (MWM). Mice were tested for their ability to find a hidden platform for 5 days (learning phase) and were subjected to the probe test on day 6. A repeated ANOVA on escape latency during the learning phase revealed a significant genotype effect [F (2, 52) = 28.03, p < 0.0001]. In the learning phase, both Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice showed a reduced ability to learn over time compared to Cdkl5 +/+ mice [Fisher's LSD: day 2: p = 0.0010 and p < 0.0001; day 3: p = 0.0033 and p < 0.0001; day 4: p = 0.0008 and p < 0.0001; day 5: p < 0.0001 and p < 0.0001, resp.] (Figure 4(a)). While Cdkl5 +/+ mice exhibited a fast learning improvement with time and learned to find the platform by the second day [Fisher's LSD: p < 0.0001], significant learning was detected in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice by the third day [Fisher's LSD: p < 0.0001 and p = 0.015, resp.], indicating a learning deficit in both genotypes. No significant differences in the learning curves were observed between the Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice in the first two days of training (Figure 4(a)), while Cdkl5 −/− mice showed a greater impairment in finding the hidden platform in the following days [Fisher's LSD: day 3: p = 0.0015; day 4: p = 0.0020; and day 5: p = 0.0049].

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Impaired hippocampus-dependent learning and memory in Cdkl5 +/− female mice. (a) Spatial learning assessed using the Morris water maze. Cdkl5 +/− (n = 20) and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 19) mice showed an increased latency to find the platform over the 5-day learning period compared to the Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 16) mice. (b, c) Maximum (b) and average (c) swim speed during the learning phase in mice as in (a). (d) Percentage of floating, defined as the percentage of time swimming at less than 4 cm/s during the learning phase in mice as in (a). (e–h) On day 6 (probe test), the platform was removed and spatial memory was assessed by evaluating different parameters. Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice showed an increased latency to enter the former platform zone (e), a reduced frequency to enter (f), and time spent in the quadrant (g) in which the platform had been located during the learning period. In addition, Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice swam a larger distance from the former platform (h) compared to the Cdkl5 +/+ mice, indicating defects in spatial memory. Values represent mean ± SEM. p < 0.05, ∗∗ p < 0.01, and ∗∗∗ p < 0.001 as compared to the Cdkl5 +/+ female mice; # p < 0.05 as compared to the Cdkl5 +/− female mice (dataset in a); p < 0.05, ∗∗ p < 0.01, and ∗∗∗ p < 0.001 as compared to the Cdkl5 +/+ or Cdkl5 +/− female mice (dataset in b–h), Fisher's LSD test after ANOVA; datasets in (d), Dunn's test after Kruskall-Wallis).

Performance in the MWM is influenced by sensorimotor function and motivation, and these parameters can be assessed using swimming patterns such as swim speed and floating (rest time). No difference in maximum swim speed was observed among Cdkl5 +/+, Cdkl5 +/−, and Cdkl5 −/− mice [F (2, 52) = 0.4431, p = 0.6445] (Figure 4(b)), indicating that the deficit of Cdkl5 +/− and −/− mice in the hidden platform test was not caused by abnormalities in locomotor activity or coordination. On the contrary, average swim speed significantly decreased in Cdkl5 +/− mice and Cdkl5 −/− mice [F (2, 52) = 28.21, p < 0.0001; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0003 and p < 0.0001, resp.] (Figure 4(c)). Even relatively short intervals of floating behavior can cause significant changes in the mean raw average speed of a trial. We quantified the number of trials with floating episodes during training across genotypes (Figure 4(d)). An analysis of floating episodes revealed that Cdkl5 −/− mice showed a significantly higher percentage of floating behaviors compared to Cdkl5 +/+ mice [Kruskal-Wallis, p < 0.0001; Dunn's test: p < 0.0001] (Figure 4(d)), while Cdkl5 +/− mice showed a slightly, but not significantly, increased number of floating episodes [Dunn's test: p = 0.1716] (Figure 4(d)). These results suggest that an increased level of floating behavior can account for the reduced swimming speed of Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice. Thigmotaxis (swimming along the walls of the pool) was not observed among Cdkl5 female genotypes.

In the probe test, the following parameters were considered as an index of spatial memory: (i) latency to enter the former platform zone (Figure 4(e)), (ii) frequency of entrances into the former platform quadrant (Figure 4(f)), (iii) time spent in the former platform quadrant (Figure 4(g)), and (iv) proximity to the former platform position (Gallagher's test) (Figure 4(h)). Performance in all examined parameters was significantly impaired in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice. Both showed an increase in the latency to enter the former platform zone [F (2, 52) = 21.70, p < 0.0001; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0026 and p < 0.0001, resp.] (Figure 4(e)), a reduction in the frequency of entrances [F (2, 52) = 22.72, p < 0.0001; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0003 and p < 0.0001, resp.] (Figure 4(f)), a reduction in the time spent there [F (2, 52) = 11.33, p < 0.0001; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0195 and p < 0.0001, resp.] (Figure 4(g)), and a reduction in the proximity to the platform quadrant [F (2, 52) = 10.25, p = 0.0002; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0176 and p < 0.0001, resp.] (Figure 4(h)).

To confirm the observed defects in memory, we performed a second test, the passive avoidance test. In this task, animals are conditioned with a single aversive event (mild foot shock) and 24 h later are tested for recollection of that experience. Figures 5(a) and 5(b) show the latency to enter the dark compartment on the first and second days of the test. While no difference in step-through latency was observed on the first day [Kruskal-Wallis, p = 0.8028] (Figure 5(a)), Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice show a significantly decreased latency on the second day [Kruskal Wallis, p = 0.0049; Dunn's test: p = 0.0209 and p = 0.0073, resp.] (Figure 5(b)), suggesting a defect in associative memory.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Impaired memory in Cdkl5 +/− female mice. (a, b) Passive avoidance task to measure memory that involves contributions from both the hippocampus and amygdala. On the first day (a), no difference was observed between genotypes regarding the latency to enter the dark compartment. On the second day, Cdkl5 +/− (n = 12) and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 14) mice showed a decreased latency to enter the dark compartment compared to Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 13) mice, indicating defects in memory. Values represent mean ± SEM. p < 0.05 and ∗∗ p < 0.01 (Dunn's test after Kruskall-Wallis).

3.6. Abnormal Breathing Pattern in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− Female Mice

To determine whether breathing irregularities and sleep disturbance are present in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− females, hypnic and respiratory phenotypes were assessed using noninvasive whole-body plethysmography (WBP). Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− females had more frequent apneas during NREM sleep [F (2, 25) = 13.26, p = 0.0001; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0249 and p < 0.0001, resp.] and during total sleep time [F (2, 25) = 13.57, p = 0.0001; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0238 and p < 0.0001, resp.] compared to Cdkl5 +/+ female mice (Figures 6(a) and 6(c)), but not during REM sleep [F (2, 25) = 1.384, p = 0.2691] (Figure 6(b)), confirming the presence of an impaired breathing pattern in heterozygous and homozygous Cdkl5 female mice.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Sleep apnea occurrence rate in Cdkl5 +/− female mice. (a, c) Apnea occurrence rate during NREM sleep (a), REM sleep (b), and total sleep period (c). Cdkl5 +/− (n = 11) and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 8) mice showed an increased number of apneas during NREM sleep (a) and, consequently, an increased number of total sleep apneas (c) compared to Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 9) mice. Values represent mean ± SEM. p < 0.05, ∗∗ p < 0.01, and ∗∗∗ p < 0.001 (Fisher's LSD test after ANOVA).

3.7. Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− Female Mice Exhibit Altered Neuronal Maturation and Synapse Development

Changes in neuronal morphology, including defects in neuronal maturation, alterations in the organization and stability of dendritic spines, and defects in synaptogenesis, have been consistently reported in male Cdkl5 KO mice [20, 2225, 3335]. To determine whether similar defects were also present in heterozygous and homozygous Cdkl5 female mice, we analyzed dendritic development and spine density/morphology in Golgi-stained granule neurons of the dentate gyrus (DG) from 3–4 month-old females. Total dendritic length [F (2, 8) = 40.28, p < 0.0001; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0023 and p < 0.0001, resp.] and number of dendritic branches [F (2, 8) = 37.34, p < 0.0001; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0041 and p < 0.0001, resp.] of granule neurons were reduced in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− females compared to their wild-type counterparts (Figures 7(a)7(c)). Evaluation of dendritic spine density showed that granule neurons of Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− females had reduced spine density [F (2, 8) = 9.355, p = 0.008; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0445 and p = 0.0026, resp.] in comparison with wild-type (+/+) mice (Figures 7(d) and 7(f)). Moreover, separate counts of different classes of dendritic spines revealed that granule neurons of both genotypes had a higher percentage of immature spines (filopodium-like, thin-shaped, and stubby-shaped) and a reduced percentage of mature spines (chi-squared test: p = 0.0153 and p = 0.0196, resp.) compared to +/+ mice (Figures 7(e) and 7(f)). Since dendritic spine structure is fundamental for synaptic contact maintenance, we further evaluated the number of immunoreactive puncta for PSD-95 (postsynaptic density protein 95) in the molecular layer of the hippocampal dentate gyrus of heterozygous and homozygous Cdkl5 female mice. Cdkl5 −/− females displayed a strong reduction in the number of PSD-95-positive puncta [F (2, 8) = 30.66, p = 0.0002; Fisher's LSD: p < 0.0001] compared to Cdkl5 +/+ female mice (Figures 7(g) and 7(h)), while Cdkl5 +/− mice showed intermediate levels of PSD-95 puncta (p = 0.0007) (Figures 7(g) and 7(h)).

Figure 7.

Figure 7

Impaired hippocampal development in Cdkl5 +/− female mice. (a, b) Mean total dendritic length (a) and mean number of dendritic branches (b) of Golgi-stained mature granule neurons of Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 4), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 3), and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 4) mice. (c) Examples of the reconstructed dendritic tree of Golgi-stained mature granule neurons of one animal from each experimental group. (d, e) Dendritic spine density (number of spines per μm; d) and percentage of immature and mature spines (e) in relation to the total number of protrusions of granule neurons of mice as in (a). (f) Examples of Golgi-stained dendritic branches of mature granule neurons of one animal from each experimental group. The black triangles indicate mature spines (mushroom- and cup-shaped spines). Scale bar: 1 μm. (g) Number of fluorescent puncta per μm2 exhibiting PSD-95 immunoreactivity in the molecular layer of the dentate gyrus of Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 4), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 4), and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 3) mice. (h) Confocal images of sections processed for PSD-95 immunohistochemistry of one animal from each experimental group. Scale bar: 2.5 μm. Values represent mean ± SEM. p < 0.05, ∗∗ p < 0.01, and ∗∗∗ p < 0.001 (datasets in (a–c, e–h), Fisher's LSD test after ANOVA; datasets in (d), Dunn's test after Kruskall-Wallis).

3.8. Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− Female Mice Exhibit Age-Related Alterations in AKT and ERK Signaling Pathways

We examined whether the AKT signaling pathway, known to be altered in Cdkl5 KO male mice [22, 23, 25], is similarly affected in adult Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− female mice. Surprisingly, Western blots on hippocampal and cerebellar extracts from adult (3–4 months of age) Cdkl5 KO females revealed no change in P-AKT-Ser473 levels [F (2, 13) = 1.846, p = 0.1970 and F (2, 12) = 1.460, p = 0.2720, resp.] (Figures 8(a) and 8(b)). Likewise, no change in β-catenin levels, an AKT downstream target, was observed in hippocampal extracts [F (2, 10) = 0.1222, p = 0.8863] (Supplementary Figure 1A). Interestingly, decreased levels of phosphorylated ERK1/2 were observed in extracts of the hippocampus [F (2, 13) = 7.897, p = 0.0057; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0071 and p = 0.0027, resp.] and cerebellum [F (2, 17) = 4.465, p = 0.0276; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0170 and p = 0.0191, resp.] from both Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− female mice, when compared to their wild-type counterparts (Figures 8(a) and 8(b)). In line with decreased P-ERK1/2 levels, we found decreased levels of phosphorylated MSK1 (P-MSK1-Thr581; mitogen- and stress-activated protein kinase), a ERK1/2 downstream phosphorylation target, in the hippocampus of adult Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice [F (2, 13) = 4.685, p = 0.0294; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0385 and p = 0.0119, resp.] (Supplementary Figure 1A).

Figure 8.

Figure 8

Altered ERK signaling pathway in Cdkl5 +/− female mice. (a) Western blot analysis of P-AKT-Ser 473 levels normalized to total AKT levels (left histograms) and P-ERK-Ser 42–44 levels normalized to total ERK levels (right histograms) in the hippocampus (a) and cerebellum (b) of Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 6 and n = 5, resp.), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 6 and n = 8, resp.), and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 6 and n = 5, resp.) mice. Immunoblots are examples from one animal of each experimental group. Data are expressed as a percentage of the values of Cdkl5 +/+ mice. Values are represented as means ± SE. p < 0.05 and ∗∗ p < 0.01 (Fisher's LSD after ANOVA).

In order to evaluate whether there are age-related changes in AKT and ERK1/2 signal pathways in Cdkl5 KO mice, we analyzed P-AKT-Ser473, β-catenin, P-ERK1/2, and P-MSK1-Thr581 levels in the hippocampus of juvenile (3 week old) and young adult (8 week old) Cdkl5 KO female mice. In line with previous results [23, 25, 35], we found that juvenile Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− mice show reduced P-AKT-Ser473 [F (2, 6) = 7.844, p = 0.0212; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0253 and p = 0.0094, resp.] and β-catenin levels [F (2, 10) = 6.635, p = 0.0147; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0065 and p = 0.0169, resp.] (Supplementary Figure 1B) in comparison with their wild-type counterparts. Differently, no changes in P-ERK1/2 [F (2, 10) = 1.241, p = 0.3299] and P-MSK1-Thr581 [F (2, 12) = 0.0720, p = 0.9309] levels were observed in juvenile Cdkl5 KO female mice (Supplementary Figure 1C). Western blots on hippocampal extracts from young adult Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− KO females revealed that both genotypes show decreased P-AKT-Ser473 [F (2, 11) = 5.076, p = 0.0274; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0566 and p = 0.0093, resp.] and P-ERK1/2 [F (2, 9) = 5.711, p = 0.0250; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0174 and p = 0.0124, resp.] levels (Supplementary Figure 1E), compared to their wild-type counterparts. In accordance, β-catenin [F (2, 13) = 6.907, p = 0.0090; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0560 and p = 0.0026, resp.] (Supplementary Figure 1D) and P-MSK1-Thr581 [F (2, 9) = 8.202, p = 0.0094; Fisher's LSD: p = 0.0110 and p = 0.0036, resp.] (Supplementary Figure 1E) levels were reduced in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− female mice.

4. Discussion

Animal models of neurological disorders serve as one of the most valuable tools for the development and validation of potential therapies. They enable scientists to test the safety and efficacy of new therapeutic agents in vivo and to acquire a detailed understanding of mechanisms of action for new drugs, in order to speed therapies along toward human clinical trials. Since the majority of CDKL5 patients are females with heterozygous CDKL5 mutations, the behavioral and molecular characterization of heterozygous Cdkl5 KO (Cdkl5 +/−) female mice is of utmost importance in order to advance preclinical and translational studies. In the present study, we found that heterozygous Cdkl5 female mice recapitulate several behavioral aspects of CDKL5 disorder including autistic-like behaviors, motor impaired behavior, learning and memory disability, and abnormal breathing pattern. These phenotypes have been described in CDKL5 patients [3, 4, 7] and may mimic the absence of hand skills, the intellectual disability, hyperactivity, poor response to social interactions, and breathing problems that have been described. Heterozygous Cdkl5 KO female mice, similarly to Cdkl5 male mice [20, 24, 3335], show neuroanatomical defects, including dendritic hypotrophy and defects in spine density/maturation. Moreover, we found that loss of Cdkl5 in Cdkl5 KO female mice impaired, in an age-related manner, phosphorylation levels of the protein kinase B (Akt) and the extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK1/2), important signaling mediators in brain function. Therefore, we believe that heterozygous Cdkl5 KO female mice are a valuable tool for further studies aimed at improving the neurological phenotype due to mutations in CDKL5.

4.1. General Health and Sensory Abnormalities

Although no significant difference was observed in body weight among young adult wild-type, heterozygous, and homozygous Cdkl5 female mice ([23] and present study), we found that adult heterozygous and homozygous Cdkl5 females showed a lower body weight than did age-matched wild-type females. The decreased weight gain observed in heterozygous and homozygous Cdkl5 female mice might be secondary to increased energy expenditure, resulting from both an increased basal metabolic rate and locomotor activity. Since olfactory inputs help coordinate food appreciation and selection, but have an important role in energy balance, influencing body weight [43], our finding that heterozygous and homozygous Cdkl5 female mice showed reduced olfaction could be an explanation for the decreased weight gain. On the other hand, increased locomotor activity and locomotion stereotypes, such as elevated vertical activity (vertical jumps), observed in heterozygous and homozygous Cdkl5 female mice, could influence body weight by increasing energy expenditure. With regard to reduced olfaction, it remains to be established whether alterations in the neuronal circuitry of the olfactory bulb and/or a reduced neurogenesis underlie this defect in Cdkl5 KO mice.

4.2. Autism-Like Features

Autistic-like (ASD-like) features, including deficits in social interaction, disinterest for the surrounding environment, and stereotypic/repetitive behaviors, have been commonly described in CDKL5 patients [44, 45] and male Cdkl5 KO mouse models [22, 32]. Our finding that heterozygous and homozygous Cdkl5 female mice show impaired social behaviors (reduced nest building ability), decreased interest toward the environment (reduced marble burying), and increased stereotypic behaviors (elevated vertical activity in novel environments) suggests that the heterozygous Cdkl5 female mouse model mirrors the ASD-like features of CDKL5 female patients.

4.3. Motor Abnormalities

Motor dysfunction is a prominent feature of CDKL5 deficiency, with the majority of patients having some degree of impairment in gross and fine motor skills, including gait dysfunction and defects in sitting, standing, walking, and grasping [45]. Consistent with this severe motor phenotype in female CDKL5 patients and with previous data in male Cdkl5 KO mice [22, 23, 31, 32], we found impaired motor coordination, assessed using the accelerating rotarod assay, and novel environment-dependent hyperactivity in heterozygous and homozygous Cdkl5 KO female mice. Since female Cdkl5 KO mice had a tendency to ride the rotarod beam around rather than walk forward, in addition to the latency to fall from the rotating rod, we scored the frequency of these passive rotations. While hanging onto the rotating rod is not commonly seen in wild-type mice, we found that both homozygous and heterozygous Cdkl5 KO female mice showed an increased frequency of uncoordinated rotations, indicating a serious difficulty in motor coordination. Cdkl5 KO female mice showed higher levels of activity in the open field similar to what has been described in male Cdkl5 KO mice [22, 32, 35]. This hyperactivity was not related to changes in emotionality/anxiety, as it did not affect the activity in the central part of the field (increased exploration at the edges/walls compared to the center of the arena).

4.4. Cognitive Impairments

Severe intellectual disability is a central feature of human CDKL5 deficiency [44]. Impairment in learning and memory is also a consistent feature in male Cdkl5 KO mice [20, 22, 24, 25, 30, 32, 35]. A marked phenotype of heterozygous and homozygous Cdkl5 females was attained in the spatial learning and memory paradigm. The acquisition of allocentric learning was markedly impaired in Cdkl5 KO female mice, thus reinforcing the concept that loss of Cdkl5 is affecting hippocampal learning and memory function. Since learning/memory defects evaluated in the Morris water maze task might be influenced by the differences in swimming speed or in the motivational/motor (floating behavior) aspects observed between wild-type and Cdkl5 KO female mice, we also tested memory in a passive avoidance test [24]. Again, heterozygous and homozygous Cdkl5 KO female mice showed a substantial memory impairment (−40%), reinforcing the concept that loss of Cdkl5 leads to alterations in the acquisition process.

4.5. Respiratory Problems

To complete our neurobehavioral studies, we monitored breathing pattern, a physiological parameter. Respiratory problems, including the presence and frequency of breathing irregularities (hyperventilation, apnea), are a reported feature in CDKL5 patients [46, 47]. Moreover, disrupted respiratory function and an increased number of apneas during sleep have been recently described also in male Cdkl5 KO mice [40, 48]. We observed a similar increase in apnea occurrence during NREM sleep and during the whole sleep time in heterozygous and homozygous Cdkl5 KO female mice, strengthening the statement that the sleep apnea occurrence rate can be a significant biomarker for validation of future therapies in CDKL5 disorder.

4.6. Hippocampal Neuroanatomical Defects

Several studies have shown that Cdkl5 KO male mice, in addition to various behavioral abnormalities, exhibit profound changes in neuronal morphology [20, 2225, 3335]. Moreover, these neuroanatomical alterations, including defects in neuronal survival and maturation and defects in the organization of dendritic spines as well as in the density of PSD-95 dendritic clusters, have been shown to be instrumental as preclinical readouts for testing potential therapeutics [20, 24, 33, 35]. Our present findings that heterozygous Cdkl5 KO females show altered neuronal maturation and synapse development confirm that cellular phenotypes, in addition to behavioral phenotypes, are useful outcome measures in preclinical studies.

4.7. Pathway Dysfunctions

In line with previous evidence [23, 25], we found that juvenile Cdkl5 KO female mice show an alteration in the AKT pathway but no differences in the phosphorylation levels of ERK1/2 in the hippocampal region. Our present findings that young adult (8 week old) Cdkl5 KO female mice show defects in both AKT and ERK signaling pathways, while adult (3–4 months of age) Cdkl5 KO females show a strong reduction in ERK1/2 phosphorylation, but no alterations in AKT phosphorylation levels might be explained by age-related changes in the activation and/or modulation of these pathways [49, 50]. Interestingly, AKT is highly expressed in the developing mouse brain but is present at low levels in the adult brain [51]. On the other hand, ERK1/2 is abundant in the adult brain, and its activation can play multiple roles in the activity-dependent regulation of neuronal function [52]. Consistent with its critical role in key cellular activities, dysregulation of ERK signaling has been implicated in the pathogenesis of many neuropsychiatric and neurological disorders including autism [53], mental retardation [54], and several neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease, and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis [55]. Our data, showing age-related changes in AKT and ERK signaling pathways in Cdkl5 KO mice, indicate that the changes in signaling pathways occurring during the stage of early brain development due to loss of Cdkl5 differ from those that present later on in adulthood. The direct functional effects of these changes on CDKL5 pathogenesis are not yet known. However, the age dependence of these alterations suggests that the efficacy of signaling pathway-targeted therapeutics may be limited to different developmental periods.

4.8. Mosaic Cdkl5 Expression and Phenotypic Outcomes

Heterozygous female CDKL5-deficient patients show a vast clinical heterogeneity that spans from milder forms—with less severe epilepsy and less severe comorbidities—to more severe forms featuring intractable seizures and the lack of achievement of developmental milestones [45]. As seen for other dominant X-linked disorders [56], this clinical heterogeneity might be, at least in part, dependent on the phenomenon of variable XCI in heterozygous females. Blood analysis in CDKL5 female patients indicates a random (50%) XCI pattern [44, 5759], suggesting that different phenotypes are not the result of X-inactivation status. However, XCI skewing is difficult to prove in human brains, since it is frequently measured indirectly from peripheral blood samples and it is difficult to determine whether the same XCI ratio is also present in the brain. Furthermore, the distribution of XCI in the brain may differ from region to region due to its tissue-specific variation [60]. As previously reported [9, 10], we found that Cdkl5 levels vary between the individual brain areas, with the highest Cdkl5 levels in the cortex and hippocampus and lower levels in the cerebellum. In all analyzed brain regions, we found that Cdkl5 +/− mice show average decreased levels of Cdkl5 to ∼42% of those contained in wild-type counterparts. Our results, which show little variability in Cdkl5 protein expression among the Cdkl5 +/− brain samples, provide a first indication of balanced XCI patterns in heterozygous Cdkl5 KO mouse brains. This might explain the relatively small degree of phenotypic variability among heterozygous female mice. Moreover, the putative skewing toward the mutated X chromosome found in the hippocampus of heterozygous females (decreased levels of Cdkl5 ~ 36%) could explain the severe phenotypic outcome in hippocampus-dependent behaviors that is more similar to homozygous Cdkl5 KO female and hemizygous Cdkl5 KO male mice (Table 1) [20, 35, 61]. Further studies based on Cdkl5 single-cell based immunodetection in heterozygous brains are required to accurately evaluate whether XCI influences the phenotypic outcome of loss of Cdkl5 in heterozygous female mice.

Table 1.

Comparison of behavioral phenotypes in Cdkl5 KO female and male mice.

Cdkl5 +/− Cdkl5 −/− Cdkl5 −/Y
Olfactory performance Buried food test +++ +++ na
Autistic-like features Nest building 22 hours +++ +++ +++ [22]
Marble burying ++ +++ na
Motor function Rotarod + + ++ [22, 32]
Open field + ++ +++ [32]
Learning and memory Morris water maze learning∗∗ ++ +++ +++ [20, 24]
Morris water maze probe +++ +++ +++ [20, 24]
Passive avoidance ++ ++ +++ [24]
Breathing Sleep apneas +++ +++ +++ [40]

Phenotypes are compared to their sex-matched wild-type condition. +, 10–25% of difference; ++, 25–50% of difference; and +++, >50% of difference. na: not assessed. Average of all trials; ∗∗average of latency day 2–day 5.

By comparing the behavioral results obtained in this study with literature data (Table 1), we found that heterozygous Cdkl5 KO females develop behavioral abnormalities that are comparable, albeit milder, to defects identified in homozygous Cdkl5 KO (Cdkl5 −/−) female and hemizygous Cdkl5 KO (Cdkl5 −/Y) male mice. The assessment of specificity and sensitivity of the behavioral tests to discriminate between control and Cdkl5 KO female mice (ROC analysis; Supplementary Table 1) shows that several behaviors, including autistic-like (i.e., nest building) and cognitive behaviors (i.e., passive avoidance), and brain functions, such as sleep patterns, had AUC values > 0.8, indicating that they might be reliable preclinical readouts for testing the translational potential of therapeutics in females.

Despite the presence of numerous neurobehavioral defects, both Cdkl5 KO male and female mice do not show spontaneous epilepsy, a hallmark clinical feature of CDKL5 disorder. Even if alterations in the excitatory/inhibitory balance, frequently postulated as a mechanism underlying epileptogenesis and seizure, were observed in Cdkl5 KO mice [31, 34], it is likely that the differences of neural connectivity between humans and mice might underlie the different seizure susceptibility in the Cdkl5 KO mouse model.

4.9. Conclusions

Recent studies in Cdkl5 KO mice have revealed different mechanisms that might be therapeutically manipulated and have produced compelling evidence that signaling pathways downstream of CDKL5 can effectively be targeted to ameliorate CDKL5-related symptoms in mice [20, 24, 33]. However, the majority of work involving Cdkl5 mouse models has focused on the loss of Cdkl5 in male mice, avoiding the potential cofounding effects of variable X-chromosome inactivation (XCI) in female mice. Our study provides a comprehensive overview of neurobehavioral and physiological phenotypes of the heterozygous female Cdkl5 KO mouse and demonstrates that this mouse model recapitulates multiple features observed in CDKL5 patients; therefore, it might be considered a valuable animal model for future therapeutic studies.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants to Elisabetta Ciani from Fondazione Telethon (GGP15098) and from the Italian parent association “CDKL5 insieme verso la cura.”

Data Availability

The data used to support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Additional Points

Highlights. Heterozygous Cdkl5 KO female mice show autism-like behaviors and defects in motor coordination. Heterozygous Cdkl5 KO female mice show defects in learning and memory performance. Heterozygous Cdkl5 KO female mice show breathing abnormalities. Heterozygous Cdkl5 KO female mice show age-related alterations of the ERK1/2 and AKT pathways. Heterozygous Cdkl5 KO female mice recapitulate multiple features observed in CDKL5 patients.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Authors' Contributions

Claudia Fuchs and Laura Gennaccaro contributed equally to this work.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary 1

Supplementary Figure 1: age-dependent alteration in AKT and ERK signaling pathways in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− female mice. A: Western blot analysis of β-catenin levels normalized to GAPDH (glyceraldehyde 3-phosphate dehydrogenase) levels (left histogram) and P-MSK1-Thr 581 levels normalized to GAPDH (right histogram) in the hippocampus of 12–14 week old Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 4 and n = 6, resp.), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 5) and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 4 and n = 5, resp.) mice. B: Western blot analysis of P-AKT-Ser 473 levels normalized to total AKT levels (left histogram) and β-catenin levels normalized to GAPDH levels (right histogram) in the hippocampus of 3 week old Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 3 and n = 4, resp.), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 3 and n = 4, resp.), and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 3 and n = 5, resp.) mice. C: Western blot analysis of P-ERK-Ser 42–44 levels normalized to total ERK levels (left histograms) and P-MSK1-Thr 581 levels normalized to GAPDH (right histogram) in the hippocampus of 3 week old Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 4 and n = 5, resp.), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 4 and n = 5, resp.), and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 4 and n = 5, resp.) mice. D: Western blot analysis of P-AKT-Ser 473 levels normalized to total AKT levels (left histograms) and β-catenin levels normalized to GAPDH (right histogram) levels in the hippocampus of 8 week old Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 4 and n = 5, resp.), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 4 and n = 5, resp.), and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 6) mice. E: Western blot analysis of P-ERK-Ser 42–44 levels normalized to total ERK levels (left histograms) and P-MSK1-Thr 581 levels normalized to GAPDH (right histogram) in the hippocampus of 8 week old Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 3), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 5), and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 4) mice. Immunoblots are examples from one animal of each experimental group. Data are expressed as a percentage of the values of Cdkl5 +/+ mice. Values are represented as means ± SE. p < 0.05 and ∗∗ p < 0.01 (Fisher's LSD after ANOVA).

Supplementary 2

Supplementary Table 1: ROC analysis on neurobehavioral and functional assays. The relative area under the curve (AUC) and correspondent p values are reported. The area under the curve (AUC) reflects the specificity and sensitivity of a test and ranges from totally noninformative (AUC = 0.5) to perfect test (AUC = 1).

References

  • 1.Kalscheuer V. M., Tao J., Donnelly A., et al. Disruption of the serine/threonine kinase 9 gene causes severe X-linked infantile spasms and mental retardation. The American Journal of Human Genetics. 2003;72(6):1401–1411. doi: 10.1086/375538. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Weaving L. S., Christodoulou J., Williamson S. L., et al. Mutations of CDKL5 cause a severe neurodevelopmental disorder with infantile spasms and mental retardation. The American Journal of Human Genetics. 2004;75(6):1079–1093. doi: 10.1086/426462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bahi-Buisson N., Bienvenu T. CDKL5-related disorders: from clinical description to molecular genetics. Molecular Syndromology. 2012;2(3–5):137–152. doi: 10.1159/000331333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fehr S., Wilson M., Downs J., et al. The CDKL5 disorder is an independent clinical entity associated with early-onset encephalopathy. European Journal of Human Genetics. 2013;21(3):266–273. doi: 10.1038/ejhg.2012.156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bahi-Buisson N., Villeneuve N., Caietta E., et al. Recurrent mutations in the CDKL5 gene: genotype–phenotype relationships. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A. 2012;158A(7):1612–1619. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.35401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Guerrini R., Parrini E. Epilepsy in Rett syndrome, and CDKL5- and FOXG1-gene–related encephalopathies. Epilepsia. 2012;53(12):2067–2078. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03656.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Fehr S., Downs J., Ho G., et al. Functional abilities in children and adults with the CDKL5 disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A. 2016;170(11):2860–2869. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.37851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Montini E., Andolfi G., Caruso A., et al. Identification and characterization of a novel serine–threonine kinase gene from the Xp22 region. Genomics. 1998;51(3):427–433. doi: 10.1006/geno.1998.5391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Rusconi L., Salvatoni L., Giudici L., et al. CDKL5 expression is modulated during neuronal development and its subcellular distribution is tightly regulated by the C-terminal tail. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2008;283(44):30101–30111. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M804613200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kilstrup-Nielsen C., Rusconi L., La Montanara P., et al. What we know and would like to know about CDKL5 and its involvement in epileptic encephalopathy. Neural Plasticity. 2012;2012:11. doi: 10.1155/2012/728267.728267 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Mari F., Azimonti S., Bertani I., et al. CDKL5 belongs to the same molecular pathway of MeCP2 and it is responsible for the early-onset seizure variant of Rett syndrome. Human Molecular Genetics. 2005;14(14):1935–1946. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddi198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Bertani I., Rusconi L., Bolognese F., et al. Functional consequences of mutations in CDKL5, an X-linked gene involved in infantile spasms and mental retardation. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2006;281(42):32048–32056. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M606325200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kameshita I., Sekiguchi M., Hamasaki D., et al. Cyclin-dependent kinase-like 5 binds and phosphorylates DNA methyltransferase 1. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications. 2008;377(4):1162–1167. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2008.10.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Chen Q., Zhu Y. C., Yu J., et al. CDKL5, a protein associated with rett syndrome, regulates neuronal morphogenesis via Rac1 signaling. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(38):12777–12786. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1102-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sekiguchi M., Katayama S., Hatano N., Shigeri Y., Sueyoshi N., Kameshita I. Identification of amphiphysin 1 as an endogenous substrate for CDKL5, a protein kinase associated with X-linked neurodevelopmental disorder. Archives of Biochemistry and Biophysics. 2013;535(2):257–267. doi: 10.1016/j.abb.2013.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Zhu Y. C., Li D., Wang L., et al. Palmitoylation-dependent CDKL5–PSD-95 interaction regulates synaptic targeting of CDKL5 and dendritic spine development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2013;110(22):9118–9123. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1300003110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Ricciardi S., Ungaro F., Hambrock M., et al. CDKL5 ensures excitatory synapse stability by reinforcing NGL-1–PSD95 interaction in the postsynaptic compartment and is impaired in patient iPSC-derived neurons. Nature Cell Biology. 2012;14(9):911–923. doi: 10.1038/ncb2566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Mertz J., Tan H., Pagala V., et al. Sequential elution interactome analysis of the mind bomb 1 ubiquitin ligase reveals a novel role in dendritic spine outgrowth. Molecular & Cellular Proteomics. 2015;14(7):1898–1910. doi: 10.1074/mcp.M114.045898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Nawaz M. S., Giarda E., Bedogni F., et al. CDKL5 and shootin1 interact and concur in regulating neuronal polarization. PLoS One. 2016;11(2, article e0148634) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0148634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Trazzi S., Fuchs C., Viggiano R., et al. HDAC4: a key factor underlying brain developmental alterations in CDKL5 disorder. Human Molecular Genetics. 2016;25(18):3887–3907. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddw231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Barbiero I., Peroni D., Tramarin M., et al. The neurosteroid pregnenolone reverts microtubule derangement induced by the loss of a functional CDKL5-IQGAP1 complex. Human Molecular Genetics. 2017;26(18):3520–3530. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddx237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Wang I. T. J., Allen M., Goffin D., et al. Loss of CDKL5 disrupts kinome profile and event-related potentials leading to autistic-like phenotypes in mice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109(52):21516–21521. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216988110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Amendola E., Zhan Y., Mattucci C., et al. Mapping pathological phenotypes in a mouse model of CDKL5 disorder. PLoS One. 2014;9(5, article e91613) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Fuchs C., Rimondini R., Viggiano R., et al. Inhibition of GSK3β rescues hippocampal development and learning in a mouse model of CDKL5 disorder. Neurobiology of Disease. 2015;82:298–310. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2015.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Fuchs C., Trazzi S., Torricella R., et al. Loss of Cdkl5 impairs survival and dendritic growth of newborn neurons by altering AKT/GSK-3β signaling. Neurobiology of Disease. 2014;70:53–68. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2014.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Cosci B., Vivaldi A., Romei C., et al. In silico and in vitro analysis of rare germline allelic variants of RET oncogene associated with medullary thyroid cancer. Endocrine-Related Cancer. 2011;18(5):603–612. doi: 10.1530/ERC-11-0117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Tsai P. T., Hull C., Chu Y. X., et al. Autistic-like behaviour and cerebellar dysfunction in Purkinje cell Tsc1 mutant mice. Nature. 2012;488(7413):647–651. doi: 10.1038/nature11310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Bai X. L., Zhang Q., Ye L. Y., et al. Myocyte enhancer factor 2C regulation of hepatocellular carcinoma via vascular endothelial growth factor and Wnt/β-catenin signaling. Oncogene. 2014;34(31):4089–4097. doi: 10.1038/onc.2014.337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Okuda K., Kobayashi S., Fukaya M., et al. CDKL5 controls postsynaptic localization of GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors in the hippocampus and regulates seizure susceptibility. Neurobiology of Disease. 2017;106:158–170. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2017.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Tang S., Wang I. T. J., Yue C., et al. Loss of CDKL5 in glutamatergic neurons disrupts hippocampal microcircuitry and leads to memory impairment in mice. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37(31):7420–7437. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0539-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Sivilia S., Mangano C., Beggiato S., et al. CDKL5 knockout leads to altered inhibitory transmission in the cerebellum of adult mice. Genes, Brain, and Behavior. 2016;15(5):491–502. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Jhang C. L., Huang T. N., Hsueh Y. P., Liao W. Mice lacking cyclin-dependent kinase-like 5 manifest autistic and ADHD-like behaviors. Human Molecular Genetics. 2017;26(20):3922–3934. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddx279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Della Sala G., Putignano E., Chelini G., et al. Dendritic spine instability in a mouse model of CDKL5 disorder is rescued by insulin-like growth factor 1. Biological Psychiatry. 2016;80(4):302–311. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Pizzo R., Gurgone A., Castroflorio E., et al. Lack of Cdkl5 disrupts the organization of excitatory and inhibitory synapses and parvalbumin interneurons in the primary visual cortex. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience. 2016;10:p. 261. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2016.00261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Trazzi S., de Franceschi M., Fuchs C., et al. Cdkl5 protein substitution therapy rescues neurological phenotypes of a mouse model of Cdkl5 disorder. Human Molecular Genetics. 2018;27(9):1572–1592. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddy064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Mazziotti R., Lupori L., Sagona G., et al. Searching for biomarkers of CDKL5 disorder: early-onset visual impairment in CDKL5 mutant mice. Human Molecular Genetics. 2017;26(12):2290–2298. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddx119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lowry O. H., Rosebrough N., Farr A., Randall R. Protein measurement with the Folin phenol reagent. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1951;193(1):265–275. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Deacon R. M. Assessing nest building in mice. Nature Protocols. 2006;1(3):1117–1119. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bastianini S., Alvente S., Berteotti C., et al. Accurate discrimination of the wake-sleep states of mice using non-invasive whole-body plethysmography. Scientific Reports. 2017;7, article 41698 doi: 10.1038/srep41698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lo Martire V., Alvente S., Bastianini S., et al. CDKL5 deficiency entails sleep apneas in mice. Journal of Sleep Research. 2017;26(4):495–497. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Greiner M., Pfeiffer D., Smith R. D. Principles and practical application of the receiver-operating characteristic analysis for diagnostic tests. Preventive Veterinary Medicine. 2000;45(1-2):23–41. doi: 10.1016/S0167-5877(00)00115-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Akobeng A. K. Understanding diagnostic tests 3: receiver operating characteristic curves. Acta Paediatrica. 2007;96(5):644–647. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2006.00178.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Riera C. E., Tsaousidou E., Halloran J., et al. The sense of smell impacts metabolic health and obesity. Cell Metabolism. 2017;26(1):198–211.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2017.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bahi-Buisson N., Nectoux J., Rosas-Vargas H., et al. Key clinical features to identify girls with CDKL5 mutations. Brain. 2008;131(10):2647–2661. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Fehr S., Leonard H., Ho G., et al. There is variability in the attainment of developmental milestones in the CDKL5 disorder. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 2015;7(1):p. 2. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-7-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hagebeuk E. E. O., van den Bossche R. A. S., de Weerd A. W. Respiratory and sleep disorders in female children with atypical Rett syndrome caused by mutations in the CDKL5 gene. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2013;55(5):480–484. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04432.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Mangatt M., Wong K., Anderson B., et al. Prevalence and onset of comorbidities in the CDKL5 disorder differ from Rett syndrome. Orphanet Journal of Rare Diseases. 2016;11(1):p. 39. doi: 10.1186/s13023-016-0418-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lee K. Z., Liao W. Loss of CDKL5 disrupts respiratory function in mice. Respiratory Physiology & Neurobiology. 2018;248:48–54. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2017.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Parmar M. S., Jaumotte J. D., Wyrostek S. L., Zigmond M. J., Cavanaugh J. E. The role of ERK1, 2, and 5 in dopamine neuron survival during aging. Neurobiology of Aging. 2014;35(3):669–679. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.09.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Orellana A. M. M., Vasconcelos A. R., Leite J. A., et al. Age-related neuroinflammation and changes in AKT-GSK-3β and WNT/β-CATENIN signaling in rat hippocampus. Aging. 2015;7(12):1094–1108. doi: 10.18632/aging.100853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Chong Z. Z., Li F., Maiese K. Activating Akt and the brain’s resources to drive cellular survival and prevent inflammatory injury. Histology and Histopathology. 2005;20(1):299–315. doi: 10.14670/HH-20.299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Sun J., Nan G. The extracellular signal-regulated kinase 1/2 pathway in neurological diseases: a potential therapeutic target (Review) International Journal of Molecular Medicine. 2017;39(6):1338–1346. doi: 10.3892/ijmm.2017.2962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Yufune S., Satoh Y., Takamatsu I., et al. Transient blockade of ERK phosphorylation in the critical period causes autistic phenotypes as an adult in mice. Scientific Reports. 2015;5(1, article 10252) doi: 10.1038/srep10252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Kim S. H., Markham J. A., Weiler I. J., Greenough W. T. Aberrant early-phase ERK inactivation impedes neuronal function in fragile X syndrome. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105(11):4429–4434. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800257105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Kim K., Kong S. Y., Fulciniti M., et al. Blockade of the MEK/ERK signalling cascade by AS703026, a novel selective MEK1/2 inhibitor, induces pleiotropic anti-myeloma activity in vitro and in vivo . British Journal of Haematology. 2010;149(4):537–549. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2141.2010.08127.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Alvarez-Mora M. I., Rodriguez-Revenga L., Feliu A., Badenas C., Madrigal I., Milà M. Skewed X inactivation in women carrying the FMR1 premutation and its relation with fragile-X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome. Neurodegenerative Diseases. 2016;16(3-4):290–292. doi: 10.1159/000441566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Archer H. L., Evans J., Edwards S., et al. CDKL5 mutations cause infantile spasms, early onset seizures, and severe mental retardation in female patients. Journal of Medical Genetics. 2006;43(9):729–734. doi: 10.1136/jmg.2006.041467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Nemos C., Lambert L., Giuliano F., et al. Mutational spectrum of CDKL5 in early-onset encephalopathies: a study of a large collection of French patients and review of the literature. Clinical Genetics. 2009;76(4):357–371. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0004.2009.01194.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Zhao Y., Zhang X., Bao X., et al. Clinical features and gene mutational spectrum of CDKL5-related diseases in a cohort of Chinese patients. BMC Medical Genetics. 2014;15(1):p. 24. doi: 10.1186/1471-2350-15-24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Sharp A., Robinson D., Jacobs P. Age- and tissue-specific variation of X chromosome inactivation ratios in normal women. Human Genetics. 2000;107(4):343–349. doi: 10.1007/s004390000382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Aad G., Abbott B., Abdallah J., et al. Search for new phenomena in dijet angular distributions in proton-proton collisions at s=8TeV measured with the ATLAS detector. Physical Review Letters. 2015;114(22, article 221802) doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.114.221802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary 1

Supplementary Figure 1: age-dependent alteration in AKT and ERK signaling pathways in Cdkl5 +/− and Cdkl5 −/− female mice. A: Western blot analysis of β-catenin levels normalized to GAPDH (glyceraldehyde 3-phosphate dehydrogenase) levels (left histogram) and P-MSK1-Thr 581 levels normalized to GAPDH (right histogram) in the hippocampus of 12–14 week old Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 4 and n = 6, resp.), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 5) and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 4 and n = 5, resp.) mice. B: Western blot analysis of P-AKT-Ser 473 levels normalized to total AKT levels (left histogram) and β-catenin levels normalized to GAPDH levels (right histogram) in the hippocampus of 3 week old Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 3 and n = 4, resp.), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 3 and n = 4, resp.), and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 3 and n = 5, resp.) mice. C: Western blot analysis of P-ERK-Ser 42–44 levels normalized to total ERK levels (left histograms) and P-MSK1-Thr 581 levels normalized to GAPDH (right histogram) in the hippocampus of 3 week old Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 4 and n = 5, resp.), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 4 and n = 5, resp.), and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 4 and n = 5, resp.) mice. D: Western blot analysis of P-AKT-Ser 473 levels normalized to total AKT levels (left histograms) and β-catenin levels normalized to GAPDH (right histogram) levels in the hippocampus of 8 week old Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 4 and n = 5, resp.), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 4 and n = 5, resp.), and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 6) mice. E: Western blot analysis of P-ERK-Ser 42–44 levels normalized to total ERK levels (left histograms) and P-MSK1-Thr 581 levels normalized to GAPDH (right histogram) in the hippocampus of 8 week old Cdkl5 +/+ (n = 3), Cdkl5 +/− (n = 5), and Cdkl5 −/− (n = 4) mice. Immunoblots are examples from one animal of each experimental group. Data are expressed as a percentage of the values of Cdkl5 +/+ mice. Values are represented as means ± SE. p < 0.05 and ∗∗ p < 0.01 (Fisher's LSD after ANOVA).

Supplementary 2

Supplementary Table 1: ROC analysis on neurobehavioral and functional assays. The relative area under the curve (AUC) and correspondent p values are reported. The area under the curve (AUC) reflects the specificity and sensitivity of a test and ranges from totally noninformative (AUC = 0.5) to perfect test (AUC = 1).

Data Availability Statement

The data used to support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.


Articles from Neural Plasticity are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES