Abstract
Despite the many studies focusing on epilepsy, a lot of the basic mechanisms underlying seizure susceptibility are mainly unclear. Here, we studied cellular electrical excitability, as well as excitatory and inhibitory synaptic neurotransmission of CA1 pyramidal neurons from the dorsal hippocampus of a genetic model of epilepsy, the Wistar Audiogenic Rat (WARs) in which limbic seizures appear after repeated audiogenic stimulation. We examined intrinsic properties of neurons, as well as EPSCs evoked by Schaffer-collateral stimulation in slices from WARs and Wistar parental strain. We also analyzed spontaneous IPSCs and quantal miniature inhibitory events. Our data show that even in the absence of previous seizures, GABAergic neurotransmission is reduced in the dorsal hippocampus of WARs. We observed a decrease in the frequency of IPSCs and mIPSCs. Moreover, mIPSCs of WARs had faster rise times, indicating that they probably arise from more proximal synapses. Finally, intrinsic membrane properties, firing and excitatory neurotransmission mediated by both NMDA and non-NMDA receptors are similar to the parental strain. Since GABAergic inhibition towards CA1 pyramidal neurons is reduced in WARs, the inhibitory network could be ineffective to prevent the seizure-dependent spread of hyperexcitation. These functional changes could make these animals more susceptible to the limbic seizures observed during the audiogenic kindling.
Introduction
Epilepsy is a set of neurological disorders that has as a common symptom the appearance of sudden events of hypersynchronization and hyperactivity of neurons1. Despite the great amount of scientific literature on epilepsy, the basic mechanisms of seizures onset and spread still remain unclear. Thus, animal models offer ways to test hypothesis on those mechanisms, and can help to identify novel targets and consequently more efficient tools2.
Acute audiogenic seizures are generalized reflex tonic-clonic seizures induced by a high intensity sound (e.g. 120 dB), regardless of its frequency. Although they are rare in humans, audiogenic seizures are very well characterized in several strains of rodents, with stereotyped behaviors and epileptiform electroencephalographic activity initially restricted to the auditory brainstem3–5. Nonetheless, upon repetition of the sound, the animals start to exhibit behavioral patterns typical of limbic seizures, with epileptiform discharges spreading through amygdala, hippocampus and auditory cortex, a phenomenon known as limbic recruitment5–8.
Auditory stimulation impacts the hippocampus in complex ways. There are evidences, for example, that hippocampal place cells respond to an auditory dimension task9. Also, we showed that repeated high intensity sound stimulation inhibits hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP)10,11.
According to our previous findings, naïve Wistar Audiogenic Rats (WARs) showed memory impairments in spatial performance in the Morris Water Maze and a slower development of LTP10. Additionally, a decreased GABAergic inhibition was observed in dissociated neurons from the hippocampus of newborn WARs12 as well as field potentials13 recorded in hippocampal slices of adult WARs. Finally, a recent study showed, in addition to other morphological alterations, marked decrease in the volume in the CA3 of adult WARs14. In the current study we characterized membrane electrical properties of CA1 pyramidal neurons from the dorsal hippocampus of naive WARs, as well as synaptic neurotransmission, to verify possible alterations that could be relevant for the seizure-dependent spread of hyperexcitation when challenged by acute or chronic audiogenic seizures.
Results
CA1 pyramidal cells of WARs are more hyperpolarized than those from Wistar cells
CA1 pyramidal neurons from WARs (n = 17) and Wistar rats (n = 14) had similar firing patterns (Fig. 1A) typical to what has been previously reported for CA1 pyramidal neurons (Wheeler et al., 2015). The resting membrane potential of WAR neurons are more hyperpolarized than Wistar neurons (Wistar = −72 ± 0.9 mV; WAR = −77 ± 1.04 mV; P = 0.0008; Fig. 1B), but we did not observe statistical differences in input resistance, membrane time constant or depolarization sag (Table 1) between neurons from WAR and Wistar rats. The analysis of FI curves showed that WAR cells fire less action potentials than Wistar cells [F (1,26) = 4.39, p = 0.046, for main effect], although at a fixed value of 100 pA over rheobase, we did not see statistical differences between the two groups (Wistar = 10.7 ± 0.95 Hz; WAR = 9.14 ± 0.89 Hz; P = 0.23; Fig. 1C,D), suggesting that this difference was caused by the more hyperpolarized resting membrane potential of WAR neurons. Also, action potential parameters did not differ between groups (Table 1).
Table 1.
Intrinsic Membrane Properties and Action Potential Parameters | |||
---|---|---|---|
Wistar | WAR | p | |
Rinput (MΩ) | 69 ± 3 | 64 ± 3 | 0.27 |
τ (ms) | 16.8 ± 1.2 | 13.7 ± 1 | 0.06 |
Sag (mV) | 3.1 ± 0.5 | 3.1 ± 0.3 | 0.99 |
APamplitude (mV) | 104.5 ± 1.6 | 105.6 ± 1.3 | 0.61 |
Half width (ms) | 0.97 ± 0.02 | 0.9 ± 0.1 | 0.91 |
FastAHP (mV) | −69.4 ± 0.9 | −70.6 ± 1.5 | 0.52 |
APthreshold (mV) | −61.9 ± 0.7 | −60.4 ± 0.9 | 0.24 |
Data are represented as mean ± SEM. Unpaired t-test with p < 0.05 considered as significant.
Glutamatergic excitatory neurotransmission is similar in WAR and Wistar pyramidal neurons
In Fig. 2 we show EPSCs mediated by AMPA/KA and NMDA receptors. Our data show that AMPA/KA-mediated EPSCs from both strains were similar in all tested voltages (Fig. 2B, n = 12 for Wistar and n = 15 for WARs). The amplitudes of EPSCs at −70 mV are similar in cells from Wistar (n = 14) and WARs (n = 15) (Wistar = −523.8 ± 71.07 pA; WAR = −388.3 ± 56.6 pA; P = 0.13; Fig. 2C), as well as the rise times (Wistar = 5.54 ± 0.6 ms; WAR = 5.36 ± 0.24 ms; P = 0.77), decay times (Wistar = 44.4 ± 4.13 ms; WAR = 46.7 ± 3.7 ms; P = 0.68) and half-widths (Wistar = 14.39 ± 1.3 ms; WAR = 12.9 ± 0.72 ms; P = 0.32). The AMPA/KA conductances were similar between the two groups (Wistar = 14.39 ± 1.3 ms, n = 12; WAR = 12.9 ± 0.72 ms, n = 15; P = 0.32). We also found that synapses from both animals presented similar short-term facilitation of AMPA/KA currents [F (1, 29) = 1.063; P = 0.3112, for main effect, n = 14 for Wistar and n = 15 for WARs] (Fig. 2D).
NMDA EPSCs had similar amplitudes in all tested voltage steps in the two groups of neurons (Fig. 2F, n = 12 for Wistar and n = 15 for WARs). NMDA at +80 mV EPSCs from CA1 neurons from Wistar and WARs had similar amplitudes (INMDA@+80 mV: Wistar = 313.9 ± 51.8 pA, n = 14; WAR = 348.3 ± 36.6 pA, n = 15; P = 0.58, Fig. 2G; rise times (Wistar = 3.3 ± 0.5 ms; WAR = 3.4 ± 0.34 ms; P = 0.86), decay times (Wistar = 305.3 ± 21.5 ms; WAR = 344 ± 22.6 ms; P = 0.23) and half-widths (Wistar = 14.39 ± 1.3 ms; WAR = 12.9 ± 0.72 ms; P = 0.32). We did not observe any differences in NMDA conductances in depolarized potentials (Wistar = 4.68 ± 0.8 nS; WAR = 5.29 ± 0.48 nS; P = 0.5) The ratio between AMPA/KA and NMDA EPSCs was not significantly different (Wistar = 0.398 ± 0.075; WAR = 0.67 ± 0.11; P = 0.2; Fig. 2H).
Neurons from WAR animals receive less GABAergic spontaneous IPSCs
Spontaneous GABAergic IPSCs recorded in CA1 pyramidal cells, reflect the activity of the interneuron network over principal excitatory cells. To determine whether genetic selection of the audiogenic phenotype had impacted GABAergic neurotransmission, we analyzed spontaneous IPSCs. Figure 3A shows raster plots of the sIPSCs over a 1-minute window. In Fig. 3B, in turn, we show typical IPSCs from Wistar and WAR neurons, and below (in green) the effect of applying the GABAA receptor antagonist picrotoxin (20 and 100 µM), confirming the GABAergic nature of the currents.
We counted more events in total from Wistar neurons than from WAR neurons (Fig. 3C) suggesting a more active inhibitory network onto the CA1 pyramidal cell from Wistar animals. Accordingly, the analysis of IEI distributions showed significant differences between the two groups (KS test, p = 0.002. Fig. 3D) with a distribution of IEIs shifted to the right in WARs. The mean frequency of IPSCs was significantly smaller in cells from WARs as compared with Wistar cells (Wistar = 4.11 ± 0.68 Hz; WAR = 1.8 ± 0.23 Hz; P = 0. 0014; Fig. 3E). The analysis of the coefficient of variation (CVs) of the IEI was similar in both groups indicating the same irregular pattern of activity in time in the two groups (Wistar = 99.35 ± 5.24%; WAR = 107.1 ± 7.07%; P = 0.4; Fig. 3F).
The amplitudes distributions followed a skewed Gaussian pattern and were similar between the groups (Fig. 3F). We did not observe differences in amplitude cumulative distribution (KS test, p = 0.95), and in mean amplitudes (Wistar = 71.6 ± 8.4 pA; WAR = 87.9 ± 7.4 pA; p = 0.15, Fig. 3H) and the amplitude CVs were similar between the two groups, showing similar variabilities among them (Wistar = 63.29 ± 6.3%; WAR = 63.38 ± 4.3%; p = 0.99). ISPCs from both groups of animals presented similar rise times (Wistar = 1.15 ± 0.08 ms; WAR = 1.1 ± 0.05 ms; p = 0.6), decay times (τfast, Wistar = 3.52 ± 0.47 ms; WAR = 2.7 ± 0.23 ms; p = 0.11 and τslow, Wistar = 38.65 ± 5.7 ms; WAR = 28.78 ± 3 ms; p = 0.1) and half-widths (Wistar = 3.15 ± 0.3 ms; WAR = 2.5 ± 0.2 ms; p = 0. 12).
Miniature GABAergic currents are faster and less frequent in WARs
GABAergic currents were recorded in the presence of TTX. Application of TTX inhibited more the frequency of the IPSCs in neurons from Wistar rats than from cells of WAR rats (Wistar = 49% and WAR = 30.5%). In Fig. 4 we show raster plots of mIPSCs in 1-minute windows for each cell together with typical mIPSCs (A-B). After TTX, the cumulative distributions of the IEIs from WARS were again shifted to the right, with more events separated by longer IEIs (>1 second; KS test, p < 0.0001; Fig. 4C,D) and the mean frequency of WAR mIPSCs was lower than that of Wistar mIPSCs, although this difference did not reach significance (Wistar = 1.25 ± 0.12 Hz; WAR = 0.89 ± 0.14 Hz; P = 0.084; Fig. 4E). The CVs were similar indicating the same irregular pattern of mIPSCs appearance for both groups of animals (Wistar = 114.7 ± 11.7%; WAR = 106 ± 2.7%; p = 0.4).
Interestingly, amplitudes of the IPSCs were not affected by TTX. The amplitude distributions were similar in both groups (Fig. 4F,G. KS test, p = 0.9621) and the mean amplitudes of mIPSCs were not different between groups (Wistar = 74.46 ± 3.74 pA; WAR = 81.68 ± 4.4 pA; p = 0.32; Fig. 4H). Also, the CVs of the amplitudes were similar (Wistar = 59.7 ± 5.56%; WAR = 57.93 ± 4.83%; p = 0.81). We did not find differences in the half-widths (Wistar = 2.6 ± 0.12 ms; WAR = 2.5 ± 0.06 ms; p = 0.5; Fig. 5A) of the mIPSCs.
The decay phase of the mIPSCs could be well fitted with double exponentials and characterized by a fast and a slow time constant both similar between groups (τfast: Wistar = 2.53 ± 0.1 ms; WAR = 2.48 ± 0.09 ms; p = 0.7 and τslow: Wistar = 16.9 ± 1.2 ms; WAR = 15.08 ± 0.48 ms; P = 0.13; Fig. 5B–D). Interestingly, we observed significant differences in the mean rise times (Wistar = 0.95 ± 0.04 ms; WAR = 0.84 ± 0.017 ms; p = 0.0078; Fig. 6A), indicating that mIPSCs in WAR cells were faster. In fact, the distribution of the rise times of both group of cells exhibited two peaks: a large peak at small values and a smaller peak for bigger values, indicating the existence of fast-rising mIPSCs (<1 ms) and slow-rising mIPSCs (>1 ms) (Fig. 6B–D). These distributions were statistically different between groups (KS test, p = 0.0027). We thus, separated the rise times in two groups (cutoff = 1 ms) and tested if the two populations of mIPSCs presented a similar pattern. We found that slow mIPSCs in cells from WARs had shorter rise times than from Wistars (Wistar = 1.5 ± 0.04 ms; WAR = 1.29 ± 0.019 ms; p < 0.0001, Fig. 6E), whereas both groups had similar fast mIPSCs (Wistar = 0.648 ± 0.01 ms; WAR = 0.65 ± 0.006 ms; P = 0.79, Fig. 6F). The fast and slow mIPSCs were similarly distributed among both strains, being 70.8% fast and 29.2% slow in cells from WARs and 65.4% fast and 34.6% slow in cells from Wistars. This result points to a particular change in the kinetics of mIPSPs. Whereas fast rise times are kept, longer rise times are altered. We propose that one subpopulation is kept (the fast), one subpopulation is lost (the slower) and one subpopulation is enhanced (peak close to 1.4 ms) (Fig. 6G). Markedly, if we combine the statistics from both groups (Fig. 6) the behavior is preserved. Amplitude is the same in both Wistar and WAR but we can still observe a second subgroup only in the WAR’s rise times as a darker region in the joint plot (Fig. 6H). Finally, no correlations were observed between rise times and amplitudes or half-widths (data not shown).
Discussion
Although acute audiogenic seizures involve mainly the activation of primary brainstem auditory areas, chronic acoustic stimuli or audiogenic kindling often lead to the spread of epileptic activity to limbic areas, such as cortex, hippocampus and amygdala, resulting in a change in behavior and electrical activity5–8,15,16. At some point of audiogenic kindling, brainstem seizures and limbic seizures start to co-exist and this phenomenon can be easily observed with all audiogenic strains4,16. Interestingly, although, a few non-susceptible animals sometimes exhibit low score brainstem seizures, limbic seizures are rare, no matter how many stimulations the animals experience17, which leads to the hypothesis that in the audiogenic strains, the auditory-limbic pathways are altered. In fact, previous findings indicated that in the WAR strain, auditory-limbic circuitry is facilitated, even in the absence of seizures18. In this respect, audiogenic kindling, firstly described by Marescaux et al.6 and its electrophysiological counterparts subsequently reproduced in Genetically Epilepsy-Prone Rats (GEPRs) by Naritoku et al.7 and in WARs by Dutra Moraes et al.8 could be considered as a model of limbic recruitment or TLE4,8, the most common epilepsy syndrome in human adults19, in which a massive activation of limbic structures can be observed during seizures.
We have previously reported that, endogenously, dorsal hippocampi of WARs exhibited delayed LTP in Schaffer-CA1 synapses, which was accompanied by spatial memory impairments in the Morris Water Maze, suggestive of strain specific changes in hippocampal circuits10. In the current study, we investigated intrinsic and synaptic properties of CA1 pyramidal cells of WARs to verify the differences in the excitation/inhibition balance in the hippocampus of these animals that could underlie limbic recruitment. We found that CA1 pyramidal neurons have similar electrophysiological properties in WAR and Wistar control strains, except for a more hyperpolarized resting membrane potential in neurons from WARs.
To begin with, since it is well established that glutamatergic neurotransmission mediated by either AMPA/KA or NMDA receptors play an important role in seizure susceptibility in many animal models2, we first investigated possible alterations in excitatory synapses in the CA1 field of WAR. Our results show no differences in the excitatory glutamatergic AMPA/kainate and NMDA currents in both Wistar and WAR strains, suggesting that the fast-excitatory synapses in the CA1 are similar and could not be the substrate for the seizure susceptibility.
Similar to other cortical structures, most hippocampal cells are excitatory, and in CA1, only 11% of cells are GABAergic interneurons20. Despite that, inhibitory interneurons control the activity of principal cells and the loss of inhibition targeting excitatory neurons in the CA1, might have a profound impact not only in the hippocampus, but in areas that receive its connections20. In our study, we observed a decrease in spontaneous GABAergic currents whether the network was active or not. Since the spontaneous GABAergic inhibition strongly regulates pyramidal neuron firing21, the observed reduction in the frequency of sIPSCs in WARs could potentially impact the excitatory-inhibitory balance in these neurons, and potentiate their firing, when the Schaffer pathway is stimulated. In that context, a decreased GABAergic inhibition could favor the susceptibility to limbic seizures, as observed in audiogenically kindled WARs4,8,16,22. In fact, the impairment in the excitation/inhibition balance is thought as a possible trigger for seizures in many animal models of epilepsy and in clinical disorders2.
A decrease in the frequency of GABAergic currents could be consequence to a reduction in the number of interneurons or the number of synapses23,24. Many models of acquired and genetic epilepsy report alterations in GABAergic neurotransmission in the hippocampus. Knopp et al.24 observed a decrease in the frequency of mIPSCs in the subiculum of rats two weeks after Status Epilepticus (SE) induced by pilocarpine (PILO). These authors observed a marked decrease in the number of fast spiking parvalbumin+ interneurons in the CA1 pyramidal layer and subiculum. Although we did not see a decrease in mIPSC frequency, our data showed that mIPSCs in WAR were separated by longer inter-event intervals, what could also reflect a change in the number of active synapses or release probability. According to Wierenga and Wadman25, hippocampal kindling induces a reduction in the frequency of mIPSCs in CA1 pyramidal cells. It is important to remember that in the PILO model, intense death of interneurons is always reported26, in contrast to what happens in hippocampal kindling27, where a modest cell loss can be observed. In both models, a re-organization of synaptic inputs take place in the hippocampus preceding the appearance of seizures and continuing as seizures occur. In our model, WAR animals do not present Fluorojade+ (Schmued et al., 1997) neurodegenerated cell counts or mossy fiber sprouting, such as the one presented after, for example, PILO-induced SE (Leite et al., 2001) different from Wistar controls after audiogenic kindling in the hippocampus22 with a subtle but significant reduction of cell counts in perirhinal cortex and amygdala10 after audiogenic kindling. However, in order to have a precise evaluation of the impact of these alterations in WARs limbic (hippocampal-amygdala, perirhinal) networks, an estimation of the number of GABAergic interneurons and their subtypes compared to Wistar parental strain is eagerly needed. We found a fast-rising and a slow-rising mIPSCs, as previously described in animals exposed to hippocampal kindling25. However, in contrast to those findings, we observed a change in mIPSC kinetics, with a decrease in the mean rise times of mIPSCs. We found that “slow” rise time population of WARs is faster, which could be explained by changes in GABAA receptor subunit composition. Furthermore, this effect could also be a result of the presence of more proximal synapses (to be proven by microscopy studies) in WARs, resulting in decreased dendritic filtering, even in the absence of correlation between rise time amplitude or decay time constants of recorded mIPSCs28–30. In fact, Labrakakis et al.31 showed that events with slower rise times originating from dendritic synapses are not always smaller in amplitude, as if they were a result of electrotonic filtering. We thus believe that this change in mIPSC kinetics may result from a combination of subunit composition of the GABAA receptors and synapse location. Llano et al.28 found also in Purkinje cells two populations of mIPSCs regarding their rise times. A fast population that originate from synapses located on the soma or proximal dendrites and slow rise times population that originate at distal dendrites. Therefore, the finding that WARs mIPSCs rise times are faster would indicate that most events come from somatic origin and consequently, neurons from these animals have less synapses coming from dendrites, which might impair the ability for dendritic inhibitory inputs to efficiently hamper the excitatory signals, contributing to the susceptibility to seizures. We are currently investigating if this is the case using electronic microscopy.
We finally report that WAR hippocampal pyramidal CA1 neurons are more hyperpolarized and fire less action potentials than Wistar hippocampal cells. Since the action potential threshold was not different in WARs, we consider that the decrease in firing is a consequence of lower resting membrane potential. In our interpretation, this alteration in resting membrane potential, could be a result of intrinsic homeostatic plasticity that counterbalances the deficient inhibition, protecting the cell from hyperexcitation. Indeed, it has been demonstrated that after chronic increases in network excitability, cellular changes will often reduce intrinsic excitability32.
We conclude that due to specific changes in fast GABAergic synapses, WAR, a genetically developed strain susceptible to epilepsy, can be a good model to understand how deficient GABAergic neurotransmission behaves when challenged, with potential impact in the control of excitability, such as the one associated to epilepsy. Although the animals studied in the present study are seizure naïve, we can speculate that if further audiogenic kindling is imposed to them, the presence of hyperexcitability would be facilitated and this is the aim of ongoing and future investigations. We therefore propose that the interaction of WAR genetic background with the chronic seizure experience, in this case, driven by high intensity sound stimulation, could mimic processes that underlie some types of human epileptic disorders.
Methods
All experiments were performed according to rules for animal research from the National Council for Animal Experimentation Control in Brazil (CONCEA). All experiments in this study were approved by the Ethics Committee for Animal Use (CEUA) at the University of São Paulo in Ribeirão Preto, protocol # 015/2013.
Animals
Wistar Audiogenic Rats (WARs) were inbred and raised at the Central Animal Housing Facility at the University of São Paulo as previously described33. For both strains, we used male rats (60–80 days, 5–8, per group), kept in Plexiglas cages (2–3 animals per cage), food and water ad libitum and 12-h dark/light cycle (lights on at 7:00 a. m.) and controlled temperature (22 °C). We used naïve animals to avoid seizure-related effects.
Hippocampal Slices
Animals were anesthetized with isoflurane and quickly decapitated. Brains were carefully removed and placed in an ice-cold solution containing (mM): 87 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 25 NaHCO3, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 75 Sucrose, 25 Glucose, 0.2 CaCl2, 7 MgCl2, bubbled with 95% O2 and 5% CO2. Brains were glued with cyanoacrylate glue to a support, placed inside the cutting chamber of a vibratome (1000 plus, Vibratome, USA) and cut in 200 µm transversal slices containing the dorsal hippocampus. The slices were placed in aCSF solution containing (mM): 125 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 10 Glucose, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2 for one hour at 34–35 °C and thereafter for at least one hour at room temperature before recordings. Only sections 3.6 to 4.5-mm anterior-posterior with respect to bregma were used34.
Whole Cell Patch Clamp Recordings
CA1 pyramidal neurons were visualized with an Olympus BX51WI Microscope (Olympus, Japan) with infrared differential interference contrast (IR-DIC). Neurons were chosen based on the morphology (pyramidal shape) and position in the pyramidal layer. We chose neurons located in the middle of the layer to avoid electrophysiological differences between superficial and deep pyramidal cells35.
Patch clamp recordings were performed using a Heka EPC10 (HEKA Elektronik, Germany) amplifier with 50 kHz sampling rate and low pass filtered at 3 kHz (Bessel). The slices were placed in the recording chamber filled with aCSF and controlled temperature at 34 °C (Scientifica, UK). To record intrinsic properties of neurons, we used an internal solution consisting of (mM): 138 K-gluconate, 8 KCl, 10 Hepes, 0.5 EGTA, 10 phosphocreatine, 4 Mg-ATP, 0.3 Na-GTP adjusted to pH 7.3 with KOH and ≈290 mOsm/kgH2O. To record glutamatergic excitatory post-synaptic currents (EPSCs) we stimulated Schaffer collaterals with a concentric bipolar microelectrode (FHC – Bowdoin, ME, USA), always placed in a visually-controlled distance from the recorded cell, connected to a SD9 Grass voltage stimulator (Natus Medical Incorporated, Warwick, RI, USA). To record AMPA and NMDA-EPSCs, we used an internal solution consisting mM): 130 CsCl, 10 Hepes, 5 EGTA, 5 phosphocreatine, 4 Mg-ATP, 0.5 Na-GTP, 10 TEA, 5 QX 314 adjusted to pH 7.3 with CsOH and ≈290 mOsm/kgH2O. We used the minimum voltage necessary to evoke the maximum current and recorded the EPSCs at holding potentials from −70 mV to +80 mV, increments of +30 mV. We applied DNQX to block AMPA/KA currents and isolate NMDA currents. AMPA/KA IVs were obtained by subtracting the currents before and after DNQX and the NMDA-AMPA ratio was obtained in the same cell by dividing the current evoked at +80 by the current evoked at −70 mV. We confirmed that currents were mediated by NMDA receptors using the NMDA antagonist, D-AP5 (10 µM). In some experiments, we recorded AMPA-mediated glutamatergic evoked currents with KGlu based internal solution. EPSCs recorded with KGlu and CsCl were not significantly different, so we decided to group them together. Glutamatergic currents were recorded in the presence of picrotoxin (20 µM). All evoked and spontaneous synaptic currents were blocked by a combination of PTX (20 µM), DNQX (10 µM) and D-AP5 (10 µM).
Spontaneous GABAergic currents were recorded in the presence of 6,7-dinitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (DNQX) with an internal solution consisting of (mM): 145 KCl, 10 Hepes, 0.5 EGTA, 10 phosphocreatine, 4 Mg-ATP, 0.3 Na-GTP, adjusted to pH 7.3 with KOH and ≈290 mOsm/kgH2O. In order to test if we could improve the detection of dendritic synaptic currents we a recorded some cells (n = 6) with CsCl-based internal solution, which could improve space-clamp control in the dendrites by reducing leak potassium conductances and compared the IPScs with the ones recorded in KCl. We found that IPSCs recorded in CsCl had similar parameters than IPSCs recorded in KCl, except by a small increase in the decay time (Data not shown). We conclude that the CsCl internal solution does not improve the detection of distal IPSCs, as suggested by a previous work36.
Electrodes were fabricated from borosilicate glass (BF150-86-10, Sutter Instruments, Novato CA) with tip resistances of 3–5 MΩ. Series resistance (<20 MΩ) was compensated in 60%. Any neuron with series resistance increased over 20% during experiments, as well as resting membrane potential higher than −60 mV, was discarded. Voltages were corrected off-line for a liquid junction potential for each internal solution calculated with Clampex software (Molecular Devices).
Drugs
The following drugs were used: picrotoxin (Sigma, 20 and 100 µM), 6,7-dinitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (DNQX - Sigma, 10 µM), D-(-)-2-Amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid (D-AP5 - Tocris, 10 µM) and tetrodotoxin (TTX - Cayman Chemical, 0.5 µM). DNQX was dissolved in DMSO and then added to bath from fresh stock solutions. The final concentration of DMSO in the experiments was 0.1% and since we did not find differences between DMSO and aCSF, we used only aCSF as control vehicle. All salts were of reagent grade.
Data Analysis
Data was analyzed using Mini Analysis (Synaptosoft 6.0.3, Fort Lee, NJ, USA) and custom written routines in IgorPro (Wavemetrics, Portland, OR, USA) and Matlab (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). Voltage-current relations (VI) were built measuring the steady-state voltage change in response to hyperpolarizations and we measured membrane input resistance, membrane time constants and the depolarization sag. Step depolarizations were used to produce the firing frequency-intensity curves (FI). We used the first action potential fired to obtain from phase-plane plots (dV/V): threshold, rate of rise (ROR), half-width and fast afterhyperpolarization (AHP).
The peaks of the EPSCs were used to build IV relationships, to calculate the reversal potential and to estimate the chord conductances. We measured rise times from baseline to peak and decay times from peak to baseline.
GABAergic currents were recorded at −70 mV. Spontaneous inhibitory postsynaptic currents (sIPSCs) were recorded for 10 minutes. We next applied TTX to block action potentials and record miniature IPSCs (mIPSCs). IPSCs and mIPSCs were selected manually, and only currents with good signal to noise ration were chosen. For each recording file, we tested the RMS noise and compared among groups. Since the electrical noise was similar among recordings, we chose not to use any type of offline filter to avoid distortions in the kinetics of events. Histograms for interevent intervals, amplitudes and rise times were built with same fixed bins for different groups of cells and cumulative frequency distributions were tested for significance with Kolmogorov-Smirnov (KS) test. Analysis of decay kinetics for inhibitory currents was performed by Mini Analysis group analysis with individual currents fitted with double exponential functions. Fast and slow time constants were presented as average and compared between groups. Further analysis of the mIPSC amplitude and rise time distributions was made using kernel density estimation (KDE). KDE was estimated using Seaborn library for the Python programming language (Python Software Foundation, https://www.python.org/).
Data from other experiments are reported as mean ± SEM and were tested with unpaired t-test, considering p < 0.05. Also, we used two-way ANOVA to test for significance in FI curves and train stimulation of Schaffer collaterals.
Ethical Publication Statement
We confirm that we have read the Journals position on issues involved in ethical publication and affirm that this report is consistent with those guidelines.
Acknowledgements
Authors thank José Fernando Aguiar and André Luiz Andreotti Dagostin for excellent technical assistance. Funding from FAPESP: grant # 2015/22327-7 (A.O.S.C.); grant # 2007/50261-4 (N.G.C.); grant # 2016/01607-4 (R.M.L.); grant # 2015/50122-0 and DFG-IRTG 1740/2 (A.C.R. and R.F.O.P.). R.F.O.P. and A.C.R. are also part of the Research, Innovation and Dissemination Center for Neuromathematics (FAPESP grant # 2013/07699-0). C.C.C., J.L.D. and R.F.O.P. are Capes, CNPq and Fapesp PhD student fellows, respectively. R.M.L., N.G.C. and A.C.R. hold CNPq Research Fellowships.
Author Contributions
A.O.S.C., J.L.D., C.C.C. performed experiments. A.O.S.C., J.L.D., C.C.C. and R.F.O.P. analyzed the data. A.O.S.C., J.L.D., C.C.C., N.G.C. and R.M.L. conceived and designed the study. J.A.C.O. and N.G.C. made the genetic selection of WAR strain. A.O.S.C., J.L.D., C.C.C., R.F.O.P., A.C.R., N.G.C. and R.M.L. planned the experiments, wrote, reviewed and contributed to the final manuscript.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Fisher RS, et al. ILAE official report: a practical clinical definition of epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2014;55:475–482. doi: 10.1111/epi.12550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Löscher W. Critical review of current animal models of seizures and epilepsy used in the discovery and development of new antiepileptic drugs. Seizure. 2011;20:359–368. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2011.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ross KC, Coleman JR. Developmental and genetic audiogenic seizure models: behavior and biological substrates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2000;24:639–653. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00029-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Garcia-Cairasco N, Umeoka EHL, Cortes de Oliveira JA. The Wistar Audiogenic Rat (WAR) strain and its contributions to epileptology and related comorbidities: History and perspectives. Epilepsy Behav. 2017;71(Part B):250–273. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2017.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Vinogradova LV. Audiogenic kindling and secondary subcortico-cortical epileptogenesis: Behavioral correlates and electrographic features. Epilepsy Behav. 2017;71:142–153. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2015.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Marescaux C, et al. Kindling of audiogenic seizures in Wistar rats: An EEG study. Exp. Neurol. 1987;97:160–168. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(87)90290-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Naritoku DK, Mecozzi LB, Aiello MT, Faingold CL. Repetition of audiogenic seizures in genetically epilepsy-prone rats induces cortical epileptiform activity and additional seizure behaviors. Exp. Neurol. 1992;115:317–324. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(92)90197-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Dutra Moraes MF, Galvis-Alonso OY, Garcia-Cairasco N. Audiogenic kindling in the Wistar rat: a potential model for recruitment of limbic structures. Epilepsy Res. 2000;39:251–259. doi: 10.1016/S0920-1211(00)00107-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Aronov D, Nevers R, Tank DW. Mapping of a non-spatial dimension by the hippocampal-entorhinal circuit. Nature. 2017;543:719–722. doi: 10.1038/nature21692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cunha AOS, de Oliveira JAC, Almeida SS, Garcia-Cairasco N, Leão RM. Inhibition of long-term potentiation in the schaffer-CA1 pathway by repetitive high-intensity sound stimulation. Neuroscience. 2015;310:114–127. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.09.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.de Deus JL, et al. A single episode of high intensity sound inhibits long-term potentiation in the hippocampus of rats. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:14094. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-14624-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Mesquita F, et al. Electrophysiological properties of cultured hippocampal neurons from Wistar Audiogenic Rats. Brain Res. Bull. 2005;65:177–183. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Drumond LE, et al. Reduced hippocampal GABAergic function in Wistar audiogenic rats. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. Rev. Bras. Pesqui. Médicas E Biológicas Soc. Bras. Biofísica Al. 2011;44:1054–1059. doi: 10.1590/s0100-879x2011007500118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lee Y, et al. Divergent brain changes in two audiogenic rat strains: A voxel-based morphometry and diffusion tensor imaging comparison of the genetically epilepsy prone rat (GEPR-3) and the Wistar Audiogenic Rat (WAR) Neurobiol. Dis. 2017;111:80–90. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2017.12.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Garcia-Cairasco N, et al. Neuroethological and morphological (Neo-Timm staining) correlates of limbic recruitment during the development of audiogenic kindling in seizure susceptible Wistar rats. Epilepsy Res. 1996;26:177–192. doi: 10.1016/S0920-1211(96)00050-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Garcia-Cairasco N. A critical review on the participation of inferior colliculus in acoustic-motor and acoustic-limbic networks involved in the expression of acute and kindled audiogenic seizures. Hear. Res. 2002;168:208–222. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(02)00371-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Galvis-Alonso OY, Cortes De Oliveira JA, Garcia-Cairasco N. Limbic epileptogenicity, cell loss and axonal reorganization induced by audiogenic and amygdala kindling in wistar audiogenic rats (WAR strain) Neuroscience. 2004;125:787–802. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.01.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Magalhães LHM, Garcia-Cairasco N, Massensini AR, Doretto MC, Moraes MFD. Evidence for augmented brainstem activated forebrain seizures in Wistar Audiogenic Rats subjected to transauricular electroshock. Neurosci. Lett. 2004;369:19–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Cascino GD. Temporal lobe epilepsy: more than hippocampal pathology. Epilepsy Curr. 2005;5:187–189. doi: 10.1111/j.1535-7511.2005.00059.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hu H, Gan J, Jonas P. Interneurons. Fast-spiking, parvalbumin+ GABAergic interneurons: from cellular design to microcircuit function. Science. 2014;345:1255263. doi: 10.1126/science.1255263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cohen I, Miles R. Contributions of intrinsic and synaptic activities to the generation of neuronal discharges in in vitro hippocampus. J. Physiol. 2000;524(Pt 2):485–502. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.00485.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Romcy-Pereira RN, Garcia-Cairasco N. Hippocampal cell proliferation and epileptogenesis after audiogenic kindling are not accompanied by mossy fiber sprouting or Fluoro-Jade staining. Neuroscience. 2003;119:533–546. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00191-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Edwards FA, Konnerth A, Sakmann B. Quantal analysis of inhibitory synaptic transmission in the dentate gyrus of rat hippocampal slices: a patch-clamp study. J. Physiol. 1990;430:213–249. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp018289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Knopp A, Frahm C, Fidzinski P, Witte OW, Behr J. Loss of GABAergic neurons in the subiculum and its functional implications in temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain J. Neurol. 2008;131:1516–1527. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wierenga CJ, Wadman WJ. Miniature inhibitory postsynaptic currents in CA1 pyramidal neurons after kindling epileptogenesis. J. Neurophysiol. 1999;82:1352–1362. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.3.1352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Dinocourt C, Petanjek Z, Freund TF, Ben-Ari Y, Esclapez M. Loss of interneurons innervating pyramidal cell dendrites and axon initial segments in the CA1 region of the hippocampus following pilocarpine-induced seizures. J. Comp. Neurol. 2003;459:407–425. doi: 10.1002/cne.10622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Sutula T, He XX, Cavazos J, Scott G. Synaptic reorganization in the hippocampus induced by abnormal functional activity. Science. 1988;239:1147–1150. doi: 10.1126/science.2449733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Llano I, et al. Presynaptic calcium stores underlie large-amplitude miniature IPSCs and spontaneous calcium transients. Nat. Neurosci. 2000;3:1256–1265. doi: 10.1038/81781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Cossart R, et al. Distribution of spontaneous currents along the somato-dendritic axis of rat hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Neuroscience. 2000;99:593–603. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(00)00231-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Cossart R, et al. Dendritic but not somatic GABAergic inhibition is decreased in experimental epilepsy. Nat. Neurosci. 2001;4:52–62. doi: 10.1038/82900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Labrakakis C, Rudolph U, De Koninck Y. The heterogeneity in GABAA receptor-mediated IPSC kinetics reflects heterogeneity of subunit composition among inhibitory and excitatory interneurons in spinal lamina II. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2014;8:424. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2014.00424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Beck H, Yaari Y. Plasticity of intrinsic neuronal properties in CNS disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:357–369. doi: 10.1038/nrn2371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Doretto MC, et al. Quantitative study of the response to genetic selection of the Wistar audiogenic rat strain (WAR) Behav. Genet. 2003;33:33–42. doi: 10.1023/A:1021099432759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Paxinos, G. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. (Academic Press, 1998).
- 35.Maroso M, et al. Cannabinoid Control of Learning and Memory through HCN Channels. Neuron. 2016;89:1059–1073. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Williams SR, Mitchell SJ. Direct measurement of somatic voltage clamp errors in central neurons. Nature neuroscience. 2008;11(7):790. doi: 10.1038/nn.2137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]