Abstract
Microglia, the innate immune cells of the central nervous system, regulate brain development by promoting cell genesis, pruning synapses, and removing dying, newly-born or progenitor cells. However, the role of microglia in the early life programming of behavior under normal conditions is not well characterized. We used central infusion of liposomal clodronate to selectively deplete microglia from the neonatal rat brain and subsequently assessed the impact of microglial depletion on programming of juvenile and adult motivated behaviors. Liposomal clodronate treatment on postnatal days one and four led to greater than 70% loss of forebrain microglia by postnatal day 6 that lasted for approximately ten days. Neonatal microglia depletion led to reduced juvenile and adult anxiety behavior on the elevated plus maze and open field test, and increased locomotor activity. On a test of juvenile social play, microglial depletion led to decreased chase behaviors relative to control animals. There was no change in active social behavior in adults on a reciprocal social interaction test, but there was decreased passive interaction time and an increased number of social avoidance behaviors in clodronate treated rats relative to controls. There was an overall decrease in behavioral despair on the forced swim test in adult rats treated neonatally with clodronate. Females, but not males, treated neonatally with clodronate showed a blunted corticosterone response after acute stress in adulthood. These results show that microglia are important for the early life programming of juvenile and adult motivated behavior.
1. Introduction
Microglia are the innate immune cells of the central nervous system. Microglia colonize the rodent brain beginning on embryonic day 9.5 and reach peak numbers by the third postnatal week (Ginhoux et al., 2010; Kim et al., 2015; Nikodemova et al., 2015). They release a variety of diffusible factors, such as chemokines and cytokines, and express a variety of receptors that allow them to respond to and modulate events under normal and abnormal circumstances in the brain. In the developing brain, microglia have been shown to prune synapses and regulate neurogenesis, apoptosis, and axonal innervation (Cunningham et al., 2013; Paolicelli et al., 2011; Schafer et al., 2012; Shigemoto-Mogami et al., 2014; Squarzoni et al., 2014). While many of these studies have investigated microglia-regulated processes in a cellular developmental context, few studies have examined whether microglia influence behavioral development in the absence of inflammation, stress or other pathology.
Previous studies have investigated the behavioral effects of depleting microglia from the adult rodent brain, and these studies have found very limited and transient effects of microglial depletion on social and anxiety behaviors (Elmore et al., 2014; Torres et al., 2016). In development, models that are thought to activate microglia or alter their function, such as perinatal stress or inflammatory challenge, show changes in social, anxiety and despair-like behavior, as well as stress reactivity (Choi et al., 2016; Hellwig et al., 2016; Lin et al., 2012; Majidi-Zolbanin et al., 2013; Meyer, 2006; Vallée et al., 1997; Wei et al., 2010; Zhan et al., 2014). These results suggest that microglia may be more important for developmental programming of behavior than the maintenance of behavior in adulthood. However, it is still unclear whether basal microglial function during development directly impacts the development of these behaviors and their expression later in life.
One strategy to determine the role of microglia on the development of later life behavior is to selectively deplete microglia through the use of liposomal clodronate. Clodronate is a cytotoxic drug that, when encapsulated in lipid droplets, is selectively taken up by phagocytic cells, where it subsequently induces apoptosis (van Rooijen et al., 1996). Liposomal clodronate specifically depletes macrophages while sparing other cell types, such as neurons, oligodendrocytes and astrocytes, when centrally injected into the central nervous system (Faustino et al., 2011; Lee et al., 2012; van Rooijen and Hendrikx, 2010). In the current studies we used central infusion of liposomal clodronate to deplete microglia from the developing brain and test whether microglia regulate the development of motivated behaviors in male and female rats. We found that microglia depletion in the early postnatal period led to decreased anxiety behavior and depressive-like behavior and increased locomotor activity in male and female rats, and reductions in stress-induced corticosterone release in females. The current studies underscore that microglia are necessary for the normal development of several motivated behaviors.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1 Animals
All procedures were conducted in accordance with The Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals published by the National Institutes of Health and approved by The Ohio State University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. One cohort of age matched juvenile and virgin adult male and female Sprague Dawley rats from four separate litters were used for all behavioral testing. Adult Sprague Dawley rats (Harlan) were mated in our facilities, or timed pregnant animals were ordered to deliver within a week of arrival at the animal facility (Harlan). Two experimental litters yielded from in house breeding and two litters yielded from ordered timed-pregnant females. Animals were housed in sex-matched pairs in a temperature and humidity controlled room with ad libitum access to food and water, and the room was maintained on a 12h/12h light/dark cycle (lights on at 20 hrs). Pregnant females were allowed to deliver naturally and the day of birth was designated as postnatal day (P) 0.
2.2 In vivo manipulations
Bilateral intracerebroventricular (icv) injections were performed on neonates on P1 and P4 under brief cryoanesthesia. A 23 gauge Hamilton syringe attached to a stereotaxic manipulator was placed 1 mm caudal to Bregma and 1 mm lateral to the midline, lowered 3 mm into the brain, and then backed out 1 mm. A total of 1μl of liposomal clodronate (Encapsula NanoSciences, Cat. 8092) or control liposomes (vehicle) was infused over 60s, and the procedure was repeated on the other hemisphere. For all procedures, the separation of pups from the dam was kept to a for the duration of time spent away from the dam. Other than icv treatment of pups on P1 and P4, experimental animals were otherwise left undisturbed with the maternal dam until sacrifice or weaning. The experimental timeline is shown in Figure 1.
2.3 Immunohistochemistry
A time-course study of microglial numbers following injection of liposomal clodronate was performed to 1) verify that liposomal clodronate effectively depleted microglia and 2) to determine the time course of microglial depletion and repopulation. After liposomal clodronate treatment, rats were sacrificed on either P2 (after receiving only one injection on P1), P6, P12, or P22 (after receiving injections on P1 and P4). Rats were deeply anesthetized with FatalPlus (Vortech Pharmaceuticals), transcardially perfused with 0.1M phosphate buffered saline (PBS) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS. Brains were then removed, postfixed overnight in the same fixative, and cryoprotected in 30% sucrose in 0.1M PBS until they sank. Brains were coronally sectioned on a cryostat at a thickness of 45 μm and mounted on charged slides into two alternate series. Brain sections underwent immunofluorescence staining for the microglia/macrophage specific marker, ionized calcium-binding adaptor molecule 1 (Iba1; Wako Chemicals). Slide mounted sections were extensively rinsed with 0.1M PBS then incubated for 20 minutes in 50% methanol. Sections were rinsed in 0.1M PBS then treated in 10 mM sodium citrate solution (pH 9.5) heated to 70° for 20 minutes, rinsed in 0.1M PBS then blocked for 1 hr in 0.1M PBS + 0.4% Triton-X + 5% normal donkey serum (NDS). Sections were then incubated at 4°C overnight in antiserum to Iba1 (1:1000) in 0.1M PBS + 0.4% Triton-X + 2.5% NDS. On day 2, sections were rinsed in 0.1M PBS and incubated in the dark for 2 hr at room temperature with anti-rabbit AlexaFluor 488 (ThermoFisher Scientific, 1:333) in 0.1M PBS + 0.4% Triton-X + 2.5% NDS, rinsed in the dark in 0.1M PBS, counterstained with DAPI, and coverslipped with VectaShield HardSet mounting medium (Vector Laboratories). Sections were then imaged using a Zeiss AxioImager.M2 microscope, Zeiss AxioCam MRm camera, and StereoInvestigator software (MBF Biosciences).
2.4 Densitometry and microglia cell counts
Six animals from each treatment group (three male, three female) were imaged for each time point, except one clodronate treated male was removed from the P22 time point due to low body weight. To quantify gross changes in microglia within the brain following liposomal clodronate treatment, digital image analysis (DIA) of Iba-1 staining was performed (Donnelly et al. 2009). The amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) were chosen for analysis because they are involved in regulating the motivated behaviors assayed in these studies and are relatively distant from one another on both dorsal-ventral and anterior-posterior axes. For densitometric analysis four images were taken from four separate sections mPFC and amygdala in one hemisphere at 10× magnification. The hemisphere used for analysis was counterbalanced within and across conditions. A threshold for positive staining was determined for each image and was processed by densitometric scanning of the threshold targets using ImageJ software (NIH) following a previously reported protocol (Donnelly et al., 2009). In brief, a threshold for positive labeling was determined for each image that included all cell bodies and processes, but excluded background staining. The proportional area was reported as the average percentage area in the positive threshold for all representative images (Wohleb et al., 2013). In addition to microglial densitometry, the number of microglia per region of interest was also determined in the mPFC and amygdala. To quantify the number of microglia, image stacks were taken at 20x for all time points and the total number of microglia was counted within a region of interest defined as 448μm × 336 μm (area = 0.15 μm2) for four sections with a physical distance of 45 μm between each section. The area of each region of interest was the same for all animals and time points, thus data is expressed as the number of microglia per region of interest.
2.5 Behavior
All behavioral testing was performed by one experimenter. Behavioral testing was videotaped for later analysis, and all testing and scoring of behavior was done blind to treatment and sex. Juvenile rats were weaned at P24 and housed in same-sex groups of 2–3. Behavioral testing was performed on the same cohort of animals in the juvenile and adult periods in order to assess any changes in behavior over development. All behavioral testing for adult females was performed on diestrus except for social interaction, which has been reported to be constant across estrous phase under low light levels (Stack et al., 2010). Diestrus is the phase of the estrous cycle when females have low levels of progestins and estrogens, and experience higher levels of anxiety in the open field and elevated plus maze compared to proestrus and estrous (Hiroi and Neumaier, 2006; Marcondes et al., 2001; Walf and Frye, 2007). Adult females were vaginally swabbed to determine the day of estrous, beginning seven days prior to initial testing to habituate animals to the procedure. Following initial habituation, females were swabbed 2–3 days prior to testing when there was a break following previous behavioral tests. On the day of behavioral testing, females were swabbed approximately 1 hr before testing began. For behavioral testing, group sizes were as follows: Vehicle treated males n = 10, clodronate treated males n = 9, clodronate treated females n = 11, and vehicle treated females n = 11. Due to technical difficulties with videotaping, two behavioral tests had modified animal numbers. For the juvenile elevated plus maze task: Vehicle treated males n = 10, clodronate treated males n = 8, vehicle treated females n = 11, and clodronate treated females n = 10, and for the adult open field test: Vehicle treated males n = 9, clodronate treated males n = 9, clodronate treated females n = 11 and vehicle treated females n = 11.
2.6 Juvenile social play
Juvenile social play testing was performed on P28–29. All juvenile social play was performed without prior social isolation. Juvenile paired play and group play were both assessed as there can be sex differences in number of play behaviors depending on the paradigm used (Argue and McCarthy, 2015; Auger and Olesen, 2009). Paired play consisted randomly assigned age-matched, same-sex pairs. Group play consisted of mixed treatment groups of 4–5 animals, consisting of 2–3 pair-housed males and 2–3 pair-housed females. All social play tests were conducted for 13 minutes and the last 10 minutes was used for analysis. Animals were marked with a sharpie to differentiate between them during testing. For both paired play and group play, we assessed the frequency of chasing, pouncing, wrestling, boxing, pinning, and social exploration (e.g., sniffing conspecifics). Behaviors were divided into rough and tumble play (e.g., wrestling, pinning, boxing, and pouncing), chase behaviors, and number of social explorations for statistical analysis.
2.7 Juvenile open field test
Juvenile open field was performed on P30–32. The open field arena was a Plexiglas cage (60 cm x 60 cm × 40 cm) divided by gridlines into 36 10 × 10 cm squares. The inner square consisted of 16 10 × 10 cm squares. Open field behavior was run under dim red light and analyzed for 5 minutes. Data collected included the number of lines crossed, center entries, time spent in the center, and number of rears.
2.8 Juvenile elevated plus maze
The elevated-plus maze (EPM) consisted of a cross-shaped platform (height: 50 cm) with four arms (width: 10 cm, length: 50 cm), two of which were enclosed by walls 50 cm in height. The EPM test was performed under bright white light during the dark phase of the light cycle. Rats were placed in the center of the platform (10 × 10 cm), facing a junction between an open and closed arm and allowed to explore for 10 min. The number of open arm entries and the time spent in the open arms were quantified. There were four clodronate males and three clodronate females that fell or jumped off the EPM, thus data for the juvenile EPM is expressed as the percent of total time on open arms.
2.9 Adult social interaction
The social interaction test was performed from P80–90 in the same arena as used for open field behavior detailed above. Stimulus animals were novel sex- and age-matched rats that had not previously been used in prior behavioral testing. Stimulus animals were used for no more than five pairings and were not used for consecutive tests. Stimulus animals were habituated to the open field for 30 minutes 24 hr before testing. The social interaction test was run under dim red light conditions, because pre-habituation and dim red light conditions elicit the lowest amount of anxiety and the highest amount of social interaction, thus it is best for detecting changes in sociability rather than anxiety (File and Seth, 2003). Females were run regardless of estrous cycle as female social behavior has been reported to be constant across estrous phase under low light levels (Stack et al., 2010). The test consisted of placing a stimulus animal in one corner, and placing an experimental animal in the opposite corner. Open-ended interaction was observed for 10 min. Data collected included total active interaction time, number of active bouts, passive interaction time, and number of social avoidance behaviors. Active interactions include sniffing, climbing on, or playing with the stimulus rat. Active bouts had to be separated by a least 5 seconds to be considered a separate bout. Passive interaction consists of a rat being within 5 cm of the stimulus rat, but not actively interacting with or investigating the stimulus rat. Social avoidance behaviors were counted when the stimulus rat attempted to actively interact with the experimental rat, but the experimental rat moved away from the stimulus rat thereby avoiding interaction. Animals were marked with a sharpie to differentiate between them during testing.
2.10 Adult open field test
The open field arena used had the same dimensions and layout as the open field used for juvenile open field (section 2.4). The open field was performed from P70–80. Open field behavior was run under dim red light and analyzed for 5 minutes. Analysis consisted of number of lines crossed, center entries, time spent in the center, and number of rears.
2.11 Adult elevated plus maze
The EPM apparatus was the same to that used for juvenile EPM behavior (section 2.5). The elevated plus maze was performed from P60–70. EPM was run under bright white lights. Rats were placed in the center of the platform, facing a junction between an open and closed arm and allowed to explore for 10 min. The number of open arm entries, open arm time, closed arm entries and closed arm time was analyzed.
2.12 Adult forced swim test
The forced swim test (FST) was used to assess behavioral despair on P90–100. Females were assessed during diestrus. Plexiglas cylinders (diameter: 30.5 cm, height: 49 cm) were filled to a depth of 30 cm with 25 ± 0.5°C water. FST was run under bright white lights. Rats were placed individually into the FST cylinders for 10 min, towel dried, and returned to their home cage. 24 h later, rats were returned to the same apparatus for 5 min and the session digitally recorded. The percentage of time spent immobile [(time spent floating in the water only making movements necessary to maintain the head above water/total test time) × 100] was later measured blind to condition by a trained observer.
2.13 Adult restraint stress and serum corticosterone assay
To determine acute stress reactivity, adult rats were restrained in ventilated clear Plexiglas cylinders (21 cm long, 6 cm internal diameter; Plas-Labs 554-BSRR) for 30 min under bright light illumination immediately prior to sacrifice on P120. Females were stressed on diestrus. Restriant cyclinders were thoroughly cleaned after each use with soap and water followed by 70% ethanol and dried. Immediately after restraint, rats were deeply anesthetized with FatalPlus (Vortech Pharmaceuticals) and blood was obtained via cardiac puncture. Blood samples were placed on ice and serum was collected after centrifuging samples at 4,000 × rpm for 15 min. Serum was stored at −80°C until assay. A corticosterone ELISA (Arbor Assays Cat. K014-H1) was used to determine serum concentrations of corticosterone and run according to the manufacturer’s instructions. All standards and samples were assayed in duplicate, and corticosterone levels were assayed on two separate plates due to the large cohort of animals. Corticosterone levels were normalized using a control animal sample run on both plates to account for any inter-plate variability.
2.14 Statistics
All data were analyzed using two-way ANOVA, with treatment (vehicle or clodronate) and sex (male or female) as the two main factors, yielding a 2 × 2 design. When a significant main effect was obtained in ANOVA, planned post-hoc comparisons were made using Bonferroni post-hoc tests with p values corrected for multiple comparisons. Male and females that were analyzed separately for all experiments except were grouped for analysis of microglia colonization of the brain due to lack of sex differences seen in the clodronate analysis and low power to detect sex differences. When a significant interaction effect was obtained, Tukey post hoc analyses were performed. For all analyses, differences were considered significant when the corrected p < 0.05. All data are expressed as mean with standard error of the mean (SEM). Statistical analyses were conducted in Prism 6.0 for Mac/PC.
3. Results
3.1 Iba1 immunofluorescence analysis
Figure 2 shows representative pictures of microglia from each time point in the mPFC (left) and amygdala (right), and figure 3 summarizes microglial counts and densitometry across development following neonatal treatment with liposomal clodronate or vehicle.
In the mPFC, microglia counts per region of interest (ROI) indicated that microglia numbers were significantly reduced by 87.3% in clodronate treated rats compared to control treated rats at P2 (Fig. 3a; treatment: F1,8 = 85.4, p < 0.001; sex: F1,8 = 0.318, ns; interaction: F1,8 = 0.5443, ns) and multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated males and females had significantly fewer microglia than vehicle treated males and females. The number of microglia was significantly reduced by 72.4% in clodronate treated rats compared to control treated rats at P6 (Fig. 3a; treatment: F1,8 =12.07, p = 0.0084; sex: F1,8 = 1.05, ns; interaction: F1,8 = 0.00222, ns). The number of microglia were significantly increased by 61.3% in the mPFC of clodronate treated rats compared to vehicle treated rats at P12 (Fig. 3a; treatment: F1,8 = 29.3, p = 0.0006; sex: F1,8 = 0.0609, ns; interaction: F1,8 = 3.66, ns) and multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated females had significantly more microglia than vehicle treated females. Microglia density in clodronate treated rats returned to control levels at P22 (Fig. 3a; treatment: F1,7 = 0.421, ns; sex: F1,7 = 0.0362, ns; interaction: F1,7 = 6.84, p = 0.0346).
Densitometric analysis indicated that the percent area of Iba1 staining was significantly reduced in clodronate treated rats compared to control treated rats at P2 in the mPFC (Fig. 3c; treatment: F1,8 = 94.2, p < 0.0001; sex: F1,8 = 0.096, ns; interaction: F1,8 = 0.399, ns) and multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated males and females had less Iba1 staining compared to vehicle males and females respectively. Iba1 staining was significantly reduced in clodronate treated rats compared to control treated rats at P6 (Fig. 3c; treatment: F1,8 =187, p < 0.0001; sex: F1,8 = 5.02, ns; interaction: F1,8 = 2.39, ns) and multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated males and females had significantly less area stained by Iba1 compared to vehicle treated males and females respectively. There was no difference in the percent area stained by Iba1 at P12 (Fig. 3c; treatment: F1,8 = 4.31, ns; sex: F1,8 = 0.267, ns; interaction: F1,8 = 2.59, ns), and P22 (Fig. 3c; treatment: F1,7 = 0.142, ns; sex: F1,7 = 0.814, ns; interaction: F1,7 = 0.812, p = 0.0346).
In the amygdala (Figures 2 and 3, right panels). microglia counts were significantly reduced by 39.6% compared to control injected animals at P2 (Fig. 3b; treatment: F1,8 = 15.2, p = 0.0046; sex: F1,8 = 0.0224, ns; interaction: F1,8 = 1.04, ns) and multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated males had significantly fewer microglia compared to vehicle treated microglia. The number of microglia was significantly reduced by 83.7% compared to control injected animals at P6 (Fig. 3b; treatment: F1,8 = 71.4, p < 0.0001; sex: F1,8 = 6.99, p = 0.0295; interaction: F1,8 = 0.0128, ns). Multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated males and females had significantly fewer microglia than vehicle treated males and females respectively. There were no differences in number of microglia between treatments at P12 (Fig. 3b; treatment: F1,8 = 0.494, ns; sex: F1,8 = 0.193, ns; interaction: F1,8 = 0.0774, ns), and P22 (Fig. 3b; treatment: F1,7 = 2.29, ns; sex: F1,7 = 4.29, ns; interaction: F1,7 = 0.684, ns).
Densitometric analysis indicated that Iba1 staining area was significantly reduced in clodronate treated rats compared to control treated rats at P2 in the amygdala (Fig. 3d; treatment: F1,8 = 45.7, p = 0.0001; sex: F1,8 = 0.676, ns; interaction: F1,8 = 3.58, ns) and multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated males and females had significantly less Iba1 staining than vehicle treated males and females respectively. Iba1 staining area was significantly reduced in clodronate treated rats compared to control treated rats at P6 (Fig. 3d; treatment: F1,8 = 85.5, p < 0.0001; sex: F1,8 = 2.03, ns; interaction: F1,8 = 0.256, ns). Multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated males and females had significantly reduced Iba1 staining compared to vehicle treated males and females respectively. There were no differences in Iba1 staining area at P12 (Fig. 3d; treatment: F1,8 = 1.82, ns; sex: F1,8 = 0.666, ns; interaction: F1,8 = 0.706, ns), or P22 (Fig. 3d; treatment: F1,7 = 1.11, ns; sex: F1,7 = 0.0995, ns; interaction: F1,7 = 0.0324, ns).
To better understand the normal trajectory microglial colonization in the mPFC versus amygdala, we compared microglial number and Iba1 staining density between these two brain regions in control animals. Differences between males and females were not tested due to low power (n = 3 per sex per time point). There was a significant effect of age, area, and a significant interaction in the number of microglia in the mPFC and amygdala (Age: F3,40 = 47.4, p < 0.0001; brain region: F3,40 = 35.8, p < 0.0001; interaction: F3,40 = 4.77, p = 0.0062). Post-hoc analyses showed that there was an increase in the number of microglia in the mPFC from P6 to P12 and a decrease from P12 to P22. There was a significant increase in number of microglia in the amygdala from P2 to P6 and a decrease from P12 to P22. There were significantly more microglia in the amygdala compared to the mPFC at P2 and P6.
There was a significant effect of age and a significant interaction in the percent area stained by Iba1 in the mPFC and amygdala (Age: F3,40 = 139, p < 0.0001; brain region: F3,40 = 2.37, ns; interaction: F3,40 = 21.9, p < 0.0001). Post-hoc analyses showed that there was a significant increase in the percent area stained by Iba1 from P2 to P6 and from P6 to P12, but not from P12 to P22 in the mPFC. There was a significant increase in the percent area stained by Iba1 in the amygdala from P2 to P6, but from P6 to P12 and P12 to P22. There was significantly more area stained by Iba1 in the amygdala compared to the mPFC at P2 and P6. At P12 there was significantly more area stained by Iba1 in the mPFC compared the amygdala. There was no difference in Iba1 staining between the two brain regions at P22.
3.2 Juvenile paired play
Clodronate treated rats chased partner rats less than vehicle treated rats (Fig. 4a; treatment: F1,37 = 17.9, p = 0.0001; sex: F1,37 = 1.47, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 2.41, ns). Post-hoc tests showed that clodronate treated males chased significantly less than vehicle treated males. There were no significant differences in the number of rough and tumble behaviors (Fig. 4b; treatment: F1,37 = 1.25, ns; sex, F1,37 = 0.093, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 1.24, ns). Clodronate treated animals displayed significantly more social exploration behaviors than vehicle treated animals (Fig. 4c; treatment: F1,37 = 4.72 p = 0.036; sex: F1,37 = 1.83e-005, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 2.13, ns). Post-hoc tests showed that clodronate treated females made significantly more social explorations than vehicle treated females.
3.3 Juvenile group play
Clodronate treated rats chased conspecifics significantly less than vehicle treated rats (Fig. 4d; treatment: F1,37 = 34.5, p = 0.0001; sex: F1,37 = 0.20, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 0.23, ns). Multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated males and clodronate treated females chased conspecifics significantly less than vehicle treated males and vehicle treated females respectively. There were no significant differences in the number of rough-and-tumble behaviors (Fig. 4e; treatment: F1,37 = 0.92, ns; sex: F1,37 = 0.29, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 0.92, ns) or in the number of social explorations (Fig. 4f; treatment: F1,37 = 4.21, ns; sex: F1,37 = 1.90, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 1.72, ns).
3.4 Adult social interaction
Female rats actively investigated the stimulus rat for a significantly longer period of time than male rats regardless of treatment (Fig. 5a; treatment: F1,37 = 0.15, ns; sex: F1,37 = 6.49, p = 0.015; interaction: F1,37 = 0.11, ns). Female rats performed more bouts of active investigation than male rats regardless of treatment (Fig. 5b; treatment: F1,37 = 0.023, ns; sex: F1,37 = 7.49, p = 0.0095; interaction: F1,37 = 0.11, ns). Clodronate treated rats spent significantly less time passively interacting with the stimulus rat than vehicle treated rats (Fig. 5c; treatment: F1,37 = 24.4, p < 0.0001; sex: F1,37 = 2.03, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 8.59, p = 0.0058). Vehicle treated females passively interacted more than vehicle treated males, and clodronate treated females. Clodronate treatment induced a significant increase in the number of social avoidance behaviors in adulthood relative to vehicle treatment (Fig. 5d; treatment: F1,37 = 7.21, p = 0.0108; sex: F1,37 = 1.67, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 0.10, ns). Multiple comparisons showed that vehicle treated females and clodronate treated females passively interacted more than vehicle treated males and clodronate treated males respectively.
3.5 Juvenile open field test
There was a significant effect of treatment on activity in the open field, with clodronate treated rats crossing significantly more gridlines than vehicle treated rats, as well as a significant sex difference in activity, with females crossing significantly more gridlines than males (Fig. 6a; treatment: F1,37 = 81.4, p < 0.0001; sex: F1,37 = 6.63, p = 0.014; interaction: F1,37 = 3.38, ns). Multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated females crossed significantly more lines than vehicle treated females and clodronate treated males, and clodronate treated males crossed significantly more lines than vehicle treated males. Clodronate treated rats entered the center area more frequently than vehicle treated rats (Fig. 6b; treatment: F1,37 = 21.1, p < 0.0001; sex: F1,37 = 2.22, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 4.64, p = 0.038). Multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated females entered the center area significantly more than vehicle treated females. There were no significant differences for time spent in the center of the open field (Fig. 6c; treatment: F1,37 = 2.32, ns; sex: F1,37 = 3.11, ns; interaction F1,37 = 1.25, ns). Clodronate treated rats reared significantly less than vehicle treated rats irrespective of sex (Fig. 6d; treatment: F1,37 = 8.01, p = 0.0075; sex: F1,37 = 0.0042, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 0.26, ns).
3.6 Adult open field test
There was a significant effect of treatment on activity in the open field, with clodronate treated rats crossing significantly more gridlines than vehicle treated rats, as well as a significant sex difference in activity, with females crossing significantly more gridlines than males (Fig. 7a; treatment: F1,36 = 98.0, p < 0.0001; sex: F1,36 = 12.38, p = 0.0012; interaction: F1,36 = 1.16, ns). Clodronate treated male and female rats crossed significantly more gridlines than vehicle treated male and female rats respectively. Clodronate treated female rats also crossed more gridlines than clodronate treated male rats. Clodronate treated rats entered the center significantly more than vehicle treated rats (Fig. 7b; treatment: F1,36 = 10.3, p = 0.0028; sex: F1,36 = 3.29, ns; interaction: F1,36 = 0.033, ns). Females spent significantly more time in the center of open field compared to males (Fig. 7c; treatment: F1,36 = 0.27, ns; sex: F1,36 = 10.4, p = 0.0027; interaction: F1,36 = 1.25, ns). Multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated females spent significantly more time in the center of the open field than clodronate treated males. There was a significant effect of treatment and significant interaction for number of rears in the open field (Fig. 7d; treatment: F1,36 = 27.3, p < 0.0001; sex: F1,36 = 0.95, ns; interaction: F1,36 = 12.8, p = 0.001). Multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated males reared significantly more than vehicle treated males and vehicle treated females. Vehicle treated males also reared significantly less than vehicle treated females.
3.7 Juvenile elevated plus maze
Clodronate treated rats spent significantly more time on the open arms than vehicle treated rats (Fig. 8a; treatment: F1,34 = 36.9, p < 0.0001; sex: F1,34 = 0.43, ns; interaction: F1,34 = 0.48, ns). Multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated males and clodronate treated females spent significantly more time on the open arms than vehicle treated males and vehicle treated females respectively. Clodronate treated rats entered the open arms significantly more than vehicle treated rats (Fig. 8b; treatment: F1,34 = 19.9, p < 0.0001; sex: F1,34 = 0.0099, ns; interaction: F1,34 = 0.039, ns). Multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated males and clodronate treated females entered the open arms significantly more often than vehicle treated males and vehicle treated females respectively. Additionally, four of nine clodronate treated males and three of ten clodronate treated females fell or jumped off the EPM, and zero vehicle treated animals fell or jumped off the EPM.
3.8 Adult elevated plus maze
There was a significant effect of treatment and sex on the time spent in open arms (Fig. 9a; treatment: F1,37 = 11.9, p = 0.0014; sex: F1,37 = 4.23, p = 0.047; interaction F1,37 = 0.35, ns) with clodronate treated rats spending more time on the open arms compared to vehicle treated rats. Multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated males and vehicle treated females spent significantly more time on the open arms than vehicle treated males and vehicle treated females respectively. Clodronate treated rats entered the open arms significantly more than vehicle treated rats (Fig. 9b; treatment: F1,37 = 8.74, p = 0.0054; sex: F1,37 = 3.13, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 0.077, ns). Multiple comparisons showed that clodronate treated females entered the open arms more than vehicle treated females. Clodronate treated rats spent significantly less time in the closed arms compared to vehicle treated rats (Fig. 9c; treatment: F1,37 = 7.13, p = 0.011; sex: F1,37 = 1.93, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 0.16, ns). There were no differences in the number of closed arm entries (Fig. 9d; treatment: F1,37 = 3.29, ns; sex: F1,37 = 0.019, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 0.0027, ns).
3.9 Adult forced swim test
Clodronate treated rats spent significantly less time immobile in the forced swim test than vehicle treated rats, and females spent significantly less time immobile than males regardless of treatment (Fig. 10a; treatment: F1,37 = 10.1 p = 0.0031; sex: F1,37 = 10.9, p = 0.0021; interaction: F1,37 = 0.83, ns). Multiple comparisons showed that vehicle treated males spent significantly more time immobile than vehicle treated females and clodronate treated males. Clodronate treated rats spent significantly more time swimming than vehicle treated rats and females spent significantly more time swimming than males (Fig. 10b; treatment: F1,37 = 13.3, p = 0.0008; sex: F1,37 = 11.3, p = 0.0018; interaction: F1,37 = 0.46, ns). Multiple comparisons showed that vehicle treated females and clodronate treated males spent significantly more time swimming compared to vehicle treated males. Clodronate treated rats spent significantly less time climbing the walls of the testing chamber than vehicle treated rats (Fig. 10c; treatment: F1,37 = 5.57, p = 0.24; sex: F1,37 = 1.72, ns; interaction: F1,37 = 0.19, ns).
3.10 Adult acute stress response
There was no significant main effect of clodronate treatment on corticosterone levels after acute restraint stress, but there was a significant effect of sex and significant interaction (Fig. 10d; treatment: F1,37 = 1.13, ns; sex: F1,37 = 23.1, p < 0.0001; interaction: F1,37 = 6.25, p = 0.017). Multiple comparisons showed vehicle treated females had significantly higher levels of corticosterone compared to vehicle treated males and that this sex difference was prevented by clodronate treatment.
4. Discussion
In the current study, we assessed the role of neonatal microglial depletion on behavioral development in male and female rats, and found that temporary neonatal loss of microglia perturbs the development of anxiety, social, and locomotor behaviors as well as the adult stress response. We found few sex differences in the effects of neonatal microglial depletion, suggesting that microglia during the neonatal period do not regulate baseline sex differences in the development of motivated behaviors. Overall, our studies support a role for microglia in the normal development of motivated behaviors.
4.1 Normal microglial dynamics in the developing mPFC and amygdala of males and females
We found a peak in microglial density in the mPFC and amygdala at P12. Our results corroborate the findings of previous studies showing that microglial density peaks brain-wide at P14 and the hippocampus, specifically, at P15 in mice (Kim et al., 2015; Nikodemova et al., 2015). Our data provides novel insight into the normal developmental time course of microglial colonization of the prefrontal cortex and amygdala across the early to mid-postnatal period, which has not previously been examined closely. Interestingly, at P2 and P6 microglia density was lower in the mPFC compared to the amygdala. There was also a larger increase in microglia density and a larger peak in microglial density in the mPFC. These data show different colonization kinetics between different the mPFC and amgydala. While microglial density did not differ between the amygdala and mPFC at P12 and P22, the area stained by Iba1 was lower in the amygdala compared to the mPFC at P12. These results suggest that microglia assume a more mature morphology at an earlier time in the amygdala. The differences in density and microglia morphology might indicate developmental differences between these brain areas or sensitivity to adverse events that change microglial function such as stress or inflammation.
4.2 Microglial depletion strategy
Liposomal clodronate selectively depletes resident tissue macrophages from various organs, as well as the brain, while sparing non-phagocytic cells (van Rooijen and Hendrikx, 2010). Liposomal clodronate induces apoptosis in macrophages that have phagocytized the liposomes (van Rooijen et al., 1996). Other recent studies have used central injection of liposomal clodronate to selectively deplete microglia from the early prenatal brain and adult brain, while leaving other cell types intact (Faustino et al., 2011; Kreisel et al., 2014; Torres et al., 2016). While another technique for depleting microglia, antagonism of colony stimulating factor 1 (CSF1), allows for non-invasive depletion of microglia via oral administration in adulthood, CSF1-receptor signaling is a crucial regulator of proliferation and differentiation of neural progenitor cells during development (Nandi et al., 2012). Thus liposomal clodronate is a more viable strategy to target microglia in the developing rat brain than CSF1-R antagonism. While it is possible that liposomal clodronate may induce an inflammatory reaction as microglia die, studies using diphtheria, and CSF1R antagonists to deplete microglia found no increase in inflammatory cytokines after microglia depletion (Asai et al., 2015; Elmore et al., 2014; Parkhurst et al., 2013). Increased GFAP expression has been noted following microglial depletion in these models, which suggests possible compensation by astrocytes due to loss of microglia or astrogliosis in response to microglial death (Asai et al., 2015; Elmore et al., 2014; Parkhurst et al., 2013). Increases in GFAP have similarly been found after microglia depletion using liposomal clodronate (Torres et al., 2016). These studies suggest that if there is any change in microglia cytokine release before the microglia are depleted it is likely transient. In our studies, microglia were depleted for about 1.5–2 weeks, at which point the microglia repopulated the brain. However, the percent area stained by Iba1 did not return to control levels until 3 weeks after initial injections, thus the repopulating microglia do not attain normal morphology until 1 week after repopulation. The clodronate depletion strategy employed here therefore is likely to have interfered with normal microglial function in the brain for approximately 3 weeks of development.
4.3 Microglia are important for the development of later-life behavior
Depleting microglia during the early postnatal period had subtle effects on juvenile and adult social behavior. Juvenile clodronate treated rats chased conspecifics less than vehicle treated rats during paired social play testing, while there were no differences in other active play behaviors. On the adult reciprocal social interaction test, adult clodronate treated rats actively avoided conspecifics more, and passively interacted less with conspecifics. Thus social behavior was altered by neonatal clodronate treatment, but the behavioral phenotype observed cannot be characterized as unilaterally hypersocial or antisocial in nature. While we saw no change in active social interaction on the reciprocal social interaction task, future studies will be necessary to determine whether social preference or social memory might be altered in the three chamber social choice task.
Interestingly, we found large changes in mood-related behaviors in the clodronate treated animals. Depleting microglia in the early postnatal period induced a large decrease in anxiety-like behavior in adolescence and adulthood. Clodronate treated rats spent more time in the open arms and entered the open arms more on the EPM than vehicle treated rats. We also found that neonatal microglial depletion led to decreased behavioral despair on the forced swim test. Specifically, neonatal clodronate treatment produced a decrease in immobility time, an increase in time spent swimming, and a decrease in time spent climbing the walls of the testing apparatus. Increases in anxiety often coincide with increases in depressive-like behavior as part of overall change in affective behavior (Lapiz-Bluhm et al., 2008). Thus, the results of the forced swim test align with the decreased anxiety behavior seen in the clodronate treated rats.
We also observed increased locomotor behavior on the open field test (number of grids crossed) and in the forced swim test (time spent swimming). These increases in locomotion could potentially influence the results observed in several other behavioral measures. The increase time spent swimming seen in the clodronate treated rats could be a side effect of increase locomotor activity and not necessarily reflect differences in response to behavioral despair. However, time spent immobile has been found to be negatively correlated with locomotor activity and center entries in the open field, and is suggested to be related to changes in exploratory behavior rather than locomotor behavior (Ho et al., 2002). This dissociation suggests that the observed locomotor effects are mechanistically separate from the effects on depressive-like behavior. Future testing using non-motor anhedonia measures, such as the sucrose preference test, would be useful to dissociate changes in locomotion from changes in mood. Increased locomotion could potentially explain the decrease in passive interaction time on the social interaction test, but increases in locomotor activity have also been shown to change under a variety of conditions without affecting social behavior (File and Seth, 2003). Moreover, active social interaction was not decreased in our studies, suggesting that locomotor changes alone cannot account for the social behavior effects in our experiment. Increased locomotor activity is a hallmark trait of several neurodevelopmental disorders, including attention deficit/hyperactive disorder and schizophrenia, and can indicate important changes in underlying brain function relevant to neuropsychiatric disorders (Bayless et al., 2015; van den Buuse, 2010; Wong and Josselyn, 2016). Thus, the hyperactive phenotype observed following our early life microglial manipulations warrants future mechanistic investigation.
4.4 Relevance to other models with microglial dysfunction
Interestingly, several models of early-life perturbations, thought to activate microglia, show opposite behavioral phenotypes compared to the clodronate treated rats. Prenatal immune challenge changes microglial morphology, number, distribution, maturation, and induce inflammatory signaling (Cunningham et al., 2013; Juckel et al., 2011; Manitz et al., 2016; Matcovitch-Natan et al., 2016; Van den Eynde et al., 2014). These challenges also are associated with increased anxiety, increased despair-like behavior, decreased social interaction, increased stress reactivity, and decreased locomotor behavior later in life (Choi et al., 2016; Enayati et al., 2012; Lin et al., 2012; Meyer, 2006; Rayen et al., 2011; Sominsky et al., 2012; Zhu et al., 2014). However, it is still unclear how and whether microglia function is being changed in these other models, thus making it hard to directly compare results (Cunningham et al., 2013; Giovanoli et al., 2015; Manitz et al., 2016; Smolders et al., 2015). Neonatal and prenatal stress also impacts microglial morphology, distribution, and function in both the short and long term following stress exposure, generally leading to increased microglial activation and signaling (Delpech et al., 2016; Roque et al., 2015; Œlusarczyk et al., 2015). Prenatal and early-life stress also increase anxiety, increase despair-like behavior, increase stress reactivity, and decrease social interactions later in life (Holland et al., 2014; Rayen et al., 2011; Vallée et al., 1997; Wei et al., 2010). Lastly, fractalkine knockout mice, a model with disrupted microglial physiology and lower microglial density during the early postnatal period, showed decreased reactivity to stress and social behavior deficits in adulthood (Hellwig et al., 2016; Zhan et al., 2014). However, the knockout effects are lifelong thus making it hard to determine what effects are developmental in origin. Overall, these previous results in comparison with our current study consistently suggest that microglial dysfunction during development leads to altered behavior and that type of dysfunction is important for determining the effect on later-life behavior.
4.5 Sex differences in response to microglial loss
Surprisingly, we saw few sex-specific effects of clodronate treatment. Previous studies have found that during the early postnatal period there are more amoeboid microglia in the male rat brain than the female brain, in the hippocampus, amygdala, and preoptic area of the hypothalamus (Lenz et al., 2013; Schwarz et al., 2012). Microglia can also regulate sex differences in brain development and program lifelong sex differences in behavior (Lenz et al., 2013). There is also evidence to indicate that there is higher expression of microglial genes in the male human prenatal brain suggesting that males may be more sensitive to changes in microglial function (Werling et al., 2016). Surprisingly, in our study we found limited sex differences on behavioral outcomes. It is possible that sex differences in behavior due to changes in microglial function in the developing brain may only be apparent under specific circumstances such as early life inflammation or stress. In either case, future in-depth study of microglial signaling and function is still needed to determine the timing and extent of sex differences in microglia in the developing brain.
We did observe a sex difference in the acute hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis response to acute stress, wherein clodronate treated females showed a decreased corticosterone response and males showed no effects of neonatal clodronate treatment. Interestingly, sex steroid hormones do not have an organizational effect on the acute stress response (Goel and Bale, 2008; Handa and Weiser, 2014) suggesting that microglia may organize sex differences in the HPA axis independently of sex hormones. However, since corticosterone levels were not measured at baseline in our study, we do not know the full extent of HPA axis dysregulation. Further experiments are needed to clarify to what extent microglia may influence the development of the HPA axis. Lastly, hormonally-driven sexual differentiation of the rodent brain starts at embryonic day 18 (McCarthy, 2008) and developmental sex differences may be harder to inhibit or change by the time microglia were depleted in the current studies. In order to assess sex-specific consequences of microglial loss, future studies should focus on behaviors that are sex specific such as maternal care or reproductive behavior, other behaviors with known sex differences, such as specific spatial memory tasks, or perform prenatal microglial depletion to better target the critical period for sexual differentiation.
4.6 Developmental versus adult effects of microglial loss
Our data supports a strong developmental role for microglia compared to acute microglial effects on behavior in adulthood. Past studies have shown that depleting microglia in juveniles results in impaired motor learning, fear-conditioning, and novel object recognition (Parkhurst et al., 2013). However, depleting microglia during adulthood has transient, but no long-term effects on anxiety behavior, locomotion or other cognitive measures (Elmore et al., 2014; Torres et al., 2016). Our data is also unique in that few developmental immune model experiments have documented the developmental time course of motivated behavior following microglial manipulations from the juvenile period to adulthood. Our results show that changes in anxiety, locomotion, and social behavior are evident from an early age suggesting that microglia are critical to early developmental processes and the onset of motivated behaviors in adolescence.
4.7 Potential mechanisms mediating the behavioral effects of microglial loss
Our current results suggest that microglia during this critical postnatal period are regulating the development of brain circuits involved in regulating motivated behavior. There are many possible cellular and molecular mechanisms through which microglia could affect behavioral development. Several studies point to the critical role of microglia in regulating normal processes of brain development, including cell genesis, progenitor pool size, synaptogenesis, synaptic pruning, and axonal innervation (Cunningham et al., 2013; Lenz et al., 2013; Paolicelli et al., 2011; Shigemoto-Mogami et al., 2014; Squarzoni et al., 2014). Microglial number, density, and morphology all change dramatically during the first three weeks of life (Bennett et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2015; Nikodemova et al., 2015). Thus, depleting microglia during the first postnatal weeks may inhibit one or more of several important early life microglial functions, or may delay or otherwise alter the timing of these critical processes. Future studies should focus on determining which microglial functions during the first weeks of neonatal life, such as phagocytosis of synapses or supporting cell proliferation through diffusible factors, are important for development of anxiety-like and depressive-like behaviors. Additionally, differences in microglia colonization in the neonatal period may indicate different microglia developmental processes or the sensitivity of these brains areas to experiences that alter microglial function.
4.8 Conclusions
Mood disorders have developmental origins and are accompanied by microglial dysfunction (Green et al., 2010; Yirmiya et al., 2015). Several other psychiatric disorders with developmental origins that can also have mood and affect dysregulation, such as autism spectrum disorder, Tourette’s syndrome, and schizophrenia, are similarly accompanied by dysregulation in microglial function (Fillman et al., 2013; Lennington et al., 2016; Morgan et al., 2012; Takano, 2015; Tetreault et al., 2012; Vargas et al., 2005). However, the exact developmental contribution of microglia to psychiatric disorders is not well known, especially for mood-related and affective behaviors. Our data suggest that early life programming of mood-related behavior is dependent upon the normal function of innate immune cells in the brain. Determining the function of microglia in normal brain development could also lead to a better understanding of how immune modulating experiences, such as maternal immune activation or maternal stress, disrupt or perturb processes of normal brain development that depend on microglia.
Highlights.
We used liposomal clodronate to deplete microglia in neonatal rats and study the lifelong effects on motivated behavior.
Microglia were depleted from the forebrain for approximately two weeks after central liposomal clodronate injection at postnatal days 1 and 4.
Early-life depletion of microglia led to hyperactivity, decreased anxiety and depressive behavior, and altered social behavior in adolescence and adulthood.
Females that had microglia depleted neonatally had a blunted corticosterone response to acute stress in adulthood.
Acknowledgments
These studies were funded by NIH R21MH105826, NARSAD Young Investigator Award, and The Ohio State University Startup funds to KML and OSU Graduate Fellowship to LHN.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Argue KJ, McCarthy MM. Characterization of juvenile play in rats: importance of sex of self and sex of partner. Biol Sex Differ. 2015;6 doi: 10.1186/s13293-015-0034-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asai H, Ikezu S, Tsunoda S, Medalla M, Luebke J, Haydar T, Wolozin B, Butovsky O, Kügler S, Ikezu T. Depletion of microglia and inhibition of exosome synthesis halt tau propagation. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:1584–1593. doi: 10.1038/nn.4132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Auger AP, Olesen KM. Brain Sex Differences and the Organisation of Juvenile Social Play Behaviour: Juvenile social play behaviour. J Neuroendocrinol. 2009;21:519–525. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2009.01871.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayless DW, Perez MC, Daniel JM. Comparison of the validity of the use of the spontaneously hypertensive rat as a model of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in males and females. Behav Brain Res. 2015;286:85–92. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.02.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett ML, Bennett FC, Liddelow SA, Ajami B, Zamanian JL, Fernhoff NB, Mulinyawe SB, Bohlen CJ, Adil A, Tucker A, Weissman IL, Chang EF, Li G, Grant GA, Hayden Gephart MG, Barres BA. New tools for studying microglia in the mouse and human CNS. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1525528113. 201525528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi GB, Yim YS, Wong H, Kim S, Kim H, Kim SV, Hoeffer CA, Littman DR, Huh JR. The maternal interleukin-17a pathway in mice promotes autismlike phenotypes in offspring. Science. 2016 doi: 10.1126/science.aad0314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham CL, Martinez-Cerdeno V, Noctor SC. Microglia Regulate the Number of Neural Precursor Cells in the Developing Cerebral Cortex. J Neurosci. 2013;33:4216–4233. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3441-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delpech J-C, Wei L, Hao J, Yu X, Madore C, Butovsky O, Kaffman A. Early life stress perturbs the maturation of microglia in the developing hippocampus. Brain Behav Immun. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donnelly DJ, Gensel JC, Ankeny DP, van Rooijen N, Popovich PG. An efficient and reproducible method for quantifying macrophages in different experimental models of central nervous system pathology. J Neurosci Methods. 2009;181:36–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elmore MRP, Najafi AR, Koike MA, Dagher NN, Spangenberg EE, Rice RA, Kitazawa M, Matusow B, Nguyen H, West BL, Green KN. Colony-Stimulating Factor 1 Receptor Signaling Is Necessary for Microglia Viability, Unmasking a Microglia Progenitor Cell in the Adult Brain. Neuron. 2014;82:380–397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enayati M, Solati J, Hosseini MH, Shahi HR, Saki G, Salari AA. Maternal infection during late pregnancy increases anxiety- and depression-like behaviors with increasing age in male offspring. Brain Res Bull. 2012;87:295–302. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faustino JV, Wang X, Johnson CE, Klibanov A, Derugin N, Wendland MF, Vexler ZS. Microglial Cells Contribute to Endogenous Brain Defenses after Acute Neonatal Focal Stroke. J Neurosci. 2011;31:12992–13001. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2102-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- File SE, Seth P. A review of 25 years of the social interaction test. Eur J Pharmacol. 2003;463:35–53. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(03)01273-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fillman S, Cloonan N, Catt V, Miller L, Wong J, McCrossin T, Cairns M, Weickert C. Increased inflammatory markers identified in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of individuals with schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. 2013:206–214. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ginhoux F, Greter M, Leboeuf M, Nandi S, See P, Gokhan S, Mehler MF, Conway SJ, Ng LG, Stanley ER, Samokhvalov IM, Merad M. Fate Mapping Analysis Reveals That Adult Microglia Derive from Primitive Macrophages. Science. 2010;330:841–845. doi: 10.1126/science.1194637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giovanoli S, Weber-Stadlbauer U, Schedlowski M, Meyer U, Engler H. Prenatal immune activation causes hippocampal synaptic deficits in the absence of overt microglia anomalies. Brain Behav Immun. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goel N, Bale TL. Organizational and Activational Effects of Testosterone on Masculinization of Female Physiological and Behavioral Stress Responses. Endocrinology. 2008;149:6399–6405. doi: 10.1210/en.2008-0433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green JG, McLaughlin KA, Berglund PA, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Kessler RC. Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication I: associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:113–123. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Handa RJ, Weiser MJ. Gonadal steroid hormones and the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal axis. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2014;35:197–220. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hellwig S, Brioschi S, Dieni S, Frings L, Masuch A, Blank T, Biber K. Altered microglia morphology and higher resilience to stress-induced depression-like behavior in CX3CR1-deficient mice. Brain Behav Immun. 2016;55:126–137. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hiroi R, Neumaier JF. Differential effects of ovarian steroids on anxiety versus fear as measured by open field test and fear-potentiated startle. Behav Brain Res. 2006;166:93–100. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland FH, Ganguly P, Potter DN, Chartoff EH, Brenhouse HC. Early life stress disrupts social behavior and prefrontal cortex parvalbumin interneurons at an earlier time-point in females than in males. Neurosci Lett. 2014;566:131–136. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.02.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ho YJ, Eichendorff J, Schwarting RK. Individual response profiles of male Wistar rats in animal models for anxiety and depression. Behav Brain Res. 2002;136:1–12. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00089-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juckel G, Manitz MP, Brüne M, Friebe A, Heneka MT, Wolf RJ. Microglial activation in a neuroinflammational animal model of schizophrenia — a pilot study. Schizophr Res. 2011;131:96–100. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim I, Mlsna LM, Yoon S, Le B, Yu S, Xu D, Koh S. A postnatal peak in microglial development in the mouse hippocampus is correlated with heightened sensitivity to seizure triggers. Brain Behav. 2015;5 doi: 10.1002/brb3.403. n/a–n/a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreisel T, Frank MG, Licht T, Reshef R, Ben-Menachem-Zidon O, Baratta MV, Maier SF, Yirmiya R. Dynamic microglial alterations underlie stress-induced depressive-like behavior and suppressed neurogenesis. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19:699–709. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lapiz-Bluhm MDS, Bondi CO, Doyen J, Rodriguez GA, Bédard-Arana T, Morilak DA. Behavioural Assays to Model Cognitive and Affective Dimensions of Depression and Anxiety in Rats. J Neuroendocrinol. 2008;20:1115–1137. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2008.01772.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee JC, Seong J, Kim SH, Lee SJ, Cho YJ, An J, Nam DH, Joo KM, Cha CI. Replacement of microglial cells using Clodronate liposome and bone marrow transplantation in the central nervous system of SOD1G93A transgenic mice as an in vivo model of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2012;418:359–365. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2012.01.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lennington JB, Coppola G, Kataoka-Sasaki Y, Fernandez TV, Palejev D, Li Y, Huttner A, Pletikos M, Sestan N, Leckman JF, Vaccarino FM. Transcriptome Analysis of the Human Striatum in Tourette Syndrome. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79:372–382. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.07.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenz KM, Nugent BM, Haliyur R, McCarthy MM. Microglia Are Essential to Masculinization of Brain and Behavior. J Neurosci. 2013;33:2761–2772. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1268-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin YL, Lin SY, Wang S. Prenatal lipopolysaccharide exposure increases anxiety-like behaviors and enhances stress-induced corticosterone responses in adult rats. Brain Behav Immun. 2012;26:459–468. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2011.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Majidi-Zolbanin J, Azarfarin M, Samadi H, Enayati M, Salari AA. Adolescent fluoxetine treatment decreases the effects of neonatal immune activation on anxiety-like behavior in mice. Behav Brain Res. 2013;250:123–132. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manitz MP, Plümper J, Demir S, Ahrens M, Eßlinger M, Wachholz S, Eisenacher M, Juckel G, Friebe A. Flow cytometric characterization of microglia in the offspring of PolyI:C treated mice. Brain Res. 2016;1636:172–182. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcondes FK, Miguel KJ, Melo LL, Spadari-Bratfisch RC. Estrous cycle influences the response of female rats in the elevated plus-maze test. Physiol Behav. 2001;74:435–440. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00593-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matcovitch-Natan O, Winter DR, Giladi A, Vargas Aguilar S, Spinrad A, Sarrazin S, Ben-Yehuda H, David E, Zelada Gonzalez F, Perrin P, Keren-Shaul H, Gury M, Lara-Astaiso D, Thaiss CA, Cohen M, Bahar Halpern K, Baruch K, Deczkowska A, Lorenzo-Vivas E, Itzkovitz S, Elinav E, Sieweke MH, Schwartz M, Amit I. Microglia development follows a stepwise program to regulate brain homeostasis. Science. 2016 doi: 10.1126/science.aad8670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy MM. Estradiol and the Developing Brain. Physiol Rev. 2008;88:91–134. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00010.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer U. The Time of Prenatal Immune Challenge Determines the Specificity of Inflammation-Mediated Brain and Behavioral Pathology. J Neurosci. 2006;26:4752–4762. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0099-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan JT, Chana G, Abramson I, Semendeferi K, Courchesne E, Everall IP. Abnormal microglial–neuronal spatial organization in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in autism. Brain Res. 2012;1456:72–81. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.03.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nandi S, Gokhan S, Dai XM, Wei S, Enikolopov G, Lin H, Mehler MF, Stanley ER. The CSF-1 receptor ligands IL-34 and CSF-1 exhibit distinct developmental brain expression patterns and regulate neural progenitor cell maintenance and maturation. Dev Biol. 2012;367:100–113. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2012.03.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nikodemova M, Kimyon RS, De I, Small AL, Collier LS, Watters JJ. Microglial numbers attain adult levels after undergoing a rapid decrease in cell number in the third postnatal week. J Neuroimmunol. 2015;278:280–288. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2014.11.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paolicelli RC, Bolasco G, Pagani F, Maggi L, Scianni M, Panzanelli P, Giustetto M, Ferreira TA, Guiducci E, Dumas L, Ragozzino D, Gross CT. Synaptic Pruning by Microglia Is Necessary for Normal Brain Development. Science. 2011;333:1456–1458. doi: 10.1126/science.1202529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parkhurst CN, Yang G, Ninan I, Savas JN, Yates JR, Lafaille JJ, Hempstead BL, Littman DR, Gan WB. Microglia Promote Learning-Dependent Synapse Formation through Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor. Cell. 2013;155:1596–1609. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rayen I, van den Hove DL, Prickaerts J, Steinbusch HW, Pawluski JL. Fluoxetine during Development Reverses the Effects of Prenatal Stress on Depressive-Like Behavior and Hippocampal Neurogenesis in Adolescence. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e24003. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roque A, Ochoa-Zarzosa A, Torner L. Maternal separation activates microglial cells and induces an inflammatory response in the hippocampus of male rat pups, independently of hypothalamic and peripheral cytokine levels. Brain Behav Immun. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schafer DP, Lehrman EK, Kautzman AG, Koyama R, Mardinly AR, Yamasaki R, Ransohoff RM, Greenberg ME, Barres BA, Stevens B. Microglia Sculpt Postnatal Neural Circuits in an Activity and Complement-Dependent Manner. Neuron. 2012;74:691–705. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarz JM, Sholar PW, Bilbo SD. Sex differences in microglial colonization of the developing rat brain: Sex differences in microglial colonization. J Neurochem. 2012 doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2011.07630.x. no–no. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shigemoto-Mogami Y, Hoshikawa K, Goldman JE, Sekino Y, Sato K. Microglia Enhance Neurogenesis and Oligodendrogenesis in the Early Postnatal Subventricular Zone. J Neurosci. 2014;34:2231–2243. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1619-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Œlusarczyk J, Trojan E, Głombik K, Budziszewska B, Kubera M, Lasoń W, Popiołek-Barczyk K, Mika J, Wędzony K, Basta-Kaim A. Prenatal stress is a vulnerability factor for altered morphology and biological activity of microglia cells. Front Cell Neurosci. 2015;9 doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smolders S, Smolders SMT, Swinnen N, Gärtner A, Rigo J-M, Legendre P, Brône B. Maternal immune activation evoked by polyinosinic:polycytidylic acid does not evoke microglial cell activation in the embryo. Front Cell Neurosci. 2015;9 doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sominsky L, Walker AK, Ong LK, Tynan RJ, Walker FR, Hodgson DM. Increased microglial activation in the rat brain following neonatal exposure to a bacterial mimetic. Behav Brain Res. 2012;226:351–356. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.08.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squarzoni P, Oller G, Hoeffel G, Pont-Lezica L, Rostaing P, Low D, Bessis A, Ginhoux F, Garel S. Microglia Modulate Wiring of the Embryonic Forebrain. Cell Rep. 2014;8:1271–1279. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.07.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stack A, Carrier N, Dietz D, Hollis F, Sorenson J, Kabbaj M. Sex differences in social interaction in rats: role of the immediate-early gene zif268. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:570. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takano T. Role of Microglia in Autism: Recent Advances. Dev Neurosci. 2015;37:195–202. doi: 10.1159/000398791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tetreault NA, Hakeem AY, Jiang S, Williams BA, Allman E, Wold BJ, Allman JM. Microglia in the Cerebral Cortex in Autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42:2569–2584. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1513-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torres L, Danver J, Ji K, Miyauchi JT, Chen D, Anderson ME, West BL, Robinson JK, Tsirka SE. Dynamic microglial modulation of spatial learning and social behavior. Brain Behav Immun. 2016;55:6–16. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vallée M, Mayo W, Dellu F, Le Moal M, Simon H, Maccari S. Prenatal stress induces high anxiety and postnatal handling induces low anxiety in adult offspring: correlation with stress-induced corticosterone secretion. J Neurosci. 1997;17:2626–2636. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-07-02626.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Buuse M. Modeling the Positive Symptoms of Schizophrenia in Genetically Modified Mice: Pharmacology and Methodology Aspects. Schizophr Bull. 2010;36:246–270. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van den Eynde K, Missault S, Fransen E, Raeymaekers L, Willems R, Drinkenburg W, Kumar-Singh S, Dedeurwaerdere S. Hypolocomotive behaviour associated with increased microglia in a prenatal immune activation model with relevance to schizophrenia. Behav Brain Res. 2014;258:179–186. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Rooijen N, Hendrikx E. Liposomes for Specific Depletion of Macrophages from Organs and Tissues. In: Weissig V, editor. Liposomes. Humana Press; Totowa, NJ: 2010. pp. 189–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Rooijen N, Sanders A, van den Berg TK. Apoptosis of macrophages induced by liposome-mediated intracellular delivery of clodronate and propamidine. J Immunol Methods. 1996;193:93–99. doi: 10.1016/0022-1759(96)00056-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vargas DL, Nascimbene C, Krishnan C, Zimmerman AW, Pardo CA. Neuroglial activation and neuroinflammation in the brain of patients with autism. Ann Neurol. 2005;57:67–81. doi: 10.1002/ana.20315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walf AA, Frye CA. The use of the elevated plus maze as an assay of anxiety-related behavior in rodents. Nat Protoc. 2007;2:322–328. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2007.44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wei L, David A, Duman RS, Anisman H, Kaffman A. Early life stress increases anxiety-like behavior in Balbc mice despite a compensatory increase in levels of postnatal maternal care. Horm Behav. 2010;57:396–404. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werling DM, Parikshak NN, Geschwind DH. Gene expression in human brain implicates sexually dimorphic pathways in autism spectrum disorders. Nat Commun. 2016;7:10717. doi: 10.1038/ncomms10717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wohleb ES, Powell ND, Godbout JP, Sheridan JF. Stress-Induced Recruitment of Bone Marrow-Derived Monocytes to the Brain Promotes Anxiety-Like Behavior. J Neurosci. 2013;33:13820–13833. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1671-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong AHC, Josselyn SA. Caution When Diagnosing Your Mouse With Schizophrenia: The Use and Misuse of Model Animals for Understanding Psychiatric Disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79:32–38. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.04.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yirmiya R, Rimmerman N, Reshef R. Depression as a Microglial Disease. Trends Neurosci. 2015;38:637–658. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2015.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhan Y, Paolicelli RC, Sforazzini F, Weinhard L, Bolasco G, Pagani F, Vyssotski AL, Bifone A, Gozzi A, Ragozzino D, Gross CT. Deficient neuron-microglia signaling results in impaired functional brain connectivity and social behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17:400–406. doi: 10.1038/nn.3641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu F, Zhang L, Ding Y, Zhao J, Zheng Y. Neonatal intrahippocampal injection of lipopolysaccharide induces deficits in social behavior and prepulse inhibition and microglial activation in rats: Implication for a new schizophrenia animal model. Brain Behav Immun. 2014;38:166–174. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]