Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Exp Psychol Gen. 2018 Sep;147(9):1349–1381. doi: 10.1037/xge0000462

Table 7.

Effect sizes for Contextual Manipulations: Estimated subcategory averages (and SEM) from the Contextual manipulation-only meta-analytic mode and individual-publication effect sizes.

Subcategory or
Study
Population Manipulation DV Effect on
Impulsivity
Effect Size p
Adding Delays 0.49 (.08) <.001
Ainslie & Herrnstein (1981) Pigeons Added 4-s delay (vs. no added delay) %LLR
Added 12-s delay (vs. no added delay)
Dai & Fishbach (2013) Experiments 1–2b College students Added delay (vs. small added delay) Proportion choosing LLR 0.40*
Same, but choice made when delay to SSR 90% complete (vs. small added delay) 0.70*
Green et al. (2005)§ Experiments 1–2 College students Added 2-year delay (vs. no added delay) AUC −0.01*#
Added 5-year delay (vs. no added delay) 0.39*#
Added 10-year delay (vs. no added delay) 0.66*#
Mitchell & Wilson (2012) Experiments 1–2 Nonsmokers Added 12-week delay (vs. no added delay) k n.s. 0.37*
Smokers 0.75*
Rachlin & Green (1972) Pigeons Added 4-s delay (vs. no added delay) Relative response rate
Added 12-s delay (vs. no added delay)
Seigel & Rachlin (1995) Pigeons Added delay (vs. no added delay) %SSR 1.47
Adding Response Requirements 2.14 (1.17) .07
Fortes et al. (2015) Pigeons High response rate during LLR delay (vs. no responding during delay) Indifference delay
Low response rate during LLR delay (vs. no responding during delay)
Huskinson & Anderson (2013) Rats High pre-choice response requirement (vs. single response) AUCb 3.03
Half of the pre-choice response requirement (vs. single-response) 2.16
Mazur (2012) Rats Pre-choice response requirement (vs. single response) %LLR
Pigeons
Seigel & Rachlin (1995) Pigeons Pre-choice response requirement (vs. single response) %LLR 1.59*
Adding Outcomes (vs. no added outcome) 0.24 (0.06) < .001
Kowal & Faulkner (2015) College students Added decoy LLR alternative k 0.16*
Added decoy SSR/LLR alternative k 0.11
Scholten & Read (2014) Online volunteers Improving sequence (LLR) Proportion choosing LLR 0.17*
Deteriorating sequence (SSR) 0.49*
Urminsky & Kivetz (2011)˚ Adults Add small, near-immediate reward to SSR and LLR Proportion choosing & preference for 0.22*
Trustworthiness 0.76 (0.17) < .001
Mahrer (1956) Stimulant addicts Working memory training (vs. sham) Proportion choosing LLR 0.60
Michaelson et al. (2013) Online volunteers Trustworthy LLR provider (vs. neutral provider) Probability of choosing LLR 1.29 (within-Ss)
n.s. 0.12 (between-Ss)

n.s., No statistically significant effect; AUC, area under the discounting curve

#

The effect of the added delay was examined using an ANOVA, with the no-added delay as the reference condition; there was a main effect of added-delay, but post-hoc tests were not reported (and thus, statistical significance for each added delay in the table is unknown).

§

The effect sizes for the $10k and $100k LLRs were omitted from the meta-analysis (>3 effect sizes in the same subjects) but are included in the averages presented in the table.

˚

Effect sizes from Exp 1b ($50 and $100 token amounts), Exp 3, and Exp 5a ($100, $300, $700, and $900) were omitted from the meta-analysis (>8 effect sizes from the same publication) but are included in the averages presented in the table.

Effect sizes were not calculated due to small sample size.

*

Effect sizes averaged across experiments or comparable conditions.

Average is a combination of significant and non-significant effects.