Abstract
A wealth of research has investigated the aging brain using blood oxygenation level dependent functional MRI [Blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)]. However, many studies do not consider the aging of the cerebrovascular system, which can influence the BOLD signal independently from neural activity, limiting what can be inferred when comparing age groups. Here, we discuss the ways in which the aging neurovascular system can impact BOLD fMRI, the consequences for age-group comparisons and possible strategies for mitigation. While BOLD fMRI is a valuable tool in this context, this review highlights the importance of consideration of vascular confounds.
Keywords: aging, brain imaging, cerebral blood flow, cerebral hemodynamics, fMRI, neurovascular coupling
Introduction
With an expanding older population, research into the aging brain has become increasingly important. Blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) an functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has often been applied to investigate how age affects neural function; common findings include increased task-induced activation in frontal areas (Park et al., 2003; Langenecker et al., 2004; Gutchess et al., 2005), decreased activation in occipital (Madden, 2004) and temporal areas (Park et al., 2003; Gutchess et al., 2005), and altered functional connectivity (Dørum et al., 2016) with increasing age.
However, the BOLD signal does not directly index changes in neural activity (as summarized in Figure 1). The BOLD signal reflects the balance between changes in cerebral blood flow (CBF), determined by the characteristics of neurovascular coupling (NVC), and changes in tissue oxygen consumption. Following increased neural activation, the signaling processes that underlie NVC lead to vasodilation and increased blood flow, tending to increase BOLD signal. Simultaneously, the increased oxygen consumption, needed to fuel the increased neural activity, will decrease the BOLD signal. The picture is complicated further by the additional dependence of BOLD signal changes on the volume of the relevant venous blood compartment (cerebral blood volume, CBV) and the local vascular architecture.
This complexity leaves BOLD fMRI open to confounds when comparing age groups. If there are age-related differences in NVC or oxygen consumption, then a difference in BOLD signal would be produced independently of a difference in neural activation. Therefore, the interpretability of any BOLD fMRI studies that compare age groups without neurovascular controls must be questioned. Here, we review how aging impacts the neurovascular system and brain metabolism and how age may confound BOLD fMRI studies, as well as discuss strategies for addressing this issue.
Aging Neurovascular System
Vasculature
Firstly, the structural integrity of the cerebral vasculature is compromised during aging (Gazzaley and D’Esposito, 2004). This includes thickening of the capillary basement membrane (Alba et al., 2004) and decreases in capillary density, though the latter has more mixed evidence (Meiger-Ruge et al., 1980; Kalaria, 1996). While the differing results for capillary density may reflect different methodologies and variability in neurovascular health (especially as reported changes tend not to be large; Riddle et al., 2003), they could also be due to the age ranges included. Specifically, decreases in capillary density may only begin in later senescence; Brown et al. (2007) found a stronger negative correlation with age and density when only considering participants over 60 years old. Therefore, this trend may not be visible in younger age ranges. Structural changes (which impact vessel elasticity and functionality) would influence CBF and CBV dynamic changes, which then impact BOLD signal magnitude and timing (Brown et al., 2003). For example, it has been suggested that age-related arterial stiffness, which is especially seen centrally (O’Rourke and Hashimoto, 2007), may lead to CBF pulsatility increases despite overall CBF decreases (Tarumi et al., 2014). A loss of vessels is also especially important when comparing across aging studies, as different magnet strengths and pulse sequences bias the BOLD signal to different vessel sizes (Mueller-Bierl et al., 2007).
Cerebral Blood Flow (CBF)
Lower resting-CBF has been demonstrated in older adults using arterial spin labelling (ASL) fMRI (Restom et al., 2007), even when potential confounds such as partial volume effects and age-related cortical thinning have been corrected for (Asllani et al., 2009; Chen et al., 2011). This has been replicated with different scanning techniques, though the magnitude differs across regions (Martin et al., 1991; Bertsch et al., 2009; Chen et al., 2011). It is suggested that this CBF decrease is due to age-related differences in CO2 end-tidal partial pressure, which reflects CO2 arterial tension, rather than an independent effect of age (De Vis et al., 2015). CBF is an important parameter in determining the BOLD response, with studies finding an inverse correlation between basal CBF and BOLD signal change, (Cohen et al., 2002; Brown et al., 2003; Stefanovic et al., 2006) in line with the deoxy-hemoglobin dilution model (Hoge et al., 1999). Indeed, age-related BOLD differences have been shown to significantly decrease when lower CBF is accounted for, suggesting a substantial vascular contribution to the observed BOLD age-differences (Zebrowitz et al., 2016).
Cerebrovascular Reactivity (CVR)
Age also influences Cerebrovascular Reactivity (CVR), which refers to the increase in CBF and CBV following exposure to a vasodilatory stimulus, such as after neural activity in order to meet the increased energy demand. Neurovascular coupling relies on CVR being preserved. A decrease in vasodilatory capacity is reported in aged rats (Tamaki et al., 1995). Flück et al. (2014) used transcranial Doppler ultrasound (TCD) to investigate this in humans and concluded that older adults had reduced CVR, which may be due to vascular stiffening. However, in TCD the blood velocities through the posterior and middle cerebral arteries are measured rather than absolute CBF, so there may be unobserved regional dependences, and the lack of direct tissue measurement of CBF using TCD means that results should be interpreted with caution (Coverdale et al., 2014; Reinstrup et al., 2014). Other studies have used a CO2-inhalation task (Liu et al., 2013a) or breath-holding challenge, (Riecker et al., 2003) in which BOLD fMRI has been used monitor evoked-CBF changes. Despite breath-holding challenges having potential confounds (e.g., metabolic rate or lung function can influence the quantity of blood CO2) and not showing a linear relationship between holding time and arterial partial pressure of CO2, (Fierstra et al., 2013) similar results were found between breath-hold and CO2 inhalation. For both methods, older adults showed lower CVR. Importantly, while uncorrected BOLD fMRI in Liu et al. (2013a) implied decreases in task-related V1 and medial temporal lobe activity with age, this effect disappeared when CVR was corrected for. Additionally, a stronger increase in bilateral frontal gyrus activation in older adults was found after CVR-correction, which may reflect compensatory mechanisms as there was no difference in memory scores (Liu et al., 2013a). This provides strong evidence for an age-related decrease in CVR; this would lead to a smaller amount of vasodilation and subsequent evoked CBF, deoxyhaemoglobin, and BOLD signal, causing an under-representation of neural responses in older groups when comparing across ages.
Cerebral Metabolic Rate of Oxygen (CMRO2)
Another key factor regulating BOLD signal is the rate at which oxygen is extracted from the blood and used for energy release (rate of cerebral metabolic oxygen consumption; CMRO2). An increase in CMRO2 would lead to an increase in deoxy-hemoglobin in the venous vessels, and thus a decrease in BOLD signal due to the paramagnetic properties of deoxy-hemoglobin (Schwarzbauer and Heinke, 1999). Although age-related changes in resting CMRO2 have been reported, there is conflicting evidence as to their direction. Peng et al. (2014) investigated this using CBF, blood oxygen saturation percentage, and total blood oxygen capacity to estimate resting CMRO2, which is suggested to be a reliable method (Liu et al., 2013b). They found a significant increase in CMRO2 in older adults, which could act as a compensatory mechanism for declining cognitive functions or weakening CVR and CBF. Conversely, De Vis et al. (2015) reported a decrease in baseline CMRO2, which was present in all regions except for the occipital cortex (although the parietal cortex lost significance once end-tidal partial pressure of CO2 was controlled for). This conflicting result is reflected in other studies, using similar and alternative techniques (Lu et al., 2011; Aanerud et al., 2012). It is possible that the method by which CMRO2 was derived influenced results, as different methods were used (e.g., ASL vs. phase-contrast flow velocity). It may also simply reflect the greater within- and between-subject variability that has been reported in older participants’ BOLD signals (Kannurpatti et al., 2010; Garrett et al., 2013; Baum and Beauchamp, 2014 but see). Of particular relevance for interpreting task-related BOLD signal changes is the difference in evoked CMRO2, of which several studies report an age-related increase (Restom et al., 2007; Hutchison et al., 2013). In the presence of an equal blood flow response across age groups this would have the effect of reducing the BOLD signal response.
Interactions between changes in NVC and oxygen consumption may have the greatest confounding effect; for example, alterations in CMRO2 and CBF may explain lower BOLD responses in older adults, as higher demand but diminished supply would lead to higher venous deoxy-hemoglobin concentrations (Lu et al., 2011; Hutchison et al., 2013). Furthermore, these effects are clinically silent and thus not excluded by controlling for health status (e.g., cardiac or neurological illness). Several studies have linked age-related changes in BOLD signal to neurovascular factors, such as signal timing (Taoka et al., 1998) and increased voxel-wise noise (D’Esposito et al., 1999). Another finding that confounds the comparison of age groups using BOLD fMRI is that of increased within- and between-subject variability that has been reported in older adults (e.g., Kannurpatti et al., 2010; Baum and Beauchamp, 2014). It may be that variations in NVC and levels of CMRO2 contribute to this greater variability (though this cannot be the only contributor, as increased neural variation is also found electro-physiologically; McIntosh et al., 2014). However, the extent of this increased variability with age was found to vary greatly based on the motion correction pipeline, despite the pipelines having similar goals, introducing extra difficulty in age group comparisons (Turner et al., 2015). Therefore, to better interpret age-related changes in BOLD signal, these factors must be considered and examined.
Drug-Related Factors
Other age-related factors may also indirectly impact the neurovascular system. For example, older adults tend to use more prescription and over-the-counter drugs (Francis et al., 2005). Although studies often exclude certain medications, such as beta-blockers or anti-depressants, they rarely account for over-the-counter products such as non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs). A survey of aspirin use (a common NSAID) in adults over 45 years old found that 52% reported current use, many did so for primary prevention, and that regular use was associated with markers of a healthy lifestyle (Williams et al., 2015). This indicates that over-the-counter NSAID use is higher in older adults, even if they are relatively healthy. If these medications alter NVC, it may confound the BOLD signal. There has been little research into this, though aspirin was found to reduce resting-CBF in an in vivo rabbit model, which was suggested to be due to inhibiting prostacyclin and/or nitric oxide (Bednar and Gross, 1999). A human study investigated the effects of two other NSAIDs, naproxen (available over-the-counter in lower doses) and indomethacin (prescription only). Using transcranial Doppler sonography, it found a significant decrease in resting and visually evoked blood velocity, possibly due to inhibiting vasodilatory processes (Szabo et al., 2014). These results may also be more reflective of average older adult NSAID use than other work as the medication was administered orally in usual doses over 2 days, rather than an injection (Jensen et al., 1993) or a single high dose (Markus et al., 1994). However, they used young adults so it is inconclusive whether the same effect would be evoked in the aged neurovascular system. Other types of NSAIDs may also have differing effects; for example, CBV and CBF didn’t change after short-term exposure to ibuprofen in piglets (Pellicer et al., 1999). While under-researched in older adults, increased use of NSAIDs may contribute to an inaccurate estimation of neural activity differences in older adults. Further studies should examine the influence of over-the-counter medications in older adults and on the BOLD response.
Potential Controls
As non-neuronal factors independently alter the BOLD response, such factors should be understood and controlled for as far as possible to maximize the interpretability of fMRI data. One approach involves normalizing BOLD fMRI data with measure of the effectiveness of NVC, CVR being commonly used under the hypothesis that it explains a lot of variability in NVC. CVR can be estimated by altering arterial blood CO2 (a vasodilator), and measuring the CBF/BOLD increase. One approach involves a CO2-inhalation task with simultaneous fMRI recording and using the change in BOLD to normalize the task-related response (see Liu et al., 2013a). However, an inhalation task may prove strenuous for older participants. Breath-holding paradigms, which involve the participant naturally increasing blood CO2 by holding their breath, may be more tolerable and have shown comparable results (Kastrup et al., 2001; Handwerker et al., 2007). Breath-holding is suggested to be reliable even in those with poor breath-hold performance, such as older adults (Bright and Murphy, 2013). However, breath-holding can cause severe motion artifacts, particularly in high-field strengths, such as 7T. Confounds could also be caused by patient anxiety over a breath-hold or inhalation task, such as increased movement or increased heart rate, which may occur if an older participant finds the task difficult to perform.
Another possibility is resting state fluctuation amplitude (RSFA), which examines the signal amplitude variation when the participant is ‘task free’, thus reducing confounds such as head movement which may be seen with a breath-holding or a hypercapnia challenge (e.g., Hall et al., 2011). The RSFA has been suggested as an index of vascular contributions to variability in the BOLD response as it will depend on local CVR and CBV. Tsvetanov et al. (2015) used RSFA to scale BOLD data and found that the magnitude of age-related differences significantly decreased. Furthermore, though RSFA has been criticized for confounding together neural and neurovascular properties (Lipp et al., 2015), these contributions have been separated using magnetoencephalography and neurovascular function measures (Tsvetanov et al., 2015). Only vascular factors mediated age-effects on RSFA, which suggests that RSFA is largely driven by neurovascular contributions (Wolpe et al., 2016). However, RSFA has also been reported to have poorer repeatability and model fit than breath holding tasks (Lipp et al., 2015). Although these methods of estimating CVR have major limitations when applied to an older population, they offer an empirical way of reducing non-neural variability.
An alternative to trying to ‘correct’ the BOLD response is to measure a process that is likely to more closely reflect levels of neural activity, such as CMRO2. By calibrating fMRI to quantitatively examine the amount of oxygen metabolism, reflective of oxidative energy release, we avoid the influence of factors such as resting-CBF with established age differences. Methods have been proposed to map CMRO2 reliably, such as using combined hyperoxia and hypercapnia (Wise et al., 2013; Germuska et al., 2016). However, this again involves gas inhalation, which may be too strenuous or uncomfortable for very old participants. A method involving diffuse optical tomography with BOLD and ASL fMRI may therefore be better as it does not involve gas inhalation but was found to be highly correlated with previous methods (Yücel et al., 2014). This also does not assume a constant CMRO2, which is not always found (Xu et al., 2011).
Another option is implementing a control task that is assumed to not show neural age-related changes (e.g., a simple motor task; D’Esposito et al., 1999). A difference between groups would therefore suggest a global NVC confound. The signal and noise characteristics (i.e., the haemodynamic response function) within each group could then be characterized to create a global normalization factor (Samanez-Larkin and D’Esposito, 2008), assuming that it can be extended across the brain. However, this approach assumes equivalent neural activity across age groups, which may not be valid even with simple motor tasks(Sailer et al., 2000; Inuggi et al., 2011). It also does not account for region-dependant variation in neurovascular function (Ances et al., 2008; Devonshire et al., 2012). Additionally, the neurovascular system has different influences on BOLD signal depending on the task (Kannurpatti et al., 2010), possibly because they utilize different functional and structural networks. Signals characterized in control tasks therefore may not be generalizable.
The above controls may be best when used in tandem with designs intended to reduce neurovascular confounds, which also have the advantage of being easier to implement. Investigating relative change within groups or varying the conditions parametrically may be useful as they provide an internal control (Gazzaley and D’Esposito, 2004; Ankudowich et al., 2016). However, these designs will still be confounded by resting levels of CBF influencing the magnitude of evoked BOLD signal (Cohen et al., 2002). Another possible method is event-related fMRI, which allows the separation of cognitive processes; if age-related differences are found in one stage of a cognitive task but not another, which both use similar brain areas (e.g., memory encoding vs. retrieval), observed differences are more likely to be neuronal in origin (Rypma and D’Esposito, 2000; Gazzaley and D’Esposito, 2004). Although the methodological design depends heavily on the research question, designs such as the above may be useful for comparing age groups with BOLD fMRI.
Conclusion
The BOLD signal reflects the complex interaction of many factors, such as CVR, CBF, and CMRO2. When using BOLD fMRI to compare across age groups, researchers must be aware that age can impact the majority of these non-neural factors, so differences across age groups cannot be solely contributed to neuronal changes. Neuronal changes, if present, may be exaggerated or masked by age-related alterations in factors such as resting-CBF. This requires careful consideration during study design and interpretation, so as not to falsely highlight neuronal changes that are not present or miss out on identifying neuronal changes that are present.
Finally, it should be noted that most of the neurovascular changes noted are part of the normal aging process and may have an independent effect on cognition (Moorhouse and Rockwood, 2008; Bangen et al., 2014). For example, neurovascular uncoupling, without CBF changes, leads to significant cognitive impairment in mice (Tarantini et al., 2015). Monitoring these factors using fMRI also has interesting applications in clinically assessing neurovascular health. While BOLD fMRI requires careful consideration if interpreting signals as neuronal in origin, more broadly it demonstrates great potential as a tool, in aging research, that is sensitive to neurovascular and neurometabolic changes.
Author Contributions
MW conceived the review, literature search, and wrote the manuscript. RW conceived the review and revised the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
- Aanerud J., Borghammer P., Chakravarty M. M., Vang K., Rodell A. B., Jónsdottir K. Y., et al. (2012). Brain energy metabolism and blood flow differences in healthy ageing. J. Cerebral Blood Flow Metab. 32 1177–1187. 10.1038/jcbfm.2012.18 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alba C., Vidal L., Díaz F., Villena A., de Vargas I. P. (2004). Ultrastructural and quantitative age-related changes in capillaries of the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. Brain Res. Bull. 64 145–153. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2004.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ances B. M., Leontiev O., Perthen J. E., Liang C., Lansing A. E., Buxton R. B. (2008). Regional differences in the coupling of cerebral blood flow and oxygen metabolism changes in response to activation: implications for BOLD-fMRI. Neuroimage 39 1510–1521. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ankudowich E., Pasvanis S., Rajah M. N. (2016). Changes in the modulation of brain activity during context encoding vs. context retrieval across the adult lifespan. Neuroimage 139 103–113. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asllani I., Habeck C., Borogovac A., Brown T. R., Brickman A. M., Stern Y. (2009). Separating function from structure in perfusion imaging of the aging brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30 2927–2935. 10.1002/hbm.20719 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bangen K. J., Nation D. A., Clark L. R., Harmell A. L., Wierenga C. E., Dev S. I., et al. (2014). Interactive effects of vascular risk burden and advanced age on cerebral blood flow. Front. Ageing Neurosci. 6:159. 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baum S. H., Beauchamp M. S. (2014). Greater BOLD variability in older compared with younger adults during audiovisual speech perception. PLoS One 9:e111121. 10.1371/journal.pone.0111121 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bednar M. M., Gross C. E. (1999). Aspirin reduces experimental cerebral blood flow in vivo. Neurol. Res. 21 488–490. 10.1080/01616412.1999.11740963 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertsch K., Hagemann D., Hermes M., Walter C., Khan R., Naumann E. (2009). Resting cerebral blood flow, attention, and ageing. Brain Res. 1267 77–88. 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bright M. G., Murphy K. (2013). Reliable quantification of BOLD fMRI cerebrovascular reactivity despite poor breath-hold performance. Neuroimage 83 559–568. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown G. G., Zorrilla L. T. E., Georgy B., Kindermann S. S., Wong E. C., Buxton R. B. (2003). BOLD and perfusion response to finger-thumb apposition after acetazolamide administration: differential relationship to global perfusion. J. Cerebral Blood Flow Metab. 23 829–837. 10.1097/01.WCB.0000071887.63724.B2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown W. R., Moody D. M., Thore C. R., Challa V. R., Anstrom J. A. (2007). Vascular dementia in leukoaraiosis may be a consequence of capillary loss not only in the lesions, but in normal-appearing white matter and cortex as well. J. Neurol. Sci. 257 62–66. 10.1016/j.jns.2007.01.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen J. J., Rosas H. D., Salat D. H. (2011). Age-associated reductions in cerebral blood flow are independent from regional atrophy. Neuroimage 55 468–478. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen E. R., Ugurbil K., Kim S.-G. (2002). Effect of basal conditions on the magnitude and dynamics of the blood oxygenation level-dependent fMRI response. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 22 1042–1053. 10.1097/00004647-200209000-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coverdale N. S., Gati J. S., Opalevych O., Perrotta A., Shoemaker J. K. (2014). Cerebral blood flow velocity underestimates cerebral blood flow during modest hypercapnia and hypocapnia. J. Appl. Physiol. 117 1090–1096. 10.1152/japplphysiol.00285.2014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Vis J. B., Hendrikse J., Bhogal A., Adams A., Kappelle L. J., Petersen E. T. (2015). Age-related changes in brain hemodynamics; a calibrated MRI study: age-related changes in brain hemodynamics. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36 3973–3987. 10.1002/hbm.22891 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Esposito M., Zarahn E., Aguirre G. K., Rypma B. (1999). The effect of normal ageing on the coupling of neural activity to the bold hemodynamic response. Neuroimage 10 6–14. 10.1006/nimg.1999.0444 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devonshire I. M., Papadakis N. G., Port M., Berwick J., Kennerley A. J., Mayhew J. E. W., et al. (2012). Neurovascular coupling is brain region-dependent. Neuroimage 59 1997–2006. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dørum E. S., Alnaes D., Kaufmann T., Richard G., Lund M. J., Tønnesen S., et al. (2016). Age-related differences in brain network activation and co-activation during multiple object tracking. Brain Behav. 6:e00533. 10.1002/brb3.533 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fierstra J., Sobczyk O., Battisti-Charbonney A., Mandell D. M., Poublanc J., Crawley A. P., et al. (2013). Measuring cerebrovascular reactivity: what stimulus to use? Measuring cerebrovascular reactivity. J. Physiol. 591 5809–5821. 10.1113/jphysiol.2013.259150 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flück D., Beaudin A. E., Steinback C. D., Kumarpillai G., Shobha N., McCreary C. R., et al. (2014). Effects of ageing on the association between cerebrovascular responses to visual stimulation, hypercapnia and arterial stiffness. Front. Physiol. 5:49. 10.3389/fphys.2014.00049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis S.-A., Barnett N., Denham M. (2005). Switching of prescription drugs to over-the-counter status: is it a good thing for the elderly? Drugs Ageing 22 361–370. 10.2165/00002512-200522050-00001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garrett D. D., Kovacevic N., McIntosh A. R., Grady C. L. (2013). The modulation of BOLD variability between cognitive states varies by age and processing speed. Cereb. Cortex 23 684–693. 10.1093/cercor/bhs055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gazzaley A. H., D’Esposito M. (2004). “BOLD functional MRI and cognitive ageing,” in Cognitive Neuroscience of Ageing: Linking Cognitive and Cerebral Ageing, eds Cabeza R., Nyberg L., Park D. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press; ). [Google Scholar]
- Germuska M., Merola A., Murphy K., Babic A., Richmond L., Khot S., et al. (2016). A forward modelling approach for the estimation of oxygen extraction fraction by calibrated fMRI. Neuroimage 139 313–323. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gutchess A. H., Welsh R. C., Hedden T., Bangert A., Minear M., Liu L. L., et al. (2005). Ageing and the neural correlates of successful picture encoding: frontal activations compensate for decreased medial-temporal activity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17 84–96. 10.1162/0898929052880048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall E. L., Driver I. D., Croal P. L., Francis S. T., Gowland P. A., Morris P. G., et al. (2011). The effect of hypercapnia on resting and stimulus induced MEG signals. Neuroimage 58 1034–1043. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Handwerker D. A., Gazzaley A., Inglis B. A., D’Esposito M. (2007). Reducing vascular variability of fMRI data across ageing populations using a breathholding task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28 846–859. 10.1002/hbm.20307 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoge R. D., Atkinson J., Gill B., Crelier G. R., Marrett S., Pike G. B. (1999). Investigation of BOLD signal dependence on cerebral blood flow and oxygen consumption: The deoxyhemoglobin dilution model. Magn. Reson. Med. 42 849–863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutchison J. L., Lu H., Rypma B. (2013). Neural mechanisms of age-related slowing: the CBF/ CMRO2 ratio mediates age-differences in BOLD signal and human performance. Cereb. Cortex 23 2337–2346. 10.1093/cercor/bhs233 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inuggi A., Amato N., Magnani G., González-Rosa J. J., Chieffo R., Comi G., et al. (2011). Cortical control of unilateral simple movement in healthy ageing. Neurobiol. Ageing 32 524–538. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.02.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen K., Freundlich M., Bünemann L., Therkelsen K., Hansen H., Cold G. E. (1993). The effect of indomethacin upon cerebral blood flow in healthy volunteers. The influence of moderate hypoxia and hypercapnia. Acta Neuroch. 124 114–119. 10.1007/BF01401132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalaria R. N. (1996). Cerebral vessels in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Pharmacol. Therap. 72 193–214. 10.1016/S0163-7258(96)00116-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kannurpatti S. S., Motes M. A., Rypma B., Biswal B. B. (2010). Neural and vascular variability and the fMRI-BOLD response in normal ageing. Magn. Reson. Imaging 28 466–476. 10.1016/j.mri.2009.12.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kastrup A., Krüger G., Neumann-Haefelin T., Moseley M. E. (2001). Assessment of cerebrovascular reactivity with functional magnetic resonance imaging: comparison of CO2 and breath holding. Magn. Reson. Imaging 19 13–20. 10.1016/S0730-725X(01)00227-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langenecker S. A., Nielson K. A., Rao S. M. (2004). fMRI of healthy older adults during Stroop interference. Neuroimage 21 192–200. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipp I., Murphy K., Caseras X., Wise R. G. (2015). Agreement and repeatability of vascular reactivity estimates based on a breath-hold task and a resting state scan. Neuroimage 113 387–396. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu P., Hebrank A., Rodrigue K., Kennedy K., Section J., Park D., et al. (2013a). Age-related differences in memory-encoding fMRI responses after accounting for decline in vascular reactivity. Neuroimage 78 415–425. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu P., Xu F., Lu H. (2013b). Test-retest reproducibility of a rapid method to measure brain oxygen metabolism. Magn. Reson. Med. 69 675–681. 10.1002/mrm.24295 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu H., Xu F., Rodrigue K. M., Kennedy K. M., Cheng Y., Flicker B., et al. (2011). Alterations in cerebral metabolic rate and blood supply across the adult lifespan. Cereb. Cortex 21 1426–1434. 10.1093/cercor/bhq224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madden D. J. (2004). Age-related changes in neural activity during visual target detection measured by fMRI. Cereb. Cortex 14 143–155. 10.1093/cercor/bhg113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markus H. S., Vallance P., Brown M. M. (1994). Differential effect of three cyclooxygenase inhibitors on human cerebral blood flow velocity and carbon dioxide reactivity. Stroke 25 1760–1764. 10.1161/01.STR.25.9.1760 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin A. J., Friston K. J., Colebatch J. G., Frackowiak R. S. J. (1991). Decreases in regional cerebral blood flow with normal ageing. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 11 684–689. 10.1038/jcbfm.1991.121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIntosh A. R., Vakorin V., Kovacevic N., Wang H., Diaconescu A., Protzner A. B. (2014). Spatiotemporal dependency of age-related changes in brain signal variability. Cereb. Cortex 24 1806–1817. 10.1093/cercor/bht030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meiger-Ruge W., Hunziker O., Schulz U., Tobler H., Schweizer A. (1980). Stereological changes in the capillary network and nerve cells of the ageing human brain. Mech. Ageing Dev. 14 684–689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moorhouse P., Rockwood K. (2008). Vascular cognitive impairment: current concepts and clinical developments. Lancet Neurol. 7 246–255. 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70040-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mueller-Bierl B. M., Uludag K., Pereira P. L., Schick F. (2007). Magnetic field distribution and signal decay in functional MRI in very high fields (up to 9.4 T) using monte carlo diffusion modeling. Int. J. Biomed. Imaging 2007:70309. 10.1155/2007/70309 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Rourke M. F., Hashimoto J. (2007). Mechanical factors in arterial ageing. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 50 1–13. 10.1016/j.jacc.2006.12.050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park D. C., Welsh R. C., Marshuetz C., Gutchess A. H., Mikels J., Polk T. A., et al. (2003). Working memory for complex scenes: age differences in frontal and hippocampal activations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15 1122–1134. 10.1162/089892903322598094 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellicer M., Aparicio F., Cabaǹas E. A. (1999). Effect of the cyclo-oxygenase blocker ibuprofen on cerebral blood volume and cerebral blood flow during normocarbia and hypercarbia in newborn piglets. Acta Paediatr. 88 82–88. 10.1080/08035259950170664 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peng S.-L., Dumas J. A., Park D. C., Liu P., Filbey F. M., McAdams C. J., et al. (2014). Age-related increase of resting metabolic rate in the human brain. Neuroimage 98 176–183. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.078 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reinstrup P., Ryding E., Asgeirsson B., Hesselgard K., Unden J., Romner B. (2014). Cerebral blood flow and transcranial doppler sonography measurements of CO2-Reactivity in acute traumatic brain injured patients. Neurocrit. Care 20 54–59. 10.1007/s12028-012-9727-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Restom K., Bangen K. J., Bondi M. W., Perthen J. E., Liu T. T. (2007). Cerebral blood flow and BOLD responses to a memory encoding task: A comparison between healthy young and elderly adults. Neuroimage 37 430–439. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riddle D. R., Sonntag W. E., Lichtenwalner R. J. (2003). Microvascular plasticity in ageing. Ageing Res. Rev. 2 149–168. 10.1016/S1568-1637(02)00064-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riecker A., Grodd W., Klose U., Schulz J. B., Groschel K., Erb M., et al. (2003). Relation between regional functional MRI activation and vascular reactivity to carbon dioxide during normal ageing. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 23 565–573. 10.1097/01.WCB.0000056063.25434.04 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rypma B., D’Esposito M. (2000). Isolating the neural mechanisms of age-related changes in human working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 3 509–515. 10.1038/74889 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sailer A., Dichgans J., Gerloff C. (2000). The influence of normal ageing on the cortical processing of a simple motor task. Neurology 55 979–985. 10.1212/WNL.55.7.979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samanez-Larkin G. R., D’Esposito M. (2008). Group comparisons: imaging the ageing brain. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 3 290–297. 10.1093/scan/nsn029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarzbauer C., Heinke W. (1999). Investigating the dependence of BOLD contrast on oxidative metabolism. Magn. Reson. Med. 41 537–543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stefanovic B., Warnking J. M., Rylander K. M., Pike G. B. (2006). The effect of global cerebral vasodilation on focal activation hemodynamics. Neuroimage 30 726–734. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szabo K., Rosengarten B., Juhasz T., Lako E., Csiba L., Olah L. (2014). Effect of non-steroid anti-inflammatory drugs on neurovascular coupling in humans. J. Neurol. Sci. 336 227–231. 10.1016/j.jns.2013.10.048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamaki K., Nakai M., Yokota T., Ogata J. (1995). Effects of ageing and chronic hypertension on cerebral blood flow and cerebrovascular CO2 reactivity in the rat. Gerontology 41 11–17. 10.1159/000213657 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taoka T., Iwasaki S., Uchida H., Fukusumi A., Nakagawa H., Kichikawa K., et al. (1998). Age correlation of the time lag in signal change on EPI-fMRI. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. 22 514–517. 10.1097/00004728-199807000-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tarantini S., Hertelendy P., Tucsek Z., Valcarcel-Ares M. N., Smith N., Menyhart A., et al. (2015). Pharmacologically-Induced Neurovascular uncoupling is associated with cognitive impairment in mice. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 35 1871–1881. 10.1038/jcbfm.2015.162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tarumi T., Khan M. A., Liu J., Tseng B. M., Parker R., Riley J., et al. (2014). Cerebral hemodynamics in normal ageing: central artery stiffness, wave reflection, and pressure pulsatility. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 34 971–978. 10.1038/jcbfm.2014.44 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsvetanov K. A., Henson R. N. A., Tyler L. K., Davis S. W., Shafto M. A., Taylor J. R., et al. (2015). The effect of ageing on fMRI: Correction for the confounding effects of vascular reactivity evaluated by joint fMRI and MEG in 335 adults: vascular influences on BOLD signal with ageing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36 2248–2269. 10.1002/hbm.22768 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner B. O., Lopez B., Santander T., Miller M. B. (2015). One dataset, many conclusions: BOLD variability’s complicated relationships with age and motion artifacts. Brain Imag. Behav. 9 115–127. 10.1007/s11682-014-9351-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams C. D., Chan A. T., Elman M. R., Kristensen A. H., Miser W. F., Pignone M. P., et al. (2015). Aspirin use among adults in the U.S. Am. J. Prevent. Med. 48 501–508. 10.1016/j.amepre.2014.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise R. G., Harris A. D., Stone A. J., Murphy K. (2013). Measurement of OEF and absolute CMRO2: MRI-based methods using interleaved and combined hypercapnia and hyperoxia. Neuroimage 83 135–147. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpe N., Ingram J. N., Tsvetanov K. A., Geerligs L., Kievit R. A., Henson R. N., et al. (2016). Ageing increases reliance on sensorimotor prediction through structural and functional differences in frontostriatal circuits. Nat. Commun. 7:13034. 10.1038/ncomms13034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu F., Uh J., Brier M. R., Hart J., Yezhuvath U. S., Gu H., et al. (2011). The influence of carbon dioxide on brain activity and metabolism in conscious humans. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 31 58–67. 10.1038/jcbfm.2010.153 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yücel M. A., Evans K. C., Selb J., Huppert T. J., Boas D. A., Gagnon L. (2014). Validation of the hypercapnic calibrated fMRI method using DOT–fMRI fusion imaging. Neuroimage 102 729–735. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zebrowitz L., Ward N., Boshyan J., Gutchess A., Hadjikhani N. (2016). Dedifferentiated face processing in older adults is linked to lower resting state metabolic activity in fusiform face area. Brain Res. 1644 22–31. 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.05.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]