Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Dec 6.
Published in final edited form as: Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2018 Jul 23;26(6):525–540. doi: 10.1037/pha0000216

Table 2:

Results of the inferential tests used to analyze the raw proportion of responses for the large magnitude reinforcer and AUCs at the end of baseline training for rats trained in either delay discounting or probability discounting.

Delay Discounting Probability Discounting

Raw Proportion of Responses Raw Proportion of Responses

Factor DF F Statistic P ηp2 Factor DF F Statistic P ηp2

Delay 1.714, 37.703  118.543 <.001 .843 Delay 1.701, 37.412 166.274 <.001 .883
Schedule 1, 22 98.025 <.001 .817 Schedule 1, 22 6.574 .018 .230
Delay × Schedule 1.714, 37.703 16.513 < .001a .429 Delay × Schedule 1.701, 37.412 3.869  0.036b .150

AUC AUC

Factor DF t Statistic P d Factor DF t Statistic P d

Schedule 22 −9.802 <.001 −4.002 Schedule 22 −2.666 .014 −1.089
a

To probe the significant interaction, independent-samples t tests (with Bonferroni correction) were used to compare the proportion of responses between rats trained on the ascending and descending schedules at each delay. Rats trained on the descending schedule showed greater responding for the large magnitude reinforcer at each delay (all t’s ≥ −4.905, all p’s < .001, all d’s ≥ 2.002), with the exception of the 0-s delay, t(11.000) = −2.756, p = .019, d = 1.166

b

To probe the significant interaction, independent-samples t tests (with Bonferroni correction) were used to compare the proportion of responses between rats trained on the ascending and descending schedules at each odds against. There were no significant differences between rats trained on the ascending and descending schedules at any of the odds against (all t’s ≤ −2.817, all p’s > .011, all d’s ≥ .063).