Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Cogn Emot. 2018 Oct 18;33(1):67–76. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2018.1535428

Concepts Dissolve Artificial Boundaries in the Study of Emotion and Cognition, Uniting Body, Brain, and Mind

Katie Hoemann a, Lisa Feldman Barrett a,b
PMCID: PMC6399041  NIHMSID: NIHMS1509995  PMID: 30336722

Abstract

Theories of emotion have often maintained artificial boundaries: for instance, that cognition and emotion are separable, and that an emotion concept is separable from the emotional events that comprise its category (e.g., ‘fear’ is distinct from instances of fear). Over the past several years, research has dissolved these artificial boundaries, suggesting instead that conceptual construction is a domain-general process—a process by which the brain makes meaning of the world. The brain constructs emotion concepts, but also cognitions and perceptions, all in the service of guiding action. In this view, concepts are multimodal constructions, dynamically prepared from a set of highly variable instances. This approach obviates old questions (e.g., how does cognition regulate emotion?) but generates new ones (e.g., how does a brain learn emotion concepts?). In this paper, we review this constructionist, predictive coding account of emotion, considering its implications for health and well-being, culture and development.

Keywords: prediction, construction, conceptualisation, embodiment, language


Theories of emotion have long been guided by folk intuitions about the mind. One intuition is that cognition and emotion are distinct, biologically-based categories of phenomena that cause one another and compete for the control of behaviour. Another intuition is that an emotion concept (i.e., a mental representation of emotion) is distinct from the physiological changes, actions, and experience of an emotional event itself. These distinctions are called into question by recent accounts that offer a common computational framework for understanding how a brain predictively constructs thoughts, feelings, and other experiences in the service of action (Clark, 2013; Friston, 2010; Hohwy, 2013), and place concepts at the centre of the construction process (Barrett, 2017a, 2017b). In the first section of this paper, we introduce a constructionist, predictive coding account of brain function and consider its consequences for the relationship between cognition and emotion. In the second section, we discuss the hypothesis that emotion concepts are embodied, highly variable, and dynamic prediction signals. In the third and final section, we consider the role of language in the development and construction of emotion concepts.

Cognitions and Emotions are Constructed by the Brain as a Dynamic, Predictive Biological System

Cognition and emotion are often viewed as separate mental forces: at times opposing, at times interdependent (e.g., Clore & Huntsinger, 2007; Damasio & Carvalho, 2013). In many modern accounts, cognitions are hypothesised to cause or be caused by emotions (e.g., Lazarus, 1991; Oatley & Johnson-Laird, 1987; Ortony, Clore, & Collins, 1990; Schwarz & Clore, 1996). According to a causal appraisal theory (e.g., Scherer, 1999), for example, hearing a sudden noise while walking home in the dark would evoke cognitive evaluations (e.g., of threat), which then trigger the experience of fear: the racing heart and urge to run that motivate decisions and direct attention. Cognitions are also hypothesised to regulate emotions after the fact (e.g., Ochsner & Gross, 2005). Fear could be attenuated by considering the situation from a different perspective (e.g., it could be interesting wildlife), reinterpreting sensations (e.g., as excitement), remembering previous situations in which no harm occurred, etc. Phenomenologically, there seems to be a clear distinction between the aspects of such a scenario that correspond to emotion (e.g., physiological changes, actions, feelings of (un)pleasantness), and those that correspond to cognition (e.g., conscious decisions, memories, experiences of effort and volition). Consequently, scientific theories have traditionally assumed that emotions and cognitions are ontologically distinct categories of experience, generated by architecturally separate systems in the brain as it reacts to its environment.

Accumulating evidence does not support these assumptions, however (e.g., Duncan & Barrett, 2007). Instead, there is growing consensus that all experiences are constructed via the interaction of domain-general systems, in a brain that predictively, rather than reactively, guides behaviour. These recent accounts offer a common computational framework for how the brain guides action and makes meaning of sensation – to create cognitions, emotions, and perceptions – through the process of predictive coding (e.g., Barrett, 2017a, 2017b; Clark, 2013; Friston, 2010; Hohwy, 2013; Huang & Rao, 2011; Spratling, 2016).

A predictive coding account seeks to explain how the brain optimises energy efficiency while keeping the body’s physiological systems in balance. To minimise metabolic costs, the brain needs to infer the causes of the sensations it receives from both exteroceptive (world) and interoceptive (body) sensory channels. By accurately inferring causes, the brain can anticipate the needs of the body, and prepare to meet those needs before they arise (Sterling, 2012; Sterling & Laughlin, 2015). However, sensory input is noisy, incomplete, and can – like the sudden noise in the dark – have many different causes. According to a predictive coding account, the brain identifies which cause is most likely by comparing the current sensory array to prior experiences and determining what is most similar. As an internal model of the world (Buckner, 2012; Hassabis & Maguire, 2009), including the body and its internal milieu (Barrett & Simmons, 2015; Garfinkel, Seth, Barrett, Suzuki, & Critchley, 2015), the brain uses the statistical regularities of the past to predict which sensations are most probable in the future, and which actions are most beneficial to deal with those sensations (e.g., Barrett & Simmons, 2015; Chanes & Barrett, 2016).

Prediction is neither a deliberate nor a consciously accessible act. Rather, it is the process by which neurons communicate to generate behaviour and construct experience. Predictions prepare the brain by flexibly changing the firing of sensory and motor neurons in anticipation of the next moment (Denève & Jardri, 2016; Denève & Machens, 2016). These changes emerge as updated brain states, or patterns of distributed neural activity. Predictions also guide which sensory inputs are attended to and which are ignored. Anticipated inputs confirm predictions, categorising sensations and making them psychologically meaningful (Lochmann & Deneve, 2011). Unanticipated sensory inputs are prediction errors (the discrepancy between what was predicted and what actually occurred), creating an opportunity to modify the internal model, so the brain can predict more accurately in the future.

When past experiences of an emotion (e.g., fear) are the best fit for the current sensory array, the brain uses this emotion as its best guess at what will cause sensory inputs and what should be done about them. Once this prediction is sufficiently corrected by any prediction error, sensations are categorised and explained as emotion. That is, the emotion is understood as the cause of actions and physical changes in the body, giving rise to the folk intuition that emotions are central drivers of behaviour and experience. Cognitions, as well as perceptions, are constructed in a similar way (Huang & Rao, 2011; Spratling, 2016). What distinguishes between apparent categories of experience is the brain’s attentional focus, or which inputs are foregrounded (Barrett, Wilson-Mendenhall, & Barsalou, 2015). The experience of cognition occurs when the brain foregrounds mental contents and processes. The experience of emotion occurs when, in relation to the current situation, the brain foregrounds bodily changes. When walking home late at night, the brain may use past experience (of a sudden noise, an elevated pulse, the dark) to predict fear.

Every categorisation of sensation (e.g., as fear) updates the neural context in which the brain is making predictions for the body. The brain will subsequently prioritise perceptions, actions, emotions, and cognitions that have previously been reinforced in similar situations. This iterative process of constructing and confirming predictions gives rise to the folk intuition that cognition and emotion cause one another or compete for control. For instance, when a cognition (e.g., mental speech) precedes a change in emotion (e.g., attenuated fear), this is understood as emotion regulation. However, a predictive coding account argues that regulation does not exist separately from construction. Rather than having separate causes, cognitions and emotions are constructed based on the temporal dynamics of the brain (Spivey, 2007). As the brain transitions through all possible patterns of neural activation (i.e., state space), the current brain state, in combination with inputs from the body and world, influences the probability of future brain states (Barrett, 2009). A predictive coding account therefore revises hypotheses about the relationship between cognition and emotion. Cognition does not control emotion in a top-down fashion, nor do emotions provoke cognitions; the transition from one to the other occurs in an uninterrupted, domain-general meaning-making process (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Schematic depiction of the dynamics of a mental event (e.g., an instance of emotional experience) from prediction to categorisation. Blue lines indicate top-down signal; red lines indicate bottom-up signal. Based on the current brain state, previous experience is used to generate a cascade of predictions focused on meeting the body’s expected needs for action (i.e., allostasis). As depicted, changes in sensory input (i.e., prediction error) may result in further tuning of the predictions. When predictions are confirmed, the current sensory array has been categorised and a new brain state instantiated. In turn, visceromotor changes and actions impact sensory inputs from the body and world, respectively. Current experience also updates the internal model, becoming part of the previous experience that will be brought to bear in future predictions.

A predictive coding account has many implications for the study of experience. For one, it suggests that traditional laboratory paradigms may limit the generalisability of experimental findings to real-world predictive processing. These paradigms typically present randomised sequences of stimulus and response, with trials treated as independent so they can be analysed in aggregate. As such, they put a continuously predicting brain into an unnatural environment, disrupting rather than modelling the temporal dependencies inherent in brain function. A predictive coding account suggests that experience is better assessed using a holistic approach, in which continuous measures of activity in the brain and body are used to capture cognitions and emotions unfolding over time (e.g., Ariff, Donchin, Nanayakkara, & Shadmehr, 2002; Müller et al., 2008), and at different levels of analysis (e.g., Mack, Preston, & Love, 2013; Purcell et al., 2010). Using computational models that account for complex, nonlinear dynamics (e.g., Friston, Harrison, & Penny, 2003; McClelland et al., 2010; Pezzulo et al., 2013), scientists can examine behaviour and experience as the brain continues on its probabilistic trajectory through state space. These recommendations lend themselves to empirically testable hypotheses and questions about the nature of cognition and emotion (Table 1, numbers 1–3).

Table 1.

Empirically Testable Hypotheses and Questions Generated by a Predictive Coding Account of Cognition and Emotion

Cognitions and Emotions are Constructed by the Brain as a Dynamic, Predictive Biological System
  • 1

    Hypotheses: Continuous measures of neural activity (e.g., EEG, fMRI) will reveal that the spatiotemporal patterns for instances of the same category of mental event (e.g., fear) vary from one another as much as from instances of different categories. Similarly, detailed self-report measures will reveal variation in the associated mental features.

  • 2

    Questions: How do phenomenological boundaries in the experience of cognitive and emotional events (e.g., Zacks & Swallow, 2007) map to continuous measures of neural activity? Are the same boundaries observed in cultures where there is no linguistic distinction made between ‘thinking’ and ‘feeling’ (e.g., Ifaluk ‘nunuwan’; Lutz, 1985)?

  • 3

    Hypothesis: Brain states, and their associated mental events, evidence properties of complex, non-linear, dynamical systems (e.g, 1/f scaling, fractality; Richardson & Chemero, 2014).

Concepts, as Predictions, are Intrinsically Embodied and Highly Variable
  • 4

    Hypotheses: When measured at an idiographic level, the mental and physical features of emotion categories might be more consistent and specific than at a nomothetic level. There will be individual differences in the number of emotion categories and variability of their instances.

  • 5

    Hypothesis: The physical features and internal bodily sensations associated with categories of mental events (e.g., Nummenmaa, Hari, Hietanen, & Glerean, 2018) will vary across cultures.

  • 6

    Question: Which leads to more efficient physiological regulation: increasing variability in category instances (i.e., within-category diversity for a given emotion category such as fear), or increasing the precision and number of emotion categories (i.e., more fine grained categories with less variation from one another)?

Language Plays a Central Role in the Development and Construction of Concepts
  • 7

    Questions: How is the conceptual system (i.e., the brain’s internal model) updated when new emotion words are acquired, either by observation or instruction, and how does this impact embodied experience?

  • 8

    Hypotheses: Increased similarity in individuals’ momentary emotion concepts (and therefore in their emotional experiences and perceptions) will result in synchrony, as well as decreased interpersonal tension and associated metabolic costs.

  • 9

    Question: What is the most effective way to teach emotion concepts (e.g., Maurer & Brackett, 2004) to improve cross-cultural communication and acculturation?

Concepts, as Predictions, are Intrinsically Embodied and Highly Variable

Scientific theories have traditionally assumed that a firm boundary exists between categories and concepts. Members of a category are instances, events, or objects that exist in the natural world, while a concept is a mental representation of that category inside the head (for reviews, see Goldstone & Kersten, 2003; Smith & Medin, 1981).1 For example, the concept of ‘fear’ is dissociable from the actions and sensations of actual fear events. In many of these views, concepts are considered relatively stable objects of cognition that have a set of automatically-activated, context-independent properties (for reviews, see Lebois, Wilson‐Mendenhall, & Barsalou, 2015; Mahon & Hickok, 2016). The central features of ‘fear’ (e.g., typical physical sensations, behaviours, affect) are maintained regardless of whether it is instantiated when walking home in the dark or giving a public speech. Accordingly, concepts are understood as amodal symbols that operate independently of the brain’s systems for perception and action (e.g., Mahon, 2015).

In contrast, a predictive coding account is consistent with proposals that concepts and categories are constructed ad hoc, according to situation-specific functions (Barsalou, 1991, 2003; Barsalou, Simmons, Barbey, & Wilson, 2003; Casasanto & Lupyan, 2015). In these views, concepts are multimodal, grounded simulations represented by the activation of the same neurons that underlie sensation and movement (e.g., Barsalou, 2008; Kan, Barsalou, Olseth Solomon, Minor, & Thompson-Schill, 2003; Pulvermüller & Fadiga, 2010). A concept’s features are fully context dependent: when walking home in the dark, ‘fear’ may involve a racing heart and the propensity to shriek; when giving a public speech, ‘fear’ may involve a tense stomach and a stammering voice. In other words, concepts are the predictions that the brain uses to categorise sensory inputs and motor actions (Barrett, 2017a, 2017b). When the brain constructs an emotion concept, the result is emotional meaning. In turn, these category members become part of the internal model used as a basis for future predictions (Hoemann, Gendron, & Barrett, 2017). The emotion categories that emerge from this process are conceptual categories (Barrett, 2012), in that within-category similarities and between-category differences are not based on perceptual features, but imposed by the brain according to the function that category serves.

The core task of the brain is to keep the body’s physiological systems in balance (Sterling, 2012). Because of this, all concepts – whether they deal with emotion or not – involve information about the body in the world (Barrett, 2017a, 2017b). Accordingly, emotion concepts are partial re-enactments of visceromotor, motor, and other sensory changes that were engaged in past emotional experiences (e.g., Niedenthal, Barsalou, Winkielman, Krauth-Gruber, & Ric, 2005). Brain areas responsible for movement and physiological regulation are activated by emotion words (Moseley et al., 2015), and observing and producing a smile activate the same facial muscles (Foroni & Semin, 2009). Embodiment also plays a constitutive role in the construction of emotion concepts (Niedenthal, 2007). Deficits in emotion perception are observed after spontaneous activity of associated facial muscles is inhibited (e.g., Niedenthal, Brauer, Halberstadt, & Innes-Ker, 2001), and after neural processes associated with embodiment are disrupted via transcranial magnetic stimulation (Pitcher, Garrido, Walsh, & Duchaine, 2008). Further, emotional experiences are constructed by brain networks involved in implementing emotion concepts (Lindquist, Wager, Kober, Bliss-Moreau, & Barrett, 2012; Wilson-Mendenhall, Barrett, & Barsalou, 2015; Wilson-Mendenhall, Barrett, Simmons, & Barsalou, 2011) – the same networks that contain the visceromotor circuitry that regulates the systems of the body (Kleckner et al., 2017).

When the brain constructs embodied, context-sensitive emotion concepts, it produces variation in the associated physiological and motor responses – a heterogeneity that is apparent in meta-analyses of faces (Barrett, Adolphs, Marsella, Martinez, & Pollak, forthcoming), brains (e.g., Clark-Polner, Wager, Satpute, & Barrett, 2016), and bodies (Siegel et al., 2018). Notably, significant variation within emotion categories has been observed across 202 studies measuring autonomic nervous system activity during lab-based inductions (Siegel et al, 2018). Patterns of activity did not consistently or specifically distinguish between emotion categories (e.g., both anger and fear inductions resulted in increased heart rate when compared to a neutral baseline, but with significant statistical heterogeneity; see also Cacioppo, Berntson, Larsen, Poehlmann, & Ito, 2000; Stemmler, 2004). This variability could not be fully explained by induction method or other experimental moderators (Siegel et al, 2018).2 Likewise, brain activations for the same emotion have been shown to differ as a function of situation-specific features (Wilson-Mendenhall et al., 2015; Wilson-Mendenhall et al., 2011). These descriptive features (mental, physical, internal, external) do not overlap completely with other instances in the same emotion category, but can (and often do) occur in other emotion categories (Hoemann et al., 2017; Wilson-Mendenhall, Barrett, & Barsalou, 2013). A particular instance of ‘fear’ may be more similar to an instance of ‘anger’ (e.g., both involve social threat and intense stomach sensations) than to another instance of ‘fear’ (e.g., that involves pleasant thrill-seeking, such as a haunted house).

A predictive coding account considers this variation meaningful rather than random, which carries implications for theory and measurement. Foremost, it suggests that studies must account for individual and context-based variation. Although one might argue that variation discredits physiological perspectives on emotion and cognition, we disagree. Emotion concepts have a biological basis, even if emotion categories do not cut nature at its joints with distinct, diagnostic sets of features. Physiological variation in emotional experience is functional: it occurs because concepts are created to meet body’s present and predicted metabolic needs. By acknowledging variation, theories can generalise beyond the lab and account for the vicissitudes of everyday life. A predictive coding account suggests that experience is better assessed using an idiographic approach, in which experience sampling methods (e.g., Conner & Mehl, 2015; Nezlek, Vansteelandt, Van Mechelen, & Kuppens, 2008) are used to test whether consistent and specific emotion categories exist within individuals (Table 1, number 4). Variation within a given emotion category can be modelled by manipulating fine-grained contextual features (e.g., situational demands; Wilson-Mendenhall et al., 2011), allowing scientists to better map cross-cultural variation (Table 1, number 5). By modelling individual differences in momentary experience and physiology, scientists can assess person-specific impacts for mental and physical health (e.g., Barrett, 2017a; Kashdan, Barrett, & McKnight, 2015; Lumley, Beyer, & Radcliffe, 2008), as well as design targeted interventions (Table 1, number 6).

Language Plays a Central Role in the Development and Construction of Concepts

Variation poses a challenge: the brain needs a way to learn the statistical regularities necessary to make accurate predictions. Language may serve this purpose, playing a key role in concept acquisition by directing attention and communicating intentionality (Chen & Waxman, 2013; Ferry, Hespos, & Waxman, 2010; Gelman, 2009). Words serve as invitations to make meaning from sensory input, creating similarity between exemplars that do not share perceptual features (e.g., Graham, Booth, & Waxman, 2012). Emotion categories may be especially reliant on the cohesion provided by words to achieve conceptual consistency. Contrary to accounts that discrete emotion concepts such as ‘fear’ and ‘anger’ are a form of inborn or early-to-develop knowledge (e.g., Izard, 1994; Kobiella, Grossmann, Reid, & Striano, 2008), data suggest they develop gradually across childhood as the brain learns from experience. Emotion categories and their corresponding words are initially applied broadly and then their use narrows over time, suggesting concepts are being refined (e.g., Widen & Russell, 2003, 2008). While young children anchor on valence-based information (pleasure, displeasure), adults have a more elaborated, multidimensional organisation that includes arousal (i.e., level of activation). This conceptual development has been shown to be uniquely mediated by increasing verbal knowledge (Nook, Sasse, Lambert, McLaughlin, & Somerville, 2017), further underscoring the role of language in emotional learning.

Language may also play an active role in shaping experience (for reviews, see Boroditsky, 2010; Lupyan, 2012). Rather than a means of simply activating stored knowledge, words are a special type of sensory input in the predictive process (Elman, 2009; Lupyan & Clark, 2015). Words highlight functional similarity between past and present experiences, forming networks of semantic associations, such that hearing the word “fear” may cue prior experiences of ‘anxiety’, ‘tense stomach’, or ‘public speaking’. As such, words create a flexible context for the online construction of concepts (Barrett, 2017a; Casasanto & Lupyan, 2015). Hearing the word “fear” while preparing a public speech may make the construction of ‘nervous’ more likely; while riding a roller coaster, it may encourage ‘thrilled’. The brain uses words to tune prediction, as shown by studies of object recognition (e.g., Boutonnet & Lupyan, 2015; Lupyan & Thompson-Schill, 2012), category learning (e.g., Lupyan & Casasanto, 2014; Lupyan, Rakison, & McClelland, 2007), and visual awareness (e.g., Lupyan & Ward, 2013; Ostarek & Huettig, 2017). These effects have recently been demonstrated for the prediction, perception, and memory of emotional expressions (Chanes, Wormwood, Betz, & Barrett, 2018; Doyle & Lindquist, 2018; Fugate, Gendron, Nakashima, & Barrett, 2017). Further, labelling or writing about emotional experiences can help reduce their intensity, with important therapeutic implications (e.g., Kircanski, Lieberman, & Craske, 2012; Lieberman et al., 2007; Pennebaker, 1997).

Language structures both individual and shared experience. Emotions can be shared through language, allowing predictions to be collectively constructed (e.g., Rimé, 2007, 2009). Concepts are inherited through language: through devices such as labels (e.g., “fear”) and generic statements (e.g., “people scream in fear”), language aligns concepts and cultural practices across generations (Gelman & Roberts, 2017). For example, as children hear their parents use emotion labels in a variety of perceptually dissimilar situations (e.g., “fear” as applied to both public speaking and the dark), they come to associate these instances as functionally similar. This implies that both the ontological and evolutionary development of emotion concepts are shaped by the language practices in a given culture (e.g., Richerson & Boyd, 2005). Future work can investigate how emotion concepts systematically influence the brain (Kitayama & Salvador, 2017) as well as the body (Niedenthal, Winkielman, Mondillon, & Vermeulen, 2009; Seth, 2013) by examining the social and linguistic context of their use (for discussion, see Barrett, 2017a; Gendron, Mesquita, & Barrett, in press) (Table 1, number 7). Moreover, emotion concepts might function as a tool for cultural coordination (e.g., De Leersnyder, Boiger, & Mesquita, 2013; Mesquita, Boiger, & De Leersnyder, 2016), helping individuals physiologically regulate one another (Barrett, 2017a) (Table 1, number 8). Interventions designed to teach emotion language and concepts (e.g., Hagelskamp, Brackett, Rivers, & Salovey, 2013) may therefore lead to shifts in emotional meaning-making, facilitating communication and acculturation (Table 1, number 9).

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to J. Theriault for his comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.

Funding

The paper was supported by grants to from the U.S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences (W911NF-16-1-0191), the National Cancer Institute (U01 CA193632) and the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH113234 and R01 MH109464) to L.F. Barrett; and the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (1 F31 HL140943–01) to K. Hoemann. The views, opinions, and/or findings contained in this paper are those of the authors and shall not be construed as an official U.S. Department of the Army position, policy, or decision, unless so designated by other documents.

Footnotes

Declaration of Interest Statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

1

There are, of course, exceptions to this theoretical assumption. For example, Fiske and Neuberg’s (1990) model of impression formation regards both concepts and categories as mental constructs. This model is in keeping with our definition of conceptual categories.

2

Even studies that use identical methods have been unable to replicate multivariate pattern classifiers across experiments (e.g., Stephens, Christie, & Friedman, 2010 vs. Kragel & LaBar, 2013).

References

  1. Ariff G, Donchin O, Nanayakkara T, & Shadmehr R (2002). A real-time state predictor in motor control: study of saccadic eye movements during unseen reaching movements. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(17), 7721–7729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barrett LF (2009). The future of psychology: Connecting mind to brain. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(4), 326–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01134.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barrett LF (2012). Emotions are real. Emotion, 12, 413–429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barrett LF (2017a). How Emotions are Made: The Secret Life the Brain and What It Means for Your Health, the Law, and Human Nature. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. [Google Scholar]
  5. Barrett LF (2017b). The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1–23. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barrett LF, Adolphs R, Marsella S, Martinez A, & Pollak S (forthcoming)Emotional expressions reconsidered: Challenges to inferring emotion in human facial movements. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Barrett LF, & Simmons WK (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(7), 419–429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barrett LF, Wilson-Mendenhall CD, & Barsalou LW (2015). The Conceptual Act Theory: A roadmap In Barrett LF & Russell JA (Eds.), The Psychological Construction of Emotion. New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  9. Barsalou LW (1991). Deriving categories to achieve goals. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 27, 1–64. [Google Scholar]
  10. Barsalou LW (2003). Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 513–562. [Google Scholar]
  11. Barsalou LW (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Barsalou LW, Simmons WK, Barbey AK, & Wilson CD (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 84–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Boroditsky L (2010). How the languages we speak shape the ways we think: The FAQs In Spivey MJ, McRae K, & Joanisse M (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 615–632). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  14. Boutonnet B, & Lupyan G (2015). Words jump-start vision: A label advantage in object recognition. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(25), 9329–9335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Buckner RL (2012). The serendipitous discovery of the brain’s default network. Neuroimage, 62(2), 1137–1145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cacioppo JT, Berntson GG, Larsen JH, Poehlmann KM, & Ito TA (2000). The psychophysiology of emotion In Lewis R & Haviland-Jones JM (Eds.), The Handbook of Emotion (2 ed., pp. 173–191). New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  17. Casasanto D, & Lupyan G (2015). All concepts are ad hoc concepts In Margolis E & Laurence S (Eds.), The Conceptual Mind: New Directions in the Study of Concepts (pp. 543–566). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  18. Chanes L, & Barrett LF (2016). Redefining the role of limbic areas in cortical processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(2), 96–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chanes L, Wormwood JB, Betz N, & Barrett LF (2018). Facial expression predictions as drivers of social perception. Journal of personality and social psychology, 114(3), 380–396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chen ML, & Waxman SR (2013). “Shall we blick?”: Novel words highlight actors’ underlying intentions for 14-month-old infants. Developmental psychology, 49(3), 426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Clark-Polner E, Wager TD, Satpute AB, & Barrett LF (2016). Neural fingerprinting: Meta-analysis, variation, and the search for brain-based essences in the science of emotion In Barrett LF, Lewis M, & Haviland-Jones JM (Eds.), The Handbook of Emotion (4th ed.). New York: New York: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  22. Clark A (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 281–253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Clore GL, & Huntsinger JR (2007). How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(9), 393–399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Conner TS, & Mehl MR (2015). Ambulatory assessment - Methods for studying everyday life In Scott R, Kosslyn S, & Pinkerton N (Eds.), Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences: An Interdisciplinary, Searchable, and Linkable Resource (pp. 1–15). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  25. Damasio A, & Carvalho GB (2013). The nature of feelings: evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(2), 143–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. De Leersnyder J, Boiger M, & Mesquita B (2013). Cultural regulation of emotion: Individual, relational, and structural sources. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(55), 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Denève S, & Jardri R (2016). Circular inference: mistaken belief, misplaced trust. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 11, 40–48. [Google Scholar]
  28. Denève S, & Machens CK (2016). Efficient codes and balanced networks. Nature neuroscience, 19(3), 375–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Doyle CM, & Lindquist KA (2018). When a word Is worth a thousand pictures: Language shapes perceptual memory for emotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(1), 62–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Duncan S, & Barrett LF (2007). Affect is a form of cognition: A neurobiological analysis. Cognition and emotion, 21(6), 1184–1211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Elman JL (2009). On the meaning of words and dinosaur bones: Lexical knowledge without a lexicon. Cognitive Science, 33(4), 547–582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ferry AL, Hespos SJ, & Waxman SR (2010). Categorization in 3‐and 4‐month‐old infants: an advantage of words over tones. Child development, 81(2), 472–479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fiske ST, & Neuberg SL (1990). A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1–74): Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
  34. Foroni F, & Semin GR (2009). Language that puts you in touch with your bodily feelings: The multimodal responsiveness of affective expressions. Psychological science, 20(8), 974–980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Friston K (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 127–138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Friston K, Harrison L, & Penny W (2003). Dynamic causal modelling. Neuroimage, 19(4), 1273–1302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fugate J, Gendron M, Nakashima S, & Barrett LF (2017). Emotion words: Adding face value. Emotion, Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/emo0000330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Garfinkel SN, Seth AK, Barrett AB, Suzuki K, & Critchley HD (2015). Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biological psychology, 104, 65–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gelman SA (2009). Learning from others: Children’s construction of concepts. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 115–140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gelman SA, & Roberts SO (2017). How language shapes the cultural inheritance of categories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 7900–7907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gendron M, Mesquita B, & Barrett LF (in press). Concepts, actions and experiences within the human affective niche In Kirmayer L, Kitayama S, Lemelson R, & Worthman C (Eds.), Culture, Mind, and Brain. [Google Scholar]
  42. Goldstone RL, & Kersten A (2003). Concepts and categorization In Healy AF & Proctor RW (Eds.), Handbook of psychology (Vol. IV, pp. 597–621): John Wiley & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  43. Graham SA, Booth AE, & Waxman SR (2012). Words are not merely features: Only consistently applied nouns guide 4-year-olds’ inferences about object categories. Language Learning and Development, 8(2), 136–145. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2011.599304 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hagelskamp C, Brackett MA, Rivers SE, & Salovey P (2013). Improving classroom quality with the RULER approach to social and emotional learning: Proximal and distal outcomes. American Journal of Community Psychology, 51(3–4), 530–543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hassabis D, & Maguire EA (2009). The construction system of the brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 1263–1271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hoemann K, Gendron M, & Barrett LF (2017). Mixed emotions in the predictive brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 15, 51–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hohwy J (2013). The Predictive Mind: OUP Oxford. [Google Scholar]
  48. Huang Y, & Rao RP (2011). Predictive coding. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(5), 580–593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Izard CE (1994). Innate and universal facial expressions: evidence from developmental and cross-cultural research. Psychological bulletin, 115(2), 288–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kan IP, Barsalou LW, Olseth Solomon K, Minor JK, & Thompson-Schill SL (2003). Role of mental imagery in a property verification task: fMRI evidence for perceptual representations of conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3–6), 525–540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kashdan TB, Barrett LF, & McKnight PE (2015). Unpacking emotion differentiation: Transforming unpleasant experience by perceiving distinctions in negativity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(1), 10–16. doi: 10.1177/0963721414550708 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kircanski K, Lieberman MD, & Craske MG (2012). Feelings into words: Contributions of language to exposure therapy. Psychological science, 23(10), 1086–1091. doi: 10.1177/0956797612443830 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kitayama S, & Salvador CE (2017). Culture embrained: Going beyond the nature-nurture dichotomy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(5), 841–854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kleckner IR, Zhang J, Touroutoglou A, Chanes L, Xia C, Simmons WK, … Barrett, L. F. (2017). Evidence for an intrinsic brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans. Nature Human Behavior(1), 0069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kobiella A, Grossmann T, Reid VM, & Striano T (2008). The discrimination of angry and fearful facial expressions in 7-month-old infants: An event-related potential study. Cognition and emotion, 22(1), 134–146. [Google Scholar]
  56. Kragel PA, & LaBar KS (2013). Multivariate pattern classification reveals autonomic and experiential representations of discrete emotions. Emotion, 13(4), 681–690. doi: 10.1037/a0031820 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Lazarus RS (1991). Cognition and motivation in emotion. American Psychologist, 46(4), 352–367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Lebois LA, Wilson‐Mendenhall CD, & Barsalou LW (2015). Are automatic conceptual cores the gold standard of semantic processing? The context‐dependence of spatial meaning in grounded congruency effects. Cognitive Science, 39(8), 1764–1801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Lieberman MD, Eisenberger NI, Crockett MJ, Tom SM, Pfeifer JH, & Way BM (2007). Putting feelings into words: Affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. Psychological science, 18(5), 421–428. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01916.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lindquist KA, Wager TD, Kober H, Bliss-Moreau E, & Barrett LF (2012). The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(3), 121–143. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X11000446 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lochmann T, & Deneve S (2011). Neural processing as causal inference. Current opinion in neurobiology, 21(5), 774–781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lumley M, Beyer J, & Radcliffe A (2008). Alexithymia and physical health problems: A critique of potential pathways and a research agenda. Emotion Regulation, 43–68. [Google Scholar]
  63. Lupyan G (2012). Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: The label-feedback hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(54), 1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lupyan G, & Casasanto D (2014). Meaningless words promote meaningful categorization. Language and Cognition, 7(02), 167–193. doi: 10.1017/langcog.2014.21 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lupyan G, & Clark A (2015). Words and the world predictive coding and the language-perception-cognition interface. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(4), 279–284. [Google Scholar]
  66. Lupyan G, Rakison DH, & McClelland JL (2007). Language is not just for talking: Redundant labels facilitate learning of novel categories. Psychological science, 18(12), 1077–1083. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02028.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Lupyan G, & Thompson-Schill SL (2012). The evocative power of words: Activation of concepts by verbal and nonverbal means. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 170–186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Lupyan G, & Ward EJ (2013). Language can boost otherwise unseen objects into visual awareness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(35), 14196–14201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Lutz C (1985). Ethnopsychology compared to what? Explaining behavior and consciousness among the Ifaluk In White GM & Kirkpatrick J (Eds.), Person, Self, and Experience: Exploring Pacific Ethnopsychologies (pp. 35–79). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  70. Mack ML, Preston AR, & Love BC (2013). Decoding the brain’s algorithm for categorization from its neural implementation. Current Biology, 23(20), 2023–2027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Mahon BZ (2015). What is embodied about cognition? Language, cognition and neuroscience, 30(4), 420–429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Mahon BZ, & Hickok G (2016). Arguments about the nature of concepts: Symbols, embodiment, and beyond. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 23(4), 941–958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Maurer M, & Brackett MA (2004). Emotional Literacy in the Middle School: A 6-Step Program to Promote Social, Emotional, and Academic Learning. Port Chester, NY: National Professional Resources. [Google Scholar]
  74. McClelland JL, Botvinick MM, Noelle DC, Plaut DC, Rogers TT, Seidenberg MS, & Smith LB (2010). Letting structure emerge: connectionist and dynamical systems approaches to cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(8), 348–356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Mesquita B, Boiger M, & De Leersnyder J (2016). The cultural construction of emotions. Current opinion in psychology, 8, 31–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Moseley RL, Shtyrov Y, Mohr B, Lombardo MV, Baron-Cohen S, & Pulvermüller F (2015). Lost for emotion words: What motor and limbic brain activity reveals about autism and semantic theory. Neuroimage, 104, 413–422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Müller K-R, Tangermann M, Dornhege G, Krauledat M, Curio G, & Blankertz B (2008). Machine learning for real-time single-trial EEG-analysis: from brain–computer interfacing to mental state monitoring. Journal of neuroscience methods, 167(1), 82–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Nezlek JB, Vansteelandt K, Van Mechelen I, & Kuppens P (2008). Appraisal-emotion relationships in daily life. Emotion, 8(1), 145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Niedenthal PM (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316(5827), 1002–1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Niedenthal PM, Barsalou LW, Winkielman P, Krauth-Gruber S, & Ric F (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and social psychology review, 9(3), 184–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Niedenthal PM, Brauer M, Halberstadt JB, & Innes-Ker ÅH (2001). When did her smile drop? Facial mimicry and the influences of emotional state on the detection of change in emotional expression. Cognition and emotion, 15(6), 853–864. [Google Scholar]
  82. Niedenthal PM, Winkielman P, Mondillon L, & Vermeulen N (2009). Embodiment of emotion concepts. Journal of personality and social psychology, 96(6), 1120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Nook EC, Sasse SF, Lambert HK, McLaughlin KA, & Somerville LH (2017). Increasing verbal knowledge mediates development of multidimensional emotion representations. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(12), 881–889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Nummenmaa L, Hari R, Hietanen JK, & Glerean E (2018). Maps of subjective feelings. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(37), 9198–9203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Oatley K, & Johnson-Laird PN (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotions. Cognition and emotion, 1(1), 29–50. [Google Scholar]
  86. Ochsner KN, & Gross JJ (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Ortony A, Clore GL, & Collins A (1990). The Cognitive Structure of Emotions: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  88. Ostarek M, & Huettig F (2017). Spoken words can make the invisible visible-Testing the involvement of low-level visual representations in spoken word processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(3), 499–508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Pennebaker JW (1997). Writing about emotional experiences as a therapeutic process. Psychological science, 8(3), 162–166. [Google Scholar]
  90. Pezzulo G, Barsalou L, Cangelosi A, Fischer M, McRae K, & Spivey M (2013). Computational Grounded Cognition: A new alliance between grounded cognition and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(612), 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00612 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Pitcher D, Garrido L, Walsh V, & Duchaine BC (2008). Transcranial magnetic stimulation disrupts the perception and embodiment of facial expressions. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(36), 8929–8933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Pulvermüller F, & Fadiga L (2010). Active perception: Sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(5), 351–360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Purcell B, Heitz R, Cohen J, Schall J, Logan G, & Palmeri T (2010). Neurally constrained modeling of perceptual decision making. Psychological review, 117(4), 1113–1143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Richardson MJ, & Chemero A (2014). Complex dynamical systems and embodiment In Shapiro L (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition (pp. 39–50): Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  95. Richerson PJ, & Boyd R (2005). Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  96. Rimé B (2007). The social sharing of emotion as an interface between individual and collective processes in the construction of emotional climates. Journal of social issues, 63(2), 307–322. [Google Scholar]
  97. Rimé B (2009). Emotion elicits the social sharing of emotion: Theory and empirical review. Emotion Review, 1(1), 60–85. [Google Scholar]
  98. Scherer KR (1999). Appraisal theory. In Dalgleish T & Power MJ (Eds.), Handbook of Cognition and Emotion (pp. 637–663). Chichester, England: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  99. Schwarz N, & Clore GL (1996). Feelings and phenomenal experiences In Kruglanski A & Higgins ET (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (pp. 433–465). New York: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  100. Seth AK (2013). Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(11), 565–573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Siegel EH, Sands MK, Condon P, Chang Y, Dy J, Quigley KS, & Barrett LF (2018). Emotion fingerprints or emotion populations? A meta-analytic investigation of autonomic features of emotion categories. Psychological bulletin, 144(4), 343–393. doi: 10.1037/bul0000128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Smith EE, & Medin DL (1981). Categories and Concepts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  103. Spivey MJ (2007). The Continuity of Mind. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  104. Spratling MW (2016). Predictive coding as a model of cognition. Cognitive processing, 17(3), 279–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Stemmler G (2004). Physiological processes during emotion In Phillipot P & Feldman RS (Eds.), The regulation of emotion (pp. 48–85): Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  106. Stephens CL, Christie IC, & Friedman BH (2010). Autonomic specificity of basic emotions: evidence from pattern classification and cluster analysis. Biological psychology, 84(3), 463–473. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Sterling P (2012). Allostasis: A model of predictive regulation. Physiology & Behavior, 106(1), 5–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Sterling P, & Laughlin S (2015). Principles of Neural Design: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  109. Widen SC, & Russell JA (2003). A closer look at preschoolers’ freely produced labels for facial expressions. Developmental psychology, 39(1), 114–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Widen SC, & Russell JA (2008). Children acquire emotion categories gradually. Cognitive development, 23(2), 291–312. [Google Scholar]
  111. Wilson-Mendenhall CD, Barrett LF, & Barsalou LW (2013). Situating emotional experience. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Wilson-Mendenhall CD, Barrett LF, & Barsalou LW (2015). Variety in emotional life: within-category typicality of emotional experiences is associated with neural activity in large-scale brain networks. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(1), 62–71. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Wilson-Mendenhall CD, Barrett LF, Simmons WK, & Barsalou LW (2011). Grounding emotion in situated conceptualization. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1105–1127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Zacks JM, & Swallow KM (2007). Event segmentation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 80–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES