Abstract
Active avoidance is the prototypical paradigm for studying aversively-motivated instrumental behavior. However, avoidance research stalled amid heated theoretical debates and the hypothesis that active avoidance is essentially Pavlovian flight. Here I reconsider key “avoidance problems” and review neurobehavioral data collected with modern tools. Although the picture remains incomplete, these studies strongly suggest that avoidance has an instrumental component and is mediated by brain circuits that resemble appetitive instrumental actions more than Pavlovian fear reactions. Rapid progress may be possible if investigators consider important factors like safety signals, response-competition, goal-directed vs. habitual control and threat imminence in avoidance study design. Since avoidance responses likely contribute to active coping, this research has important implications for understanding human resilience and disorders of control.
Introduction.
Active avoidance responses (ARs) are learned defensive behaviors that escape or prevent aversive stimuli. “Avoidance” has a negative connotation, but AR learning may be a powerful coping mechanism, giving subjects the flexibility to suppress innate defensive reactions like fleeing, fighting or freezing in favor of more adaptive actions. AR learning may underlie basic protective behaviors to distant or uncertain threats, like wearing a helmet to prevent head injury or paying taxes to prevent an audit. It may also help people deal more effectively with intense threats. For instance, first responders and soldiers undergo extensive training to suppress fear reactions and select actions that produce a better outcome in lifethreatening situations. However, dysfunctional ARs may also contribute to disorders of control. In OCD and addiction, compulsive actions that escape anxiety or withdrawal symptoms are repeated despite punishing consequences and even cognitive awareness that they are maladaptive [1–4].
Active avoidance was the dominant paradigm for studying aversive conditioning and defensive behavior for much of the last century. The field lost momentum in 1980s, however, when simpler Pavlovian paradigms rapidly advanced psychological and neurobiological explanations of defensive behavior. Avoidance has experienced something of a revival recently as model paradigm for studying aversively-motivated instrumental behavior [5] (but see Figure 1). However, these efforts have been dogged by unresolved theoretical debates and a general notion that avoidance brain research was unproductive in the past and likely will be again. Shifting focus to Pavlovian processes and the topography of defensive responding was the right move for the field at the time. Today an impressive body of literature details the brain systems, microcircuits, and physiological/molecular mechanisms of Pavlovian defensive responding. However, it is increasingly clear that Pavlovian processes alone cannot explain avoidance and many basic questions relevant to human psychopathology and resilience remain unanswered.
In this essay, I will briefly describe the avoidance paradigm and some key theoretical considerations. I will then focus on recent neurobiological data that may help reveal the “natural fracture lines” of avoidance behavior [6]. These studies suggest that rapid progress is possible if researchers apply modern neurobiological and behavioral tools to longstanding avoidance problems.
Active Avoidance Behavior.
In signaled active avoidance (SigAA), subjects receive warning signals (WSs; e.g. tone) that predict an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US; e.g. footshock; Figure 2). However, unlike Pavlovian conditioning, subjects can exert control over events in the SigAA paradigm: they can escape the WS and prevent the US by emitting a specific action determined by the experimenter (e.g. shuttle or barpress). WS-US pairings occur early in training, before the subject gains exposure to instrumental contingencies, and Pavlovian reactions like freezing dominate. As training progresses and subjects gain accidental exposure to instrumental contingencies, ARs emerge and Pavlovian reactions are drastically diminished [7–13].
Theoretical Considerations.
From the beginning, avoidance posed a problem for theoretical accounts of learned behavior, mainly because learning occurred on trials where the US was omitted. It was difficult to understand how a nonevent could support learning, especially since US omission does not coincide in time with the AR (at the time, cognitive expectancies were not widely accepted). The most influential account of SigAA was two-factor fear theory [14,15]. According to fear theory, rats first acquired conditional fear through Pavlovian conditioning (Factor 1). Then, on later trials, an instrumental stimulus-response (WS-AR) association was reinforced by “fear reduction” associated with WS-termination (Factor 2). Conditional fear was an acquired drive and (negative) reinforcement occurred because of drive reduction. Fear theory also hypothesized a motivational role for the WS: once learning occurred, WS presentations triggered fear and motivated ARs that reduce fear. Importantly, fear theory solved the problem of explaining how a response could be reinforced by the nonoccurrence of an aversive US. As Seligman and Johnston [16] explain it: “rather than responding to ward off a shock in the future, an “avoider” is actually escaping from fear evoking stimuli in the present”, which puts the AR in contact with the reinforcing event (WS-termination).
Although enormously influential, fear theory eventually ran into significant difficulty on several fronts. First, several groups demonstrated avoidance learning with no explicit WS [17]. Other experiments found that both WS-termination and US-omission contribute to learning, and the relative contribution depended on the task [18]. It was also criticized for its reliance on WSinduced “fear”, mainly because researchers found little correspondence between the level of fear (i.e. defensive and autonomic reactions) and ARs [7]. Evidence for fear-reduction at WStermination was also lacking [19]. Lastly, avoidance is extremely resistant to extinction; when shock is turned off ARs may continue to be emitted for thousands of trials [20].
The most damaging blow to active avoidance research came with Robert Bolles’ interpretation of ARs through the lens of SSDR (Species-Specific Defense Reactions) theory. Bolles was bothered that the most important determinant of SigAA learning was the response choice [18]. Initially, he suggested that aversive stimuli restrict behavior to innate SSDRs, and thus only SSDRs or responses compatible with SSDRs can be reinforced. However, subsequent experiments found that the most prominent rodent SSDRs, freezing and flight, were insensitive to reinforcement [18, 21]. Compared to yoked controls, freezing and locomotion are not increased when they delay or prevent shock delivery. Similarly, freezing is not decreased by punishment. Bolles then reasoned: if aversive stimuli restrict behavior to SSDRs, and SSDRs are insensitive to reinforcement, then response learning is not possible in the avoidance situation. Instead, apparent ARs likely represent SSDRs that come under stimulus control through Pavlovian learning, where rats flee threats to the safest spot available and then freeze [22]. Note that safety stimuli may include internally perceived stimuli such as proprioceptive feedback – so animals may approach (or flee towards) feedback stimuli by emitting the response. Although this explanation cannot easily account for “avoidance” with ratio schedules or arbitrary responses like bar-pressing [23,24], Bolles and others argue that such ARs require extensive training and are not a major component of defense (since animals could not have evolved a defense mechanism that allows for many unsuccessful encounters with a predator) [22].
Reconsidering Major Avoidance Problems.
Since the decline of avoidance research, there has been remarkable progress in the understanding of Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning with appetitive outcomes. Newer behavioral and neurobiological tools are already resolving longstanding questions about avoidance. Here I will reconsider past reasons for abandoning the avoidance paradigm in light of new data and models of learning, behavior and brain function. Although many brain regions have been implicated in avoidance, I will focus on the amygdala, striatum and medial prefrontal cortex because of their known roles in fear conditioning, instrumental learning and response selection. Unless otherwise noted, studies refer to active avoidance in rodents.
Is the avoidance paradigm useful for studying defensive brain circuits?
Early research on the role of the amygdala in aversive conditioning illustrates why neuroscientists favored the Pavlovian paradigm over avoidance. Conflicting reports suggested that amygdala damage could impair, facilitate or have no effect on ARs [25,26]. This contrasts with initial studies of fear conditioning. Independent laboratories, using different conditioned stimuli (CSs) and measuring different responses, provided converging evidence that basolateral (BLA) and central (CeA) amygdala were essential for the learning, storage and expression of Pavlovian fear memories [27]. However, recent work with more precise manipulations suggests that clarity is possible if experimenters account for competing Pavlovian responses and the amount of training.
Specificity of brain manipulations.
Many early studies investigating avoidance brain circuits targeted the entire amygdala with large lesions that inconsistently damaged amygdala subregions. More recent work evaluating specific amygdala nuclei paints a clearer picture. For instance, muscimol inactivation of BLA blocks wheel-turning ARs in a rabbit discrimination task [28] and platform-mediated avoidance in rats [13]. Consistent with this, selective lesions of lateral amygdala (LA) or basal amygdala (BA) severely impair the learning and performance of shuttlebox ARs [8,9]. Measures of neural activity also support a role for BLA; plasticity of WSevoked responses occurs rapidly with SigAA training and correlates with ARs [29]. Interestingly, lesions of CeA produce a very different profile: they have no effect in good avoiders (rats who acquired the AR without difficulty) but facilitate ARs in poor avoiders (rats emitting few ARs even after significant training) [8,9]. Considered with results from Pavlovian studies, this suggests that threat memories stored in LA can be used by to generate Pavlovian reactions (via CeA), or instrumental actions (via BA; Figure 3). They also illustrate how different labs using different tasks and subjects can provide converging neurobehavioral evidence when more precise methods are used.
Competition between incompatible responses.
Although early researchers recognized that incompatible responses like freezing interfered with avoidance, they treated this mostly as a nuisance and engineered chambers and tasks to minimize freezing. Bolles considered the freezing hypothesis of poor avoidance something of a lazy explanation, remarking “…the real issue is whether the animal fails to [avoid] because it is freezing, or freezes because it has not learned to [avoid]”, clearly favoring the latter description [18]. However, the CeA lesion data discussed above suggest that behavior in the avoidance chamber should be viewed as a competition between incompatible Pavlovian reactions and instrumental actions. In support of this, Pavlovian reactions like freezing are 1) inversely correlated with ARs, 2) unusually strong in poor avoiders, and 3) abolished by CeA lesions that reverse poor avoidance [8,9,11–13,30]. Note that CeA lesions in poor avoiders produce near-asymptotic avoidance behavior in the first post-lesion session, indicating that these rats acquired instrumental associations but failed to express them [8,9]. Thus, at least in shuttlebox tasks, rats fail to avoid because they freeze.
More recent studies examining response competition in avoidance have focused on infralimbic (IL) prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens (NAc). c-Fos in IL [12] and NAc [31] correlates positively with ARs and negatively with freezing after moderate avoidance training. To test whether IL suppresses CeA-mediated reactions during SigAA, Moscarello and LeDoux [11] evaluated permanent and temporary lesions in a rat shuttlebox task. Disrupting IL early in training impaired SigAA and produced corresponding increases in freezing. An opposite pattern was observed with CeA inactivation. Subsequent work by Greg Quirk’s group evaluated IL in a novel platform-mediated avoidance task [13]. Here, rats lever-pressing for food must step onto a safe platform to avoid shocks signaled by a tone. ARs reach asymptote in session 2, and Pavlovian fear reactions gradually disappear by session 10. Using muscimol inactivation of IL during session 11, they also observed increased fear reacitons, but these weren’t so strong as to prevent ARs. Together, these data suggest that IL is most important early in avoidance training to suppress incompatible Pavlovian reactions like freezing.
Two recent SigAA studies examined ARs and Pavlovian reactions after NAc inactivation. The first, using the platform-mediated avoidance task, found that inactivation of the entire NAc impaired ARs after 10d of training [13]. The second, using a disconnection strategy and shuttlebox avoidance, found that BA and NAc-shell operate in series to motivate AR expression after three training sessions [32]. In both studies, disrupting ARs led to corresponding increases in Pavlovian freezing. Interestingly, NAc-shell inactivation had no effect on freezing when the opportunity to avoid was removed by blocking the shuttlebox door, consistent with the responsecompetition model and many earlier studies showing that response prevention leads to a rapid return of Pavlovian fear reactions [20].
Amount of training.
Some early researchers recognized that the amount of avoidance training could impact brain lesion effects, but this was not widespread. Recent studies show a dramatic shift in the brain circuits mediating avoidance across training. First, manipulations of BLA that impair avoidance early in training are ineffective after overtraining [9,28]. Second, ventral striatum, which receives direct projections from BLA, appears necessary for moderately trained, but not overtrained, ARs [13,32,33]. Finally, in dopamine-deficient mice with severe impairments in SigAA learning, replacing dopamine in amygdala and striatum is sufficient to enable learning [34]. However, overtrained mice require dopamine only in striatum to perform the AR. This dramatic shift from amygdala-dependent to amygdala-independent avoidance is reminiscent of the shift from goal-directed to habitual responding in appetitive conditioning [35]. In appetitive studies, instrumental behavior that is sensitive to outcome-devaluation requires dorsomedial striatum (DMS) but not dorsolateral striatum (DLS). However, with overtraining, behavior is DLS-dependent and insensitive to devaluation. It is unclear if the shift to amygdalaindependent avoidance behavior reflects a transition to S-R control with overtraining. Outcome manipulations after moderate training vs. overtraining have not been examined in avoidance and early attempts to address the role of dorsal striatum in habitual avoidance have been inconclusive [36–38]. But these studies demonstrate that the amount of training is a critical factor for neurobehavioral analyses of avoidance.
Are ARs merely Pavlovian flight reactions?
SSDR theory generated little new avoidance research as it argued that avoidance was a poor method for studying defense. Nevertheless, several findings cast doubt on the conditioned flight interpretation of ARs. First, Pavlovian flight is rarely observed in the laboratory, even when rats are familiar with escape routes [21]. This makes sense, as the CS functions to prepare the animal to react to the US, not substitute for it. One study attempted to show conditioned flight to a familiar enclosure during a shock-free test [39]. They observed CS-elicited movement to the enclosure, but these responses were so slow (~0.25cm/s) that they would not have avoided the shock had it been present. Perhaps the best demonstration of conditioned flight was recently published using a serial compound CS in mice (tone→noise→shock; [40]). Mice froze to the tone but exhibited rapid (~8cm/s) movement to the noise. However, even here conditioned flight was severely constrained; flight was only observed in the context where shock had previously occurred. Second, although some ARs resemble flight, the notion that rats flee to a safe spot to freeze is not supported by the data. Once rats reach a high level of avoidance responding, Pavlovian reactions including freezing approach zero [8,11–13]. Finally, if ARs are Pavlovian flight reactions one would expect the core underlying brain circuitry to match shared circuitry for other well-known fear reactions. But there are stark differences. CeA is required for fear conditioning in a variety of tasks but is unnecessary for avoidance [8,9,11,41]. The opposite is true for BA [8,9,42,43]. Most strikingly, the BLA-dependence of avoidance is transient. With overtraining, lesions of BLA have no effect on ARs [9,28] and WS-elicited neural activity declines [29]. This contrasts with fear conditioning, where LA is required for the lifetime of the memory [44] and remains essential even after overtraining [45–47]. And notably, when Pavlovian flight can be demonstrated it depends on threat processing in the LA→CeA pathway, unlike avoidance [40].
Fear and reinforcement problems.
Although not fatal to the paradigm, avoidance research was severely bogged down by theoretical debates about reinforcement and the mediating role of fear. Neurobiological studies are beginning to shed light on these important questions. For instance, outcome revaluation procedures that paired morphine injections with shock or safety signals suggest that US-omission is important for goal-directed ARs and safety signals are important for habitual ARs [48–50]. Both effects appear to depend on NAc-shell. In other “escape from fear” studies, aversive CS-termination supports new response learning in chambers where shock and explicit safety signals are never presented [51]. Amygdala lesions show that the underlying neurocircuitry mirrors AR learning; LA and BA are required, but CeA is not [52]. Appropriately-timed US-omission signals have not yet been identified in the brain, however, the lateral habenula, a region implicated in negative prediction errors in appetitive tasks inhibits AR learning [53]. Dopaminergic ventral tegmental area (VTA) projections to NAcshell may play a role, as these have been implicated in learning via omission of an expected aversive US [54]. These are important first steps towards understanding reinforcement in avoidance, but many questions remain. For instance, are WS-termination and US-omission involved in AR learning directly or indirectly via the generation of response-produced safety signals? And to what degree do these events behave as response-linked outcomes vs. S-R reinforcers?
One intriguing possibility that could explain the lack of correspondence between ARs and fear is that WSs come to behave as discriminative stimuli (DSs) or reflex triggers with extended training [55]. A DS would signal the opportunity to avoid harm (or obtain safety) and a reflex trigger would directly elicit the AR. Neither should elicit fear reactions. NAc and prelimbic (PL) prefrontal cortex, regions known to mediate goal-directed responding in appetitive tasks [35], may mediate DS-elicited ARs after moderate training [13,56,57]. VTA dopamine transients in NAc-core indicate that WSs predict harm early in training, predict safety later in training and ultimately behave as reflex triggers after overtraining [33,58]. WS-elicited dopamine responses are negative on early trials when freezing is high. However, on trials where the AR is emitted, positive dopamine responses are observed during the WS and especially during the safety interval. Further, avoidance can be bi-directionally modulated by manipulating these dopamine signals after moderate training, but not after overtraining. A very recent study observed inhibitory WS responses in rostral PL during avoidance conditioning, but not Pavlovian conditioning [59]. Although these didn’t correlate with ARs and were observed even on failed trials, optogenetically blocking PL inhibitory responses severely impaired AR expression. Thus, recruitment of PL appears to track the transition of the WS from a CS to a DS, as residual Pavlovian responses are decoupled from AR performance [31]. This NAc- and PL-dependent stage may bridge threat-motivated avoidance and habitual avoidance.
Together these findings indicate that fear as an intervening variable in avoidance may be an outdated, unnecessary and counterproductive concept [60–62]. Fear-mediation was introduced to explain S-R reinforcement via a nonevent (US-omission) without appealing to cognitive expectancies. However, behavioral and autonomic reactions thought to reflect a central fear state are anticorrelated with ARs and evidence for fear-reduction at WS-termination is lacking. Today it is also clear that animals form associations with outcome representations and neurally register prediction errors when expected outcomes are omitted. Safety conditioning, which appears important for avoidance, depends on such violated expectations [63]. A modern conception of AR learning based on appetitive conditioning has been more productive, where ARs are generated by Pavlovian and instrumental prediction errors and controlled by different brain circuits as subjects gain experience with associative contingencies.
Insensitivity of SSDRs to reinforcement.
Flight and freezing SSDRs are insensitive to reinforcement [21,64,65]. Arbitrary responses like bar-pressing can be exceedingly difficult to train with an avoidance protocol, often requiring procedural tricks like shaping, long ITI safety cues, rest days and extended periods of intermittent shock. Locomotor responses are acquired more readily. However, even in these tasks, ~25% of subjects never acquire the AR despite significant exposure to instrumental contingencies (poor avoiders). Phenomena like “warm-up”, where even good avoiders won’t avoid until receiving a shock, can also be a major nuisance [66]. If animals can learn about the consequences of their actions when threatened, why is this so difficult to reliably demonstrate in the laboratory?
One possibility is that avoidance researchers have been operating at the wrong end of the defensive behavior continuum (Figure 4). Avoidance is typically trained in small chambers with few behavioral options using high density shock protocols. This may maximally activate survival circuits, thoroughly suppress non-defensive behavior and restrict responding to a few SSDRs that are insensitive to reinforcement. These “post-encounter” and “circa-strike” behaviors likely evolved as rapid, inflexible reactions to predators that are detected and/or attacking [21].
Although dominant when predatory imminence is high, these behaviors represent a small fraction of the defensive behavioral repertoire. Animals spend most of their time under lower threat levels engaging in anxiety-related “pre-encounter” behaviors, intermixed with nondefensive behaviors like foraging. Perhaps these evolved to be sensitive to reinforcement, since trial-and-error learning would be much less likely to result in death. Unfortunately, very little is known about pre-encounter defensive behavior, especially in the context of avoidance. However, there is renewed interest in more ecologically valid models that evaluate defensive behavior in conflict situations where threats are less certain, safety signals modulate responding and rewards are available [30,59,67–73]. It will be important to evaluate instrumental contingencies in these paradigms to determine if there is an inverse relationship between US imminence and the effectiveness of reinforcement for available ARs [74].
Conclusions.
Converging evidence from studies using modern neurobiological and behavioral tools strongly suggests that ARs are established via instrumental response learning. Conditioned threats and safety signals may be necessary for avoidance, however, generating ARs with purely Pavlovian contingencies is exceedingly difficult. Further, the neural circuits mediating ARs have more in common with appetitive instrumental behavior than Pavlovian SSDRs. Recent advances indicate that avoidance problems identified in the 1970s can be resolved using modern tools, especially if investigators consider important factors like safety signal reinforcement, response-competition, goal-directed vs. habitual control of ARs and threat imminence in the design of avoidance studies [61,63]. This latter factor represents the greatest hurdle for progress in understanding avoidance, and more work is needed to determine if preencounter defensive behaviors (or co-occurring non-defensive behaviors) are more sensitive to reinforcement than circa-strike and post-encounter responses like flight and freezing. Research in this vein may shed light on a neglected class of learned defensive behavior and advance our understanding of human disorders of control. Indeed, recent studies in humans have confirmed, to a first approximation, the neural circuits of avoidance identified in rodent studies [10,75,76]. They also demonstrate that adaptive ARs can be effective active coping strategies [10], whereas maladaptive or habitual ARs may contribute to disorders ranging from OCD [4,62] to addiction [77].
Highlights.
Avoidance research stalled amid debates about fear, reinforcement and Pavlovian control
Recent neurobehavioral data are inconsistent with a Pavlovian flight account of avoidance
Avoidance brain circuits resemble those of appetitive instrumental actions more than Pavlovian SSDRs
Feedback, response-competition, goal-directedness and threat-imminence are important factors in avoidance
Warning signals may behave as conditioned stimuli, discriminative stimuli and habit triggers across training
Acknowledgements
The author would like to thank Robert Sears, Vincent Campese, Greg Quirk and Joseph LeDoux for helpful discussions and comments on this manuscript. Funding: This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under award number [R01MH114931] to CKC.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest statement
Nothing declared.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Baker TB, Piper ME, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR, Fiore MC: Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychol Rev (2004) 111(1):33–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Everitt BJ, Robbins TW: Drug addiction: Updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on. Annual review of psychology (2016) 67(23–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.•.Gillan CM, Morein-Zamir S, Urcelay GP, Sule A, Voon V, Apergis-Schoute AM, Fineberg NA, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW: Enhanced avoidance habits in obsessivecompulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry (2014) 75(8):631–638. This paper uses a novel outcome-devaluation procedure to demonstrate that habits play a larger role in AR performance for OCD patients relative than controls. After disconnecting shock cables in view of the participants, OCD patients emitted significantly more ARs in extinction. Outcome-devaluation effects are rare in the avoidance literature, and this laboratory has published related effects in rats implicating US-omission and possibly safety signals in goal-directed avoidance [48–50]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Vaghi MM, Cardinal RN, Apergis-Schoute AM, Fineberg NA, Sule A, Robbins TW: Action-outcome knowledge dissociates from behavior in obsessive-compulsive disorder following contingency degradation. bioRxiv (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Krypotos AM, Effting M, Kindt M, Beckers T: Avoidance learning: A review of theoretical models and recent developments. Front Behav Neurosci (2015) 9(189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Thomas GJ, Hostetter G, Barker DJ: Behavioral functions of the limbic system In: Progress in physiological psychology. 2 Stellar, Sprague (Eds), Academic Press, New York: (1968):229–311. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kamin CJ, Brimer CJ, Black AH: Conditioned suppression as a monitor of fear of the cs in the course of avoidance training. J Comp Physiol Psychol (1963) 56(497–501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Choi JS, Cain CK, LeDoux JE: The role of amygdala nuclei in the expression of auditory signaled two-way active avoidance in rats. Learn Mem (2010) 17(3):139–147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.•.Lazaro-Munoz G, LeDoux JE, Cain CK: Sidman instrumental avoidance initially depends on lateral and basal amygdala and is constrained by central amygdalamediated pavlovian processes. Biol Psychiatry (2010) 67(12):1120–1127. Clearly demonstrates the transition from BLA-dependent to BLA-independent avoidance with overtraining. Also shows that CeA is unnecessary for AR learning and CeA lesions immediately reverse poor avoidance by eliminating freezing. This supports the notion that Pavlian reactions compete with instrumental ARs and indicates that poor avoidance is mainly a performance, not learning, deficit. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Boeke EA, Moscarello JM, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA, Hartley CA: Active avoidance: Neural mechanisms and attenuation of pavlovian conditioned responding. J Neurosci (2017) 37(18):4808–4818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.•.Moscarello JM, LeDoux JE: Active avoidance learning requires prefrontal suppression of amygdala-mediated defensive reactions. J Neurosci (2013) 33(9):3815–3823. Shows that IL plays a crucial role early in avoidance training by suppressing CeAmediated Pavlovian reactions, similar to it’s role in fear extinction. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Martinez RC, Gupta N, Lazaro-Munoz G, Sears RM, Kim S, Moscarello JM, LeDoux JE, Cain CK: Active vs. Reactive threat responding is associated with differential c-fos expression in specific regions of amygdala and prefrontal cortex. Learn Mem (2013) 20(8):446–452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bravo-Rivera C, Roman-Ortiz C, Brignoni-Perez E, Sotres-Bayon F, Quirk GJ: Neural structures mediating expression and extinction of platform-mediated avoidance. J Neurosci (2014) 34(29):9736–9742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Levis DJ: The case for a return to a two-factor theory of avoidance: The failure of non-fear interpretations In: Contemporary learning theories: Pavlovian conditioning and the status of traditional learning theory. Klein SB, Mowrer RR (Eds), Lawrence Erlbaum Ass., Hillsdale: (1989):227–277. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mowrer OH, Lamoreaux RR: Fear as an intervening variable in avoidance conditioning. J Comp Psychol (1946) 39(29–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Seligman ME, Johnston JC: A cognitive theory of avoidance learning In: Contemporary approaches to conditioning and learning. McGuigan FJ, Lumsden DB (Eds), Hemisphere Publishing Corporation, Washington D.C. (1975):69–110. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sidman M: Avoidance conditioning with brief shock and no exteroceptive warning signal. Science (1953) 118(3058):157–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bolles RC: The avoidance learning problem In: The psychology of learning and motivation. 6 Bower GH, Spence JT (Eds), Academic Press, Oxford: (1972):97–145. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Rescorla RA, Solomon RL: Two process learning theory: Relationships between pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. Psychological Review (1967) 74(151–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Solomon RL, Wynne LC: Traumatic avoidance learning: The principles of anxiety conservation and partial irreversibility. Psychol Rev (1954) 61(353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.•.Fanselow MS, Lester LS: A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: Predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior. In: Evolution and learning. Bolles RC, Beecher MD (Eds), Erlbaum, Hillsdale NJ. (1988):185–211. Introduces the notion that threat imminence selects between pre-encounter, post encounter and circa-strike antipredator SSDRs. Also confirms earlier work showing that freezing is insensitive to avoidance or punishment contingencies using a discrete-trial SigAA procedure. [Google Scholar]
- 22.• •.Fanselow MS: Species-specific defense reactions: Retrospect and prospect In: Learning, motivation, and cognition: The functional behaviorism of robert c. Bolles. Bouton ME, Fanselow MS (Eds), American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C. (1997):321–341. An excellent review by one of Bolles’ former students explaining the evolution of SSDR theory througout the 1970s. Although Bolles initially entertained some non-traditional explanations of AR learning (e.g. punishment of non-target responses, safety signal reinforcement), he ultimately concluded that threats restrict behavior to SSDRs which are insensitive to reinforcement. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Fragale JE, Beck KD, Pang KC: Use of the exponential and exponentiated demand equations to assess the behavioral economics of negative reinforcement. Front Neurosci (2017) 11(77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Overmier JB, Schwartzkopf KH: Summation of food and shock based responding. Learning and Motivation (1974) 5(42–52. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Goddard G: Functions of the amygdala. Psychol Rev (1964) 62(89–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sarter MF, Markowitsch HJ: Involvement of the amygdala in learning and memory: A critical review, with emphasis on anatomical relations. Behavioral Neuroscience (1985) 99(342–380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Cain CK, LeDoux JE: Brain mechanisms of pavlovian and instrumental aversive conditioning In: Handbook of anxiety and fear. 17 Nutt DJ, Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC, Griebel G (Eds), Elsevier Academic Press, Amsterdam: (2008):103–125. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Poremba A, Gabriel M: Amygdala neurons mediate acquisition but not maintenance of instrumental avoidance behavior in rabbits. J Neurosci (1999) 19(21):9635–9641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Maren S, Poremba A, Gabriel M: Basolateral amygdaloid multi-unit neuronal correlates of discriminative avoidance learning in rabbits. Brain Research (1991) 549(311–316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gentry RN, Lee B, Roesch MR: Phasic dopamine release in the rat nucleus accumbens predicts approach and avoidance performance. Nature communications (2016) 7(13154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bravo-Rivera C, Roman-Ortiz C, Montesinos-Cartagena M, Quirk GJ: Persistent active avoidance correlates with activity in prelimbic cortex and ventral striatum. Front Behav Neurosci (2015) 9(184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.•.Ramirez F, Moscarello JM, LeDoux JE, Sears RM: Active avoidance requires a serial basal amygdala to nucleus accumbens shell circuit. J Neurosci (2015) 35(8):34703477 First demonstration that a long-hypothesized amygdalostriatal circuit mediates ARs (moderately-trained). Also shows that impairing ARs via NAcS inactivation increases Pavlovian freezing, but only when performance of the AR is possible. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.• •.Wenzel JM, Oleson EB, Gove WN, Cole AB, Gyawali U, Dantrassy HM, Bluett RJ, Dryanovski DI, Stuber GD, Deisseroth K, Mathur BN et al. : Phasic dopamine signals in the nucleus accumbens that cause active avoidance require endocannabinoid mobilization in the midbrain. Current biology : CB (2018) 28(9):1392–1404 e1395. Elegant study building on results of reference [58]. Using optogenetics and pharmacology, they show that VTA dopamine transients in NAcC during the CS promote AR learning via activation of D1 receptors. Like appetitive instrumental conditioning, these signals are modulated by endocannabinnoids in VTA. However, after overtraining, this pathway no longer influences AR performance. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Darvas M, Fadok JP, Palmiter RD: Requirement of dopamine signaling in the amygdala and striatum for learning and maintenance of a conditioned avoidance response. Learn Mem (2011) 18(3):136–143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Peak J, Hart G, Balleine BW: From learning to action: The integration of dorsal striatal input and output pathways in instrumental conditioning. Eur J Neurosci (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wendler E, Gaspar JC, Ferreira TL, Barbiero JK, Andreatini R, Vital MA, Blaha CD, Winn P, Da Cunha C: The roles of the nucleus accumbens core, dorsomedial striatum, and dorsolateral striatum in learning: Performance and extinction of pavlovian fear-conditioned responses and instrumental avoidance responses. Neurobiol Learn Mem (2014) 109(27–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Boschen SL, Wietzikoski EC, Winn P, Da Cunha C: The role of nucleus accumbens and dorsolateral striatal d2 receptors in active avoidance conditioning. Neurobiol Learn Mem (2011) 96(2):254–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wietzikoski EC, Boschen SL, Miyoshi E, Bortolanza M, Dos Santos LM, Frank M, Brandao ML, Winn P, Da Cunha C: Roles of d1-like dopamine receptors in the nucleus accumbens and dorsolateral striatum in conditioned avoidance responses. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2012) 219(1):159–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.De Oca BM, Minor TR, Fanselow MS: Brief flight to a familiar enclosure in response to a conditional stimulus in rats. J Gen Psychol (2007) 134(2):153–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.• •.Fadok JP, Krabbe S, Markovic M, Courtin J, Xu C, Massi L, Botta P, Bylund K, Muller C, Kovacevic A, Tovote P et al. : A competitive inhibitory circuit for selection of active and passive fear responses. Nature (2017) 542(7639):96–100. One of the few clear examples of Pavlovian flight in the literature. Also shows that Pavlovian flight depends on CS processing in the LA to CeA pathway, unlike instrumental ARs that depend on the LA to BA pathway and never require CeA. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Roozendaal B, Koolhaas JM, Bohus B: The central amygdala is involved in conditioning but not in retention of active and passive shock avoidance in male rats. Behav Neural Biol (1993) 59(2):143–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Nader K, Majidishad P, Amorapanth P, LeDoux JE: Damage to the lateral and central, but not other, amygdaloid nuclei prevents the acquisition of auditory fear conditioning. Learn Mem (2001) 8(3):156–163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Sotres-Bayon F, Bush DE, LeDoux JE: Emotional perseveration: An update on prefrontal-amygdala interactions in fear extinction. Learn Mem (2004) 11(5):525535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Gale GD, Anagnostaras SG, Godsil BP, Mitchell S, Nozawa T, Sage JR, Wiltgen B, Fanselow MS: Role of the basolateral amygdala in the storage of fear memories across the adult lifetime of rats. J Neurosci (2004) 24(15):3810–3815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.•.Maren S: Neurotoxic basolateral amygdala lesions impair learning and memory but not the performance of conditional fear in rats. J Neurosci (1999) 19(19):8696–8703. This paper shows that Pavlovian freezing responses require BLA even after overtraining. This contrasts with multiple studies showing that overtrained ARs do not require BLA. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Poulos AM, Li V, Sterlace SS, Tokushige F, Ponnusamy R, Fanselow MS: Persistence of fear memory across time requires the basolateral amygdala complex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2009) 106(28):11737–11741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Maren S: Auditory fear conditioning increases cs-elicited spike firing in lateral amygdala neurons even after extensive overtraining. Eur J Neurosci (2000) 12(11):4047–4054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Fernando AB, Urcelay GP, Mar AC, Dickinson A, Robbins TW: Safety signals as instrumental reinforcers during free-operant avoidance. Learn Mem (2014) 21(9):488–497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Fernando A, Urcelay G, Mar A, Dickinson A, Robbins T: Free-operant avoidance behavior by rats after reinforcer revaluation using opioid agonists and damphetamine. J Neurosci (2014) 34(18):6286–6293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Fernando AB, Urcelay GP, Mar AC, Dickinson TA, Robbins TW: The role of the nucleus accumbens shell in the mediation of the reinforcing properties of a safety signal in free-operant avoidance: Dopamine-dependent inhibitory effects of damphetamine. Neuropsychopharmacology (2014) 39(6):1420–1430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Cain CK, LeDoux JE: Escape from fear: A detailed behavioral analysis of two atypical responses reinforced by cs termination. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process (2007) 33(4):451–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Amorapanth P, LeDoux JE, Nader K: Different lateral amygdala outputs mediate reactions and actions elicited by a fear-arousing stimulus. Nat Neurosci (2000) 3(1):74–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Song M, Jo YS, Lee YK, Choi JS: Lesions of the lateral habenula facilitate active avoidance learning and threat extinction. Behav Brain Res (2017) 318(12–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Luo R, Uematsu A, Weitemier A, Aquili L, Koivumaa J, McHugh TJ, Johansen JP: A dopaminergic switch for fear to safety transitions. Nature communications (2018) 9(1):2483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Moscarello JM, Maren S: Flexibility in the face of fear: Hippocampal-prefrontal regulation of fear and avoidance. Curr Opin Behav Sci (2018) 19(44–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Beck KD, Jiao X, Smith IM, Myers CE, Pang KC, Servatius RJ: Iti-signals and prelimbic cortex facilitate avoidance acquisition and reduce avoidance latencies, respectively, in male wky rats. Front Behav Neurosci (2014) 8(403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Piantadosi PT, Yeates DCM, Floresco SB: Cooperative and dissociable involvement of the nucleus accumbens core and shell in the promotion and inhibition of actions during active and inhibitory avoidance. Neuropharmacology (2018) 138(5771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.•.Oleson EB, Gentry RN, Chioma VC, Cheer JF: Subsecond dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens predicts conditioned punishment and its successful avoidance. J Neurosci (2012) 32(42):14804–14808. Shows positive phasic dopamine responses in NAc during the CS and especially the during AR-produced safety cues when animals are moderately trained (50% successful avoidance). However, dopamine transients during the CS are negative on failed trials when animals freeze rather than avoid. These dopamine transients were later manipulated to bi-directionally modulate SigAA behavior [33]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.• •.Diehl MM, Bravo-Rivera C, Rodriguez-Romaguera J, Pagan-Rivera PA, Burgos-Robles A, Roman-Ortiz C, Quirk GJ: Active avoidance requires inhibitory signaling in the rodent prelimbic prefrontal cortex. Elife (2018) 77:e34657 Shows avoidance-training-specific inhibitory responses in rostral PL are crucial for ARs when Pavlovian reactions no longer compete. Since these inhibitory responses correlate poorly with ARs, they may reflect the transfomation of the CS into a DS/occasion-setter that signals the opportunity to avoid (or attain safety). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.LeDoux JE, Pine DS: Using neuroscience to help understand fear and anxiety: A two-system framework. Am J Psychiatry (2016) 173(11):1083–1093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.LeDoux JE, Moscarello J, Sears R, Campese V: The birth, death and resurrection of avoidance: A reconceptualization of a troubled paradigm. Molecular psychiatry (2017) 22(1):24–36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Gillan CM, Urcelay G, Robbins TW: An associative account of avoidance In: The wiley handbook on the cognitive neuroscience of learning. Murphy RA, Honey RC (Eds), John Wiley & Sons, LTD, West Sussex, UK: (2016):442–467. [Google Scholar]
- 63.•.Lloyd K, Dayan P: Safety out of control: Dopamine and defence. Behav Brain Funct (2016) 12(1):15 Excellent review of reinforcement learning principles and dopamine signals as they may apply to active avoidance conditioning. Response-produced safety signals are considered as crucial outcomes/reinforcers that also function to oppose incompatible Pavlovian reactions. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Bolles RC, Riley AL: Freezing as an avoidance response: Another look at the operant-respondent distinction. Learning and Motivation (1973) 4(268–275. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Brener J, Goesling WJ: Avoidance conditioning of activity and immobility in rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol (1970) 70(2):276–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Spiegler KM, Fortress AM, Pang KCH: Differential use of danger and safety signals in an animal model of anxiety vulnerability: The behavioral economics of avoidance. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2018) 82(195–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Paré D, Quirk GJ: When scientific paradigms lead to tunnel vision: Lessons from the study of fear. npj Science of Learning (2017) 2(1):6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Rigoli F, Chew B, Dayan P, Dolan RJ: Multiple value signals in dopaminergic midbrain and their role in avoidance contexts. NeuroImage (2016) 135(197–203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Saga Y, Richard A, Sgambato-Faure V, Hoshi E, Tobler PN, Tremblay L: Ventral pallidum encodes contextual information and controls aversive behaviors. Cereb Cortex (2017) 27(4):2528–2543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Kim JJ, Jung MW: Fear paradigms: The times they are a-changin ’. Curr Opin Behav Sci (2018) 24(38–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Ng KH, Pollock MW, Urbanczyk PJ, Sangha S: Altering d1 receptor activity in the basolateral amygdala impairs fear suppression during a safety cue. Neurobiol Learn Mem (2018) 147(26–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Schwartz N, Miller C, Fields HL: Cortico-accumbens regulation of approachavoidance behavior is modified by experience and chronic pain. Cell Rep (2017) 19(8):1522–1531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Gentry RN, Roesch MR: Neural activity in ventral medial prefrontal cortex is modulated more before approach than avoidance during reinforced and extinction trial blocks. J Neurosci (2018) 38(19):4584–4597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Rigoli F, Pezzulo G, Dolan RJ: Prospective and pavlovian mechanisms in aversive behaviour. Cognition (2016) 146(415–425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Lewis AH, Niznikiewicz MA, Delamater AR, Delgado MR: Avoidance-based human pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer. Eur J Neurosci (2013) 38(12):3740–3748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Collins KA, Mendelsohn A, Cain CK, Schiller D: Taking action in the face of threat: Neural synchronization predicts adaptive coping. J Neurosci (2014) 34(44):1473314738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Koob GF, Volkow ND: Neurobiology of addiction: A neurocircuitry analysis. The lancet Psychiatry (2016) 3(8):760–773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Rogan MT, Leon KS, Perez DL, Kandel ER: Distinct neural signatures for safety and danger in the amygdala and striatum of the mouse. Neuron (2005) 46(2):309–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Ostroff LE, Cain CK, Bedont J, Monfils MH, LeDoux JE: Fear and safety learning differentially affect synapse size and dendritic translation in the lateral amygdala. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2010) 107(20):9418–9423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Sangha S, Chadick JZ, Janak PH: Safety encoding in the basal amygdala. J Neurosci (2013) 33(9):3744–3751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Likhtik E, Stujenske JM, Topiwala MA, Harris AZ, Gordon JA: Prefrontal entrainment of amygdala activity signals safety in learned fear and innate anxiety. Nat Neurosci (2014) 17(1):106–113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Hormigo S, Vega-Flores G, Castro-Alamancos MA: Basal ganglia output controls active avoidance behavior. J Neurosci (2016) 36(40):10274–10284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]