Tsai 2012.
Methods | Quasi‐RCT. | |
Participants | Number screened: 368. Number included: 52 (30 children with DCD who were quasi‐randomised plus 22 typically developing children). Number followed up: 52. Number of withdrawals: 0. Diagnosis of DCD: DSM‐IV. Presence and absence of comorbid conditions: children were excluded if they had comorbid conditions that may impact on the tests or intervention. Regarding participants completing the study Age: overall range 9 to 10 years: DCD soccer training (mean ± SD): 116.81 ± 5.37 months; DCD inactive control (mean ± SD): 114.00 ± 3.68 months). Sex: DCD soccer training (9 boys and 7 girls); DCD inactive control (9 boys and 5 girls); non‐DCD control (12 boys and 10 girls). Ethnicity: Taiwanese. Potentially very low rate of aborigine.* Country: Taiwan. Setting: Primary schools.* Sociodemographics: urban residents of Kaohsiung.* Inclusion criteria
Exclusion criteria
|
|
Interventions |
Intervention: DCD soccer training vs DCD inactive control vs non‐DCD inactive control. Intervention schedule: 50 min at 5/wk for 10 wk. Duration of intervention: 10 wk.* Mode of delivery: face‐to‐face group sessions. Intervention material: soccer balls, obstacles, goal posts. Intervention procedure: warm‐up, main part of soccer training, playing a game, cool down. emphasis first on general skills and then on task‐specific skills. Intervention provider: trained soccer coach. Place of intervention: school. Intervention compliance: no absence. All enjoyed participation.* |
|
Outcomes |
Primary
Secondary
Adverse effects or events: none reported by the instructor.* Measures of participation: not reported. |
|
Notes | Study start date: not available. Study end date: not available. Sample calculation: no. Ethics approval: yes. Comments from study authors Limitations:*
Key conclusions of study authors Soccer training could significantly facilitate the development of motor skills and improve inhibitory control and neuroelectric indices of attention networks. Comment from review authors A larger effect size of intervention outcome than other studies. This may be due to the quality and the high frequency of training or quasi‐RCT design. * Email correspondence with study authors: May 2014 and February 2016. We contacted the first study author several times and obtained raw data for meta‐analysis and supplementary information. |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | Quasi‐randomly subdivided. If consent was not forthcoming then the child was moved to the non‐training group. This is going against the randomisation process. |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Randomisation was broken. |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Single blind, but impossible to achieve double blind. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Low risk | After enquiry, the first author stated that blinding of outcome assessment ensured, and unlikely that the blinding could have been broken. |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | After enquiry, the first author stated no missing outcome data. |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Low risk | No protocol obtained. All prespecified outcomes of interest reported. |
Other bias | Low risk | Funding: research funded by a grant from the National Science Council in Taiwan (NSC 99‐2314‐B‐006‐006‐MY2 and NSC 98‐2410‐H‐006‐106‐MY2). Conflicts of interest: no information available. Small numbers. |