Abstract
The adolescent brain undergoes profound structural changes which is influenced by many factors. Screen media activity (SMA; e.g., watching television or videos, playing video games, or using social media) is a common recreational activity in children and adolescents; however, its effect on brain structure is not well understood. A multivariate approach with the first cross-sectional data release from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study was used to test the maturational coupling hypothesis, i.e. the notion that coordinated patterns of structural change related to specific behaviors. Moreover, the utility of this approach was tested by determining the association between these structural correlation networks and psychopathology or cognition. ABCD participants with usable structural imaging and SMA data (N=4277 of 4524) were subjected to a Group Factor Analysis (GFA) to identify latent variables that relate SMA to cortical thickness, sulcal depth, and gray matter volume. Subject scores from these latent variables were used in generalized linear mixed-effect models to investigate associations between SMA and internalizing and externalizing psychopathology, as well as fluid and crystalized intelligence. Four SMA-related GFAs explained 37% of the variance between SMA and structural brain indices. SMA-related GFAs correlated with brain areas that support homologous functions. Some but not all SMA-related factors corresponded with higher externalizing (Cohen’s d effect size (ES) 0.06–0.1) but not internalizing psychopathology and lower crystalized (ES: 0.08–0.1) and fluid intelligence (ES: 0.04–0.09). Taken together, these findings support the notion of SMA related maturational coupling or structural correlation networks in the brain and provides evidence that individual differences of these networks have mixed consequences for psychopathology and cognitive performance.
1. Introduction
Brain structure undergoes remarkable changes in the second decade of life (Pfefferbaum et al., 2016), characterized by a reduction of gray matter and an increase in white matter (Giedd et al., 2015), with enduring impacts on cognition (Walhovd et al., 2016). Specifically, coordinated cortical thinning (Ducharme et al., 2016) is governed by evolutionary novelty and functional specialization (Sotiras et al., 2017), showing regional and temporal specificity with development (Houston et al., 2014). Evidence from several recent studies is consistent with the hypothesis that changes of brain structure are correlated across areas with similar function that recapitulate functional networks (Geng et al., 2017), which has been termed maturational coupling or structural correlation networks (SCNs), and has been proposed as a putative region-specific biomarker for developmental psychopathology (Saggar et al., 2015). Thus, brain regions that change together, i.e. increase or decrease in volume at the same rate over the course of years in the same individual, show structural covariance (Vandekar et al., 2015) or anatomical connectivity across individuals, reflecting synchronized developmental change in distributed cortical regions (Alexander-Bloch et al., 2013). For example, developmental changes in maturational coupling within the default-mode network (DMN) align with developmental changes in structural and functional DMN connectivity (Khundrakpam et al., 2017). These structural changes can also be affected by environmental characteristics, such as childhood abuse (Gold et al., 2016) or urban upbringing (Besteher et al., 2017), and have direct implications for brain functions such as general cognitive ability (Vuoksimaa et al., 2016), behavioral inhibition (Sylvester et al., 2016), and subjective ratings of empathy (Bernhardt et al., 2014). Finally, these maturational differences seem to be triggered by regional variation of gene expression having a direct impact on cortical thickness (Fjell et al., 2015). Together, structural brain changes are a consequence of a coordinated process that reflects an interaction between environment and genes that impact specific neural functions.
Screen media activity (SMA) (e.g., watching television or videos, playing video games, or using social media) is among the most common recreational activity in children and adolescents (Kenney and Gortmaker, 2017; Loprinzi and Davis, 2016). As many as 99% of adolescents use the internet, approximately 85% engage in electronic gaming (Rikkers et al., 2016), and nearly 97% of US youth have at least one electronic item in their bedroom (Hale and Guan, 2015). Relatively few studies have examined the relationship between SMA and brain structure or function. In one study with 18 year-old college students, individuals with internet gaming addiction showed less gray matter volume in bilateral anterior cingulate cortex, precuneus, supplementary motor area, superior parietal cortex, left dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex, left insula, and bilateral cerebellum (Wang et al., 2015) than matched controls. Among young adult female habitual internet users, more gray matter volume of bilateral putamen and right nucleus accumbens and lower gray matter volume of orbitofrontal cortex were associated with more frequent use (Altbacker et al., 2016). Functional neuroimaging studies have provided some evidence that those with internet addiction fail to recruit frontal-basal pathways that are important in inhibiting unwanted actions (Li et al., 2014). However, there are no large studies in youth in general, and prepubescent adolescents in particular, focused on SMA and structural or functional brain characteristics.
There is some controversy about whether excessive SMA is associated with problematic outcomes among youth and adolescents. Whereas some have reported that frequent SMA is associated with internalizing psychopathology including depression (Goldfield et al., 2016) and anxiety (Holfeld and Sukhawathanakul, 2017), externalizing psychopathology (Cerniglia et al., 2016), greater risk behaviors (Fischer et al., 2011), and even suicide (Twenge et al., 2017), others have not found evidence for an association between SMA and problematic outcomes (Ferguson, 2015, 2017). Even fewer studies have examined the relationship between different types of SMA and brain structure in healthy youth. In a recent cross-sectional study of healthy children ages 8–12, time spent reading was positively correlated with higher functional connectivity between the Brodmann Area 37 and left-sided language, visual, and cognitive control regions, but screen time was related to lower connectivity between the left visual word form area and regions related to language and cognitive control (Horowitz-Kraus and Hutton, 2017). However, this study had a small number of participants. The goal of this investigation was to determine whether SMA is related to specific SCN, i.e. whether exposure to SMA correlates to specific brain areas across cortical thickness, volume, and sulcal depth. Moreover, the second goal was to determine whether such SCN can be related to individual differences in psychopathology and cognition. Based on the maturational coupling hypothesis (Alexander-Bloch et al., 2013; Raznahan et al., 2011), i.e. coordinated patterns of structural change related to specific behaviors, we hypothesized that those individuals engaged in significant SMA relative to those with less SMA exposure would show greater maturity in sensorimotor areas, i.e. lower cortical thickness associated with SMA in primary sensory and motor areas. Moreover, based on the emerging findings of disorganized SCNs in psychiatric populations (Wang et al., 2016; Xia et al., 2018), we hypothesized that SMA-related SCNs related to mismatch between sensorimotor and executive and value-based processing areas are associated with psychopathology or cognitive performance.
2. Materials and Methods
The Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development Study (ABCD) is a multi-site, longitudinal neuroimaging study following 9–10 year-old youth through adolescence. The ABCD study team employed a rigorous epidemiologically informed school-based recruitment strategy, designed with consideration of the demographic composition of the 21 ABCD sites and the US as a whole (Volkow et al., 2017). The total sample size for the ABCD Study is projected to be 11,500; the first data release (February 2018) included 4,524 youth who completed the baseline protocol before September 2017, and is the basis for these analyses (https://ndar.nih.gov/study.html?id=500, dx.doi.org/10.15154/1412097, accessed 3/22/2018).
2.1. Screen Media Activity (youth-report):
Youth were asked to indicate how long they were engaged in the following SMA activities during the weekday and weekend: (1) Watch TV shows or movies? (2) Watch videos (such as YouTube)? (3) Play video games on a computer, console, phone or other device (Xbox, Play Station, iPad)? (4) Text on a cell phone, tablet, or computer (e.g. GChat, WhatsApp, etc.)? (5) Visit social networking sites like Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, etc.? (6) Video chat (Skype, FaceTime, etc.)? Seven potential answer choices included: none, < 30 minutes, 30 minutes, 1 hour, 2 hours, 3 hours, and 4+ hours. In addition, two questions that were included in the analyses focused on specific types of videos and games: (1) How often do you play mature-rated video games (e.g., Call of Duty, Grand Theft Auto, Assassin’s Creed, etc.)? (2) How often do you watch R-rated movies? Four potential answer choices included: (1) never, (2) once in a while, (3) regularly, and (4) all the time. The youth report was used in the multivariate analyses to characterize and quantify SMA.
2.2. Mental Health Symptoms (parent report):
Youth’s behavior was assessed using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (Achenbach, 2009). The CBCL’s syndrome scales t-scores of externalizing, and internalizing psychopathologies were used for the analyses.
2.3. Cognition (youth performance):
The neurocognitive assessment included the NIH Toolbox (Luciana et al., 2018). For this report, measures of fluid intelligence (Li et al., 2004) (i.e., those abilities that rely on solving problems, thinking, acting quickly, and encoding new episodic memories) comprised of the Toolbox Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test, List Sorting Working Memory Test, Picture Sequence Memory Test, Flanker Task, and Dimensional Change Card Sort Task. Crystalized intelligence (i.e., those abilities that are more dependent on experience, representing accumulated store of verbal knowledge or skills and rely more heavily on education and cultural exposure) was measured with the Toolbox Picture Vocabulary Task and Oral Reading Recognition Task (Akshoomoff et al., 2013).
2.4. Structural Image Processing:
All structural neuroimaging processing was completed using FreeSurfer version 5.3.0 according to standardized processing pipelines (Casey et al., 2018). Subjects that did not pass FreeSurfer Quality Control measure were excluded from further analyses (n= 247; 5% of total sample), which left n=4,277 participants. Cortical reconstruction and volumetric segmentation was performed by the ABCD Data Acquisition and Integration Core using the FreeSurfer image analysis suite (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/, accessed 3/22/2018). Details of these procedures are described in prior publications (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999). Briefly, this process includes removal of non-brain tissue using a hybrid watershed/surface deformation procedure (Segonne et al., 2004), automated Talairach transformation, segmentation of the subcortical white matter and deep gray matter volumetric structures (including hippocampus, amygdala, caudate, putamen, ventricles) (Fischl et al., 2002), intensity normalization, tessellation of the gray/white matter boundary, automated topology correction (Fischl et al., 2001), and surface deformation following intensity gradients to optimally place the gray/white and gray/cerebrospinal fluid borders at the location where the greatest shift in intensity defines the transition to the other tissue class (Fischl and Dale, 2000). Images were registered to a spherical atlas, which is based on individual cortical folding patterns to match cortical geometry across subjects and the cerebral cortex was parcellated into 34 regions per hemisphere based on the gyral and sulcal structure (Desikan et al., 2006). Cortical thickness was measured as the shortest distance between the pial and the white matter tessellated surfaces (Dale et al., 1999). For sulcal depth, regions that moved outward during inflation were positive and represent the depths of sulci, and regions that moved inward were negative and represent the height of gyri (Fischl et al., 1999). FreeSurfer morphometric measures are related to histological measurements (Cardinale et al., 2014) and demonstrate good test-retest reliability (Han et al., 2006; Iscan et al., 2015).
2.5. Statistical Analysis:
All statistical analyses were conducted in R 3.4.0 (2017–04-21)(2010) using RStudio (Team, 2016b) and RMarkdown (Team, 2016a). Descriptive statistics were obtained using the tableone package in R (Yoshida and Bohn., 2017). Group Factor Analyses (GFA) were conducted using the R package GFA (Leppäaho et al., 2017). Mixed-effects model analyses were conducted using the R package gamm4 (Wood, 2017). Missing data were imputed during the GFA; however, all mixed model analyses were conducted with complete cases only. All R and R Markdown scripts are available as html documents in the supplemental materials.
2.6. Group Factor Analysis:
Group factor analysis (Klami et al., 2015) identified latent variables (LVs) related to SMA, cortical thickness, sulcal depth, and gray matter volume. The goal of GFA is to find factors that separate relationships within groups of variables from those between groups. Thus, given a collection X1,...,XM of M groups of variables of dimension D1,...,DM, the task is to find K < D1+ … + Dm factors that describe dependencies between multiple groups Xm, while allowing for within-group factors that account for covariance unique to each group. The GFA solution differs from canonical correlation analysis or standard exploratory factor analysis (EFA) by utilizing a Bayesian inferential framework to place an Automatic Relevance Determination (ARD) prior on the factor solution (Tipping, 2001). The ARD prior assumes a low-rank representation of the factor loadings. The main advantages of GFA are that (i) it is conceptually simple, essentially a Bayesian EFA model which differentiates within-group from between-group associations, and (ii) it enables factor analysis in scenarios with two or more groups of data, giving factor solutions not driven by method variance particular to one variable grouping. The solution comprises as a set of factors that contain a projection vector for each of the variable groups having non-zero weights for that factor. The number of factors presented here were chosen based on SMA loadings with brain structure variables and accounting for at least 1% of the variance across all variable groupings.
2.7. Generalized Linear Mixed Models:
Subsequent association analyses were conducted within a Generalized Linear Mixed Models (GLMM) framework. Specifically, GFAs that accounted for at least 1% of the variance across SMA and structural brain imaging estimates were used as independent variables to predict the dependent variables (CBCL-Internalizing, CBCL-Externalizing, Cognition-Fluid Intelligence, Cognition-Crystallized Intelligence). All variables were standardized to zero mean and unit variance to be able to calculate coefficients that correspond to Cohen’s d effect sizes. We used the R gamm4 library (Wood, 2017), estimating the parameters of the mixed model using Maximum Likelihood Estimation (MLE). Site and family nested within site were used as random effects and age, sex, race/ethnicity, parental education, marital status, parental income, parental age, and body mass index were used as covariates. Model comparisons were conducted using the anova function in R and the Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC).
3. Results
3.1. Demographics and sample characteristics.
Table 1 shows general demographics of the sample by quartiles of youth-reported total SMA. First, there was no difference in average age across the quartiles of screen time. Second, males were more frequently in the higher quartiles. Third, youth in the higher quartile had a higher BMI. Fourth, Black and Hispanic youth reported significantly more screen time use than White and Asian youth. Fifth, parents of youth in the higher quartiles were slightly younger, less well educated, were less likely to be married, and had lower household income.
Table 1:
Quartile table showing the relationship between youth-reported total screen activity and socio-demographic variables.
Total Week Screen Media | 1st Quartile | 2nd Quartile | 3rd Quartile | 4th Quartile | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Activity (hours) [youth report] | 0–11 | 11–19.5 | 19.5–33.8 | 33.8–163 | p |
n | 1076 | 1085 | 1034 | 1062 | |
Average Screen Time (hours/week) | 6.94 (2.76) | 15.25 (2.48) | 26.08 (4.05) | 54.01 (19.13) | <0.001 |
Age in Months (mean (sd)) | 119.38 (7.23) | 120.32 (7.26) | 120.38 (7.57) | 120.04 (7.26) | 0.006 |
Height (in) (mean (sd)) | 55.26 (2.92) | 55.43 (3.12) | 55.49 (3.15) | 55.56 (3.41) | 0.147 |
Weight (lbs) (mean (sd)) | 77.71 (19.23) | 81.09 (21.32) | 82.58 (23.02) | 87.65 (25.89) | <0.001 |
Body Mass Index (mean (sd)) | 17.75 (3.45) | 18.40 (3.75) | 18.67 (4.04) | 19.72 (4.39) | <0.001 |
Gender (female) (%) | 625 (58.1) | 520 (47.9) | 453 (43.8) | 421 (39.6) | <0.001 |
Race/ Ethnicity (%) | <0.001 | ||||
White | 745 (69.3) | 700 (64.5) | 587 (56.8) | 487 (45.9) | |
Black | 43 (4.0) | 55 (5.1) | 99 (9.6) | 202 (19.0) | |
Hispanic | 149 (13.9) | 210 (19.4) | 222 (21.5) | 246 (23.2) | |
Asian | 36 (3.3) | 30 (2.8) | 21 (2.0) | 10 (0.9) | |
Other | 102 (9.5) | 90 (8.3) | 105 (10.2) | 116 (10.9) | |
Parental Education (%) | <0.001 | ||||
<= 12 grades | 87 (8.1) | 117 (10.8) | 152 (14.7) | 172 (16.2) | |
HS Degree | 25 (2.3) | 38 (3.5) | 46 (4.4) | 50 (4.7) | |
Some College | 160 (14.9) | 221 (20.4) | 300 (29.0) | 406 (38.2) | |
Bachelor | 363 (33.7) | 400 (36.9) | 290 (28.0) | 263 (24.8) | |
Higher | 441 (41.0) | 308 (28.4) | 246 (23.8) | 168 (15.8) | |
Not known | 0 (0.0) | 1 (0.1) | 0 (0.0) | 3 (0.3) | |
Parents married (yes) (%) | 872 (81.0) | 829 (76.4) | 719 (69.5) | 615 (57.9) | <0.001 |
Parental Income (%) | <0.001 | ||||
[<50K] | 144 (13.4) | 171 (15.8) | 252 (24.4) | 399 (37.6) | |
[>=50K & <100K] | 243 (22.6) | 313 (28.8) | 327 (31.6) | 310 (29.2) | |
[>=100K] | 610 (56.7) | 517 (47.6) | 378 (36.6) | 262 (24.7) | |
Refused to state | 79 (7.3) | 84 (7.7) | 77 (7.4) | 91 (8.6) | |
Parental Age (mean (sd)) | 41.55 (6.09) | 41.03 (6.31) | 40.16 (7.16) | 39.16 (7.19) | <0.001 |
3.2. Group factor analysis (GFA) relating SMA to structural estimates.
Cortical thickness (Supp. Figure 1), sulcal depth (Supp. Figure 2), and gray matter volume (Supp. Figure 3) distributions did not show excessive extreme values and were similar to those previously reported (Tamnes et al., 2017; Vandekar et al., 2015). Examining the correlational structure between SMA variables, cortical thickness, sulcal depth, and gray matter volume revealed that variables are relatively strongly correlated within variable groupings, i.e. within a particular measurement domain, but weakly correlated across groups with sulcal depth being the exception showing relatively weak correlation within the group (Suppl. Figure 4). The GFA extracted 132 factors accounting for 58% of the variance (Suppl. Figure 5a) of which 10 factors (each of which accounted for > 1% of the variance) accounted for a total of 37% of the variance between SMA and structural brain indices (Supp. Figure 5b) that were strongly orthogonal (Supp. Figure 6). Only the four SMA-related GFAs that accounted for >1% of the variance across all groups were used in subsequent analyses (i.e., GFAs 1–3, 5, which will now be referred to as the 4th GFA).
The first SMA-related factor, which accounted for about 16% percent of the variance (Figure 1), loaded positively on SMA and negatively on both cortical thickness (most negative for occipital areas and least for anterior cingulate) and GMV (most negative for orbitofrontal areas and least for entorhinal and anterior cingulate) with a mixed loading on sulcal depth (most positive on posterior cingulate, most negative for medial orbitofrontal and temporal pole). The second SMA-related factor, which accounted for 9% of the variance loaded most strongly on gaming with a negative loading on thickness (most negative for superior frontal areas and least for visual cortical areas) but a positive loading on GMV (most positive for orbitofrontal areas and least temporal pole) and a mixed loading on sulcal depth (most positive on superior frontal cortex, most negative for posterior cingulate) (Figure 2). The third SMA-related factor, which accounted for 2% of the variance, loaded strongly on all SMA with mixed and selective loadings on cortical thickness (most negative for visual areas and most positive for temporal and orbitofrontal areas), sulcal depth (most posterior parietal areas, most negative for primary visual cortex), and volumes (most positive for posterior cingulate and temporal cortex, most negative for primary visual cortices) (Figure 3). The fourth SMA-related factor, which accounted for 2% of the variance, also loaded positively on SMA (relatively more on social media activity) and selectively on some areas for cortical thickness (most negative for hippocampus and most positive for visual cortices), sulcal depth (most negative for fusiform area and most positive for visual areas) and volumes (most negative for inferior temporal cortex, most positive for primary visual cortices) (Figure 4).
Figure 1.
General factor analysis of screen media activity, cortical thickness, sulcal depth and gray matter volume related loadings for GFA 1 with 95% credible interval. The median factor loadings and the 95% credible interval drawn from the posterior distribution of the GFA W matrix are shown for cortical thickness, sulcal depth, and gray matter volume.
Figure 2.
General factor analysis of screen media activity, cortical thickness, sulcal depth and gray matter volume related loadings for GFA 2 with 95% credible interval. Panels analogous to Figure 1.
Figure 3.
General factor analysis of screen media activity, cortical thickness, sulcal depth and gray matter volume related loadings for GFA 3 with 95% credible interval. Panels analogous to Figure 1.
Figure 4.
General factor analysis of screen media activity, cortical thickness, sulcal depth and gray matter volume related loadings for GFA 5 with 95% credible interval. Panels analogous to Figure 1.
3.3. Generalized mixed models predicting psychopathology and cognition.
Mixed model analysis showed that the SMA-related GFA model for predicting overall internalizing behaviors (df=25, AIC: 11726, 2.53% accounted covariance(AC)) did not significantly improve over the base model (df=21, AIC=11727, 2.35% AC) (Chî2 = 9.2492, p=0.05516), that comprised age, sex, BMI, race/ethnicity, parental education, parental marital status, parental age, and parental income (Figure 5). In comparison, the SMA-related GFA model (AIC=11705, df=25, 4.36% AC) for predicting overall externalizing behaviors significantly improved (Chî2 = 48.081, p= 9.076e-10) the base model (df=21, AIC=11746, 3.29% AC). Specifically, those individuals with higher SMA-related GFA 1 (β=0.059) and GFA 4 (β=0.095) scores had significantly higher externalization scores (see Figure 6). The SMA-related GFA model for predicting fluid intelligence (df = 25, AIC=10333, 10% AC) significantly (Chî2 = 34.181, p= 6.842e-07) improved over the base model (df=21, AIC=10359, 9.2% AC). Specifically, those individuals with higher SMA-related GFA 2 (β=0.043) had higher fluid abilities whereas those with higher SMA-related GFA 4 (β=−0.086) scored lower on fluid abilities (Figure 7). Lastly, the SMA-related GFA model for predicting crystalized intelligence (df = 25, AIC: 9878.6, 19.1% AC) significantly (Chî2 = 87.655, p= 2.2e-16) improved the base model (df=21, AIC: 9958.3, 17.2% AC). Specifically, those individuals with higher SMA-related GFA 1 (β=−0.109) and higher SMA-related GFA 4 (β=−0.080) had lower crystalized abilities whereas those with higher SMA-related GFA 2 (β=0.0871) showed better performance on crystalized abilities (Figure 8).
Figure 5.
Mixed model results for internalizing pathology. (a) median coefficients and 95% confidence interval, (b) marginal effect plots separated by sex.
Figure 6.
Mixed model results for externalizing pathology, panels analogous to Figure 5.
Figure 7.
Mixed model results for fluid intelligence, panels analogous to Figure 5.
Figure 8.
Mixed model results for crystallized intelligence, panels analogous to Figure 5.
4. Discussion
This investigation applied a multivariate exploratory approach to the first data release of the ABCD study (1) to parse the relationship between SMA and structural brain indices (i.e., cortical thickness, sulcal depth, and gray matter volume) and (2) to evaluate its impact on psychopathology and overall cognitive functioning. First, the Group Factor Analysis extracted four SMA-related factors that integrated across cortical thickness, sulcal depth, and gray matter volume. Second, these factors revealed that different brain regions supporting similar functions were correlated with SMA. Third, some but not all of the factors related to SMA were related to greater externalizing as opposed to internalizing psychopathology and were predominately related to crystalized intelligence and, to a lesser extent, fluid intelligence. Taken together, this brain behavior investigation shows that SMA is significantly related to brain structure with mixed consequences for psychopathology and cognitive performance.
4.1. Screen Media Activity and Brain Structure
The SMA-related GFAs linking to brain structure showed the greatest effect size and the most consistent loadings across factors for visual areas including occipital cortex, calcarine areas, and other primary visual cortices. Thus, those individuals with significant exposure to activities that engage the visual system (TV or video watching, gaming, and social network activities) show structural patterns suggestive of greater maturation in the visual system (i.e., thinner cortex, reduced volume, and a more complex pattern of changes in sulcal depth). Several studies have shown that maturational patterns show a strong covariance across brain areas of similar function (Alexander-Bloch et al., 2013; Geng et al., 2017; Sotiras et al., 2017). Specifically, several investigations have suggested that homologous brain areas (Khundrakpam et al., 2017) undergo coordinated cortical thinning guided by evolutionary novelty and functional specialization (Sotiras et al., 2017). Moreover, these structural brain-related changes have been related to individual differences for specific brain areas, e.g. the anterior cingulate to behavioral inhibition (Sylvester et al., 2016), insula to empathy (Bernhardt et al., 2014), ventromedial prefrontal cortex to anxiety (Newman et al., 2016), prefrontal cortex to levels of depression (Vijayakumar et al., 2017), and general cortical thinning to personality characteristics (Ferschmann et al., 2018). Other investigators reported altered resting state connectivity patterns between sensorimotor and cognitive control regions as function of screen-based media use and suggested that excessive screen time may adversely affect cognitive control (Horowitz-Kraus and Hutton, 2017). The current investigation adds an important new element to these prior studies. Specifically, SMA is related across subjects to areas that are important for sensory processing but also for higher order cortical functions, i.e. the prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate. However, it is important to point out that SMA is related to several correlated brain structural patterns that are orthogonal, i.e. the latent variables describing the relationship between brain structure and SMA are not correlated. Thus, screen media activity cannot be reduced to a unidimensional impact on brain structure. Taken together, although there is some evidence that SMA-related latent variables are associated with more psychopathology and poorer performance on cognitive tests, there are other latent variables that show no such relationship. Thus, it is difficult to conclude that SMA related brain structural characteristics have uniformly negative consequences.
4.2. Psychopathology, Cognition, and SMA Related Brain Structures
The complex relationship between SMA and brain structure is further supported by examining its psychopathological and cognitive correlates. There was little evidence that SMA related brain structure differences were associated with internalizing pathology, i.e. increase in anxiety, depression, or other avoidance or withdrawal behaviors. Some have suggested that reduced volume and cortical thickness in frontolimbic regions may serve as a neurobiological predictors of emergent internalizing psychopathology in adolescence (Jones et al., 2017). However, others have observed delayed thinning in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in high anxious children (Newman et al., 2016). Thus, internalizing psychopathology may play a role in the development of cortical structures during adolescence; however, the current ABCD sample provides little evidence that SMA contributes to brain structure characteristics that can be related to internalizing behaviors. In comparison, we did find a strong relationship to externalizing psychopathology. Specifically, those youth who had higher scores on GFA 1 and 4, i.e. had thinner occipital cortices and smaller volume in orbitofrontal areas as well as thinner hippocampi and smaller inferior-temporal cortical volumes, showed greater levels of externalizing psychopathology. Some investigators have reported, among other areas, increased externalizing in individuals with thinner parahippocampal gyrus (Gold et al., 2016), whereas others showed that youths with conduct disorder had reduced cortical thickness in the superior temporal gyrus and sulcal pathology in orbitofrontal cortex, as well as increased cortical folding in the insula (Fairchild et al., 2015). The current study adds to these findings that some of these effects may also be related to SMA.
The complexity of SMA associated structural brain characteristics extends to the relationship with cognitive performance. In particular, for both fluid and crystalized intelligence, some GFAs were related to better performance whereas others related to poorer performance on these tests. For fluid intelligence, GFA2, which loaded on gaming activities and greater cortical thinning in prefrontal areas and increased orbitofrontal volume, showed a positive relationship, whereas GFA4 which loaded on social media and reduced hippocampal thickness and lower inferior-temporal cortical gray matter volume, showed a strong negative relationship. Others have shown a relationship between cognitive abilities and structural variability in medial frontal lobes and paracingulate (Fornito et al., 2008). Moreover, a series of investigations have examined the relationship between brain structures and cognitive control in the context of internet gaming disorder. Some of the investigators found widespread reduction of cortical volume (Lin et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2015; Yuan et al., 2013), whereas others have found more circumscribed cortical thickness and gray matter volume reduction most notably within the orbitofrontal cortex (Altbacker et al., 2016; Park et al., 2017; Zhou et al., 2017), but also in frontal pole volume (Kuhn and Gallinat, 2015). The diversity of these findings underscores that SMA does not have a simple effect on fluid intelligence but rather exerts differential effects via different brain networks. The SMA associated structural brain characteristics were most strongly related to crystalized intelligence, which are more dependent on experience, represent accumulated store of verbal knowledge and skills, and are influenced by education and cultural exposure (Akshoomoff et al., 2013). Here, GFA 1 and 4, which were general and social media related SMA that loaded strongly on occipital and orbitofrontal areas, showed the strongest negative relationship to crystallized intelligence. In contrast, GFA2, which loaded strongly gaming and was associated with thinner prefrontal cortices but greater orbitofrontal cortical volume had a positive relationship to this ability. Since crystallized cognitive abilities have a strong cultural and educational component and it should not be surprising that these GFAs are also related to parental education, race/ethnicity, and parental income (Suppl. Table 1). Thus, future investigations will need to further delineate the complex relationship between SMA, structural brain characteristics, and other moderating influences. Nevertheless, these results support the general conclusion that SMA associated brain characteristics are not uniformly related to better or poorer cognitive performance. Instead, specific components that affect particular brain areas contribute differentially to cognition.
4.3. Limitations
First, this is a cross-sectional assessment, which enables establishment of associations but does not allow drawing causal inferences. Although multivariate methods such as GFA enable differential examination of the impact of SMA and structural brain characteristics on outcome variables, they cannot address the “chicken and egg” question. Therefore, the longitudinal component of ABCD is essential to begin to delineate causal longitudinal pathways. Second, the current analysis pathway used the standard FreeSurfer regions (Desikan et al., 2006); however, others have utilized a higher resolution approach (Vandekar et al., 2015) or have even suggested a connection-based approach, which may be more anatomically meaningful than the traditional lobar structure (Kruggel, 2018). Third, although we have focused on the relationship between SMA and brain structure, others have pointed out that developmental trajectories of structural brain changes are influenced in complex ways by multiple factors (Foulkes and Blakemore, 2018) and that better control for these variables may yield more precise results (LeWinn et al., 2017). Fourth, subcortical structures, which also undergo significant development, were not available from the official ABCD data release, thus it was not possible to relate SMA to striatal characteristics, which could be highly informative. Therefore, future investigations may need to employ complex machine learning approaches aimed at identifying individual-level patterns that meaningfully relate variables across levels of analyses, i.e. examine cognitively relevant brain structures to specific SMA in socio-demographically select individuals exposed to particular environmental characteristics. Fifth, the SMA-related factors provide a latent variable approach to examine media related structural brain correlation networks with respect to other youth activities. Currently, we did not take into account the engagement of youth in other recreational activities. Moreover, school-related screen media activity was no considered in the screen media assessment, which may ultimately under-estimate the time spent on SMA. However, it will be important to determine whether these factors can be used to predict other recreational activities.
4.4. Conclusions
This investigation of the ABCD cohort aimed at relating an important youth behavior, i.e. screen media activity, to structural characteristics of the brain and revealed that there are significant associations but that they are complex. Whereas some SMA associated brain structures have relevance for externalizing psychopathology, fluid and crystallized intelligence, others do not. Moreover, whereas some SMA associated brain structures are related to poorer cognitive performance, others are related to better cognitive performance. This diversity of findings provides an important public health message, i.e. screen media activity is not simply “bad for the brain” or “bad for brain related functioning”. Instead, future investigations will need to examine how various forms of screen media activity influence specific psychopathology and cognitive functions, and how this influences changes throughout development.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements:
Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children age 9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041022, U01DA041025, U01DA041028, U01DA041048, U01DA041089, U01DA041093, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041120, U01DA041134, U01DA041148, U01DA041156, U01DA041174, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/nih-collaborators. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/principal-investigators.html. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The ABCD data used in this report came from https://dx.doi.org/10.15154/1412097.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest: The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
References
- 2010. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, 2009. The Achenbach System of Emprically Based Assessment (ASEBA): Development, Findings, Theory and Applications University of Vermong Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families, Burlington, VT. [Google Scholar]
- Akshoomoff N, Beaumont JL, Bauer PJ, Dikmen SS, Gershon RC, Mungas D, Slotkin J, Tulsky D, Weintraub S, Zelazo PD, Heaton RK, 2013. VIII. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): composite scores of crystallized, fluid, and overall cognition. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 78, 119–132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander-Bloch A, Raznahan A, Bullmore E, Giedd J, 2013. The convergence of maturational change and structural covariance in human cortical networks. J Neurosci 33, 2889–2899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Altbacker A, Plozer E, Darnai G, Perlaki G, Horvath R, Orsi G, Nagy SA, Bogner P, Schwarcz A, Kovacs N, Komoly S, Clemens Z, Janszky J, 2016. Problematic internet use is associated with structural alterations in the brain reward system in females. Brain Imaging Behav 10, 953–959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernhardt BC, Klimecki OM, Leiberg S, Singer T, 2014. Structural covariance networks of the dorsal anterior insula predict females’ individual differences in empathic responding. Cereb Cortex 24, 2189–2198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Besteher B, Gaser C, Spalthoff R, Nenadic I, 2017. Associations between urban upbringing and cortical thickness and gyrification. J Psychiatr Res 95, 114–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardinale F, Chinnici G, Bramerio M, Mai R, Sartori I, Cossu M, Lo Russo G, Castana L, Colombo N, Caborni C, De Momi E, Ferrigno G, 2014. Validation of FreeSurfer-estimated brain cortical thickness: comparison with histologic measurements. Neuroinformatics 12, 535–542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey BJ, Cannonier T, Conley MI, Cohen AO, Barch DM, Heitzeg MM, Soules ME, Teslovich T, Dellarco DV, Garavan H, Orr CA, Wager TD, Banich MT, Speer NK, Sutherland MT, Riedel MC, Dick AS, Bjork JM, Thomas KM, Chaarani B, Mejia MH, Hagler DJ Jr., Daniela Cornejo M, Sicat CS, Harms MP, Dosenbach NUF, Rosenberg M, Earl E, Bartsch H, Watts R, Polimeni JR, Kuperman JM, Fair DA, Dale AM, 2018. The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev Cogn Neurosci 32, 43–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cerniglia L, Zoratto F, Cimino S, Laviola G, Ammaniti M, Adriani W, 2016. Internet Addiction in adolescence: Neurobiological, psychosocial and clinical issues. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 76, 174–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI, 1999. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, Buckner RL, Dale AM, Maguire RP, Hyman BT, Albert MS, Killiany RJ, 2006. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968–980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ducharme S, Albaugh MD, Nguyen TV, Hudziak JJ, Mateos-Perez JM, Labbe A, Evans AC, Karama S, 2016. Trajectories of cortical thickness maturation in normal brain development--The importance of quality control procedures. Neuroimage 125, 267–279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fairchild G, Toschi N, Hagan CC, Goodyer IM, Calder AJ, Passamonti L, 2015. Cortical thickness, surface area, and folding alterations in male youths with conduct disorder and varying levels of callous-unemotional traits. Neuroimage Clin 8, 253–260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson CJ, 2015. Do Angry Birds Make for Angry Children? A Meta-Analysis of Video Game Influences on Children’s and Adolescents’ Aggression, Mental Health, Prosocial Behavior, and Academic Performance. Perspect Psychol Sci 10, 646–666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson CJ, 2017. Everything in Moderation: Moderate Use of Screens Unassociated with Child Behavior Problems. Psychiatr Q 88, 797–805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferschmann L, Fjell AM, Vollrath ME, Grydeland H, Walhovd KB, Tamnes CK, 2018. Personality Traits Are Associated With Cortical Development Across Adolescence: A Longitudinal Structural MRI Study. Child Dev [DOI] [PubMed]
- Fischer P, Greitemeyer T, Kastenmuller A, Vogrincic C, Sauer A, 2011. The effects of risk-glorifying media exposure on risk-positive cognitions, emotions, and behaviors: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull 137, 367–390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Dale AM, 2000. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97, 11050–11055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Liu A, Dale AM, 2001. Automated manifold surgery: constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 20, 70–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, van der Kouwe A, Killiany R, Kennedy D, Klaveness S, Montillo A, Makris N, Rosen B, Dale AM, 2002. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33, 341–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Sereno MI, Dale AM, 1999. Cortical surface-based analysis. II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9, 195–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fjell AM, Grydeland H, Krogsrud SK, Amlien I, Rohani DA, Ferschmann L, Storsve AB, Tamnes CK, Sala-Llonch R, Due-Tonnessen P, Bjornerud A, Solsnes AE, Haberg AK, Skranes J, Bartsch H, Chen CH, Thompson WK, Panizzon MS, Kremen WS, Dale AM, Walhovd KB, 2015. Development and aging of cortical thickness correspond to genetic organization patterns. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112, 15462–15467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fornito A, Wood SJ, Whittle S, Fuller J, Adamson C, Saling MM, Velakoulis D, Pantelis C, Yucel M, 2008. Variability of the paracingulate sulcus and morphometry of the medial frontal cortex: associations with cortical thickness, surface area, volume, and sulcal depth. Hum Brain Mapp 29, 222–236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foulkes L, Blakemore SJ, 2018. Studying individual differences in human adolescent brain development. Nat Neurosci 21, 315–323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geng X, Li G, Lu Z, Gao W, Wang L, Shen D, Zhu H, Gilmore JH, 2017. Structural and Maturational Covariance in Early Childhood Brain Development. Cereb Cortex 27, 1795–1807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giedd JN, Raznahan A, Alexander-Bloch A, Schmitt E, Gogtay N, Rapoport JL, 2015. Child psychiatry branch of the National Institute of Mental Health longitudinal structural magnetic resonance imaging study of human brain development. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 43–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold AL, Sheridan MA, Peverill M, Busso DS, Lambert HK, Alves S, Pine DS, McLaughlin KA, 2016. Childhood abuse and reduced cortical thickness in brain regions involved in emotional processing. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 57, 1154–1164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldfield GS, Murray M, Maras D, Wilson AL, Phillips P, Kenny GP, Hadjiyannakis S, Alberga A, Cameron JD, Tulluch H, Sigal RJ, 2016. Screen time is associated with depressive symptomatology among obese adolescents: a HEARTY study. Eur J Pediatr 175, 909–919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hale L, Guan S, 2015. Screen time and sleep among school-aged children and adolescents: a systematic literature review. Sleep Med Rev 21, 50–58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han X, Jovicich J, Salat D, van der Kouwe A, Quinn B, Czanner S, Busa E, Pacheco J, Albert M, Killiany R, Maguire P, Rosas D, Makris N, Dale A, Dickerson B, Fischl B, 2006. Reliability of MRI-derived measurements of human cerebral cortical thickness: the effects of field strength, scanner upgrade and manufacturer. Neuroimage 32, 180–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holfeld B, Sukhawathanakul P, 2017. Associations Between Internet Attachment, Cyber Victimization, and Internalizing Symptoms Among Adolescents. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 20, 91–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horowitz-Kraus T, Hutton JS, 2017. Brain connectivity in children is increased by the time they spend reading books and decreased by the length of exposure to screen-based media. Acta Paediatr 107, 685–693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houston SM, Herting MM, Sowell ER, 2014. The neurobiology of childhood structural brain development: conception through adulthood. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 16, 3–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iscan Z, Jin TB, Kendrick A, Szeglin B, Lu H, Trivedi M, Fava M, McGrath PJ, Weissman M, Kurian BT, Adams P, Weyandt S, Toups M, Carmody T, McInnis M, Cusin C, Cooper C, Oquendo MA, Parsey RV, DeLorenzo C, 2015. Test-retest reliability of freesurfer measurements within and between sites: Effects of visual approval process. Hum Brain Mapp 36, 3472–3485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones SA, Morales AM, Lavine JB, Nagel BJ, 2017. Convergent neurobiological predictors of emergent psychopathology during adolescence. Birth Defects Res 109, 1613–1622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenney EL, Gortmaker SL, 2017. United States Adolescents’ Television, Computer, Videogame, Smartphone, and Tablet Use: Associations with Sugary Drinks, Sleep, Physical Activity, and Obesity. J Pediatr 182, 144–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khundrakpam BS, Lewis JD, Jeon S, Kostopoulos P, Itturia Medina Y, Chouinard-Decorte F, Evans AC, 2017. Exploring Individual Brain Variability during Development based on Patterns of Maturational Coupling of Cortical Thickness: A Longitudinal MRI Study. Cereb Cortex [DOI] [PubMed]
- Klami A, Virtanen S, Leppaaho E, Kaski S, 2015. Group Factor Analysis. IEEE Trans Neural Netw Learn Syst 26, 2136–2147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kruggel F, 2018. The macro-structural variability of the human neocortex. Neuroimage 172, 620–630. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn S, Gallinat J, 2015. Brains online: structural and functional correlates of habitual Internet use. Addict Biol 20, 415–422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leppäaho E, Ammad-ud-din M, Kaski S, 2017. GFA: Exploratory Analysis of Multiple Data Sources with Group Factor Analysis. Journal of Machine Learning Research 18, 1–5. [Google Scholar]
- LeWinn KZ, Sheridan MA, Keyes KM, Hamilton A, McLaughlin KA, 2017. Sample composition alters associations between age and brain structure. Nat Commun 8, 874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li B, Friston KJ, Liu J, Liu Y, Zhang G, Cao F, Su L, Yao S, Lu H, Hu D, 2014. Impaired frontal-basal ganglia connectivity in adolescents with internet addiction. Sci Rep 4, 5027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li SC, Lindenberger U, Hommel B, Aschersleben G, Prinz W, Baltes PB, 2004. Transformations in the couplings among intellectual abilities and constituent cognitive processes across the life span. Psychol Sci 15, 155–163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin X, Dong G, Wang Q, Du X, 2015. Abnormal gray matter and white matter volume in ‘Internet gaming addicts’. Addict Behav 40, 137–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loprinzi PD, Davis RE, 2016. Secular trends in parent-reported television viewing among children in the United States, 2001–2012. Child Care Health Dev 42, 288–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luciana M, Bjork JM, Nagel BJ, Barch DM, Gonzalez R, Nixon SJ, Banich MT, 2018. Adolescent neurocognitive development and impacts of substance use: Overview of the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) baseline neurocognition battery. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Newman E, Thompson WK, Bartsch H, Hagler DJ Jr., Chen CH, Brown TT, Kuperman JM, McCabe C, Chung Y, Libiger O, Akshoomoff N, Bloss CS, Casey BJ, Chang L, Ernst TM, Frazier JA, Gruen JR, Kennedy DN, Murray SS, Sowell ER, Schork N, Kenet T, Kaufmann WE, Mostofsky S, Amaral DG, Dale AM, Jernigan TL, 2016. Anxiety is related to indices of cortical maturation in typically developing children and adolescents. Brain Struct Funct 221, 3013–3025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park B, Han DH, Roh S, 2017. Neurobiological findings related to Internet use disorders. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 71, 467–478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfefferbaum A, Rohlfing T, Pohl KM, Lane B, Chu W, Kwon D, Nolan Nichols B, Brown SA, Tapert SF, Cummins K, Thompson WK, Brumback T, Meloy MJ, Jernigan TL, Dale A, Colrain IM, Baker FC, Prouty D, De Bellis MD, Voyvodic JT, Clark DB, Luna B, Chung T, Nagel BJ, Sullivan EV, 2016. Adolescent Development of Cortical and White Matter Structure in the NCANDA Sample: Role of Sex, Ethnicity, Puberty, and Alcohol Drinking. Cereb Cortex 26, 4101–4121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raznahan A, Lerch JP, Lee N, Greenstein D, Wallace GL, Stockman M, Clasen L, Shaw PW, Giedd JN, 2011. Patterns of coordinated anatomical change in human cortical development: a longitudinal neuroimaging study of maturational coupling. Neuron 72, 873–884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rikkers W, Lawrence D, Hafekost J, Zubrick SR, 2016. Internet use and electronic gaming by children and adolescents with emotional and behavioural problems in Australia -results from the second Child and Adolescent Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing. BMC Public Health 16, 399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saggar M, Hosseini SM, Bruno JL, Quintin EM, Raman MM, Kesler SR, Reiss AL, 2015. Estimating individual contribution from group-based structural correlation networks. Neuroimage 120, 274–284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Segonne F, Dale AM, Busa E, Glessner M, Salat D, Hahn HK, Fischl B, 2004. A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. Neuroimage 22, 1060–1075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sotiras A, Toledo JB, Gur RE, Gur RC, Satterthwaite TD, Davatzikos C, 2017. Patterns of coordinated cortical remodeling during adolescence and their associations with functional specialization and evolutionary expansion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114, 3527–3532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sylvester CM, Barch DM, Harms MP, Belden AC, Oakberg TJ, Gold AL, White LK, Benson BE, Troller-Renfree S, Degnan KA, Henderson HA, Luby JL, Fox NA, Pine DS, 2016. Early Childhood Behavioral Inhibition Predicts Cortical Thickness in Adulthood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 55, 122–129.e121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamnes CK, Herting MM, Goddings AL, Meuwese R, Blakemore SJ, Dahl RE, Guroglu B, Raznahan A, Sowell ER, Crone EA, Mills KL, 2017. Development of the Cerebral Cortex across Adolescence: A Multisample Study of Inter-Related Longitudinal Changes in Cortical Volume, Surface Area, and Thickness. J Neurosci 37, 3402–3412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Team, R., 2016a. rmarkdown: R Markdown Document Conversion. [Google Scholar]
- Team, R., 2016b. RStudio: Integrated Development Environment for R. RStudio, Inc., Boston, MA. [Google Scholar]
- Tipping ME, 2001. Sparse Bayesian learning and the relevance vector machine. Journal of Machine Learning Research 1, 211–244. [Google Scholar]
- Twenge JM, Joiner TE, Rogers ML, Martin GN, 2017. Increases in Depressive Symptoms, Suicide-Related Outcomes, and Suicide Rates Among U.S. Adolescents After 2010 and Links to Increased New Media Screen Time. Clinical Psychological Science 6, 3–17. [Google Scholar]
- Vandekar SN, Shinohara RT, Raznahan A, Roalf DR, Ross M, DeLeo N, Ruparel K, Verma R, Wolf DH, Gur RC, Gur RE, Satterthwaite TD, 2015. Topologically dissociable patterns of development of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci 35, 599–609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vijayakumar N, Allen NB, Dennison M, Byrne ML, Simmons JG, Whittle S, 2017. Cortico-amygdalar maturational coupling is associated with depressive symptom trajectories during adolescence. Neuroimage 156, 403–411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Koob GF, Croyle RT, Bianchi DW, Gordon JA, Koroshetz WJ, Perez-Stable EJ, Riley WT, Bloch MH, Conway K, Deeds BG, Dowling GJ, Grant S, Howlett KD, Matochik JA, Morgan GD, Murray MM, Noronha A, Spong CY, Wargo EM, Warren KR, Weiss SRB, 2017. The conception of the ABCD study: From substance use to a broad NIH collaboration. Dev Cogn Neurosci 32, 4–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vuoksimaa E, Panizzon MS, Chen CH, Fiecas M, Eyler LT, Fennema-Notestine C, Hagler DJ Jr., Franz CE, Jak AJ, Lyons MJ, Neale MC, Rinker DA, Thompson WK, Tsuang MT, Dale AM, Kremen WS, 2016. Is bigger always better? The importance of cortical configuration with respect to cognitive ability. Neuroimage 129, 356–366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walhovd KB, Krogsrud SK, Amlien IK, Bartsch H, Bjornerud A, Due-Tonnessen P, Grydeland H, Hagler DJ Jr., Haberg AK, Kremen WS, Ferschmann L, Nyberg L, Panizzon MS, Rohani DA, Skranes J, Storsve AB, Solsnes AE, Tamnes CK, Thompson WK, Reuter C, Dale AM, Fjell AM, 2016. Neurodevelopmental origins of lifespan changes in brain and cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113, 9357–9362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang H, Jin C, Yuan K, Shakir TM, Mao C, Niu X, Niu C, Guo L, Zhang M, 2015. The alteration of gray matter volume and cognitive control in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Front Behav Neurosci 9, 64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang T, Wang K, Qu H, Zhou J, Li Q, Deng Z, Du X, Lv F, Ren G, Guo J, Qiu J, Xie P, 2016. Disorganized cortical thickness covariance network in major depressive disorder implicated by aberrant hubs in large-scale networks. Sci Rep 6, 27964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood SN, 2017. Generalized additive models: an introduction with R, 2nd Edition ed. Chapman Hall/CRC, New York. [Google Scholar]
- Xia CH, Ma Z, Ciric R, Gu S, Betzel RF, Kaczkurkin AN, Calkins ME, Cook PA, García de la Garza A, Vandekar SN, Cui Z, Moore TM, Roalf DR, Ruparel K, Wolf DH, Davatzikos C, Gur RC, Gur RE, Shinohara RT, Bassett DS, Satterthwaite TD, 2018. Linked dimensions of psychopathology and connectivity in functional brain networks. Nat Commun 9, 3003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoshida K, Bohn. J, 2017. tableone: Create ‘Table 1’ to Describe Baseline Characteristics
- Yuan K, Cheng P, Dong T, Bi Y, Xing L, Yu D, Zhao L, Dong M, von Deneen KM, Liu Y, Qin W, Tian J, 2013. Cortical thickness abnormalities in late adolescence with online gaming addiction. PLoS One 8, e53055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou F, Montag C, Sariyska R, Lachmann B, Reuter M, Weber B, Trautner P, Kendrick KM, Markett S, Becker B, 2017. Orbitofrontal gray matter deficits as marker of Internet gaming disorder: converging evidence from a cross-sectional and prospective longitudinal design. Addict Biol [DOI] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.