Skip to main content
The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews logoLink to The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews
. 2018 Aug 9;2018(8):CD007813. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD007813.pub3

Amphetamines for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults

Xavier Castells 1,, Lídia Blanco‐Silvente 1, Ruth Cunill 2
Editor: Cochrane Developmental, Psychosocial and Learning Problems Group
PMCID: PMC6513464  PMID: 30091808

Abstract

Background

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a childhood‐onset disorder characterised by inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity. ADHD can persist into adulthood and can affects individuals' social and occupational functioning, as well as their quality of life and health. ADHD is frequently associated with other mental disorders such as substance use disorders and anxiety and affective disorders. Amphetamines are used to treat adults with ADHD, but uncertainties about their efficacy and safety remain.

Objectives

To examine the efficacy and safety of amphetamines for adults with ADHD.

Search methods

In August 2017, we searched CENTRAL, MEDLINE, Embase, PsycINFO, 10 other databases, and two trials registers, and we ran citation searches for included studies. We also contacted the corresponding authors of all included studies, other experts in the field, and the pharmaceutical company, Shire, and we searched the reference lists of retrieved studies and reviews for other published, unpublished, or ongoing studies. For each included study, we performed a citation search in Web of Science to identify any later studies that may have cited it.

Selection criteria

We searched for randomised controlled trials comparing the efficacy of amphetamines (at any dose) for ADHD in adults aged 18 years and over against placebo or an active intervention.

Data collection and analysis

Two review authors extracted data from each included study. We used the standardised mean difference (SMD) and the risk ratio (RR) to assess continuous and dichotomous outcomes, respectively. We conducted a stratified analysis to determine the influence of moderating variables. We assessed trials for risk of bias and drew a funnel plot to investigate the possibility of publication bias. We rated the quality of the evidence using the GRADE approach, which yielded high, moderate, low, or very low quality ratings based on evaluation of within‐trial risk of bias, directness of evidence, heterogeneity of data; precision of effect estimates, and risk of publication bias.

Main results

We included 19 studies that investigated three types of amphetamines: dexamphetamine (10.2 mg/d to 21.8 mg/d), lisdexamfetamine (30 mg/d to 70 mg/d), and mixed amphetamine salts (MAS; 12.5 mg/d to 80 mg/d). These studies enrolled 2521 participants; most were middle‐aged (35.3 years), Caucasian males (57.2%), with a combined type of ADHD (78.8%). Eighteen studies were conducted in the USA, and one study was conducted in both Canada and the USA. Ten were multi‐site studies. All studies were placebo‐controlled, and three also included an active comparator: guanfacine, modafinil, or paroxetine. Most studies had short‐term follow‐up and a mean study length of 5.3 weeks.

We found no studies that had low risk of bias in all domains of the Cochrane 'Risk of bias’ tool, mainly because amphetamines have powerful subjective effects that may reveal the assigned treatment, but also because we noted attrition bias, and because we could not rule out the possibility of a carry‐over effect in studies that used a cross‐over design.

Sixteen studies were funded by the pharmaceutical industry, one study was publicly funded, and two studies did not report their funding sources.

Amphetamines versus placebo

Severity of ADHD symptoms: we found low‐ to very low‐quality evidence suggesting that amphetamines reduced the severity of ADHD symptoms as rated by clinicians (SMD −0.90, 95% confidence interval (CI) −1.04 to −0.75; 13 studies, 2028 participants) and patients (SMD −0.51, 95% CI −0.75 to −0.28; six studies, 120 participants).

Retention: overall, we found low‐quality evidence suggesting that amphetamines did not improve retention in treatment (risk ratio (RR) 1.06, 95% CI 0.99 to 1.13; 17 studies, 2323 participants).

Adverse events: we found that amphetamines were associated with an increased proportion of patients who withdrew because of adverse events (RR 2.69, 95% CI 1.63 to 4.45; 17 studies, 2409 participants).

Type of amphetamine: we found differences between amphetamines for the severity of ADHD symptoms as rated by clinicians. Both lisdexamfetamine (SMD −1.06, 95% CI −1.26 to −0.85; seven studies, 896 participants; low‐quality evidence) and MAS (SMD −0.80, 95% CI −0.93 to −0.66; five studies, 1083 participants; low‐quality evidence) reduced the severity of ADHD symptoms. In contrast, we found no evidence to suggest that dexamphetamine reduced the severity of ADHD symptoms (SMD −0.24, 95% CI −0.80 to 0.32; one study, 49 participants; very low‐quality evidence). In addition, all amphetamines were efficacious in reducing the severity of ADHD symptoms as rated by patients (dexamphetamine: SMD −0.77, 95% CI −1.14 to −0.40; two studies, 35 participants; low‐quality evidence; lisdexamfetamine: SMD −0.33, 95% CI −0.65 to −0.01; three studies, 67 participants; low‐quality evidence; MAS: SMD −0.45, 95% CI −1.02 to 0.12; one study, 18 participants; very low‐quality evidence).

Dose at study completion: different doses of amphetamines did not appear to be associated with differences in efficacy.

Type of drug‐release formulation: we investigated immediate‐ and sustained‐release formulations but found no differences between them for any outcome.

Amphetamines versus other drugs

We found no evidence that amphetamines improved ADHD symptom severity compared to other drug interventions.

Authors' conclusions

Amphetamines improved the severity of ADHD symptoms, as assessed by clinicians or patients, in the short term but did not improve retention to treatment. Amphetamines were associated with higher attrition due to adverse events. The short duration of studies coupled with their restrictive inclusion criteria limits the external validity of these findings. Furthermore, none of the included studies had an overall low risk of bias. Overall, the evidence generated by this review is of low or very low quality.

Plain language summary

Amphetamines for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults

Background

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a childhood‐onset psychiatric disorder that can persist into adulthood in up to 50% of patients. From a clinical point of view, ADHD is characterised by hyperactivity, mood instability, irritability, difficulties in maintaining attention, lack of organisation, and impulsive behaviours. Occurrence of other disorders at the the same time is common, especially mood disorders and substance abuse. Amphetamines (a type of stimulant) are thought to improve ADHD symptoms, but there are concerns about how safe they are for regular use by patients with ADHD.

Review question

We examined whether treatment with amphetamines improves the symptoms of ADHD in adults.

Study characteristics

Reviewers found 19 studies, which enrolled 2521 patients. Most patients were male (57.2%), middle‐aged (mean age 35.3 years) Caucasians (84.5%). These studies compared amphetamines to placebo (something that looks like an amphetamine but with no active ingredient), and three studies also compared amphetamines with other drugs such as guanfacine, modafinil, and paroxetine. In this review, we assessed the effects of three different kinds of amphetamines: dexamphetamine (from 10.2 to 21.8 mg/d), lisdexamfetamine (from 30 to 70 mg/d), and mixed amphetamine salts (MAS) (from 12.5 to 80 mg/d). Treatment length ranged from one to 20 weeks. Eighteen studies were conducted in the USA and one study in Canada and the USA. Ten studies were conducted at multiple sites. Study funding was reported in all but two studies. Sixteen studies were funded by the manufacturer, and one was funded by government agencies.

All amphetamines reduced the severity of ADHD symptoms as rated by patients. Lisdexamfetamine and MAS also reduced the severity of ADHD symptoms as rated by clinicians, but dexamphetamine did not. Overall, amphetamines did not make people more likely to stay in treatment and were associated with higher risk of treatment ending early as the result of adverse events. We found no evidence suggesting that higher doses worked better than lower ones. We did not find any difference in effectiveness between amphetamines that act for longer periods of time versus those that act for shorter periods of time. Therefore, it appears that short‐term treatment with amphetamines reduces the severity of ADHD symptoms, but studies assessing the effects of amphetamines for longer periods of time are needed. We found no differences in effectiveness between amphetamines and guanfacine, modafinil, or paroxetine.

Quality of the evidence

The quality of the evidence was low to very low for all outcomes for several reasons, namely, it was possible for patients to know the treatment they were taking; the number of studies and included patients was low, leading to imprecise results for many outcomes; the studies had problems in their design; and, for some outcomes, results varied across trials.

Summary of findings

Summary of findings for the main comparison. Amphetamines versus placebo for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults.

Amphetamines versus placebo for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults
Patient or population: adult patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
 Settings: outpatients
 Intervention: amphetamines
 Comparison: placebo
Outcomes Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI) Relative effect
 (95% CI) Number of participants
 (studies) Quality of the evidence
 (GRADE) Comments
Assumed risk Corresponding risk
Placebo Amphetamines
Dexamphetamine
ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated
 Assessed with ADHD‐RS‐IV
 Follow‐up: post intervention 
 (mean 20 weeks) Mean clinician‐rated ADHD symptom severity score in the intervention groups was 0.24 standard deviations lower (0.80 lower to 0.32 higher) 49
 (1 study) ⊕⊝⊝⊝
 Very lowa,b,c An SMD of 0.24 can be considered a small effect size.
ADHD symptom severity: patient rated
 Assessed with DSM‐IV ADHD Behavior Checklist for Adults
 Follow‐up: post intervention 
 (mean 2 weeks) Mean patient‐rated ADHD symptom severity score in the intervention groups was 0.77 standard deviations lower (1.14 lower to 0.4 lower) 35
 (2 studies) ⊕⊕⊝⊝
 Lowa,c,d An SMD of 0.77 can be considered a medium effect size.
Lisdexamfetamine
ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated
 Assessed with ADHD‐RS‐IV and CAARS
 Follow‐up: post intervention 
 (1‐10 weeks) Mean clinician‐rated ADHD symptom severity score in the intervention groups was 1.06 standard deviations lower (1.26 lower to 0.85 lower) 896
 (7 studies) ⊕⊕⊝⊝
 Lowc,e,f,g An SMD of 1.06 can be considered a large effect size.
ADHD symptom severity: patient rated
 Assessed with CAARS
 Follow‐up: post intervention 
 (1‐4 weeks) Mean patient‐rated ADHD symptom severity score in the intervention groups was 0.33 standard deviations lower (0.65 lower to 0.01 lower) 67
 (3 studies) ⊕⊕⊝⊝
 Lowc,d,h An SMD of 0.33 can be considered a medium effect size.
Mixed amphetamine salts
ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated
 Assessed with ADHD‐RS‐IV and AISRS
 Follow‐up: post intervention 
 (3‐13 weeks) Mean clinician‐rated ADHD symptom severity score in the intervention groups was 0.80 standard deviations lower (0.93 lower to 0.66 lower) 1083
 (5 studies) ⊕⊕⊝⊝
 Lowc,e An SMD of 0.8 can be considered a small effect size.
ADHD symptom severity: patient rated
 Assessed with CAARS
 Follow‐up: post intervention
(mean 1 week)
Mean patient‐rated ADHD symptom severity score in the intervention groups was 0.45 standard deviations lower (1.02 lower to 0.12 higher) 18
 (1 study) ⊕⊝⊝⊝
 Very lowb,c,h An SMD of 0.45 can be considered a medium effect size.
All amphetamines
Retention in treatment
Assessed with the proportion of randomised participants that completed the study
 Follow‐up: post intervention 
 (1‐20 weeks)
Study population RR 1.06 
 (0.99 to 1.13) 2323
 (17 studies) ⊕⊕⊝⊝
 Lowa,i
708 per 1000 750 per 1000
 (701 to 800)
Moderate
800 per 1000 848 per 1000
 (792 to 904)
*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI).
 ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; ADHD‐RS‐IV: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scale, Fourth Version; AISRS: Adult Attention Deficity Hyperactivity Disorder Investigator Rating Scale; CAARS: Conners' Adult Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scales;CI: confidence interval; DSM‐IV:Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition;SMD: standardised mean difference.
GRADE Working Group grades of evidence.
 High quality: we are very confident that the true effect lies close to that of the estimate of the effect.
 Moderate quality: we are moderately confident in the effect estimate: the true effect is likely to be close to the estimate of the effect, but there is a possibility that it is substantially different.
 Low quality: our confidence in the effect estimate is limited: the true effect may be substantially different from the estimate of the effect.
 Very low quality: we have very little confidence in the effect estimate: the true effect is likely to be substantially different from the estimate of effect.

aThe certainty of the evidence was downgraded by one level owing to unclear risk of detection and performance bias because it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved in placebo‐controlled studies given the powerful behavioural effects of amphetamines.
 bThe certainty of the evidence was downgraded by two levels owing to imprecision because the 95% CI is wide, indicating that the intervention effect for this outcome can range from a small, worsening effect to a large benefit.
 cThe statistical power to detect publication bias for this comparison in this review is low.
 dThe certainty of the evidence was downgraded by one level owing to imprecision because the 95% CI is rather wide, indicating that the intervention effect for this outcome can range from a moderate to a large benefit.
 eThe certainty of the evidence was downgraded by two levels owing to unclear risk of detection and performance bias (it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved in placebo‐controlled studies given the powerful behavioural effects of amphetamines), high risk of attrition bias (large proportion of participants discontinued treatment or differences between study groups in discontinuation rates), and high risk of other bias (such as the possibility of carry‐over effect in cross‐over studies without a washout phase).
 fThe certainty of the evidence was downgraded by one level owing to moderate statistical heterogeneity.
 gThe certainty of the evidence was upgraded by one level because a large and precise effect size was observed.
 hThe certainty of the evidence was downgraded by one level owing to unclear risk of detection and performance bias (it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved in placebo‐controlled studies given the powerful behavioural effects of amphetamines) and high risk of other bias (such as the possibility of carry‐over effect in cross‐over studies without a washout phase).
 iThe certainty of the evidence was downgraded by one level owing to inconsistency (this comparison includes three different types of amphetamines at a wide range of doses, and the analysis showed moderate heterogeneity).

Background

Description of the condition

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder affecting 7% of children and adolescents worldwide (Thomas 2015). ADHD is reported to continue into adulthood in 15% to 50% of children given this diagnosis (Faraone 2006; Lara 2009). Factors associated with persistence of the disorder into adulthood are the presence of comorbidity, ADHD severity, and ADHD treatment (Caye 2016). The prevalence of ADHD in adults has been estimated at 2.5% to 5% (Simon 2009; Willcutt 2012).

Childhood ADHD is characterised by inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity. Inattention is often presented as distractibility, difficulty in sustaining attention on tasks or activities, trouble in organising tasks or activities, and forgetfulness. Hyperactivity and impulsivity are usually manifested as an inability to be still or to undertake quiet activities, being fidgety, talking excessively, or having trouble awaiting turns (Thapar 2016). The clinical characteristics of ADHD in adults are more subtle than in children, with hyperactivity and impulsivity often manifesting as restlessness and talkativeness (Kessler 2010; Kooij 2009). In addition, symptoms of emotional dysregulation, such as irritability, emotional lability, and emotional reactivity, are usually described in adults with ADHD (Corbisiero 2013; Retz 2012). Symptoms of ADHD thus vary across the lifespan, with improvements in hyperactivity and impulsivity usually observed (Kessler 2010). However, inattention is thought to remain unchanged, and executive functions are significantly altered (Riccio 2005). This leads to an inability to perform complex activities as a consequence of lack of activity planning, inadequate time management, high distractibility, and lack of attention (Riccio 2005). The cluster of ADHD symptoms includes the clinical expression of neuropsychological dysfunction in several executive functions, such as working memory and impulse inhibition (Schoechlin 2005), and in reward and motivation (Castellanos 2006; Sonuga‐Barke 2008).

Adults with ADHD are at higher risk of developing comorbid psychiatric disorders such as anxiety and mood and substance use disorders (Kessler 2006). In addition, a high prevalence of antisocial personality disorder has been observed in this population (Biederman 2006; Young 2005). Particularly worrying is the prevalence of substance misuse amongst adults with ADHD, which has been reported to be twice as high as that of the general population (Biederman 2006; Levin 1998). Similarly, an inverse association has been found in patients with substance use disorders, in whom the estimated prevalence of ADHD is 23% (Van Emmerik‐van Oortmerssen 2012). Adults with ADHD tend to have more social problems that affect their work and family life (Biederman 1993). Furthermore, they have poorer driving performance and are more frequently involved in car accidents (Barkley 2002). Recently, ADHD has been associated with increased mortality (Dalsgaard 2015).

ADHD is usually diagnosed using the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders (DSM) or theInternational Classification of Diseases (ICD). Diagnostic criteria differ between the DSM and the ICD, and these criteria have varied across different versions of the DSM. For example, to qualify for ADHD, both DSM‐IV and DSM‐IV‐TR require that patients have six out of nine symptoms of inattention or hyperactivity/impulsivity, that these symptoms have begun before the age of seven years, and that they clearly impair social, academic, or occupational function in two or more settings. In contrast, the latest version of the DSM ‐ DSM‐5 ‐ requires only five out of nine symptoms of inattention or hyperactivity/impulsivity that have begun before the age of 12 years and that interfere with social, academic, or occupational functioning in two or more settings, for adults to qualify for ADHD. In addition, and for the first time, DSM‐5 allows a diagnosis of ADHD to be made in patients with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). For a diagnosis of ADHD based on ICD‐10 criteria, six symptoms of inattention, three symptoms of hyperactivity, and one symptom of impulsivity that are present before the age of six years, and that impair social, academic, or occupational function in two or more settings, are needed. The presence of a comorbid ASD is incompatible with a diagnosis of ADHD according to ICD‐10 diagnostic criteria.

Description of the intervention

Amphetamines are drugs, structurally related to catecholamines, that increase dopamine (DA) and norepinephrine (NE) concentrations at the synapse. In healthy individuals, these catecholaminergic actions result in psychostimulant effects (Hardman 2001). Because of their stimulant activity within the central nervous system, amphetamines have been studied for the treatment of several disorders, including narcolepsy (Nishino 2007), obesity (Ioannides‐Demos 2005), amphetamine dependence (Shearer 2002), cocaine dependence (Castells 2016), and ADHD (Wilens 2003).

Use of amphetamines for the treatment of adults with ADHD has been increasing during the past decade and recently surpassed use of methylphenidate in the USA (Safer 2016). Different types of amphetamines are available for the treatment of ADHD, such as lisdexamfetamine, dexamphetamine (or dextroamphetamine), and mixed amphetamine salts (MAS), which contain d‐amphetamine and l‐amphetamine at a ratio of 3:1. Amphetamines are metabolised in the liver, and their half‐lives are 10 to 15 hours for MAS (10 to 12 hours for d‐amphetamine and 12 to 15 hours for l‐amphetamine) and around 12 hours for dexamphetamine (Markowitz 2017). Lisdexamfetamine is a prodrug with a half‐life of around 0.6 hours that is metabolised to dexamphetamine (Markowitz 2017). All amphetamine derivatives are administered orally. Lisdexamfetamine is administered once a day, and MAS and dextroamphetamine may be administered once or twice a day depending on the formulation (immediate‐release versus extended‐release) (Markowitz 2017). Recommended dosages range from 5 mg/d to 40 mg/d for MAS (FDA 2015a), from 30 mg/d to 70 mg/d for lisdexamfetamine (FDA 2015b), and from 5 mg/d to 40 mg/d for dexamphetamine (FDA 2007).

How the intervention might work

From a neurobiological perspective, ADHD is characterised by a hypofunction in frontal‐striatal, cerebellar circuits that results in executive function impairment, including decreased attention and reduced ability to plan activities and inhibit inappropriate actions. A dysfunction in dopaminergic neurotransmission has been observed in these circuits. Amphetamines increase dopamine (DA) and norepinephrine (NE) concentrations at the synapse. Although the precise mechanism of action is not well understood, it seems that these drugs act on the dopamine transporter (DAT) and presumably cause inversion of the transport direction of DAT, resulting in an efflux of dopamine from the presynaptic neuron towards the synapse. Some have proposed that amphetamines get into the presynaptic neuron through the DAT and cause exocytosis of vesicles containing DA. In addition to increased DA release, amphetamines inhibit catecholamine metabolism through catechol‐O‐methyltransferasse (COMT) (for a review of the mechanism of action of amphetamines, see Carboni 2004; Fleckenstein 2007; Heal 2013; and Sulzer 2005). Thus, by promoting DA release from the presynaptic neuron and inhibiting COMT, amphetamines increase dopamine at the synapse, which yields improvement in executive function and ADHD symptoms (for a review of the neurobiological basis of ADHD and the mechanism of action of psychostimulants, see Arnsten 2006; Grace 2002; Heal 2013; and Swanson 2007).

Why it is important to do this review

The number of medicines containing amphetamines and the number of clinical trials assessing the efficacy of these medicines for adults with ADHD have been increasing over past decades (Cunill 2016; Heal 2013). Furthermore, prescription of amphetamines for adults with ADHD has also increased (Safer 2016). In addition, after publication of the first version of this review in 2011 (Castells 2011a), lisdexamfetamine was approved for the treatment of adults with ADHD in several European countries (Ermer 2016; MHRA 2015). Despite this increase in the number of clinical trials and prescriptions of amphetamines, no new systematic review has focused on the efficacy of amphetamines in adults.

A number of factors appear to modify the efficacy of drugs used to treat ADHD. For instance, the efficacy of other stimulants seems to be lower in patients with ADHD and comorbid substance use disorders (Cunill 2015; Koesters 2008), implying that stimulants may be less useful in these patients and thus stressing the importance of adapting ADHD treatment to patient characteristics. Furthermore, the efficacy of methylphenidate is lower with lower doses (Castells 2011b; Faraone 2004), as well as with long‐acting drug‐release formulations (Peterson 2007). For this reason, we plan to carry out subgroup analyses of these factors. In addition, because pharmaceutical industry funding has been associated with positive trial results (Bekelman 2003; Riera 2017), the type of funding (i.e. with and without pharmaceutical industry funding) also merits a subgroup analysis.

Amphetamines have been blamed for causing 20 deaths among patients in Canada receiving these drugs for the treatment of ADHD, and these drugs were temporarily pulled from the market in that country (Kondro 2005). Amphetamines moreover can cause withdrawal effects (Phillips 2014), and they can be misused (Weyandt 2016). Therefore, we also aim to review the adverse effects of amphetamines, with a special emphasis on cardiovascular and psychiatric outcomes.

Finally, the change in diagnostic criteria with the introduction of DSM‐5, which permits a diagnosis of ADHD in individuals with autism spectrum disorders, will allow investigation of the efficacy and safety of amphetamines in patients with this comorbidity.

Objectives

To examine the efficacy and safety of amphetamines for adults with ADHD.

Methods

Criteria for considering studies for this review

Types of studies

Randomised controlled trials (RCTs).

Types of participants

Adults (over 18 years of age) with ADHD, diagnosed by any standardised criteria (e.g. DSM‐III, DSM‐III‐R, DSM‐IV, DSM‐IV‐TR, DSM‐5, ICD‐10). The presence of comorbid disorders was not an exclusion criterion.

Types of interventions

Any amphetamine (including amphetamine, dextroamphetamine, lisdexamfetamine, or mixed amphetamine salts (MAS; Adderall)), given at any dose, compared with placebo or an active intervention(s).

We did not exclude studies with additional interventions if these were provided to both study groups.

Types of outcome measures

Primary outcomes
  1. Severity of ADHD symptoms, assessed by clinicians and patients using a standardised instrument (e.g. the ADHD Rating Scale‐IV (ADHD‐RS‐IV; DuPaul 1998), Conners' Adult ADHD Rating Scale (CAARS; Conners 1999))

Secondary outcomes
  1. Efficacy outcomes

    1. Clinical impression of severity at study end, measured by the Clinical Global Impression (CGI) ‐ Severity (CGI‐S) scale (Guy 1976)

    2. Clinical impression of improvement at study end, assessed by the CGI‐Improvement (CGI‐I) scale (Guy 1976)

    3. Proportion of participants achieving a reduction of at least 30% in severity of ADHD symptoms

    4. Proportion of participants achieving a CGI‐I score of 1 or 2

    5. Proportion of participants achieving a reduction of at least 30% in severity of ADHD symptoms and a CGI‐I score of 1 or 2

    6. Global functioning: social, occupational, and psychological functioning of adults with ADHD at study end, assessed by a standardised instrument

    7. Depressive symptoms: severity of depressive symptoms at study completion, assessed by a standardised instrument

    8. Anxiety: severity of anxiety symptoms at study completion, assessed by a standardised instrument

    9. Retention: proportion of randomised participants that completed the study

  2. Adverse events

    1. Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any cardiovascular adverse event

    2. Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to medication abuse

    3. Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any psychiatric adverse event

    4. Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event

Search methods for identification of studies

Electronic searches

We searched the following databases and trial registers in July 2016 and again in August 2017.

  1. Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL; 2017, Issue 7) in the Cochrane Library, which contains the Cochrane Developmental, Psychosocial and Learning Problems Specialised Register (searched 21 August 2017).

  2. MEDLINE Ovid (1946 to August week 2 2017).

  3. MEDLINE In‐Process and Other Non‐indexed Citations Ovid (searched 21 August 2017).

  4. MEDLINE Epub Ahead of Print Ovid (searched 21 August 2017).

  5. Embase Ovid (1974 to 2017 week 34).

  6. PsycINFO Ovid (1967 to August week 2 2017).

  7. CINAHL Plus EBSCOhost (Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature; 1937 to 23 August 2017).

  8. Science Citation Index Web of Science (SCI; 1970 to 22 August 2017).

  9. Social Science Citation Index Web of Science (SSCI; 1970 to 22 August 2017).

  10. Conference Proceedings Citation Index ‐ Science Web of Science (CPCI‐S; 1990 to 22 August 2017).

  11. Conference Proceedings Citation Index ‐ Social Science and Humanities Web of Science (CPCI‐SS&H; 1990 to 22 August 2017).

  12. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (CDSR; 2017, Issue 8), part of the Cochrane Library (searched 21 August 2017).

  13. Database of Abstracts of Reviews of Effects (DARE; 2015, Issue 2), part of the Cochrane Library (final issue searched 29 July 2016).

  14. Worldcat (www.worldcat.org; searched 23 August 2017).

  15. Clinicaltrials.gov (clinicaltrials.gov; searched 23 August 2017).

  16. World Health Organization International Clinical Trials Registry Platform (WHO ICTRP; apps.who.int/trialsearch; searched 23 August 2017).

We did not apply any language or date restrictions.

We have listed the search strategies for this update in Appendix 1, along with previous search strategies in Appendix 2.

Searching other resources

We contacted the corresponding authors of all included studies, experts in the field, and the pharmaceutical company, Shire, and we inspected the reference lists of retrieved studies and relevant reviews to identify any other published, unpublished, or ongoing studies. In addition, for each included study, we performed a citation search in Web of Science to identify any later studies that may have cited it.

Data collection and analysis

Selection of studies

Having removed duplicates, two review authors (XC and RC) independently assessed the titles and abstracts of all remaining records yielded by the search strategy for eligibility, discarding those that were clearly irrelevant. Next, we acquired the full‐text reports of those records deemed potentially eligible and assessed them against our inclusion criteria (see Criteria for considering studies for this review). When we identified unpublished trials, we contacted the study co‐ordinators to request the data. We resolved disagreements by discussion, until reaching a consensus, or in consultation with a third review author (LB). We recorded our selection process in a PRISMA diagram (Moher 2009).

Data extraction and management

Two review authors (XC and RC) independently inspected the full‐text reports of included studies and extracted data onto a piloted data extraction sheet (Appendix 3). We resolved disagreements by discussion, until reaching a consensus, or in consultation with a third review author (LB).

Regarding our primary outcomes (severity of ADHD symptoms), we collected both change scores (the difference between ADHD symptom severity score at study end compared to baseline) and endpoint scores (ADHD symptom severity score at study end) but gave preference to change scores over endpoint scores. For all secondary outcomes (efficacy outcomes and adverse events), we collected endpoint scores.

We emailed study authors to request any missing data or information, when necessary. We also contacted the authors of all cross‐over trials to obtain first period data on ADHD symptoms. We made a second approach if no answer was obtained by one month after the first email (see Dealing with missing data).

Two review authors (XC and RC) entered data into Review Manager 5 (RevMan 5) (Review Manager 2014).

Assessment of risk of bias in included studies

In accordance with the Cochrane 'Risk of bias' tool (Higgins 2017a), as well as the criteria set out in Appendix 4, two review authors (XC and RC) independently assessed the risk of bias in each included study as high, low, or unclear, for each of the following domains: sequence generation, allocation concealment, blinding of participants and personnel, blinding of outcome assessment, incomplete outcome data, selective outcome reporting, and other bias. Review authors sought to resolve any differences by discussion, with MC adjudicating in cases for which this was not possible.

Measures of treatment effect

Continuous outcome data

We calculated the standardised mean difference (SMD) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) because included studies used different scales to assess the severity of ADHD symptoms. We used Hedges’ method for calculating SMD with individual study weights calculated as the inverse of the variance.

For both clinician‐ and patient‐rated severity of ADHD symptoms, we entered data into RevMan using the generic inverse variance to combine data from parallel and cross‐over studies in the manner recommended by Elbourne 2002 (see Unit of analysis issues section for additional details).

Dichotomous outcome data

We calculated the risk ratio (RR) and presented it with 95% CIs.

Unit of analysis issues

Cross‐over trials

To combine parallel‐group studies with cross‐over studies, we calculated the correlation coefficient between active and control periods and used it to calculate effect sizes (Elbourne 2002). We used data from the first study period, when available, when we could not apply these recommendations.

We could calculate the correlation coefficient of the outcome score between active and control periods from only two studies (Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001). We applied the correlation coefficient to the other cross‐over studies using the most conservative correlation coefficient (r = 0.44) in the main analysis (Taylor 2001); we used the least conservative one (r = 0.61) in a sensitivity analysis (Taylor 2000).

Multiple treatment groups

When several independent treatment groups were available (e.g. amphetamine + psychotherapy versus placebo + psychotherapy; amphetamine + fake psychotherapy versus placebo + fake psychotherapy), we included them as independent studies. In studies with multiple and correlated interventions (e.g. amphetamine 10 mg versus placebo; amphetamine 20 mg versus placebo), we combined the intervention groups into a single group and included them in the meta‐analysis as a single comparison. For binary data, we summed sample sizes and numbers of participants with the event across groups. We combined continuous data using the formulae described in Section 7.7.3.8, "Combining groups", of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011a).

Dealing with missing data

We used the number of randomised participants as the denominator for dichotomous variables. For continuous data, we entered into Review Manager 2014 the sample size used in calculations of the mean and the standard deviation. We did not use any imputations to deal with missing data.

We emailed study authors to request missing data or information, when necessary. We also contacted Shire after the corresponding authors directed us to this pharmaceutical company to obtain the information requested (Castells 2009b [pers comm]). See Characteristics of included studies for data requested and subsequently provided.

Assessment of heterogeneity

We assessed clinical heterogeneity by comparing differences in study populations, interventions, and outcomes, and we evaluated methodological heterogeneity by comparing study designs.

We investigated statistical heterogeneity using tau², Chi² test, and the I² statistic, which determines the proportion of variability due to heterogeneity (I² from 0 to 40% = not important statistical heterogeneity; I² from 30% to 60% = moderate heterogeneity; I² from 50% to 90% = substantial heterogeneity; and I² from 75% to 100% = considerable heterogeneity (Deeks 2017)).

Assessment of reporting biases

We drew funnel plots to investigate any relationship between effect size and study precision (closely related to sample size) when we identified a sufficient number of studies (al least 10 studies) (Egger 1997).

Data synthesis

We used RevMan 5 to perform the analyses (Review Manager 2014).

We calculated weighted averages and 95% CIs using the inverse variance method for continuous outcomes and the Mantel‐Haenszel method for dichotomous outcomes. We pooled data using a random‐effects model because we noted marked between‐study heterogeneity as regards study design (studies with cross‐over and parallel designs were included) and length of follow‐up (from two to 20 weeks).

We examined the efficacy of amphetamines for reducing the severity of ADHD symptoms in adults by means of continuous outcome variables (using change scores, e.g. change in ADHD symptom severity score from baseline to study completion; and endpoint scores, e.g. ADHD symptom severity score at study completion) and binary ones (e.g. proportion of patients achieving a reduction of at least 30% in the severity of ADHD symptoms). The primary efficacy outcome (severity of ADHD symptoms) combined change scores and endpoint scores, but we prioritised change scores when both types of scores were available in the same study. We preferred change scores because they are more precise than endpoint scores, as long as they were adjusted for baseline severity. We analysed studies reporting response rates separately.

Summarising the quality of the evidence

Using GRADEpro (GRADEPro GDT 2015), we constructed a 'Summary of findings’ table for the comparison of amphetamines versus placebo for ADHD in adults, for the following outcomes assessed post intervention: 'severity of ADHD symptoms' (primary outcome) assessed by clinicians and patients for each amphetamine; and retention in treatment (secondary outcome). Two review authors (XC and RC) assessed the quality of the evidence for each of these outcomes using the GRADE approach, resolving disagreements by discussion until reaching a consensus. GRADE offers a structured process for appraising the quality of evidence and developing recommendations based on the extent to which one can be confident that the estimates of effect are correct (Guyatt 2011a). The assessment may result in high‐, moderate‐, low‐ or very low‐quality ratings based on evaluation of five categories: within‐trial risk of bias, directness of evidence, heterogeneity of data, precision of effect estimates, and risk of publication bias (Balshem 2011; Guyatt 2011b; Guyatt 2011c; Guyatt 2011d; Guyatt 2011e; Guyatt 2011f).

Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity

Irrespective of whether we found statistical heterogeneity, we conducted the following subgroup analyses when we had sufficient studies (i.e. at least one study in each subgroup).

  1. Comorbidities: the presence of a comorbidity (drug use disorder, major depressive disorder) versus no comorbidity.

  2. Type of amphetamine (amphetamine, dextroamphetamine, lisdexamfetamine, or MAS).

  3. Dose at study completion (equal to and above the median dose versus below the median dose). We performed this subgroup analysis separately for each type of amphetamine because no pharmacological equivalence was available for the three types of amphetamine that have been studied in adults with ADHD (dextroamphetamine, lisdexamfetamine, and MAS (which consists of a fixed‐dose mixture of racaemic amphetamine aspartate monohydrate, racaemic amphetamine sulphate, dextroamphetamine saccharide, and dextroamphetamine sulphate)).

  4. Type of drug‐release formulation (immediate‐release versus long‐acting release).

We conducted subgroup analyses for the following outcomes only: severity of ADHD symptoms rated by clinicians and patients; retention in treatment; and proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse events (because the number of studies measuring these outcomes was large enough to carry out these analyses). We calculated the pooled effect size (RR or SMD) for each subgroup. We investigated whether there were between‐subgroup differences by means of the Chi² test, using a random‐effects model.

We did not conduct our planned subgroup analysis for study funding (with versus without pharmaceutical industry funding) (Castells 2009a), as only was study was not funded by the pharmaceutical industry. See Differences between protocol and review.

Sensitivity analysis

We performed sensitivity analyses in which we restricted the meta‐analysis of each outcome to those studies that had low risk of bias on that outcome. We had intended to restrict the analysis to studies that had low risk of bias in all domains (Castells 2009a), but this was not possible as no studies fulfilled this criterion. Instead, we used our assessments of incomplete outcome data and other potential sources of bias, whose scores showed between‐study variability, and conducted sensitivity analyses that included only studies scoring low risk of bias on these specific domains.

We conducted three post hoc sensitivity analyses. First, we borrowed the correlation coefficient from Taylor 2000 to calculate the effect size of six cross‐over studies (Dupaul 2012; Kay 2009; Martin 2014a/Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Wigal 2010) (see Unit of analysis issues). Second, we calculated the pooled risk difference for the outcomes of 'proportion of participants withdrawn owing to cardiovascular adverse events' and 'proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event', to include studies that had no events for these outcomes. Third, we excluded from the analysis one cross‐over study (Spencer 2001), which had a carry‐over effect, to determine wether the carry‐over effect may have biased the results of this review.

Results

Description of studies

See Characteristics of included studies, Characteristics of excluded studies, and Characteristics of ongoing studies.

Results of the search

Our searches for this update yielded 3414 records, from which we identified and discarded 1391 duplicates. We screened titles and abstracts of the remaining 2023 records and retrieved 39 full‐text reports for further examination. Of these, we excluded eight reports that did not meet our inclusion criteria (Criteria for considering studies for this review), and we identified eight secondary publications of previously included studies. We included 12 new studies (from 18 reports) and identified five ongoing studies, which, when combined with studies included in the previous version of the review (Castells 2011a), gives a total of 19 included studies (from 33 reports) and seven ongoing studies (from seven reports). See Figure 1.

1.

1

Flow diagram.

Included studies

This review includes 19 studies (Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Dupaul 2012; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a/Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010).

Study design

Of the 19 studies included in this review, six used a cross‐over design. All studies compared amphetamine versus placebo, and three studies also compared amphetamine versus an active intervention: guanfacine (Taylor 2001), modafinil (Taylor 2000), or paroxetine (Weiss 2006). One study investigated two types of amphetamines (lisdexamfetamine and MAS); thus we have included two drug versus placebo comparisons in the review (Martin 2014a/Martin 2014b).

Setting

Eighteen studies were conducted in the USA (Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Dupaul 2012; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a/Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Wigal 2010). One study was conducted in both Canada and the USA (Weiss 2006).

Ten studies were multi‐centre; that is, participants were enrolled and were followed up at more than one study site (Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Brams 2012; Frick 2017; Levin 2015; Spencer 2008; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010).

Participants  

The included studies randomised 2521 participants, mostly males (57.2%). Most participants were middle‐aged Caucasians (mean age, 35.3 years) with a combined type of ADHD (78.8%). (For a detailed description of participant characteristics, see Table 2.) Sample sizes ranged from 17 participants in Taylor 2001 to 420 participants in Adler 2008.

1. Participants' baseline characteristics.
Characteristic Descriptive statistics N studies (N patients)
Gender: male N = 1435 (57.2%) 19 (2507)
Age Mean = 35.3 (range = 20.2 to 41.2) years 19 (2507)
Race: Caucasian N = 2006 (84.5%) 15 (2373)
Combined ADHD N = 1341 (78.8%) 11 (1701)
Predominantly inattentive ADHD N = 344 (20.2%)
Predominantly hyperactive/impulsive ADHD N = 28 (1.6%)
Comorbid SUD as inclusion criterion N = 158 (6.3%) 19 (2507)
Comorbid depressive disorders as inclusion criteria N = 0 19 (2507)
Comorbid anxiety disorders as inclusion criteria N = 0 19 (2507)
Treated previously with stimulants N = 306 (41.1%) 8 (744)

ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
 N: number.
 SUD: substance use disorder.

Interventions 

These studies investigated three types of amphetamines: dextroamphetamine in three studies (Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001; Weiss 2006); lisdexamfetamine in nine studies (Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Dupaul 2012; Kollins 2014; Martin 2014a; Waxmonsky 2014; Wigal 2010); and MAS in eight studies (Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Levin 2015; Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017).

Doses studied ranged from 10.2 mg/d in Taylor 2001 to 21.8 mg/d in Taylor 2000 for dextroamphetamine; from 30 mg/d in Adler 2008 and Dupaul 2012 to 70 mg/d in Adler 2008 and Dupaul 2012 for lisdexamfetamine; and from 12.5 mg/d in Weisler 2017 to 80 mg/d in Levin 2015 for MAS.

Duration

Duration of study interventions ranged from one week in Dupaul 2012 to 20 weeks in Weiss 2006, with a mean of 5.3 weeks (37.2 days). Only three studies were longer than eight weeks in duration (Adler 2013; Levin 2015; Weiss 2006).

Sponsorship

All but two studies reported their funding sources (Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001). With the exception of one study ‐ Levin 2015 ‐ all studies were funded by the pharmaceutical industry.

Excluded studies

In total, we excluded 17 studies (eight studies in this update and nine studies in the previous review) for the following reasons: 12 (70.6%) studies were not RCTs; three (17.6%) studies were not conducted in adults with ADHD (two studies were performed in children and one in individuals who had ADHD symptoms who did not qualify for the ADHD disorder), one (5.9%) study was not controlled with placebo or an active control, and one (5.9%) study did not investigate amphetamines. See Characteristics of excluded studies tables and Figure 1.

Ongoing studies

Seven clinical trials were still ongoing when we completed this update (NCT00202605; NCT00514202; NCT00928148; NCT01863459; NCT02635035; NCT02803229; NCT03153488); two of these ‐ NCT00514202 and NCT00202605 ‐ were already identified in the previous version (Castells 2011a).

Four of these studies were completed (NCT00202605; NCT00514202; NCT00928148; NCT01863459), two are recruiting participants (NCT02635035; NCT02803229), and one is not yet recruiting (NCT03153488). Five studies investigated MAS (NCT00202605; NCT00514202; NCT00928148; NCT02803229; NCT03153488), and two studies investigated lisdexamfetamine (NCT01863459; NCT02635035). Four studies have a cross‐over design (NCT00202605; NCT00928148; NCT01863459; NCT02635035), and three studies have a parallel design (NCT00514202; NCT02803229; NCT03153488). Three studies included patients with ADHD and comorbid disorders (NCT00514202; NCT01863459; NCT02803229). See Characteristics of ongoing studies.

Risk of bias in included studies

We have provided a comprehensive description of the risk of bias for each study in the 'Risk of bias' tables beneath the Characteristics of included studies tables. We have summarised this information in Figure 2.

2.

2

Risk of bias summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.

Note: scores for blinding of participants, personnel, and outcome assessors refer to amphetamines vs placebo only comparisons; scores on all remaining domains refer to amphetamines vs placebo, guanfacine, modafinil, or paroxetine.

Allocation

Sequence generation

Four studies reported on how the random sequence was generated, and so we considered their risk of bias to be low (Adler 2013; Kay 2009; Levin 2015; Weisler 2017). The 16 remaining studies did not report on how the random sequence was generated, and so we judged them to be at unclear risk of bias (Adler 2008; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Dupaul 2012; Frick 2017; Kollins 2014; Martin 2014a; Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010).

Allocation concealment

Three studies reported on their method of allocation concealment, and so we considered their risk of bias to be low (Adler 2013; Dupaul 2012; Weisler 2017). The 17 remaining studies did not report on their method of allocation concealment, and so we judged them to be at unclear risk of bias (Adler 2008; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a; Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010).

Blinding

Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias)

Blinding of participants and personnel was intended in all included studies (Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Dupaul 2012; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a/Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010). Nevertheless, we deemed all studies to be unclear risk of performance bias, given that amphetamines have powerful subjective effects that may reveal the assigned treatment (Childs 2009; Johanson 1980; Makris 2004; Makris 2007; Wachtel 1992).

Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias)

Blinding of outcome assessment was intended in all included studies (Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Dupaul 2012; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a/Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010). Nevertheless, we deemed all studies to be unclear risk of detection bias, given that amphetamines have powerful subjective effects that may reveal the assigned treatment (Childs 2009; Johanson 1980; Makris 2004; Makris 2007; Wachtel 1992).

Incomplete outcome data

Study outcomes can be influenced by attrition because reasons for dropping out from the study may differ between active intervention and placebo groups. This selective attrition makes intervention groups that were similar at baseline different at the end of the study. This appears to be the case in studies investigating the efficacy of amphetamines for adults with ADHD. As discussed later, the proportion of participants dropping out owing to AEs was higher amongst those receiving amphetamines than placebo, suggesting that attrition was somehow related to the experimental intervention. This selective attrition can lead to bias. This is particularly true for studies with a higher dropout rate (Adler 2013), and for those with statistically significant differences in the number of dropouts between study groups (Brams 2012; Frick 2017; Spencer 2008); we rated these studies at high risk of attrition bias. In such an instance, no statistical method of dealing with missing data appears to guarantee unbiased results. Conversely, this type of bias seems unlikely amongst those studies for which attrition was low (Adler 2008; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001); we considered these studies to be at low risk of attrition bias. For the remaining 12 studies, we judged the risk of attrition bias to be unclear because attrition was moderate but imputation methods for missing data were applied, or attrition was low but missing data were not imputed (Biederman 2012; Dupaul 2012; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a/Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010).

Selective reporting

For 13 studies, the protocol was available and outcomes stated in the protocol were reported in the article (Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a/Martin 2014b; Spencer 2008; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2017; Wigal 2010). We considered these studies to be at low risk of reporting bias. For the six remaining studies, the study protocol was not available and thus we considered them to be at unclear risk of reporting bias (Dupaul 2012; Spencer 2001; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001; Weisler 2006; Weiss 2006).

Other potential sources of bias

We considered seven studies to be at high risk of other bias (Dupaul 2012; Kay 2009; Martin 2014a; Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Waxmonsky 2014; Wigal 2010), mainly because they had a cross‐over design with no washout phase, and thus the possibility of a carry‐over effect could not be ruled out. Indeed, in one of these studies the carry‐over effect was evident (Spencer 2001). The carry‐over effect can yield an underestimation of the effect of the intervention and can bias the result towards the null for both effectiveness and AE outcomes. For one study ‐ Frick 2017 ‐ we judged the risk of other bias to be unclear because there was a long period of time between performance of the study and publication of the main results and, in addition, secondary results were published before the main results were published. For the 12 remaining studies, groups were balanced at baseline and no other potential source of bias was found; thus we considered them to be at low risk of other bias (Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Spencer 2008; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006).

Summary

We did not rate any study as having low risk of bias overall because we considered all of them to be at unclear or high risk of bias in at least one domain of the Cochrane 'Risk of bias' tool. For all studies, we considered the risk of performance and detection bias to be unclear because it is likely that participants or clinicians would have detected the medication, given the powerful behavioural effects of amphetamines. This bias is unlikely to occur if amphetamines are compared with other psychostimulants such as modafinil or methylphenidate. Furthermore, attrition bias is likely in several studies, and the possibility of a carry‐over effect could not be ruled out in studies using a cross‐over design.

Effects of interventions

See: Table 1

Amphetamines versus placebo

We were able to perform meta‐analyses for most outcomes, given the high availability of data: 'ADHD symptom severity: clinician' (68.4%), 'Retention in treatment' (84.2%), and 'Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event' (84.2%). We present the results for each outcome below, along with results for the outcomes of 'severity of ADHD symptoms' assessed by clinicians and patients for each amphetamine, and we present results for 'retention to treatment' in Table 1.

Primary outcomes: severity of ADHD symptoms

We found evidence to suggest that amphetamines are more efficacious than placebo in reducing the severity of ADHD symptoms whether assessed by clinicians (standardised mean difference (SMD) −0.90, 95% confidence interval (CI) −1.04 to −0.75; 13 studies, 2028 participants; Analysis 1.1; Figure 3; Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Frick 2017; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010) or by patients (SMD −0.51, 95% CI −0.75 to −0.28; six studies, 120 participants; Analysis 1.2; Dupaul 2012; Kollins 2014; Martin 2014a; Martin 2014b; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001). Statistical heterogeneity for severity of ADHD symptoms was moderate when rated by clinicians (I² = 47%) and was low (I² = 13%) when rated by patients. We drew a funnel plot for clinician‐rated efficacy and detected no asymmetry (Figure 4).

1.1. Analysis.

1.1

Comparison 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician‐rated.

3.

3

Forest plot of comparison: 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for ADHD in adults, outcome: 1.1 Severity of ADHD symptoms: clinician rated.

1.2. Analysis.

1.2

Comparison 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 2 ADHD symptom severity: patient‐rated.

4.

4

Funnel plot of comparison: 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for ADHD in adults, outcome: 1.1 Severity of ADHD symptoms: clinician rated.

Secondary outcomes
Efficacy outcomes

We found evidence that amphetamines are more efficacious than placebo in reducing the severity of ADHD symptoms, irrespective of the efficacy definition used.

  1. Clinical impression of severity at study end (SMD −1.09, 95% CI −1.57 to −0.61; two studies, 78 participants; Analysis 1.3; Spencer 2001; Waxmonsky 2014).

  2. Clinical impression of improvement at study end (one study, 263 participants; Analysis 1.4; Weisler 2017).

  3. Proportion of participants achieving a reduction of at least 30% in the severity of ADHD symptoms (risk ratio (RR) 1.52, 95% CI 1.19 to 1.95; two studies, 381 participants; Analysis 1.5; Levin 2015; Weisler 2006).

  4. Proportion of participants achieving a CGI‐I score of 1 or 2 (RR 2.47, 95% CI 2.10 to 2.90; eight studies, 1707 participants; Analysis 1.6; Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Frick 2017; Levin 2015; Spencer 2008; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weiss 2006).

  5. Proportion of participants achieving a reduction of at least 30% in the severity of ADHD symptoms and a CGI‐I score of 1 or 2 in another study (one study, 61 participants; Analysis 1.7; Biederman 2012).

1.3. Analysis.

1.3

Comparison 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 3 Clinical impression of severity at study end.

1.4. Analysis.

1.4

Comparison 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 4 Clinical impression of improvement at study end.

1.5. Analysis.

1.5

Comparison 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 5 Proportion of participants achieving a reduction ≥ 30% in severity of ADHD symptoms.

1.6. Analysis.

1.6

Comparison 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 6 Proportion of participants achieving a CGI‐Improvement score of 1 or 2.

1.7. Analysis.

1.7

Comparison 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 7 Proportion of participants achieving a reduction ≥ 30% in severity of ADHD symptoms and a CGI‐Improvement score of 1 or 2.

We found no statistical heterogeneity for any of these outcomes.

We conducted a meta‐analysis of two studies with 110 participants (Biederman 2012; Weiss 2006), which revealed no differences between the groups given amphetamines and those given placebo in global functioning (SMD 0.54, 95% CI −0.34 to 1.42; Analysis 1.8), depressive symptoms (SMD 0.16, 95% CI −0.22 to 0.53; Analysis 1.9), or anxiety symptoms (SMD 0.13, 95% CI −0.24 to 0.51; Analysis 1.10). Nevertheless, few studies provided data on these outcomes in a way that was suitable for meta‐analysis.

1.8. Analysis.

1.8

Comparison 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 8 Global functioning.

1.9. Analysis.

1.9

Comparison 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 9 Depressive symptoms.

1.10. Analysis.

1.10

Comparison 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 10 Anxiety symptoms.

In another meta‐analysis of 17 studies with 2323 participants (Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Dupaul 2012; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a; Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010), we found no evidence to suggest that amphetamines improve retention in treatment (RR 1.06, 95% CI 0.99 to 1.13; low‐quality evidence; Analysis 1.11; Figure 5). This latter analysis showed moderate statistical heterogeneity (I² = 40%), but we detected no asymmetry in the funnel plot (not shown).

1.11. Analysis.

1.11

Comparison 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 11 Retention in treatment.

5.

5

Forest plot of comparison: 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for ADHD in adults, outcome: 1.11 Retention in treatment.

Adverse events

A meta‐analysis of three studies with 699 participants showed that a higher proportion of participants in the amphetamine group than in the placebo group dropped out owing to cardiovascular adverse events. However, this difference was not statistically significant (RR 2.18, 95% CI 0.39 to 12.04; Analysis 1.12; Adler 2008; Dupaul 2012; Weisler 2006).

1.12. Analysis.

1.12

Comparison 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 12 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any cardiovascular adverse event.

We conducted a meta‐analysis of 17 studies with 2409 participants and found that the proportion of participants who dropped out owing to any adverse event was higher in the amphetamine group than in the placebo group (RR 2.69, 95% CI 1.63 to 4.42; Analysis 1.13; Figure 6) (Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Dupaul 2012; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a; Martin 2014b; Spencer 2008; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010). However, it must be noted that the proportion of participants who were withdrawn owing to any adverse event was low, even in the amphetamines arm (7.6%).

1.13. Analysis.

1.13

Comparison 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 13 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event.

6.

6

Forest plot of comparison: 1 Amphetamines vs placebo for ADHD in adults, outcome: 1.13 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event.

We found no statistical heterogeneity for any adverse events.

No study reported data on the remaining two outcomes: 'proportion of participants withdrawn owing to medication abuse' and 'proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any psychiatric adverse event'.

Subgroup analyses

Comorbidity

We found no evidence to suggest that comorbidity influenced the effects of amphetamines on:

  1. severity of ADHD symptoms assessed by clinicians (SMD −0.90, 95% CI −1.04 to −0.75; 13 studies, 2028 participants; Analysis 2.1; Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Frick 2017; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010);

  2. severity of ADHD symptoms assessed by participants (SMD −0.51, 95% CI −0.75 to −0.28; six studies, 120 participants; Analysis 2.2; Dupaul 2012; Kollins 2014; Martin 2014a; Martin 2014b; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001);

  3. retention in treatment (RR 1.06, 95% CI 0.99 to 1.13; 17 studies, 2323 participants; Analysis 2.3; Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Dupaul 2012; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a; Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010); or

  4. proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event (RR 2.69, 95% CI 1.63 to 4.42; 17 studies, 2409 participants; Analysis 2.4; Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Dupaul 2012; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a; Martin 2014b; Spencer 2008; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010).

2.1. Analysis.

2.1

Comparison 2 Subgroup analysis 1: comorbidity, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician‐rated.

2.2. Analysis.

2.2

Comparison 2 Subgroup analysis 1: comorbidity, Outcome 2 ADHD symptom severity: patient‐rated.

2.3. Analysis.

2.3

Comparison 2 Subgroup analysis 1: comorbidity, Outcome 3 Retention in treatment.

2.4. Analysis.

2.4

Comparison 2 Subgroup analysis 1: comorbidity, Outcome 4 Proportion of patients withdrawn owing to any adverse event.

Types of amphetamines

Included studies assessed the effects of three amphetamines: dexamphetamine, lisdexamfetamine, and MAS. We found differences between these three types of amphetamines in the reduction in severity of ADHD symptoms assessed by clinicians (Analysis 3.1): both lisdexamfetamine and MAS were more efficacious than placebo in reducing the severity of ADHD symptoms (lisdexamfetamine: SMD −1.06, 95% CI −1.26 to −0.85; seven studies, 896 participants; Figure 3; Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Kollins 2014; Waxmonsky 2014; Wigal 2010; MAS: SMD −0.80, 95% CI −0.93 to −0.66; five studies, 1083 participants; Figure 3; Frick 2017; Levin 2015; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Weisler 2006), but not dexamphetamine (SMD −0.24, 95% CI −0.80 to 0.32; one study, 49 participants; Figure 3; Weiss 2006).

3.1. Analysis.

3.1

Comparison 3 Subgroup analysis 2: type of amphetamine, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician‐rated.

We also found evidence to suggest that both dexamphetamine and lisdexamfetamine are more efficacious than placebo in reducing the severity of ADHD symptoms as assessed by participants (dexamphetamine: SMD −0.77, 95% CI −1.14 to −0.40; two studies, 35 participants; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001; lisdexamfetamine: SMD −0.33, 95% CI −0.65 to −0.01; three studies, 67 participants; Dupaul 2012; Kollins 2014; Martin 2014a), but not MAS (SMD −0.45, 95% CI −1.02 to 0.12; one study, 18 participants; Martin 2014b). See Analysis 3.2.

3.2. Analysis.

3.2

Comparison 3 Subgroup analysis 2: type of amphetamine, Outcome 2 ADHD symptom severity: patient‐rated.

We found no between‐group differences in retention in treatment (RR 1.06, 95% CI 0.99 to 1.13; 17 studies, 2323 participants; Analysis 3.3; Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Dupaul 2012; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a; Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010), or in the proportion of participants withdrawn owing to adverse events (RR 2.69, 95% CI 1.63 to 4.42; 17 studies, 2409 participants; Analysis 3.4; Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Dupaul 2012; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a; Martin 2014b; Spencer 2008; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010).

3.3. Analysis.

3.3

Comparison 3 Subgroup analysis 2: type of amphetamine, Outcome 3 Retention in treatment.

3.4. Analysis.

3.4

Comparison 3 Subgroup analysis 2: type of amphetamine, Outcome 4 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event.

Dose at study completion

The amphetamine maintenance dose was available for all but two studies (Biederman 2012; Weiss 2006). We studied the influence of dose by splitting available comparisons into two groups (lower versus higher doses). The cutoff for delimiting low and high doses was 16 mg/d for dexamphetamine, 53.4 mg/d for lisdexamfetamine, and 50 mg/d for MAS. Four studies compared three doses of amphetamines versus placebo (Adler 2008; Dupaul 2012; Frick 2017; Weisler 2006), thus providing three amphetamine versus placebo comparisons. We combined two of these three comparisons into the same subgroup, thereby leaving two amphetamine (higher and lower doses) versus placebo comparisons (see Unit of analysis issues for the explanation on methods used to combine multiple and correlated interventions). Two studies ‐ Levin 2015 and Weisler 2017 ‐ compared two amphetamine doses versus placebo, which we combined into the same subgroup because both were above or below the median dose (Levin 2015; Weisler 2017).

We found no evidence that dose influenced the effects of:

  1. dexamphetamine on severity of ADHD symptoms as assessed by participants (SMD −0.77, 95% CI −1.14 to −0.40; two studies, 35 participants; Analysis 4.1; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001);

  2. lisdexamfetamine on severity of ADHD symptoms as assessed by clinicians (SMD −1.02, 95% CI −1.22 to −0.82; six studies, 885 participants; Analysis 5.1; Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Brams 2012; Kollins 2014; Waxmonsky 2014; Wigal 2010), or as assessed by participants (SMD −0.35, 95% CI −0.61 to −0.10; three studies, 67 participants; Analysis 5.2; Dupaul 2012; Kollins 2014; Martin 2014a); retention in treatment (RR 1.00, 95% CI 0.93 to 1.08; five studies, 712 participants; Analysis 5.3; Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Dupaul 2012; Kollins 2014; Martin 2014a); or the proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event (RR 2.72, 95% CI 1.09 to 6.75; six studies, 828 participants; Analysis 5.4; Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Brams 2012; Dupaul 2012; Kollins 2014; Martin 2014a); and

  3. MAS on severity of ADHD symptoms as assessed by clinicians (SMD −0.81, 95% CI −0.94 to −0.69; five studies, 1083 participants; Analysis 6.1; Frick 2017; Levin 2015; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Weisler 2017); retention in treatment (RR 1.16, 95% CI 1.05 to 1.28; eight studies, 1569 participants; Analysis 6.2; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Levin 2015; Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017); or the proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event (RR 3.73, 95% CI 2.16 to 6.44; seven studies, 1539 participants; Analysis 6.3; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Levin 2015; Martin 2014b; Spencer 2008; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017).

4.1. Analysis.

4.1

Comparison 4 Subgroup analysis 3: dose of dexamphetamine, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated.

5.1. Analysis.

5.1

Comparison 5 Subgroup analysis 3: dose of lisdexamfetamine, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated.

5.2. Analysis.

5.2

Comparison 5 Subgroup analysis 3: dose of lisdexamfetamine, Outcome 2 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated.

5.3. Analysis.

5.3

Comparison 5 Subgroup analysis 3: dose of lisdexamfetamine, Outcome 3 Retention in treatment.

5.4. Analysis.

5.4

Comparison 5 Subgroup analysis 3: dose of lisdexamfetamine, Outcome 4 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event.

6.1. Analysis.

6.1

Comparison 6 Subgroup analysis 3: dose of mixed amphetamine salts, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated.

6.2. Analysis.

6.2

Comparison 6 Subgroup analysis 3: dose of mixed amphetamine salts, Outcome 2 Retention in treatment.

6.3. Analysis.

6.3

Comparison 6 Subgroup analysis 3: dose of mixed amphetamine salts, Outcome 3 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event.

Type of drug‐release formulation

We found no evidence to suggest that the type of drug‐release formulation influences the effects of amphetamines on:

  1. severity of ADHD symptoms as assessed by clinicians (SMD −0.90, 95% CI −1.04 to −0.75; 13 studies, 2028 participants; Analysis 7.1; Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Brams 2012; Frick 2017; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010);

  2. severity of ADHD symptoms as assessed by participants (SMD −0.51, 95% CI −0.75 to −0.27; six studies, 120 participants; Analysis 7.2; Dupaul 2012; Kollins 2014; Martin 2014a; Martin 2014b; Taylor 2000; Taylor 2001); or

  3. retention in treatment (RR 1.06, 95% CI 0.99 to 1.13; 17 studies, 2323 participants; Analysis 7.3; Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Biederman 2012; Dupaul 2012; Frick 2017; Kay 2009; Kollins 2014; Levin 2015; Martin 2014a; Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Spencer 2008; Waxmonsky 2014; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017; Weiss 2006; Wigal 2010).

7.1. Analysis.

7.1

Comparison 7 Subgroup analysis 4: type of drug‐release formulation, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated.

7.2. Analysis.

7.2

Comparison 7 Subgroup analysis 4: type of drug‐release formulation, Outcome 2 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated.

7.3. Analysis.

7.3

Comparison 7 Subgroup analysis 4: type of drug‐release formulation, Outcome 3 Retention in treatment.

Study funding

Twelve studies were funded by the pharmaceutical industry; only one was a non‐commercial study. We decided post hoc not to conduct a subgroup analysis of study funding given the difference in the number of studies included within each subgroup, which could compromise the validity of these analyses.

Sensitivity analyses

We performed sensitivity analyses by limiting analyses to those studies scoring low risk of bias on two specific domains of the Cochrane 'Risk of bias' tool, namely, incomplete outcome data and other potential sources of bias. Findings from these analyses were similar to those of the primary analyses (incomplete outcome data: Analysis 8.1; Analysis 8.2; other potential sources of bias: Analysis 9.1; Analysis 9.2; Analysis 9.3).

8.1. Analysis.

8.1

Comparison 8 Sensitivity analysis: incomplete subjective outcome data, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated.

8.2. Analysis.

8.2

Comparison 8 Sensitivity analysis: incomplete subjective outcome data, Outcome 2 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated.

9.1. Analysis.

9.1

Comparison 9 Sensitivity analysis: other potential sources of bias, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated.

9.2. Analysis.

9.2

Comparison 9 Sensitivity analysis: other potential sources of bias, Outcome 2 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated.

9.3. Analysis.

9.3

Comparison 9 Sensitivity analysis: other potential sources of bias, Outcome 3 Retention in treatment.

We ran another sensitivity analysis by changing the statistical model from a random‐effects model, which we used to pool data in the main analysis, to a fixed‐effect model. We observed similar results for efficacy outcomes (Analysis 10.1; Analysis 10.2; Analysis 10.3; Analysis 1.4; Analysis 1.5; Analysis 10.6; Analysis 10.7; Analysis 10.8; Analysis 10.9; Analysis 10.10), with the exception of retention in treatment, which was higher for amphetamines than for placebo (Analysis 10.11: RR 1.10, 95% CI 1.04 to 1.16; 17 studies, 2323 participants). We observed similar results for adverse events when using the fixed‐effect model (Analysis 10.12; Analysis 10.13).

10.1. Analysis.

10.1

Comparison 10 Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician‐rated.

10.2. Analysis.

10.2

Comparison 10 Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model, Outcome 2 ADHD symptom severity: patient‐rated.

10.3. Analysis.

10.3

Comparison 10 Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model, Outcome 3 Clinical impression of severity at study end.

10.6. Analysis.

10.6

Comparison 10 Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model, Outcome 6 Proportion of participants achieving a CGI‐Improvement score of 1 or 2.

10.7. Analysis.

10.7

Comparison 10 Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model, Outcome 7 Proportion of participants achieving a reduction ≥ 30% in severity of ADHD symptoms and a CGI‐Improvement score of 1 or 2.

10.8. Analysis.

10.8

Comparison 10 Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model, Outcome 8 Global functioning.

10.9. Analysis.

10.9

Comparison 10 Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model, Outcome 9 Depressive symptoms.

10.10. Analysis.

10.10

Comparison 10 Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model, Outcome 10 Anxiety symptoms.

10.11. Analysis.

10.11

Comparison 10 Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model, Outcome 11 Retention in treatment.

10.12. Analysis.

10.12

Comparison 10 Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model, Outcome 12 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any cardiovascular adverse event.

10.13. Analysis.

10.13

Comparison 10 Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model, Outcome 13 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event.

We also conducted three post hoc sensitivity analyses.

  1. We repeated the analysis of the severity of ADHD symptoms as rated by clinicians and participants after calculating the effect size of four studies (Dupaul 2012; Martin 2014a/Martin 2014b; Spencer 2001; Wigal 2010) using the least conservative correlation coefficient (see Unit of analysis issues). Results of these analyses (clinician rated: SMD −0.90, 95% CI −1.05 to −0.76; 13 studies, 2028 participants; Analysis 11.1; participant rated: SMD −0.47, 95% CI −0.69 to −0.25; six studies, 120 participants; Analysis 11.2) were comparable with results of the original analyses (clinician rated: SMD −0.90, 95% CI −1.04 to −0.75; 13 studies, 2028 participants; Analysis 1.1; patient rated: SMD −0.51, 95% CI −0.75 to −0.28; six studies, 120 participants; Analysis 1.2).

  2. In the second analysis, we re‐analysed the outcomes of 'proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any cardiovascular adverse event' and 'proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event', calculating the risk difference (RD) (Analysis 12.1: RD 0.02, 95% CI −0.00 to 0.04; three studies, 699 participants; Analysis 12.2: RD 0.04, 95% CI 0.01 to 0.06; 17 studies, 2409 participants, respectively). This yielded similar findings to the previous analyses (Analysis 1.12: RR 2.18, 95% CI 0.39 to 12.04; three studies, 699 participants; Analysis 1.13: RR 2.69, 95% CI 1.64 to 4.42; 17 studies, 2409 participants, respectively).

  3. In the third analysis, we removed one study (Spencer 2001), which was showing a carry‐over effect from the analysis on severity of ADHD symptoms (Analysis 13.1: SMD −0.90, 95% CI −1.05 to −0.74; 12 studies, 1998 participants), and obtained similar results to the primary analysis (Analysis 1.1: SMD −0.90, 95% CI −1.04 to −0.75; 13 studies, 2028 participants), suggesting that inclusion of this study did not bias the results of this review.

11.1. Analysis.

11.1

Comparison 11 Post hoc sensitivity analysis 1: calculation of effect sizes using correlation coefficient from Taylor 2000, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated.

11.2. Analysis.

11.2

Comparison 11 Post hoc sensitivity analysis 1: calculation of effect sizes using correlation coefficient from Taylor 2000, Outcome 2 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated.

12.1. Analysis.

12.1

Comparison 12 Post hoc sensitivity analysis 2: pooled risk difference for proportion of participants withdrawn owing to cardiovascular adverse events and any adverse event, Outcome 1 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any cardiovascular adverse event.

12.2. Analysis.

12.2

Comparison 12 Post hoc sensitivity analysis 2: pooled risk difference for proportion of participants withdrawn owing to cardiovascular adverse events and any adverse event, Outcome 2 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event.

13.1. Analysis.

13.1

Comparison 13 Post hoc sensitivity analysis 3: exclusion of cross‐over study, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated.

Amphetamines versus guanfacine

Only one study (17 participants) compared the efficacy of amphetamines versus guanfacine (Taylor 2001).

Primary outcomes: severity of ADHD symptoms

Taylor 2001 found no evidence to suggest that amphetamines are superior to guanfacine in reducing the severity of ADHD symptoms as rated by participants (Analysis 14.1).

14.1. Analysis.

14.1

Comparison 14 Amphetamines vs guanfacine for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated.

Secondary outcomes

Taylor 2001 did not provide data on any of our secondary outcomes.

Amphetamines versus modafinil

Only one study (22 participants) compared the efficacy of amphetamines versus modafinil (Taylor 2000).

Primary outcomes: severity of ADHD symptoms

Taylor 2000 found no evidence to suggest that amphetamines are superior to modafinil in reducing the severity of ADHD symptoms as rated by participants (Analysis 15.1).

15.1. Analysis.

15.1

Comparison 15 Amphetamines vs modafinil for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated.

Secondary outcomes

Taylor 2000 did not provide data on any of our secondary outcomes.

Amphetamines versus paroxetine

Only one study (98 participants) compared the efficacy of amphetamines versus paroxetine (Weiss 2006).

Primary outcomes: severity of ADHD symptoms

Weiss 2006 found no evidence to suggest that amphetamines are superior to paroxetine in reducing the severity of ADHD symptoms as rated by clinicians (Analysis 16.1).

16.1. Analysis.

16.1

Comparison 16 Amphetamines vs paroxetine for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated.

Secondary outcomes
Efficacy outcomes

Weiss 2006 found evidence indicating that amphetamines are more efficacious than paroxetine in increasing the proportion of participants achieving a CGI‐I score of 1 or 2 (Analysis 16.2), but are not more efficacious than paroxetine in improving global functioning (Analysis 16.3), reducing symptoms of depression (Analysis 16.4) or anxiety (Analysis 16.5), or improving retention in treatment (Analysis 16.6).

16.2. Analysis.

16.2

Comparison 16 Amphetamines vs paroxetine for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 2 Proportion of participants achieving a CGI‐Improvement score of 1 or 2.

16.3. Analysis.

16.3

Comparison 16 Amphetamines vs paroxetine for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 3 Global functioning.

16.4. Analysis.

16.4

Comparison 16 Amphetamines vs paroxetine for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 4 Depressive symptoms.

16.5. Analysis.

16.5

Comparison 16 Amphetamines vs paroxetine for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 5 Anxiety symptoms.

16.6. Analysis.

16.6

Comparison 16 Amphetamines vs paroxetine for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 6 Retention in treatment.

Adverse events

Weiss 2006 found no evidence that amphetamines are more efficacious than paroxetine in reducing the proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event (Analysis 16.7).

16.7. Analysis.

16.7

Comparison 16 Amphetamines vs paroxetine for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults, Outcome 7 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event.

Discussion

Summary of main results

Amphetamines showed mixed results in the treatment of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). We found low‐ to very low‐quality evidence suggesting that amphetamines were more efficacious than placebo in reducing the severity of ADHD symptoms, irrespective of the rater, and low‐quality evidence that they did not improve retention in treatment. Furthermore, amphetamines were associated with increased risk of dropping out owing to adverse events. Amphetamines were not effective in improving depressive and anxiety symptoms nor global functioning.

This review found that amphetamines reduced the severity of ADHD symptoms in adults in the short term. This finding was consistent across all analyses that were conducted using different efficacy definitions and statistical models. Furthermore, in most analyses, effect sizes of amphetamines for improving ADHD symptoms were moderate to high according to conventional cutoffs (Cohen 1988).

The included studies were of short duration, lasting an average of only 5.3 weeks. This is notable for three reasons. First, ADHD is a chronic disorder, and pharmacological treatment is usually administered over long periods of time. Second, because severity tends to lessen with age (Biederman 2000; Faraone 2006; Hill 1996), the possibility that the efficacy of amphetamines in adult ADHD is less after long‐term amphetamine treatment cannot be ruled out and should be studied through clinical trials with a longer follow‐up period. Third, some reports suggest that the efficacy of drugs used to treat ADHD tends to decrease progressively over time (Cunill 2016; MTA 2004; Riera 2017). Therefore, given that most included studies were of short duration, it is possible that effect sizes of amphetamines are smaller over the long term.

As a group, amphetamines did not improve retention in treatment. Retention can be interpreted as a risk‐benefit outcome because it reflects the combined evaluation of efficacy and safety (Castells 2013; Cunill 2013; Schhneider 2006; Stroup 2003). This result cannot be considered a positive one, as it is always desirable for any intervention to show a lower discontinuation rate than placebo, suggesting that the efficacy of the medication outweighs its side effects.

We found between‐study variability in relation to the severity of ADHD symptoms as assessed by clinicians. This resulted in moderate statistical heterogeneity. We investigated the source of this heterogeneity via four subgroup analyses (comorbidities, types of amphetamines, dose at study completion, and type of drug‐release formulation). Even though we found an effect for the type of amphetamine on the severity of ADHD symptoms, with lisdexamfetamine and MAS showing larger effect sizes than dexamphetamine, this factor did not entirely explain the between‐study variability, as within‐subgroup statistical heterogeneity remained evident. We also found moderate statistical heterogeneity for 'retention to treatment', but no subgroup analyses could control for such heterogeneity, which is likely to be explained by other co‐variates or a combination of them.

As stated above, we found that the type of amphetamine influenced clinician‐rated ADHD efficacy: although both lisdexamfetamine and MAS reduced the severity of ADHD symptoms compared to placebo, dexamphetamine did not. The type of amphetamine did not influence participant‐rated ADHD efficacy, retention to treatment, or adverse events. This result, along with the fact that dextroamphetamine has been infrequently studied, provides indirect and low‐quality evidence preferring lisdexamfetamine and MAS over dextroamphetamine.

Studies have investigated a wide range of doses, and higher and lower doses of amphetamine have shown similar results. This finding is consistent with that of clinical trials that have investigated the efficacy of multiple amphetamine doses and found no differences between treatment arms (Adler 2008; Weisler 2006). Methylphenidate given at a wide range of doses has also been investigated, and findings regarding its dose‐response effects have been contradictory: some studies suggested a positive relationship (Castells 2011b; Faraone 2004; Medori 2008), and others found no association with dose (Koesters 2008; Spencer 2007).

Amphetamines have a short half‐life and must be administered two or three times a day. To facilitate treatment compliance, sustained‐release formulations have been developed. This systematic review showed that amphetamines delivered via immediate‐release and slow‐release formulations had similar efficacy and tolerability. This is relevant, as long‐acting formulations have been found to be less efficacious than short‐acting ones (Peterson 2007).

Few studies included participants with comorbid disorders, which contrasts with the high prevalence of other psychiatric conditions diagnosed in patients with ADHD (Kessler 2006). The presence of comorbid disorders did not modify efficacy, retention to treatment, nor adverse events. This finding is consistent with those of a recent study that did not find comorbidity to modify the effects of pharmacological treatment in adults with ADHD (Cunill 2016).

We did not assess the effects of study sponsorship on efficacy, retention to treatment, or adverse events, because with the exception of one study, all were sponsored by the pharmaceutical industry. Other studies have shown that studies with a commercial sponsor report more favourable outcomes than are reported by independent studies (Lundh 2017; Riera 2017).

Failure to identify an impact of amphetamines on depressive and anxiety symptoms could be a consequence of the strict inclusion criteria adopted by most included studies, which excluded patients with major depressive or bipolar disorders. Baseline depression and anxiety scores therefore were low, leaving little room for improvement. Another possible interpretation is that the effects of amphetamines on ADHD symptoms are independent of effects on mood and anxiety. The number of studies included in these analyses was low, limiting our ability to draw conclusions.

Amphetamines have been compared with only three drugs (guanfacine, modafinil, and paroxetine) in three small clinical trials. Therefore, it is not surprising that no differences have been found for most outcomes.

Overall completeness and applicability of evidence

The overall completeness and applicability of evidence related to the efficacy and safety of amphetamines are limited by two factors. First, the dearth of data on patients with ADHD with comorbid disorders such as substance abuse or major depressive disorder. This is particularly notable given the high prevalence of comorbid psychiatric disorders in patients with ADHD (Biederman 2006; Levin 1998; Van Emmerik‐van Oortmerssen 2012; Young 2005), which is expected to increase further with the use of DSM‐5, as it permits a diagnosis of ADHD in patients with autism spectrum disorders. Second, the short duration of studies, which contrasts with the chronic course and long‐term treatment of the disorder. However, strengths must also be acknowledged. This review includes a systematic and exhaustive search that permitted us to identify all amphetamine trials performed in adults with ADHD. In addition, we were able to obtain a large quantity of missing data of interest from the trial authors of a number of studies included in this review.

Quality of the evidence

We did not find any study that was free of bias. Most articles reported neither on how the random sequence was generated nor how it was concealed. Therefore, we were not able to differentiate between reporting problems and study bias. However, even if these processes had been performed correctly, no study would have been rated as free of bias because amphetamines have intense behavioural effects, and participants and raters may have detected the administered study medication. This detection may have caused a blinding failure, which might have exaggerated the efficacy of the intervention (Schultz 1995); this type of bias is less likely to occur when amphetamines are compared to other psychostimulants such as modafinil (Taylor 2000). However, no study assessed whether blinding had failed, and the fact that all studies were scored at unclear risk of bias on this domain was based on the review authors' opinion, which, in turn, was based on ample evidence that amphetamines have intense behavioural and haemodynamic effects that can unmask the intervention being studied (Childs 2009; Johanson 1980; Makris 2004; Makris 2007; Wachtel 1992). Use of a nocebo (i.e. an active placebo that produces noticeable side effects that may convince the person that he/she is being treated with the active drug) has been proposed as a means of reducing the possibility of unblinding (Storebø 2015); however, this type of comparator has ethical problems, as it conflicts with the principle of non‐maleficence. A better alternative to nocebos would be the use of objective outcomes (e.g. accidents, legal or work problems), which have a lower risk of performance and detection bias than subjective outcomes (e.g. ADHD symptom severity). Use of objective, clinically meaningful outcomes, such as accidents or legal or work problems, would also improve the external validity of the findings of clinical trials including patients with ADHD. The validity of the outcome variables used to determine the efficacy of amphetamines for ADHD symptoms is an important question. The clinical interpretation of a reduction of 30% in the severity of ADHD symptoms or a change in the number of units on the ADHD Rating Scale is not straightforward. Thus, it would be helpful to use outcomes with greater clinical interpretability to improve our understanding of the effect of an intervention for this disorder; by way of example, one could monitor the proportion of patients achieving 'symptomatic remission' (i.e. the proportion of patients who fail to meet the full ADHD diagnostic criteria) (Biederman 2000; Keck 1998).

Indirectness moreover may have jeopardised the quality of evidence in this review. Indirectness can arise from combining different medications (e.g. different amphetamines), different doses of the same medication, or studies with important follow‐up differences, thereby hindering the possibility of making precise recommendations. Uncertainties regarding the indirectness of some estimations, the imprecision of some calculations, the existence of statistical heterogeneity, and the possibility of blinding failure mean that no result can be deemed to provide high‐quality or moderate‐quality evidence. Thus, it is likely that new research may change the main findings of this review.

We ran two post hoc sensitivity analyses excluding studies at unclear or high risk of bias on two specific domains of the Cochrane 'Risk of bias' tool: 'incomplete outcome data' and 'other biases'. These analyses yielded results similar to those of the primary ones, which suggests that our findings are robust against the two potential sources of bias.

Potential biases in the review process

We conducted a comprehensive search across several bibliographic databases and trial registers, without language restrictions. We also contacted the pharmaceutical industry and corresponding authors of included publications to enquire about additional studies that we may have missed. We did not, however, inspect FDA and EMA websites, and thus we cannot rule out the possibility that the review process is biased. However, we found no evidence of reporting bias, as suggested by a symmetrical funnel plot, but it must be highlighted that the sensitivity and precision of these tests are low.

We were able to obtain relevant data from almost all studies. We were able to obtain endpoint or change scores or response rates of clinician‐ or patient‐rated scales assessing the severity of ADHD symptoms in ways suitable for meta‐analysis, either directly from the study report or from the study authors. In addition, we were able to obtain data on all‐cause treatment discontinuation from 17 out of 19 studies.

With regards to the methods used, some studies applied a modified intention‐to‐treat (ITT) approach, where only participants who provided at least one post‐randomisation outcome were included in the efficacy analysis (Adler 2008; Adler 2013; Brams 2012; Spencer 2008; Weisler 2006; Weisler 2017). Not including all randomised participants may cause attrition bias. To minimise this source of bias, we used an ITT approach to calculate the risk ratio (RR) of these studies. Proceeding in this way yields more conservative efficacy results because it assumes that all individuals who left the study did not have the outcome. Provided that most studies provided short‐term follow‐up, and given that ADHD is a chronic disorder whose severity does not change after short periods of time, it seems reasonable to assume that participants who left the study were not treatment responders. Even if this is not the case, we expect this will have minimal influence on the results because the proportion of participants excluded from the efficacy analysis of those studies that used a modified ITT approach was low (consistently below 3% of the randomised sample).

We advise caution when interpreting the results of between‐subgroup comparisons. Given that these comparisons are indirect ones, head‐to‐head comparisons are needed to confirm their findings.

Agreements and disagreements with other studies or reviews

We rated the quality of the evidence in this review as low to very low (Otasowie 2014; Punja 2016; Storebø 2015), which is comparable with the quality of evidence reported by other Cochrane Reviews on this topic. Factors that limit the validity and quality of the evidence of systematic reviews of pharmacological treatment for ADHD are recurrent and include attrition bias, the possibility of blinding failure, imprecise results, and statistical heterogeneity (Castells 2011b; Castells 2013; Cunill 2016; Otasowie 2014; Peterson 2007; Punja 2016). Improving the quality of studies investigating the efficacy and safety of pharmacological treatment for ADHD has become a priority to increase the reliability of study findings.

Authors' conclusions

Implications for practice.

Amphetamines appear to improve the severity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms in adults in the short term. Nevertheless, compared to placebo, amphetamines do not increase retention in treatment overall and are associated with higher risk of dropping out as the result of adverse events. Furthermore, blinding failure could occur in the included studies, leading to an overestimation of amphetamine efficacy. For this reason, we considered evidence on the efficacy of amphetamines for ADHD in adults, generated by this review, to be of low or very low quality.

Evidence from this review does not provide a sound basis on which to support the use of higher doses of amphetamines or sustained‐release formulations to achieve greater efficacy. However, we did find differences between the types of amphetamines used: lisdexamfetamine was efficacious for reducing the severity of ADHD symptoms independently of the rater, but no evidence showed an effect of dexamphetamine or MAS on the severity of ADHD symptoms, respectively, as rated by clinicians or participants. This could provide indirect, low‐quality evidence in favour of lisdexamfetamine over dexamphetamine and MAS.

Implications for research.

The external validity of studies that have investigated the efficacy of amphetamines for ADHD in adults could be greater. This could be achieved by including patients with comorbid disorders such as substance use disorder or major depressive disorder. Studies with longer follow‐up periods are also needed to demonstrate the long‐term efficacy of amphetamines.

Use of objective outcomes that cannot be influenced by blinding failure, such as the number of accidents or problems at work or at home, would improve the reliability of findings. Nevertheless, it must be acknowledged that using these types of outcomes will make studies less feasible because large samples will be needed to demonstrate differences between the interventions studied.

Given that other drugs, such as atomoxetine or methylphenidate, have been shown to reduce ADHD symptoms in adults, it would be of great interest to compare the efficacy of amphetamines versus the efficacy of these interventions.

In addition, changes in comorbidity profiles with current DSM‐5 criteria mean that much of the work reviewed will need to be revalidated.

What's new

Date Event Description
27 September 2017 New citation required but conclusions have not changed 12 new studies included in the review; no changes made to the conclusions
21 August 2017 New search has been performed Review updated following a new search in July 2016 and a top‐up search in August 2017

History

Protocol first published: Issue 2, 2009
 Review first published: Issue 6, 2011

Date Event Description
8 December 2010 Amended Study author contact details updated

Acknowledgements

We would like to give special thanks to the current and past members of the Cochrane Developmental, Psychosocial and Learning Problems (CDPLP) Group named here: Georgia Salanti (emeritus CDPLP Statistican), as well as the editors and external referees for the many helpful comments and suggestions that we have received while we were conducting this review, which have represented a decisive contribution to this work.

Current CDPLP members of staff: Geraldine Macdonald (Coordinating Editor); Joanne Duffield (Managing Editor); and Margaret Anderson (Information Specialist).

Past CDPLP members of staff: Joanne Abbott; Chris Champion; Jane Dennis; and Laura MacDonald.

Appendices

Appendix 1. Search strategies from 2010 onwards

Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), in the Cochrane Library

#1[mh ^"attention deficit and disruptive behavior disorders"]
 #2[mh " attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity"]
 #3[mh "conduct disorder"]
 #4(ADHD or ADDH or ADHS or "AD/HD" or HKD or TDAH)
 #5((attention* or behav*) near/3 (defic* or dysfunc* or disorder*))
 #6((disrupt* near/3 disorder*) or (disrupt* near/3 behav*) or (defian* near/3 disorder*) or (defian* near/3 behav*))
 #7(impulsiv* or inattentiv* or inattention*)
 #8[mh hyperkinesis]
 #9(hyperkin* or hyper next kin*)
 #10(minimal* near/3 brain near/3 (disorder* or dysfunct* or damage*))
 #11(hyperactiv* or hyper next activ*)
 #12{or #1‐#11}
 #13[mh Amphetamines]
 #14(amphetamin* or amfetamin* or anfetamin*)
 #15benzedrin*
 #16(dexamphetamin* or dexamfetamin* or dextroamphetamin* or dextroamfetamin*)
 #17(dex next amphetamin* or dex next amfetamin* or dextro next amphetamin* or dextro next amfetamin*)
 #18(lisdexamphetamin* or lisdexamfetamin*)
 #19(lis next dexamphetamin* or lis next dexamfetamin*)
 #20(Adderall or Dexedrine or Elvanse or Vyvanse)
 #21{or #13‐#20}
 #22#12 and #21 in Trials

MEDLINE Ovid

1 "attention deficit and disruptive behavior disorders"/
 2 attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity/
 3 conduct disorder/
 4 ADHD.tw,kw.
 5 ADDH.tw,kw.
 6 ADHS.tw,kw.
 7 ("AD/HD" or HKD).tw,kw.
 8 TDAH.tw,kw.
 9 ((attention$ or behav$) adj3 (defic$ or dysfunc$ or disorder$)).tw,kw.
 10 ((disrupt$ adj3 disorder$) or (disrupt$ adj3 behav$) or (defian$ adj3 disorder$) or (defian$ adj3 behav$)).tw,kw.
 11 (impulsiv$ or inattentiv$ or inattention$).tw,kw.
 12 hyperkinesis/
 13 (hyperkin$ or hyper‐kin$).tw,kw.
 14 (minimal adj3 brain adj3 (disorder$ or dysfunct$ or damage$)).tw,kw.
 15 (hyperactiv$ or hyper‐activ$).tw,kw.
 16 or/1‐15
 17 exp Amphetamines/
 18 (amphetamin$ or amfetamin$ or anfetamin$).mp.
 19 benzedrin$.mp.
 20 (dexamphetamin$ or dexamfetamin$ or dextroamphetamin$ or dextroamfetamin$ or dexedrin$).mp.
 21 (dex‐amphetamine or dex‐amfetamine or dextro‐amphetamine or dextro‐amfetamine).mp.
 22 (lisdexamphetamin$ or lisdexamfetamin$).mp.
 23 (lis‐dexamphetamin$ or lis‐dexamfetamin$).mp.
 24 (Adderall or Dexedrine or Elvanse or Vyvanse).mp.
 25 or/17‐24
 26 16 and 25
 27 randomized controlled trial.pt.
 28 controlled clinical trial.pt.
 29 randomi#ed.ab.
 30 placebo$.ab.
 31 drug therapy.fs.
 32 randomly.ab.
 33 trial.ab.
 34 groups.ab.
 35 or/27‐34
 36 exp animals/ not humans.sh.
 37 35 not 36
 38 26 and 37

MEDLINE In‐Process and Other Non‐indexed Citations Ovid

1 ("AD/HD" or HKD).mp.
 2 TDAH.mp.
 3 ((attention$ or behav$) adj3 (defic$ or dysfunc$ or disorder$)).mp.
 4 ((disrupt$ adj3 disorder$) or (disrupt$ adj3 behav$) or (defian$ adj3 disorder$) or (defian$ adj3 behav$)).mp.
 5 (impulsiv$ or inattentiv$ or inattention$).mp.
 6 (hyperkin$ or hyper‐kin$).mp.
 7 (minimal adj3 brain adj3 (disorder$ or dysfunct$ or damage$)).mp.
 8 (hyperactiv$ or hyper‐activ$).mp.
 9 or/1‐8
 10 (amphetamin$ or amfetamin$ or anfetamin$).mp.
 11 benzedrin$.mp.
 12 (dexamphetamin$ or dexamfetamin$ or dextroamphetamin$ or dextroamfetamin$ or dexedrin$).mp.
 13 (dex‐amphetamine or dex‐amfetamine or dextro‐amphetamine or dextro‐amfetamine).mp.
 14 (Adderall or Dexedrine or Elvanse or Vyvanse).mp.
 15 or/10‐14
 16 random$.mp.
 17 control$.mp.
 18 placebo$.mp.
 19 trial.mp.
 20 groups.mp.
 21 or/16‐20
 22 9 and 15 and 21

MEDLINE Epub Ahead of Print Ovid

1 ("AD/HD" or HKD).mp.
 2 TDAH.mp.
 3 ((attention$ or behav$) adj3 (defic$ or dysfunc$ or disorder$)).mp.
 4 ((disrupt$ adj3 disorder$) or (disrupt$ adj3 behav$) or (defian$ adj3 disorder$) or (defian$ adj3 behav$)).mp.
 5 (impulsiv$ or inattentiv$ or inattention$).mp.
 6 (hyperkin$ or hyper‐kin$).mp.
 7 (minimal adj3 brain adj3 (disorder$ or dysfunct$ or damage$)).mp.
 8 (hyperactiv$ or hyper‐activ$).mp.
 9 or/1‐8
 10 (amphetamin$ or amfetamin$ or anfetamin$).mp.
 11 benzedrin$.mp.
 12 (dexamphetamin$ or dexamfetamin$ or dextroamphetamin$ or dextroamfetamin$ or dexedrin$).mp.
 13 (dex‐amphetamine or dex‐amfetamine or dextro‐amphetamine or dextro‐amfetamine).mp.
 14 (Adderall or Dexedrine or Elvanse or Vyvanse).mp.
 15 or/10‐14
 16 random$.mp.
 17 control$.mp.
 18 placebo$.mp.
 19 trial.mp.
 20 groups.mp.
 21 or/16‐20
 22 9 and 15 and 21

Embase Ovid

1 attention deficit disorder/
 2 hyperactivity/
 3 conduct disorder/
 4 ADHD.tw,kw.
 5 ADDH.tw,kw.
 6 ADHS.tw,kw.
 7 ("AD/HD" or HKD).tw,kw.
 8 TDAH.tw,kw.
 9 ((attention$ or behav$) adj3 (defic$ or dysfunc$ or disorder$)).tw,kw.
 10 ((disrupt$ adj3 disorder$) or (disrupt$ adj3 behav$) or (defian$ adj3 disorder$) or (defian$ adj3 behav$)).tw,kw.
 11 (impulsiv$ or inattentiv$ or inattention$).tw,kw.
 12 hyperkinesis/ (3903)
 13 (hyperkin$ or hyper‐kin$ or hkd).tw,kw.
 14 (minimal adj3 brain adj3 (disorder$ or dysfunct$ or damage$)).tw,kw.
 15 (hyperactiv$ or hyper‐activ$).tw,kw.
 16 or/1‐15
 17 exp amphetamine derivative/
 18 (amphetamin$ or amfetamin$ or anfetamin$).mp.
 19 benzedrin$.mp.
 20 (dexamphetamin$ or dexamfetamin$ or dextroamphetamin$ or dextroamfetamin$ or dexedrin$).mp.
 21 (dex‐amphetamine or dex‐amfetamine or dextro‐amphetamine or dextro‐amfetamine).mp.
 22 (lisdexamphetamin$ or lisdexamfetamin$).mp.
 23 (lis‐dexamphetamin$ or lis‐dexamfetamin$).mp.
 24 (Adderall or Dexedrine or Elvanse or Vyvanse).mp.
 25 or/17‐24
 26 16 and 25
 27 Randomized controlled trial/
 28 controlled clinical trial/
 29 Single blind procedure/
 30 Double blind procedure/
 31 triple blind procedure/
 32 Crossover procedure/
 33 (crossover or cross‐over).tw.
 34 ((singl$ or doubl$ or tripl$ or trebl$) adj1 (blind$ or mask$)).tw.
 35 Placebo/
 36 placebo.tw.
 37 prospective.tw.
 38 factorial$.tw.
 39 random$.tw.
 40 assign$.ab.
 41 allocat$.tw.
 42 volunteer$.ab.
 43 or/27‐42
 44 26 and 43

PsycINFO Ovid

1 exp attention deficit disorder/
 2 exp Behavior Problems/
 3 adhd.tw.
 4 addh.tw.
 5 adhs.tw.
 6 "ad/hd".tw.
 7 TDAH.tw.
 8 ((attention$ or behav$) adj3 (defic$ or dysfunc$ or disorder$)).tw.
 9 ((disrupt$ adj3 disorder$) or (disrupt$ adj3 behav$) or (defian$ adj3 disorder$) or (defian$ adj3 behav$)).tw.
 10 Impulsiveness/
 11 (impulsiv$ or inattentiv$ or inattention$).tw.
 12 hyperkinesis/
 13 (hyperkin$ or hyper‐kin$ or hkd).tw.
 14 (minimal adj3 brain$ adj3 (damag$ or disorder$ or dysfunc$)).tw.
 15 (hyperactiv$ or hyper‐activ$).tw.
 16 or/1‐15
 17 exp amphetamine/
 18 (amphetamin$ or amfetamin$ or anfetamin$).mp.
 19 benzedrin$.mp.
 20 (dexamphetamin$ or dexamfetamin$ or dextroamphetamin$ or dextroamfetamin$ or dexedrin$).mp.
 21 (dex‐amphetamine or dex‐amfetamine or dextro‐amphetamine or dextro‐amfetamine).mp.
 22 (lisdexamphetamin$ or lisdexamfetamin$).mp.
 23 (lis‐dexamphetamin$ or lis‐dexamfetamin$).mp.
 24 (Adderall or Dexedrine or Elvanse or Vyvanse).mp.
 25 or/17‐24
 26 16 and 25
 27 clinical trials/
 28 random$.tw.
 29 ((singl$ or doubl$ or trebl$ or tripl$) adj (blind$ or mask$)).tw.
 30 (crossover$ or "cross over$").tw.
 31 trial$.tw.
 32 group$.ab.
 33 control.ab.
 34 exp program evaluation/
 35 treatment effectiveness evaluation/
 36 treatment outcome clinical trial.md.
 37 ((effectiveness or evaluat$) adj3 (stud$ or research$)).tw.
 38 (allocat$ or assign$).tw.
 39 placebo.ab.
 40 or/27‐39
 41 26 and 40

CINAHL Plus EBSCOhost (Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature)

S1 (MH "Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder")
 S2 ADHD
 S3 ADDH
 S4 ADHS
 S5 TDAH
 S6 "AD/HD" or HKD
 S7 ((attention* or behav*) N3 (defic* or dysfunc* or disorder*))
 S8 ((disrupt* N3 disorder*) or (disrupt* N3 behav*) or (defian* N3 disorder*) or (defian* N3 behav*))
 S9 (impulsiv* or inattentiv* or inattention*)
 S10 (MH "Hyperkinesis")
 S11 hyperkin* or hyper‐kin*)
 S12 (minimal N3 brain N3 (disorder* or dysfunct* or damage*))
 S13 hyperactiv* or hyper‐activ*
 S14 S1 OR S2 OR S3 OR S4 OR S5 OR S6 OR S7 OR S8 OR S9 OR S10 OR S11 OR S12 OR S13
 S15 (MH "Amphetamines+")
 S16 (amphetamin* or amfetamin* or anfetamin*)
 S17 benzedrin*
 S18 (dexamphetamin* or dexamfetamin$ or dextroamphetamin* or dextroamfetamin*)
 S19 (dex‐amphetamine or dex‐amfetamin* or dextro‐amphetamin* or dextro‐amfetamin*)
 S20 lisdexamphetamin* or lisdexamfetamin*
 S21 lis‐dexamphetamin* or lis‐dexamfetamin*
 S22 (Adderall or Dexedrine or Elvanse or Vyvanse)
 S23 S15 OR S16 OR S17 OR S18 OR S19 OR S20 OR S21 OR S22
 S24 S14 AND S23
 S25 (MH "Clinical Trials+")
 S26 MH random assignment
 S27 (MH "Meta Analysis")
 S28 (MH "Crossover Design")
 S29 (MH "Quantitative Studies")
 S30 PT randomized controlled trial
 S31 PT Clinical trial
 S32 (clinical trial*) or (control* N2 trial*)
 S33 ("follow‐up study" or "follow‐up research")
 S34 (prospectiv* study or prospectiv* research)
 S35 (evaluat* N2 study or evaluat* N2 research)
 S36 (MH "Program Evaluation")
 S37 (MH "Treatment Outcomes")
 S38 TI(single N2 mask* or single N2 blind*) OR AB(single N2 mask* or single N2 blind*)
 S39 TI((doubl* N2 mask*) or (doubl* N2 blind*)) OR AB((doubl* N2 mask*) or (doubl* N2 blind*))
 S40 TI ((tripl* N2 mask*) or (tripl* N2 blind*)) or ((trebl* N2 mask*) or (trebl* N2 blind*)) OR AB((tripl* N2 mask*) or (tripl* N2 blind*)) or ((trebl* N2 mask*) or (trebl* N2 blind*)
 S41 random* N2 assign* OR random* N2 allocat*
 S42 S25 OR S26 OR S27 OR S28 OR S29 OR S30 OR S31 OR S32 OR S33 OR S34 OR S35 OR S36 OR S37 OR S38 OR S39 OR S40 OR S41 OR S43 S24 AND S42

Science Citation Index (SCI) and Social Science Citation Index (SSCI), both Web of Science

#19 #18 AND #17
 #18 TS=(RANDOM* OR TRIAL* OR GROUP* OR CONTROL* OR PLACEBO OR BLIND*)
 #17 #16 AND #8
 #16 #15 OR #14 OR #13 OR #12 OR #11 OR #10 OR #9
 #15 TS=(Adderall or Dexedrine or Elvanse or Vyvanse)
 #14 TS=(lis‐dexamphetamin* or lis‐dexamfetamin*)
 #13 TS=(lisdexamphetamin* or lisdexamfetamin*)
 #12 TS=(dex‐amphetamin* or dex‐amfetamin* or dextro‐amphetamin* or dextro‐amfetamin*)
 #11 TS=(dexamphetamin* or dexamfetamin* or dextroamphetamin* or dextroamfetamin*)
 #10 TS=benzedrin*
 #9 TS=(amphetamin* or amfetamin* or anfetamin* )
 #8 #7 OR #6 OR #5 OR #4 OR #3 OR #2 OR #1
 #7 TS=(hyperactiv* or hyper‐activ*)
 #6 TS=(minimal* near/3 brain near/3 (disorder* or dysfunct* or damage*))
 #5 TS=(hyperkin* or hyper‐kin*)
 #4 TS=(impulsiv* or inattentiv* or inattention*)
 #3 TS=((disrupt* near/3 disorder*) or (disrupt* near/3 behav*) or (defian* near/3 disorder*) or (defian* near/3 behav*))
 #2 TS=(ADHD or ADDH or ADHS or "AD/HD" or HKD or TDAH)
 #1 TS=((attention* or behav*) near/3 (defic* or dysfunc* or disorder*))

Conference Proceedings Citation Index ‐ Science (CPCI‐S) and Conference Proceedings Citation Index ‐ Social Science & Humanities (CPCI‐SS&H), both Web of Science

#19 #18 AND #17
 #18 TS=(RANDOM* OR TRIAL* OR GROUP* OR CONTROL* OR PLACEBO OR BLIND*)
 #17 #16 AND #8
 #16 #15 OR #14 OR #13 OR #12 OR #11 OR #10 OR #9
 #15 TS=(Adderall or Dexedrine or Elvanse or Vyvanse)
 #14 TS=(lis‐dexamphetamin* or lis‐dexamfetamin*)
 #13 TS=(lisdexamphetamin* or lisdexamfetamin*)
 #12 TS=(dex‐amphetamin* or dex‐amfetamin* or dextro‐amphetamin* or dextro‐amfetamin*)
 #11 TS=(dexamphetamin* or dexamfetamin* or dextroamphetamin* or dextroamfetamin*)
 #10 TS=benzedrin*
 #9 TS=(amphetamin* or amfetamin* or anfetamin* )
 #8 #7 OR #6 OR #5 OR #4 OR #3 OR #2 OR #1
 #7 TS=(hyperactiv* or hyper‐activ*)
 #6 TS=(minimal* near/3 brain near/3 (disorder* or dysfunct* or damage*))
 #5 TS=(hyperkin* or hyper‐kin*)
 #4 TS=(impulsiv* or inattentiv* or inattention*)
 #3 TS=((disrupt* near/3 disorder*) or (disrupt* near/3 behav*) or (defian* near/3 disorder*) or (defian* near/3 behav*))
 #2 TS=(ADHD or ADDH or ADHS or "AD/HD" or HKD or TDAH)
 #1 TS=((attention* or behav*) near/3 (defic* or dysfunc* or disorder*))

Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (CDSR), part of the Cochrane Library

#1[mh ^"attention deficit and disruptive behavior disorders"]
 #2[mh " attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity"]
 #3[mh "conduct disorder"]
 #4(ADHD or ADDH or ADHS or "AD/HD" or HKD or TDAH):ti,ab,kw
 #5((attention* or behav*) near/3 (defic* or dysfunc* or disorder*)):ti,ab,kw
 #6((disrupt* near/3 disorder*) or (disrupt* near/3 behav*) or (defian* near/3 disorder*) or (defian* near/3 behav*)):ti,ab,kw
 #7(impulsiv* or inattentiv* or inattention*):ti,ab,kw
 #8[mh hyperkinesis]
 #9(hyperkin* or hyper next kin*):ti,ab,kw
 #10(minimal* near/3 brain near/3 (disorder* or dysfunct* or damage*)):ti,ab,kw
 #11(hyperactiv* or hyper next activ*):ti,ab,kw
 #12{or #1‐#11}
 #13[mh Amphetamines]
 #14(amphetamin* or amfetamin* or anfetamin*):ti,ab,kw
 #15benzedrin*:ti,ab,kw
 #16(dexamphetamin* or dexamfetamin* or dextroamphetamin* or dextroamfetamin*):ti,ab,kw
 #17(dex next amphetamin* or dex next amfetamin* or dextro next amphetamin* or dextro next amfetamin*):ti,ab,kw
 #18(lisdexamphetamin* or lisdexamfetamin*):ti,ab,kw
 #19(lis next dexamphetamin* or lis next dexamfetamin*):ti,ab,kw
 #20(Adderall or Dexedrine or Elvanse or Vyvanse):ti,ab,kw
 #21{or #13‐#20} in Cochrane Reviews (Reviews and Protocols)

Database of Abstracts of Reviews of Effects (DARE), part of the Cochrane Library

#1[mh ^"attention deficit and disruptive behavior disorders"]
 #2[mh " attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity"]
 #3[mh "conduct disorder"]
 #4(ADHD or ADDH or ADHS or "AD/HD" or HKD or TDAH):ti,ab,kw
 #5((attention* or behav*) near/3 (defic* or dysfunc* or disorder*)):ti,ab,kw
 #6((disrupt* near/3 disorder*) or (disrupt* near/3 behav*) or (defian* near/3 disorder*) or (defian* near/3 behav*)):ti,ab,kw
 #7(impulsiv* or inattentiv* or inattention*):ti,ab,kw
 #8[mh hyperkinesis]
 #9(hyperkin* or hyper next kin*):ti,ab,kw
 #10(minimal* near/3 brain near/3 (disorder* or dysfunct* or damage*)):ti,ab,kw
 #11(hyperactiv* or hyper next activ*):ti,ab,kw
 #12{or #1‐#11}
 #13[mh Amphetamines]
 #14(amphetamin* or amfetamin* or anfetamin*):ti,ab,kw
 #15benzedrin*:ti,ab,kw
 #16(dexamphetamin* or dexamfetamin* or dextroamphetamin* or dextroamfetamin*):ti,ab,kw
 #17(dex next amphetamin* or dex next amfetamin* or dextro next amphetamin* or dextro next amfetamin*):ti,ab,kw
 #18(lisdexamphetamin* or lisdexamfetamin*):ti,ab,kw
 #19(lis next dexamphetamin* or lis next dexamfetamin*):ti,ab,kw
 #20(Adderall or Dexedrine or Elvanse or Vyvanse):ti,ab,kw
 #21{or #13‐#20}
 #22#12 and #21 in Other Reviews

WorldCat (www.worldcat.org)

'kw:(adhd OR "attention deficit" OR hyper*) AND (amfetamin* OR amphetamin* ) AND KW:(random* OR trial* OR placebo* OR control* OR blind* )' >'Thesis/dissertation'

Clinicaltrials.gov (clinicaltrials.gov)

Advanced search ADHD OR hyperactive OR attention deficit | AMFETAMINES OR amphetamines OR dexamphetamine or dexamfetamine or dextroamphetamine or dextroamfetamine OR lisdexamphetamin OR lisdexamfetamin* OR Adderall OR Dexedrine OR Elvanse OR Vyvanse | Adult, Senior

World Health Organization International Clinical Trials Registry Platform (WHO ICTRP; www.who.int/ictrp/en)

Advanced search Condition: ADHD OR hyperactive OR attention deficit AND Intervention:amfetamin* OR amphetamin* OR dexamphetamine or dexamfetamine or dextroamphetamine or dextroamfetamine OR lisdexamphetamin OR lisdexamfetamin* OR Adderall OR Dexedrine OR Elvanse OR Vyvanse.
 
 AUTOMATIC SYNONYMS included in search: ADDH, ATTENTION DEFICIT DIS WITH HYPERACTIVITY, ATTENTION DEFICIT DISORDER, ATTENTION DEFICIT DISORDER OF CHILDHOOD WITH HYPERACTIVITY, ATTENTION
 DEFICIT DISORDER WITH HYPERACTIVITY, ATTENTION DEFICIT DISORDERS WITH HYPERACTIVITY, ATTENTION DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DIS, ATTENTION DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, ATTENTION DEFICIT
 HYPERACTIVITY DISORDERS, ATTENTION DEFICIT‐HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, ATTENTION‐DEFICIT DISORDER, COMBINED TYPE, ATTENTION‐DEFICIT DISORDER, PREDOMINANTLY HYPERACTIVE‐IMPULSIVE TYPE, ATTN
 DEFICIT W HYPERACT, CHILDHOOD HYPERKINETIC SYNDROME, HYPERACTIVE CHILD SYNDROME, HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER NOS, HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER, PREDOMINANTLY HYPERACTIVE‐IMPULSIVE TYPE, HYPERACTIVITY
 OF CHILDHOOD, HYPERKINETIC SYND NOS, HYPERKINETIC SYNDROME, HYPERKINETIC SYNDROME NOS, HYPERKINETIC SYNDROME OF CHILDHOOD, SYNDROME HYPERKINETIC, SYNDROMES, HYPERKINETIC, UNSPECIFIED
 HYPERKINETIC SYNDROME OF CHILDHOOD, adhd: ‐ amphetamine a d ‐ d amphetamine ‐ d‐amphetamine ‐ d‐amphetamine sulfate ‐ dexamfetamine ‐ dexamphetamine ‐ dexedrine ‐ dextro amphetamine ‐
 dextro‐amphetamine ‐ dextroamphetamine ‐ lisdexamphetamin ‐ dextroamfetamine ‐ lisdexamfetamin ‐ amphetamine a d ‐ d amphetamine ‐ d‐amphetamine ‐ dexamfetamine ‐ dextro amphetamine ‐
 dextro‐amphetamine ‐ dextroamphetamine ‐ dexamphetamine ‐ amphetamine a d ‐ d amphetamine ‐ d‐amphetamine ‐ dexamphetamine ‐ dextro amphetamine ‐ dextro‐amphetamine ‐ dextroamphetamine ‐
 dexamfetamine ‐ amphetamin ‐ celltech brand of dextroamphetamine sulfate ‐ dextroamphetamine ‐ dextroamphetamine sulfate ‐ glaxosmithkline brand of dextroamphetamine sulfate ‐ mallinckrodt
 brand of dextroamphetamine sulfate ‐ dexedrine ‐ amfetamin ‐ dextroamphetamine‐amphetamine ‐ adderall ‐ vyvanse ‐ elvanse

Appendix 2. Search strategies up to 2010

Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), in the Cochrane Library

#1 MeSH descriptor Amphetamines explode all trees
 #2 adderall
 #3 lisdexamphetamine or lisdexamfetamine or vyvanse
 #4 amphetamine* or amfetamine* or amphetamine or amfetamine or benzedrine
 #5 dexamphetamine or dexamfetamine or dextroamphetamine or dextroamfetamine or dexedrine
 #6 (#1 OR #2 OR #3 OR #4 OR #5)
 #7 MeSH descriptor Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity explode all trees
 #8 adhd
 #9 addh
 #10 adhs
 #11 hyperactiv*
 #12 hyperkin*
 #13 attention* AND deficit*
 #14 attention* AND disorder*
 #15 brain dysfunction*
 #16 (#7 OR #8 OR #9 OR #10 OR #11 OR #12 OR #13 OR #14 OR #15)
 #17 (#6 AND #16)

PubMed

("amphetamine"[MeSH Terms] OR Adderall OR lisdexamphetamine OR  lisdexamfetamine OR vyvanse OR amphetamine* OR amfetamine* OR amphetamine OR amfetamine OR benzedrine OR dexamphetamine OR dexamfetamine OR "dextroamphetamine"[MeSH Terms] OR dextroamfetamine OR dexedrine) AND ("attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity"[MeSH Terms] OR ADHD OR ADDH OR ADHS OR "attention deficit" OR "brain dysfunction") AND (randomized controlled trial [pt] OR controlled clinical trial [pt] OR randomized [tiab] OR placebo [tiab] OR drug therapy [sh] OR randomly [tiab] OR trial [tiab] OR groups [tiab]) NOT (animals [mh] NOT humans [mh])

Embase Ovid

1     Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/
 2     adhd.tw.
 3     addh.tw.
 4     adhs.tw.
 5     hyperactiv$.tw.
 6     hyperkin$.tw.
 7     attention deficit$.tw.
 8     brain dysfunction.tw.
 9     or/1‐8
 10     exp Amphetamine/
 11     adderall.tw.
 12     lisdexamphetamine.tw.
 13     lisdexamfetamine.tw.
 14     vyvanse.tw.
 15     amphetamine$.tw.
 16     amfetamine*.tw.
 17     benzedrine.tw.
 18     dexamphetamine.tw.
 19     dexamfetamine.tw.
 20     exp Dextroamphetamine/
 21     dextroamfetamine.tw.
 22     dexedrine.tw.
 23     or/10‐22
 24     9 and 23
 25     random$.tw.
 26     factorial$.tw.
 27     crossover$.tw.
 28     cross over$.tw.
 29     cross‐over$.tw.
 30     placebo$.tw.
 31     (doubl$ adj blind$).tw.
 32     (singl$ adj blind$).tw.
 33     assign$.tw.
 34     allocat$.tw.
 35     volunteer$.tw.
 36     Crossover Procedure/
 37     double‐blind procedure.tw.
 38     Randomized Controlled Trial/
 39     Single Blind Procedure/
 40     or/25‐39
 41     24 and 40

CINAHL EBSCOhost (Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature)

S40 S23 and S39
 S39 S24 or S25 or S26 or S27 or S28 or S29 or S30 or S31 or S32 or S33 or S34 or S35 or S36 or S37 or S38
 S38 allocat* random*
 S37 (MH "Quantitative Studies")
 S36 (MH "Placebos")
 S35 placebo*
 S34 random* allocat*
 S33 (MH "Random Assignment")
 S32 (Randomi?ed control* trial*)
 S31 (singl* mask* )
 S30 (doubl* mask* )
 S29 (tripl* mask* )
 S28 (trebl* mask* )
 S27 (trebl* blind* )
 S26 (tripl* blind* )
 S25 (doubl* blind* )
 S24 (singl* blind* )
 S23 S9 and S22
 S22 S10 or S11 or S12 or S13 or S14 or S15 or S16 or S17 or S18 or S19 or S20 or S21
 S21 dexedrine
 S20 dextroamphetamine
 S19 dexamfetamine
 S18 dexamphetamine
 S17 benzedrine
 S16 amfetamine*
 S15 amphetamine*
 S14 vyvanse
 S13 lisdexamfetamine
 S12 lisdexamphetamine
 S11 adderall
 S10 (MH "Amphetamine+")
 S9 S1 or S2 or S3 or S4 or S5 or S6 or S7 or S8
 S8 brain dysfunction
 S7 attention deficit*
 S6 hyperkin*
 S5 hyperactiv*
 S4 adhs
 S3 addh
 S2 adhd
 S1 (MH "Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder")

PsycINFO Ovid

1 Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/
 2 adhd.tw.
 3 addh.tw.
 4 adhs.tw.
 5 hyperactiv$.tw.
 6 hyperkin$.tw.
 7 attention deficit$.tw.
 8 brain dysfunction.tw.
 9 or/1‐8
 10 exp Amphetamine/
 11 adderall.tw.
 12 lisdexamphetamine.tw.
 13 lisdexamfetamine.tw.
 14 vyvanse.tw.
 15 amphetamine$.tw.
 16 amfetamine*.tw.
 17 benzedrine.tw.
 18 dexamphetamine.tw.
 19 dexamfetamine.tw.
 20 exp Dextroamphetamine/
 21 dextroamfetamine.tw.
 22 dexedrine.tw.
 23 or/10‐22
 24 9 and 23
 25 Treatment Effectiveness Evaluation/
 26 exp Treatment Outcomes/
 27 Psychotherapeutic Outcomes/
 28 PLACEBO/
 29 exp Followup Studies/
 30 placebo$.tw.
 31 random$.tw.
 32 comparative stud$.tw.
 33 randomi#ed controlled trial$.tw.
 34 (clinical adj3 trial$).tw.
 35 (research adj3 design).tw.
 36 (evaluat$ adj3 stud$).tw.
 37 (prospectiv$ adj3 stud$).tw.
 38 ((singl$ or doubl$ or trebl$ or tripl$) adj3 (blind$ or mask$)).tw.
 39 control$.tw.
 40 39 or 31 or 29 or 37 or 36 or 32 or 25 or 30 or 26 or 38 or 34 or 28 or 27 or 35 or 33
 41 24 and 40

Clinicaltrials.gov (clinicaltrials.gov)

("amphetamine" OR Adderall OR lisdexamphetamine OR  lisdexamfetamine OR vyvanse OR amphetamine* OR amfetamine* OR amphetamine OR amfetamine OR benzedrine OR dexamphetamine OR dexamfetamine OR "dextroamphetamine" OR dextroamfetamine OR dexedrine) AND ("attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity" OR ADHD OR ADDH OR ADHS OR "attention deficit" OR "brain dysfunction")

Appendix 3. Data to be extracted from included studies

Study description and funding

Author
 Year of publication
 Country
 Author affiliation (pharmaceutical industry (Yes/No))
 Study funding (pharmaceutical industry (Yes/No))

Methods

Sequence generation
 Allocation concealment
 Blinding of patients/clinicians/therapists/assessors
 Design (cross‐over/parallel groups)
 Design (single site/multiple sites)
 Study length (from randomisation to treatment completion)
 Number of participants
 Handling of dropouts (intention‐to‐treat (ITT) vs non‐ITT)
 Outcomes (including description of instruments used)

Participants

Inclusion/exclusion criteria
 Gender (% male)
 Age (mean, standard deviation (SD))
 Race (% Caucasian, % African‐American, % other)
 Employment status  (% unemployed)
 Prior ADHD treatment (Yes/No), with psychostimulants (Yes/No)
 Type of ADHD (% with inattentive subtype, % hyperactive subtype, % combined subtype)
 Comorbid disorders (% with comorbid psychiatric disorders)

Intervention

Type of amphetamine
 Dose
 Pharmaceutical presentation
 Assessment of compliance
 Adjunctive psychological interventions

Outcomes

  1. Achievement of significant clinical improvement. Although several definitions of significant improvement of ADHD symptom are used, we will prefer a "30% reduction of the ADHD severity". When any study uses a different definition, we will collect and use it also (% patients achieving a significant clinical improvement)

  2. ADHD severity score (note: these data will be collected for each instrument used to assess ADHD symptom severity)

  3. Clinical impression of severity (% patients achieving a Clinical Global Impression (CGI) ‐ Severity score of one or two at study end, mean (SD) CGI‐Severity at the end of the study)

  4. Clinical impression of improvement (% patients achieving a CGI‐Improvement score of one or two at study end, mean (SD) CGI‐Improvement at the end of the study)

  5. Global functioning (mean (SD) score at study end)

  6. Anxiety symptom severity (mean (SD) anxiety score at study end)

  7. Depressive symptom severity (mean (SD) depression score at study end)

  8. Number of participants withdrawn owing to adverse events (% participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event and % participants withdrawn owing to a cardiovascular adverse event)

  9. Abuse of study medication (% participants who abused study medication)

  10. Retention in treatment (% participants who completed the study)

Appendix 4. Criteria for assigning 'Risk of bias' judgements

Sequence generation

Description: the method used to generate the allocation sequence is described in sufficient detail to assess whether it should have produced comparable groups.
 Review authors' judgement: was the allocation concealment sequence adequately generated?

Allocation concealment

Description: the method used to conceal the allocation sequence is described in sufficient detail to assess whether intervention schedules could have been foreseen in advance of, or during, recruitment.
 Review authors' judgement: was allocation adequately concealed?

Blinding of participants and personnel

Description: measures used to keep the intervention blinded to participants and personnel are described in sufficient detail to assess the suitability of methods used to prevent knowledge of the allocated intervention.
 Review authors' judgement: was knowledge of the allocated intervention adequately prevented during the study?

Blinding of outcome assessment

Description: measures used to keep the intervention blinded to outcome assessors are described in sufficient detail to assess the suitability of methods used to prevent knowledge of the allocated intervention.
 Review authors' judgement: was knowledge of the allocated intervention adequately prevented during the study?

Incomplete outcome data

Description: if studies did not report intention‐to‐treat analyses, we attempted to obtain the missing data by contacting the study authors. We extracted and reported data on attrition and exclusions as well as numbers involved (compared with total). We also provided reasons for attrition/exclusion when reported or obtained from investigators, and incorporated any re‐inclusions in analyses performed.
 Review authors' judgement: were incomplete data dealt with adequately by the reviewers? (see also Dealing with missing data)

Selective outcome reporting

Description: we attempted to assess the possibility of selective outcome reporting by investigators.
 Review authors' judgement: are reports of the study free of the suggestion of selective outcome reporting?

We attempted to deal with the possibility of selective outcome reporting by searching for the original protocols of each included study in trial registries and comparing these (when available) with the list of outcomes in the methods section of the final report. We constructed a table to present an outcomes matrix to compare relevant outcomes between studies. In cases where we suspected selective outcome reporting, we contacted the study author.

Other bias

Description: we assessed imbalanced baseline characteristics, blocked randomisations, and deviations from protocol, as well as the possibility of carry‐over effect in cross‐over trials.
 Review authors' judgement: was there any evidence of other potential sources of bias?

We considered a study to be (1) at low risk of bias overall if all key domains were judged at low risk of bias; (2) at unclear risk of bias overall if one or more domains were judged at unclear risk of bias and all other domains were judged at low risk of bias; and (3) at high risk of bias overall if one or more domains were judged at high risk of bias (Higgins 2017a).

Data and analyses

Comparison 1. Amphetamines vs placebo for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician‐rated 13   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.90 [‐1.04, ‐0.75]
2 ADHD symptom severity: patient‐rated 6   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.51 [‐0.75, ‐0.28]
3 Clinical impression of severity at study end 2 78 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐1.09 [‐1.57, ‐0.61]
4 Clinical impression of improvement at study end 1 263 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.75 [‐1.01, ‐0.48]
5 Proportion of participants achieving a reduction ≥ 30% in severity of ADHD symptoms 2 381 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.52 [1.19, 1.95]
6 Proportion of participants achieving a CGI‐Improvement score of 1 or 2 8 1707 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 2.47 [2.10, 2.90]
7 Proportion of participants achieving a reduction ≥ 30% in severity of ADHD symptoms and a CGI‐Improvement score of 1 or 2 1 61 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 2.54 [1.34, 4.82]
8 Global functioning 2 110 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.54 [‐0.34, 1.42]
9 Depressive symptoms 2 110 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.16 [‐0.22, 0.53]
10 Anxiety symptoms 2 110 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.13 [‐0.24, 0.51]
11 Retention in treatment 17 2323 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.06 [0.99, 1.13]
11.1 Dexamphetamine 1 49 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 0.79 [0.54, 1.17]
11.2 Lisdexamfetamine 8 873 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.01 [0.94, 1.08]
11.3 Mixed amphetamine salts 8 1401 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.14 [1.02, 1.28]
12 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any cardiovascular adverse event 3 699 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 2.18 [0.39, 12.04]
13 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event 17 2409 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 2.69 [1.64, 4.42]
13.1 Dexamphetamine 1 49 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.70 [0.31, 9.27]
13.2 Lisdexamfetamine 9 989 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.79 [0.72, 4.42]
13.3 Mixed amphetamine salts 7 1371 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 3.50 [1.86, 6.59]

Comparison 2. Subgroup analysis 1: comorbidity.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician‐rated 13   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.90 [‐1.04, ‐0.75]
1.1 With comorbidity 2   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.76 [‐1.11, ‐0.41]
1.2 Without comorbidity 11   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.91 [‐1.07, ‐0.76]
2 ADHD symptom severity: patient‐rated 6   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.51 [‐0.75, ‐0.28]
2.1 With comorbidity 1   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.66 [‐1.44, 0.12]
2.2 Without comorbidity 5   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.50 [‐0.77, ‐0.23]
3 Retention in treatment 17 2323 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.06 [0.99, 1.13]
3.1 With comorbidity 2 158 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.01 [0.77, 1.33]
3.2 Without comorbidity 15 2165 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.07 [0.99, 1.15]
4 Proportion of patients withdrawn owing to any adverse event 17 2409 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 2.69 [1.64, 4.42]
4.1 With comorbidity 2 158 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 2.67 [0.12, 60.93]
4.2 Without comorbidity 15 2251 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 2.69 [1.63, 4.45]

Comparison 3. Subgroup analysis 2: type of amphetamine.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician‐rated 13   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.90 [‐1.04, ‐0.75]
1.1 Dexamphetamine 1   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.24 [‐0.80, 0.32]
1.2 Lisdexamfetamine 7   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐1.06 [‐1.26, ‐0.85]
1.3 Mixed amphetamine salts 5   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.80 [‐0.93, ‐0.66]
2 ADHD symptom severity: patient‐rated 6   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.51 [‐0.75, ‐0.28]
2.1 Dexamphetamine 2   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.77 [‐1.14, ‐0.40]
2.2 Lisdexamfetamine 3   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.33 [‐0.65, ‐0.01]
2.3 Mixed amphetamine salts 1   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.45 [‐1.02, 0.12]
3 Retention in treatment 17 2323 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.06 [0.99, 1.13]
3.1 Dexamphetamine 1 49 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 0.79 [0.54, 1.17]
3.2 Lisdexamfetamine 8 873 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.01 [0.94, 1.08]
3.3 Mixed amphetamine salts 8 1401 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.14 [1.02, 1.28]
4 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event 17 2409 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 2.69 [1.64, 4.42]
4.1 Dexamphetamine 1 49 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.70 [0.31, 9.27]
4.2 Lisdexamfetamine 9 989 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.79 [0.72, 4.42]
4.3 Mixed amphetamine salts 7 1371 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 3.50 [1.86, 6.59]

Comparison 4. Subgroup analysis 3: dose of dexamphetamine.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated 2   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.77 [‐1.14, ‐0.40]
1.1 Lower dose 1   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.55 [‐1.10, ‐0.00]
1.2 Higher dose 1   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.93 [‐1.40, ‐0.46]

Comparison 5. Subgroup analysis 3: dose of lisdexamfetamine.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated 6   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐1.02 [‐1.22, ‐0.82]
1.1 Lower dose 2   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.98 [‐1.41, ‐0.55]
1.2 Higher dose 5   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐1.04 [‐1.31, ‐0.78]
2 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated 3   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.35 [‐0.61, ‐0.10]
2.1 Lower dose 1   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.33 [‐0.78, 0.12]
2.2 Higher dose 3   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.36 [‐0.67, ‐0.05]
3 Retention in treatment 5 712 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.00 [0.93, 1.08]
3.1 Lower dose 2 322 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.01 [0.89, 1.14]
3.2 Higher dose 4 390 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.01 [0.89, 1.14]
4 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event 6 828 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 2.72 [1.09, 6.75]
4.1 Lower dose 3 335 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 2.98 [0.56, 15.72]
4.2 Higher dose 4 493 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 2.61 [0.88, 7.75]

Comparison 6. Subgroup analysis 3: dose of mixed amphetamine salts.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated 5   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.81 [‐0.94, ‐0.69]
1.1 Lower dose 3   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.78 [‐0.94, ‐0.63]
1.2 Higher dose 3   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.86 [‐1.06, ‐0.66]
2 Retention in treatment 8 1569 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.16 [1.05, 1.28]
2.1 Lower dose (50 mg/d) 5 962 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.13 [0.96, 1.32]
2.2 Higher dose (50 mg/d) 5 607 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.21 [1.09, 1.35]
3 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event 7 1539 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 3.73 [2.16, 6.44]
3.1 Lower dose 5 962 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 3.59 [1.84, 7.00]
3.2 Higher dose 4 577 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 4.03 [1.56, 10.42]

Comparison 7. Subgroup analysis 4: type of drug‐release formulation.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated 13   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.90 [‐1.04, ‐0.75]
1.1 Immediate‐release formulations 1   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.91 [‐1.38, ‐0.44]
1.2 Sustained‐release formulations 12   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.90 [‐1.05, ‐0.74]
2 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated 6   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.51 [‐0.75, ‐0.27]
2.1 Immediate‐release formulations 3   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.67 [‐0.98, ‐0.37]
2.2 Sustained‐release formulations 3   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.33 [‐0.65, ‐0.01]
3 Retention in treatment 17 2323 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.06 [0.99, 1.13]
3.1 Immediate‐release formulations 2 41 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.13 [0.91, 1.40]
3.2 Sustained‐release formulations 15 2282 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.06 [0.98, 1.13]

Comparison 8. Sensitivity analysis: incomplete subjective outcome data.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated 1   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) Totals not selected
2 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated 2   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.77 [‐1.14, ‐0.40]

Comparison 9. Sensitivity analysis: other potential sources of bias.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated 9   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.84 [‐1.02, ‐0.66]
2 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated 3   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.75 [‐1.07, ‐0.43]
3 Retention in treatment 9 1661 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 1.04 [0.96, 1.13]

Comparison 10. Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician‐rated 13   Std. Mean Difference (Fixed, 95% CI) ‐0.89 [‐0.98, ‐0.79]
1.1 Dexamphetamine 1   Std. Mean Difference (Fixed, 95% CI) ‐0.24 [‐0.80, 0.32]
1.2 Lisdexamfetamine 7   Std. Mean Difference (Fixed, 95% CI) ‐1.04 [‐1.19, ‐0.90]
1.3 Mixed amphetamine salts 5   Std. Mean Difference (Fixed, 95% CI) ‐0.80 [‐0.93, ‐0.66]
2 ADHD symptom severity: patient‐rated 6   Std. Mean Difference (Fixed, 95% CI) ‐0.51 [‐0.73, ‐0.29]
2.1 Dexamphetamine 2   Std. Mean Difference (Fixed, 95% CI) ‐0.77 [‐1.13, ‐0.41]
2.2 Lisdexamfetamine 3   Std. Mean Difference (Fixed, 95% CI) ‐0.33 [‐0.65, ‐0.01]
2.3 Mixed amphetamine salts 1   Std. Mean Difference (Fixed, 95% CI) ‐0.45 [‐1.02, 0.12]
3 Clinical impression of severity at study end 2 78 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) ‐1.09 [‐1.57, ‐0.61]
4 Clinical impression of improvement at study end 1 263 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) ‐0.75 [‐1.01, ‐0.48]
5 Proportion of participants achieving a reduction ≥ 30% in severity of ADHD symptoms 2 381 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) 1.52 [1.18, 1.95]
6 Proportion of participants achieving a CGI‐Improvement score of 1 or 2 8 1707 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) 2.52 [2.14, 2.97]
7 Proportion of participants achieving a reduction ≥ 30% in severity of ADHD symptoms and a CGI‐Improvement score of 1 or 2 1 61 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) 2.54 [1.34, 4.82]
8 Global functioning 2 110 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) 0.56 [0.17, 0.95]
9 Depressive symptoms 2 110 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) 0.16 [‐0.22, 0.53]
10 Anxiety symptoms 2 110 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) 0.13 [‐0.24, 0.51]
11 Retention in treatment 17 2323 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) 1.10 [1.04, 1.16]
11.1 Dexamphetamine 1 49 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) 0.79 [0.54, 1.17]
11.2 Lisdexamfetamine 8 873 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) 1.03 [0.96, 1.11]
11.3 Mixed amphetamine salts 8 1401 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) 1.15 [1.07, 1.24]
12 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any cardiovascular adverse event 2 675 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) 2.51 [0.32, 19.54]
13 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event 17 2409 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) 2.99 [1.86, 4.83]
13.1 Dexamphetamine 1 49 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) 1.70 [0.31, 9.27]
13.2 Lisdexamfetamine 9 989 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) 1.77 [0.78, 4.02]
13.3 Mixed amphetamine salts 7 1371 Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) 4.05 [2.14, 7.67]

10.4. Analysis.

10.4

Comparison 10 Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model, Outcome 4 Clinical impression of improvement at study end.

10.5. Analysis.

10.5

Comparison 10 Sensitivity analysis: fixed‐effect model, Outcome 5 Proportion of participants achieving a reduction ≥ 30% in severity of ADHD symptoms.

Comparison 11. Post hoc sensitivity analysis 1: calculation of effect sizes using correlation coefficient from Taylor 2000.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated 13   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.90 [‐1.05, ‐0.76]
2 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated 6   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.47 [‐0.69, ‐0.25]

Comparison 12. Post hoc sensitivity analysis 2: pooled risk difference for proportion of participants withdrawn owing to cardiovascular adverse events and any adverse event.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any cardiovascular adverse event 3 699 Risk Difference (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 0.02 [‐0.00, 0.04]
2 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event 17 2409 Risk Difference (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 0.04 [0.01, 0.06]
2.1 Dexamphetamine 1 49 Risk Difference (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 0.05 [‐0.12, 0.23]
2.2 Lisdexamfetamine 9 989 Risk Difference (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 0.01 [‐0.02, 0.04]
2.3 Mixed amphetamine salts 7 1371 Risk Difference (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) 0.06 [0.02, 0.10]

Comparison 13. Post hoc sensitivity analysis 3: exclusion of cross‐over study.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated 12   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) ‐0.90 [‐1.05, ‐0.74]

Comparison 14. Amphetamines vs guanfacine for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated 1   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) Totals not selected

Comparison 15. Amphetamines vs modafinil for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: patient rated 1   Std. Mean Difference (Random, 95% CI) Totals not selected

Comparison 16. Amphetamines vs paroxetine for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 ADHD symptom severity: clinician rated 1   Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) Totals not selected
2 Proportion of participants achieving a CGI‐Improvement score of 1 or 2 1   Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) Totals not selected
3 Global functioning 1   Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) Totals not selected
4 Depressive symptoms 1   Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) Totals not selected
5 Anxiety symptoms 1   Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) Totals not selected
6 Retention in treatment 1   Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) Totals not selected
7 Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to any adverse event 1   Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) Totals not selected

Characteristics of studies

Characteristics of included studies [ordered by study ID]

Adler 2008.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, randomised clinical trial
Number of study sites: 48
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: modified ITT (participants with ≥ 1 postrandomisation assessment of the ADHD‐RS score were included in the efficacy analysis, which consisted of 414 (98.6%) out of 420 randomised participants). Although no statistically significant differences were found in any baseline data, mean differences were calculated after adjustment for baseline score.
Participants Sample size: 420 patients with adult ADHD, according to DSM‐IV‐TR criteria
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with a comorbid psychiatric diagnosis with significant symptoms that may preclude treatment with lisdexamfetamine
Mean age: 35.1 years
Gender: 228 (54.3%) men
Race: 349 (83.1%) Caucasian
ADHD subtype: NR
Interventions 4 groups:
  1. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 119): 30 mg/d, qd, fixed posology

  2. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 117): 50 mg/d, qd, fixed posology (30 mg/d for week 1, with forced‐dose escalation to 50 mg/d for weeks 2 to 4)

  3. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 122): 70 mg/d, qd, fixed posology (30 mg/d for week 1, with forced‐dose escalation to 50 mg/d for week 2 and 70 mg/d for for weeks 3 to 4)

  4. Placebo (n = 62)


Psychotherapy: NR
Duration: 4 weeks
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with clinician‐rated ADHD‐RS‐IV

  2. Clinical impression, assessed with CGI‐S and CGI‐I scales

  3. Proportion of responders, defined as percentage of participants with ≥ 30% reduction in ADHD‐RS total score at endpoint or CGI‐I score ≤ 2

  4. Retention in treatment

  5. Proportion of participants withdrawn owing to cardiovascular adverse events

Notes Author's affiliation: university and pharmaceutical industry
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: available (NCT00334880)
Study start and end dates: May 2006 to November 2006
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we requested additional data on efficacy and safety outcomes. Study authors directed us to Shire, from whom we requested the data again. Shire responded to our email but did not provide us with the additional data.
Other comments: we obtained data on cardiovascular adverse event‐related dropouts from a secondary publication (Adler 2009).
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Low risk Comment: attrition was low (17%), and last observation carried forward was deemed a suitable method to impute missing data.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: outcomes stated in the study protocol are reported in the article.
Other bias Low risk Comment: study groups were balanced at baseline. The study appears free of other biases.

Adler 2013.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, randomised clinical trial
Number of study sites: 35
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: modified ITT (participants who took ≥ 1 dose of study medication during the double‐blind phase and had ≥ 1 BRIEF‐A assessment, which consisted of 154 (95.7%) out of 161 randomised participants)
Participants Sample size: 161 patients with adult ADHD, according to DSM‐IV‐TR criteria, in a close domicile relationship for the previous 6 months and with a ADHD‐RS‐IV score ≥ 28 and a BRIEF‐A global executive composite T‐score ≥ 65 (2 patients (1 in each group) were included with a BRIEF‐A score lower than 65)
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with comorbid psychiatric diagnosis that was uncontrolled or was controlled with prohibited medication
Mean age: 34.5 years
Gender: 83 (52.2%) men
Race: 16 (9.9%) African American; 136 (84.5%) Caucasian; 7 (4.3%) Other
ADHD subtype: 129 (80.1%) combined; 29 (18%) inattentive; 1 (0.6%) hyperactive‐impulsive
Interventions 2 groups:
  1. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 80): 4‐week dose titration (beginning with 30 mg and titrated in 20‐mg/week increments to an optimal dose of up to 70 mg/d) and 6‐week maintenance period (up to 70 mg/d). Mean maintenance dose = 56.9 mg/d

  2. placebo (n = 81)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 10 weeks
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with clinician‐rated ADHR‐RS‐IV

  2. Clinical impression, assessed with CGI‐I scale

  3. Proportion of responders, defined as percentage of participants with CGI‐I score ≤ 2 at endpoint

  4. Retention in treatment

Notes Author's affiliation: university and pharmaceutical industry
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: available (NCT01101022)
Study start and end dates: May 2010 to November 2010
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we did not request additional data from the study authors.
Other comments: none
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: the sequence was generated using interactive, voice/web response system.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Low risk Comment: treatment allocation was assigned using interactive voice/web response system.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes High risk Comment: attrition was high (45%), and in this scenario, it is unclear whether any method used to impute missing data can provide unbiased results.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: study protocol was available, and outcomes stated in the protocol are reported in the article.
Other bias Low risk Comment: study groups were balanced at baseline. The study appears free of other biases.

Biederman 2012.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, randomised clinical trial
Number of study sites: 1
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: NR
Participants Sample size: 69 patients with adult ADHD, according to DSM‐IV criteria
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with clinically significant, comorbid psychiatric conditions or using psychotropic medication during the previous month
Mean age: 21.6 years
Gender: 38 (62%) men
Race: NR
ADHD subtype: NR
Interventions 2 groups:
  1. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 31): 2‐week dose titration (beginning with 30 mg during the first week, followed by 50 mg/d during the second week and up to 70 mg/d during the third week) and 4‐week maintenance period (up to 70 mg/d)

  2. placebo (n = 30)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 6 weeks
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with clinician‐rated ADHD‐RS‐IV

  2. Proportion of responders, defined as percentage of participants with ≥ 30% reduction in ADHD‐RS total score and a CGI‐I score ≤ 2 at endpoint

  3. Depressive symptoms, assessed with HAM‐D

  4. Anxiety symptoms, assessed with HAM‐A

  5. Retention in treatment

Notes Author's affiliation: university
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: available (NCT00801229)
Study start and end dates: December 2008 to July 2010
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we did not request additional data from the study authors.
Other comments: none
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: attrition was low (12%), and the imputation methods, if any, were not reported.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: study protocol was available. Only the primary outcome was reported. Secondary outcomes reported in the article are those that one would expect from this type of study.
Other bias Low risk Comment: study groups were balanced at baseline. The study appears free of other biases.

Brams 2012.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, randomised withdrawal study
Number of study sites: 36
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: all randomised participants who received ≥ 1 study dose and had ≥ 1 ADHD‐RS‐IV and CGI‐S assessment
Participants Sample size: 116 patients with adult ADHD, according to DSM‐IV criteria, with a baseline ADHD‐RS‐IV total score < 22 and a CGI‐S score ≥ 3, who had received lisdexamfetamine (30, 50, or 70 mg/d) for ≥ 6 months with an acceptable safety profile, and with a body mass index between 18.5 and 40
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with comorbid Axis I or II disorders that were uncontrolled with significant symptoms or were controlled with prohibited medications (psychostimulants and amphetamine‐like agents, centrally or peripherally acting antihistamines, investigational compounds, clonidine and guanfacine, and herbal preparations), as well as patients at risk of suicide or with a history of suicide attempts
Mean age: 35.8 years
Gender: 50 (43.1%) men
Race: 106 (91.4%) Caucasian; 10 (8.6%) Other
ADHD subtype: NR
Interventions All randomised participants entered an open‐label treatment phase with lisdexamfetamine (at participants' stable treatment dose) during 3 weeks. After that, participants were randomised to 2 groups:
  1. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 56): at participants' stable treatment dose. Mean dose = 61.2 mg/d

  2. Placebo (n = 60)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 6 weeks
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with ADHD‐RS‐IV

  2. Proportion of participants with symptom relapse, defined as the percentage of patients with an increase in ADHD‐RS ≥ 50% and ≥ 2‐point increase in CGI‐S at the end of the study

  3. Clinical impression, assessed with CGI‐S

  4. Retention in treatment*

Notes Author's affiliation: university
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: available (NCT00877487)
Study start and end dates: April 2009 to September 2010
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we did not request additional data from the study authors.
Other comments: *patients that met symptom relapse criteria at any point during the study were actively withdrawn. Therefore, the outcome "retention in treatment" is markedly different from the remaining studies and was not used.
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes High risk Comment: attrition was high (54%), and statistically significant differences in retention across study groups were found.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: study protocol was available, and outcomes stated in the protocol are reported in the article.
Other bias Low risk Comment: study groups were balanced at baseline. The study appears free of other biases.

Dupaul 2012.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, cross‐over clinical trial
Number of study sites: 2
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: per protocol
Participants Sample size: 24 university students with ADHD, according to DSM‐IV‐TR, who scored at or above the 90th percentile on current symptom ratings based on self‐reports
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded participants with significant symptoms of major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, or thought disorder with significant illicit substance abuse
Mean age: 20.2 years
Gender: 15 (62.5%) men
Race: 22 (91%) Caucasian
ADHD subtype: 17 (70.8%) combined; 16 ( 25%) inattentive; 1 (4.2 %) hyperactive‐impulsive
Interventions 4 groups:
  1. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 24): 30 mg/d, qd, fixed posology

  2. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 24): 50 mg/d, qd, fixed posology

  3. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 24): 70 mg/d, qd, fixed posology

  4. Placebo (n = 24)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 1 week
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with patient‐rated CAARS–SF

Notes Author's affiliation: university
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: available (NCT01342445)
Study start and end dates: September 2009 to December 2010
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: study authors declared no conflicts of interest, but the study was funded entirely by the pharmaceutical industry.
Missing data: we requested additional data on safety outcomes from the study authors, and they provided us with this information.
Other comments: none
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Low risk Comment: participants, research assistants, and professors had no access to the randomisation sequence.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: 2 out of 24 participants discontinued treatment and were not included in the statistical analysis.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: study protocol was available. Adverse events were poorly reported in the clinical trials register and in the article.
Other bias High risk Comment: no washout phase was included. The possibility of a carry‐over effect was not studied. Patients were paid to participate.

Frick 2017.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, randomised clinical trial
Number of study sites: 48
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: ITT (all randomised participants taking ≥ 1 study dose and having ≥ 1 postbaseline primary efficacy assessment and a baseline assessment)
Participants Sample size: 411 patients with adult ADHD, according to DSM‐IV‐TR criteria, with an ADHD‐RS‐IV score ≥ 32
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with Axis I disorders controlled with prohibited medications, or uncontrolled and associated with significant symptoms, or patients with symptoms that could confound clinical assessments at screening
Mean age: 37.1 years
Gender: 233 (56.7%) men
Race: 23 (5.6%) African American; 358 (87.1%) Caucasian; 27 (6.6%) Other
ADHD subtype: 332 (80.8%) combined; 75 (18.2%) inattentive; 4 (1%) hyperactive‐impulsive
Interventions 4 groups:
  1. Triple‐bead MAS (n = 104): 25 mg/d, qd, fixed posology

  2. Triple‐bead MAS (n = 101): 50 mg, qd, fixed posology (titration over 2 weeks: 25 mg/d during week 1, 37.5 mg/d during week 2, 50 mg/d during weeks 3 to 6)

  3. Triple‐bead MAS (n = 102): 75 mg, qd (fixed posology) (titration over 3 weeks: 25 mg/d during week 1, 37.5 mg/d during week 2, 50 mg/d during week 3, 75 mg/d during weeks 4 to 6)

  4. Placebo (n = 104)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 6 weeks
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with clinician‐rated ADHD‐RS‐IV

  2. Clinical impression, assessed with CGI‐I scale

  3. Retention in treatment

Notes Author's affiliation: university and pharmaceutical industry
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: available (NCT00152022)
Study start and end dates: April 2005 to April 2006
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we did not request additional data from the study authors.
Other comments: none
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes High risk Comment: attrition was moderate (29%), and statistically significant differences in retention across study groups were found.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: study protocol was available, and outcomes stated in the protocol were reported in the article.
Other bias Unclear risk Comment: study groups were balanced at baseline. There was a long period of time (approximately 10 years) between presentation of preliminary results of this study (in 2007 at the 160th annual meeting of the American Psychiatric Association in San Diego) and publication of the article with the main results. In addition, secondary results were published before the primary ones.

Kay 2009.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, cross‐over, randomised clinical trial
Number of study sites: 1
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: ITT (participants who were randomised to treatment, received ≥ 1 dose of study drug, and completed ≥ 1 test session)
Participants Sample size: 19 patients with ADHD, according to DSM‐IV‐TR criteria, with an ADHD‐RS score ≥ 24, a score ≤ 50th percentile on either the Stroop Color and Word Test or the Halstead–Reitan Category Test, and with valid driver’s license and ≥ 3 years of driving experience
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with a current comorbid psychiatric diagnosis (controlled or uncontrolled) or substance abuse or dependence for the previous 6 months, and patients who were naive to ADHD medications
Mean age: 22.3 years
Gender: 17 (89.5%) men
Race: 2 (10.5%) African American; 15 (78.9%) Caucasian; 2 (10.5%) Other
ADHD subtype: NR
Interventions 2 groups:
  1. MAS XR (n =19): 20 mg/d during the first week, 40 mg/d during the second week, and 50 mg/d during the third week, qd, fixed posology

  2. Placebo (n =19)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 3 weeks
Outcomes
  1. Proportion of responders, defined as percentage of participants with ≥ 30% reduction in ADHD‐RS total score at endpoint

  2. Clinical impression, assessed with CGI‐I scale

  3. Retention in treatment

Notes Author's affiliation: university and pharmaceutical industry
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: available (NCT00557960)
Study start and end dates: February 2004 to October 2004
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: NR
Missing data: we requested additional data on efficacy outcomes but have not obtained them as yet.
Other comments: none
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: a 4‐digit randomisation numbers table was used.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: attrition was moderate (21%), and the imputation methods, if any, were not reported.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: study protocol was available, and outcomes stated in the protocol were reported in the article.
Other bias High risk Comment: no washout phase was included. The possibility of a carry‐over effect was not studied. Medication‐naive patients with ADHD were excluded.

Kollins 2014.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, randomised clinical trial
Number of study sites: 1
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: per protocol
Participants Sample size: 32 patients with adult ADHD, according to DSM‐IV‐TR criteria, and nicotine dependence, smoking at least 10 cigarettes per day and with an expired air CO level ≥ 10 ppm
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with any other psychiatric condition or using illicit drugs (confirmed by urine drug screen)
Mean age: 31.4 years
Gender: 20 (62.5%) men
Race: 24 (78.1%) Caucasian; 7 (21.9%) Other
ADHD subtype: 19 (52.4%) combined; 12 (37.5%) inattentive; 1 (3.1%) hyperactive‐impulsive
Interventions 2 groups:
  1. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 17): 2‐week dose titration (beginning with 30 mg and titrated in 20 mg/week increments to an optimal dose of up to 70 mg/d) and 2‐week maintenance period (up to 70 mg/d). Mean maintenance dose = 56.9 mg/d

  2. Placebo (n = 15)


All participants received a transdermal nicotine patch from 2 weeks before randomisation until the end of the study (21 mg nicotine/24 h for the 2 weeks previous to randomisation and the first week after randomisation, 14 mg nicotine/24 h for the second week after randomisation, and 7 mg nicotine/24 h for the last 2 weeks of the study).
Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 4 weeks
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with clinician‐ and patient‐rated CAARS

  2. Proportion of responders, defined as percentage of participants with CGI‐I score ≤ 2 at endpoint

  3. Retention in treatment

Notes Author's affiliation: university
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: available (NCT00736255)
Study start and end dates: December 2007 to July 2011
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we requested additional data on efficacy outcomes but have not obtained them as yet.
Other comments: the study was suspended for several months when it was reported by the sponsor that some of the medication that had been provided had reached an expiration date. This affected 2 participants who were discontinued before randomisation and 1 participant who was already started on medication and subsequently was discontinued from the study.
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: attrition was low (13%), but the analysis was per protocol.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: study protocol was available, and outcomes stated in the protocol were reported in the article.
Other bias Low risk Comment: study groups were balanced at baseline. The study appears free of other biases.

Levin 2015.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, randomised clinical trial
Number of study sites: 2
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical method: ITT
Participants Sample size: 126 patients with adult ADHD, according to DSM‐IV‐TR criteria, and cocaine dependence
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with past mania, schizophrenia, or any psychotic disorder other than transient psychosis due to drug abuse, patients with an unstable psychiatric condition, or patients currently undergoing treatment
Mean age: 36.4 years
Gender: 106 (84.1%) men
Race: 22 (17.5%) African American; 72 (57.1%) Caucasian; 28 (22.2%) Other
ADHD subtype: NR
Interventions 3 groups:
  1. MAS XR (n = 40): 60 mg/d, qd, fixed posology. Mean tolerated dose = 53.3 mg/d

  2. MAS XR (n = 43): 80 mg/d, qd, fixed posology. Mean tolerated dose = 70.8 mg/d

  3. Placebo (n = 43)


Psychotherapy: all participants also received CBT and relapse prevention.
Duration: 14 weeks (1‐week placebo lead‐in phase followed by 13‐week trial)
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with adult AISRS

  2. Clinical impression of severity and improvement, assessed with CGI‐S and CGI‐I scales, respectively

  3. Proportion of responders, defined as percentage of participants with ≥ 30% reduction in AISRS total score at endpoint or CGI‐I score ≤ 2

  4. Retention in treatment

Notes Author's affiliation: university
Study funding: public
Study protocol: available (NCT00553319)
Study start and end dates: December 2007 to July 2013
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we did not request additional data from the study authors.
Other comments: none
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: the random sequence was computer‐generated.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: attrition was moderate (26%), and last observation carried forward was deemed a suitable method to impute missing data.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: study protocol was available, and outcomes stated in the protocol were reported in the article.
Other bias Low risk Comment: study groups were balanced at baseline. The study appears free of other biases.

Martin 2014a.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, 3‐period, cross‐over, randomised clinical trial
Number of study sites: 1
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: seem to be per protocol. Study completers (17 out of 18) were included in the efficacy analysis.
Participants Sample size: 18 adults aged 18 to 55 years with ADHD, according to DSM‐IV criteria, and with a history of successful treatment with an amphetamine‐based agent
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with a diagnosis of severe, comorbid Axis I or Axis II disorder
Mean age: 30.8 years
Gender: 11 (61.1%) men
Race: 15 (83.3%) Caucasian
ADHD subtype: NR
Interventions 3 groups:
  1. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 18): 50 mg/d, fixed posology

  2. MAS‐IR (n = 18): 20 mg/d, fixed posology

  3. Placebo (n = 18)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 1 week with each study intervention. No washout period was scheduled.
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with the patient‐rated CAARS: Short Version

  2. Retention in treatment

Notes Author's affiliation: university and pharmaceutical industry
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: available (NCT01010750)
Study start and end dates: January 2010 to April 2010
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we did not request additional data from the study authors.
Other comments: none
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: study had a cross‐over design, and it seems that a paired data analysis was conducted under a per‐protocol principle. Nevertheless, attrition was low (1 participant).
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: study protocol was available, and outcomes stated in the protocol were reported in the article.
Other bias High risk Comment: no washout phase was included. The possibility of a carry‐over effect was not studied. All participants had a history of responsiveness to amphetamines.

Martin 2014b.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, 3‐period, cross‐over, randomised clinical trial
Number of study sites: 1
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: seem to be per protocol. Study completers (17 out of 18) were included in the efficacy analysis.
Participants Sample size: 18 adults aged 18 to 55 years with ADHD, according to DSM‐IV criteria, and with a history of successful treatment with an amphetamine‐based agent
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with a diagnosis of severe, comorbid Axis I or II disorder
Mean age: 30.8 years
Gender: 11 (61.1%) men
Race: 15 (83.3%) Caucasian
ADHD subtype: NR
Interventions 3 groups:
  1. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 18): 50 mg/d, fixed posology

  2. MAS‐IR (n = 18): 20 mg/d, fixed posology

  3. Placebo (n = 18)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 1 week with each study intervention. No washout period was scheduled.
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with the patient‐rated CAARS: Short Version

  2. Retention in treatment

Notes Author's affiliation: university and pharmaceutical industry
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: available (NCT01010750)
 Study start and end dates: January 2010 to April 2010
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we did not request additional data from the study authors.
Other comments: none
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: study had a cross‐over design, and it seems that a paired data analysis was conducted under a per‐protocol principle. Nevertheless, attrition was low (1 participant).
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: study protocol was available, and outcomes stated in the protocol were reported in the article.
Other bias High risk Comment: no washout phase was included. The possibility of a carry‐over effect was not studied. All participants had a history of responsiveness to amphetamines.

Spencer 2001.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, randomised, cross‐over trial
Number of study sites: 1
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: per‐protocol analysis
Participants Sample size: 30 patients with adult ADHD, according to DSM‐IV criteria. The per‐protocol sample consisted of 27 (90%) patients.
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with an IQ < 80, delirium, dementia, amnesic disorders, any other clinically unstable psychiatric conditions (i.e. bipolar disorder, psychosis), drug or alcohol abuse or dependence within the 6 months preceding the study, or currently using psychotropics
Mean age: 38 years
Gender: 15 (55.6%) men
Race: 26 (96%) Caucasian
ADHD subtype: 12 (44%) combined; 15 (56%) inattentive; 0 hyperactive‐impulsive
Interventions 2 groups:
  1. MAS‐IR (n = 30): 3‐week, stepwise dose titration (20 mg/d (10 mg twice daily) by week 1, 40 mg/d (20 mg twice daily) by week 2, 60 mg/d (30 mg twice daily) by week 3, unless adverse effects emerged). Mean dose across the study = 37.4 mg/d, mean dose at study completion = 53.7 mg/d

  2. Placebo (n = 30)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 3 weeks (2 × 3‐week‐long periods separated by 1 week of washout)
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with clinician‐rated ADHD‐RS‐IV

  2. Clinical impression of severity and improvement, assessed with CGI‐S and CGI‐I, respectively

  3. Proportion of responders, defined as percentage of participants with ≥ 30% reduction in ADHD‐RS total score at endpoint or CGI‐I score ≤ 2

  4. Depressive symptom severity, assessed with HAM‐D and BDI

  5. Anxiety symptom severity, assessed with HAM‐A

  6. Retention in treatment

Notes Author's affiliation: university
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry and public funds
Study protocol: not available
Study start and end dates: NR
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: NR
Missing data: we requested additional data on efficacy outcomes from the study authors. Study authors directed us to Shire, from whom we requested the data again. Shire responded to our email but did not provide us with the additional data.
Other comments: a carry‐over effect was observed, which can lead to biased results. To avoid the influence of this bias, it is recommended to use data from the first period of the study, as if it was a parallel‐group clinical trial. However, data from the first period were available only for retention in treatment. We requested the remaining outcome data but were not able to obtain them. The carry‐over effect can underestimate the effect of the intervention and can bias the result towards the null for both effectiveness and adverse events outcomes. To determine whether this study could bias the results of our meta‐analysis, we conducted a sensitivity analysis in which we repeated the analysis with this study excluded.
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: study had a cross‐over design, and it seems that a paired data analysis was conducted under a per‐protocol principle. Nevertheless, attrition was low (10%).
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: study protocol was not available, and the possibility of reporting bias could not be assessed.
Other bias High risk Comment: a carry‐over effect was observed, and data from the first study period were not available.

Spencer 2008.

Methods Design: double‐blind, randomised, placebo‐controlled, parallel‐group clinical trial
Number of study sites: 39
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical analysis: modified ITT; 268 (97.8%) participants (out of 274 who were randomised) were included in the efficacy analysis. 6 participants were excluded from the efficacy analysis because they discontinued the study before the first assessment. Although no statistically significant differences were found in any baseline data, mean differences were calculated after adjustment for baseline score. Unadjusted mean differences were also reported, which did not differ from the adjusted ones.
Participants Sample size: 274 patients with adult ADHD, according to DSM‐IV‐TR criteria, with a baseline ADHD‐RS score ≥ 24
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with any psychiatric disorder with, in the opinion of the investigator, significant symptoms and substance use disorder (except nicotine dependence) within the 6 months preceding the screening
Mean age: 36.5 years
Gender: 136 (50%) men
Race: 21 (7.7%) African American; 231 (84.9%) Caucasian; 20 (7.4%) Other
ADHD subtype: 192 (70.6%) combined; 72 (26.5%) inattentive; 8 (2.9%) hyperactive‐impulsive
Interventions 2 parallel groups:
  1. Triple‐bead MAS (n = 137): 5‐week, stepwise dose titration (beginning with 12.5 mg/d and maximum dose 75 mg/d) followed by 2‐week maintenance dose, administered qd). Mean maintenance dose = 47.9 mg/d

  2. Placebo (n = 137)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 7 weeks
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed by clinician‐rated ADHD‐RS

  2. Clinical impression of severity and improvement, assessed with CGI‐S and CGI‐I, respectively

  3. Retention in treatment

Notes Author's affiliation: university and pharmaceutical industry
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: NCT00150579
Study start and end dates: January 2005 to December 2005
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we requested additional data on safety outcomes from the study authors. Study authors directed us to Shire, from whom we requested the data again. Shire responded to our email but did not provide us with the additional data.
Other comments: unpublished data sought but not obtained
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes High risk Comment: attrition was moderate (38%), and statistically significant differences in retention across study groups were found.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: study protocol was available, and outcomes stated in the protocol are reported in the article.
Other bias Low risk Comment: study groups were balanced at baseline. The study appears free of other biases.

Taylor 2000.

Methods Design: double‐blind, randomised, placebo‐controlled, cross‐over clinical trial
Number of study sites: 1
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical analysis: ITT (all randomised participants were included in the statistical analysis)
Participants Sample size: 22 patients with adult ADHD, according to DSM‐IV criteria. To be eligible, patients had to score above the 93th percentile on both adult and childhood versions of the DSM‐IV‐based ADHD Behaviour Checklist.
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with schizophrenia and Tourette disorder, as well as cannabis, cocaine, non‐prescription amphetamine, or heroin users during the past 6 months
Mean age: 40.8 years
Gender: 13 (59%) men
Race: NR
ADHD subtype: 9 (41%) combined; 11 (50%) inattentive; 2 (9%) hyperactive‐impulsive
Interventions 3 groups:
  1. Dextroamphetamine (n = 22): 5 to 20 mg bid. Dose was titrated up using 5‐mg increments every 1 or 2 days, as tolerated, to a maximum of 20 mg bid. Titration was achieved within 4 to 7 days and was maintained for an additional 4 to 10 days. Average dose was 21.8 mg/d.

  2. Modafinil (n = 22): 50 to 200 mg bid. Dose was titrated up using 50‐mg increments every 1 or 2 days, as tolerated, to a maximum of 200 mg bid. Titration was achieved within 4 to 7 days and was maintained for an additional 4 to 10 days.

  3. Placebo (n = 22)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 50 days (3 × 14‐day treatment periods separated by 2 × 4‐day washout periods)
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with "self‐rated DSM‐IV ADHD behaviour checklist for adults"

  2. Depressive symptom severity, assessed with 21‐item BDI

  3. Anxiety symptom severity, assessed with 14‐item HAM‐A

  4. Retention in treatment

Notes Author affiliation: university and health care system
Study funding: NR
Study protocol: not available
Study start and end dates: NR
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: NR
Missing data: we requested additional data on efficacy outcomes from the study authors, and they provided us with this information.
Other comments: none
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Low risk Comment: attrition was low (5%), and last observation carried forward was used to impute missing data.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: study protocol was not available, and the possibility of reporting bias could not be assessed.
Other bias Low risk Comment: study groups were balanced at baseline. The study appears free of other biases.

Taylor 2001.

Methods Design: double‐blind, randomised, placebo‐controlled, cross‐over clinical trial
Number of sites: 1
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical analysis: ITT (all randomised participants were included in the statistical analysis)
Participants Sample size: 17 patients with adult ADHD, according to DSM‐IV criteria. To be eligible, patients had to score above the 93th percentile on both adult and childhood versions of the DSM‐IV‐based ADHD Behaviour Checklist.
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with organic brain disorders, schizophrenia, and Tourette disorder, as well as cannabis, cocaine, amphetamine, or heroin users during the past 6 months
Mean age: 41.2 years
Gender: 7 (41%%) men
Race: NR
ADHD subtype: NR
Interventions 3 groups:
  1. Dextroamphetamine (n = 17): 2.5 to 20 mg qd. Dose was titrated up using 2.5‐mg increments every 2 days, as tolerated, to a maximum of 20 mg/d. Titration was achieved within 4 to 7 days and was maintained for an additional 4 to 10 days. Average dose was 10.2 mg/d. A short‐acting pharmaceutical presentation was used.

  2. Guanfacine (n = 17): 0.25 to 2.0 mg qd. Dose was titrated up using 0.25‐mg increments every 2 days, as tolerated, to a maximum of 2.0 mg/d. Titration was achieved within 4 to 7 days and was maintained for an additional 4 to 10 days.

  3. Placebo (n = 17)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 50 days (3 × 14‐day treatment periods separated by 2 × 4‐day washout periods)
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with "self‐rated DSM‐IV ADHD behaviour checklist for adults"

  2. Depressive symptom severity, assessed with 21‐item BDI

  3. Anxiety symptom severity, assessed with 14‐item HAM‐A

  4. Retention in treatment

Notes Author affiliation: university and health care system
Study funding: NR
Study protocol: not available
Study start and end dates: NR
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: NR
Missing data: we requested additional data on efficacy outcomes from the study authors, and they provided us with this information.
Other comments: none
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Low risk Comment: no participant dropped out.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: study protocol was not available, and the possibility of reporting bias could not be assessed.
Other bias Low risk Comment: study groups were balanced at baseline. The study appears free of other biases.

Waxmonsky 2014.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, randomised clinical trial with an open‐label dose‐optimisation phase
Number of study sites: 1
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: unclear
Participants Sample size: 27 adults with ADHD, according to DSM‐IV criteria, with an ADHD‐RS score ≥ 28 and a CGI‐S score ≥ 4, who had a child between 5 and 12 years old with a diagnosis of ADHD, according to DSM‐IV criteria, and with a minimum score of 5 on the Sheehan Disability Scale
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with comorbid psychiatric conditions that could worsen with stimulants and those who required psychotropic medications
Mean age: 41.04 years
Gender: 7 (25.9 %) men
Race: NR
ADHD subtype: NR
Interventions All participants entered a 3‐week, open‐label, dose‐optimisation phase, during which they received lisdexamfetamine that was initiated at 30 mg/d and could be increased to 50 mg/d during the second week, and to 70 mg/d during the third week, depending on efficacy and tolerability. Optimal dose was defined as a physically tolerable dose that produced a ≥ 30% reduction in the ADHD‐RS‐IV score and a CGI‐I rating of 1 or 2. After that, participants entered a 2‐week, within‐subject evaluation of 2 parent‐child interaction tasks ‐ 1 while taking lisdexamfetamine and 1 while taking placebo. Between tasks, participants were allowed to receive their optimal lisdexamfetamine dose. After this phase, participants were randomised to:
  1. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 13): at participant's optimal dose. Mean maintenance dose = 54.5 mg/d

  2. Placebo (n = 14)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 4 weeks
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptoms, assessed by clinician‐rated ADHD‐RS‐IV

  2. Proportion of responders, defined as percentage of participants with CGI‐I score ≤ 2 at endpoint

  3. Retention to treatment

Notes Author's affiliation: university and pharmaceutical industry
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: available (NCT01127607)
Study start and end dates: November 2010 to July 2012
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we requested additional data on efficacy outcomes but have not yet obtained this information.
Other comments: data on ADHD symptom severity were obtained from clinical trials
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: attrition was low (11%), and the imputation methods, if any, were not reported.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: study protocol was available, and outcomes stated in the protocol were reported in the article.
Other bias High risk Comment: before the double‐blind phase came a run‐in phase with lisdexamfetamine (up to 70 mg/d), in which 8 participants discontinued ‐ 5 because of adverse events. Thus, it seems that there might have been a pre‐selection of tolerant patients, which could have biased safety results. In addition, we found inconsistent data on discontinuation between results reported in clinical trials and those described in the article. This inconsistency could not be clarified by contacting the study authors.

Weisler 2006.

Methods Design: multi‐site, double‐blind, randomised, placebo‐controlled, parallel‐group clinical trial
Number of study sites: 18
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical analysis: modified ITT; 248 (97.3%) participants (out of 255 who were randomised) were included in the efficacy analysis. The reasons for excluding 7 participants from the ITT analysis were not reported.
Participants Sample size: 255 patients with adult ADHD (combined type), according to DSM‐IV‐TR criteria
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with an IQ < 80 and those with comorbid psychosis, bipolar illness, pervasive developmental disorder, or severe obsessive‐compulsive disorder, as well as severe depressive (17‐item HAM‐D score > 19) and anxiety disorders (14‐item HAM‐A score > 17). Also excluded participants who tested positive on drug screening, with a history of substance abuse, or living with someone with a substance abuse disorder
Mean age: 39.2 years
Gender: 149 (60.1%) men
Race: 8 (3.2%) African American; 221 (89.1%) Caucasian; 19 (7.7%) Other
ADHD subtype: 255 (100%) combined
Interventions 4 parallel groups:
  1. MAS XR (n = 66): 20 mg/d, qd, fixed posology

  2. MAS XR (n = 64): 40 mg/d, qd, fixed posology

  3. MAS XR (n = 61): 60 mg/d, qd, fixed posology

  4. Placebo (n = 64)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 4 weeks
Outcomes
  1. ADHD severity, assessed with clinician‐ and patient‐rated ADHD‐RS‐IV

  2. Clinical impression of severity and improvement, assessed with CGI‐S and CGI‐I scales, respectively

  3. Retention in treatment

Notes Author's affiliation: university and pharmaceutical industry
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: not available
Study start and end dates: February 2002 to May 2002
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we requested additional data on efficacy outcomes from the study authors. Study authors directed us to Shire, from whom we requested the data again. Shire responded to our email but did not provide us with the additional data.
Other comments: mean (SD) ADHD symptom severity at study completion was reported for the active treatment group but not for the placebo group. Nevertheless, the effect size was available, and we used these data to calculate ADHD symptom severity at study completion for the placebo group.
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: attrition was moderate (28%), and last observation carried forward was used to address missing data.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: study protocol was not available, and the possibility of reporting bias could not be assessed.
Other bias Low risk Comment: study groups were balanced at baseline. The study appears free of other biases.

Weisler 2017.

Methods Design: double‐blind, multi‐centre, placebo‐controlled, randomised clinical trial
Number of study sites: 43
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: all screened participants assigned a randomisation number, who took 1 or more study drug doses, and who had 1 or more postbaseline, on‐treatment, primary efficacy assessments were included in the efficacy analysis.
Participants Sample size: 275 patients with adult ADHD, according to DSM‐V criteria, with a baseline ADHD‐RS‐AP total score ≥ 28
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with a comorbid psychiatric diagnosis that was controlled with prohibited medications (psychostimulants and amphetamine‐like agents, centrally or peripherally acting antihistamines, investigational compounds, clonidine and guanfacine, and herbal preparations), or was uncontrolled and was associated with significant symptoms that contraindicated MAS treatment or could confound study assessments. Also excluded patients with suicide risk and those who had previously attempted suicide
Mean age: 33.6 years
Gender: 156 ( 57.6%) men
Race: 23 (8.5%) African American; 221 (57.6%) Caucasian; 27 (10%) Other
ADHD subtype: 219 (80.8%) combined; 50 (18.5%) inattentive; 2 (0.73%) hyperactive‐impulsive
Interventions 3 groups:
  1. Triple‐bead MAS (n = 92): 12.5 mg/d, qd, fixed posology

  2. Triple‐bead MAS (n = 92): 37.5 mg/d, qd, fixed posology (titration over 2 weeks: 12.5 mg/d during week 1, 25 mg/d during week 2, 37.5 mg/d from weeks 3 and 4)

  3. Placebo (n = 91)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 4 weeks
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with clinician‐rated ADHD‐RS‐AP

  2. Clinical impression of improvement, assessed with CGI‐I scale

Notes Authors; affiliations: university and pharmaceutical industry
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: available (NCT02604407)
Study start and end dates: November 2015 to March 2016
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: none
Other comments: none
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: treatment assignments were made by an interactive, web response system.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Low risk Comment: investigators, investigators’ staff, and participants were blinded to the treatment assignment.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: attrition was low (14%), and imputation methods, if any, were not reported.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: study protocol was available, and outcomes stated in the protocol are reported in the article.
Other bias Low risk Comment: study groups were balanced at baseline. The study appears free of other biases.

Weiss 2006.

Methods Design: double‐blind, randomised, placebo‐controlled, parallel‐group, clinical trial
Number of study sites: 5
Country: USA and Canada
Seting: outpatients
Statistical analysis: ITT (all randomised participants were included in the statistical analysis)
Participants Sample size: 98 patients with adult ADHD, according to DSM‐IV criteria
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded patients with comorbid psychosis, eating disorders, substance abuse disorders, organic brain syndrome, and active suicide risk. Permitted other comorbid disorders if they did not require treatment with psychotropic medications other than those provided for in the protocol
Mean age: 37.5 years
Gender: 63 (64%) men
Race: 83 (85%) Caucasian; 15 (15%) Other
ADHD subtype: 59 (60%) combined; 35 (36%) inattentive; 4 (4%) hyperactive‐impulsive
Interventions 4 parallel groups:
  1. Dexamphetamine (n = 23): 4‐week dose titration (beginning with 5 mg bid and maximum dose 20 mg bid) followed by 16‐week maintenance dose

  2. Paroxetine (n = 24): 4‐week dose titration (beginning with 20 mg qd and maximum dose 40 mg qd) followed by 16‐week maintenance dose

  3. Paroxetine + dexamphetamine (n = 25*)

  4. Placebo (n = 26)


Psychotherapy: 9 sessions of problem‐focused psychotherapy
Duration: 20 weeks
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with clinician‐rated ADHD‐RS‐IV

  2. Clinical impression, assessed with CGI‐I

  3. Anxiety symptom severity, assessed with HAM‐A

  4. Depressive symptom severity, assessed with HAM‐D

  5. Global activity functioning

Notes Author's affiliation: university
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: not available
Study start and end dates: NR
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we requested additional data on efficacy outcomes from the study authors, and they provided us with this information.
Other comments: *outcome data from this group were not included in this systematic review.
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: attrition was moderate (35%), and in this scenario, it is unclear whether any method used to impute missing data can provide unbiased results.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: study protocol was not available, and the possibility of reporting bias could not be assessed.
Other bias Low risk Comment: study groups were balanced at baseline. The study appears free of other biases.

Wigal 2010.

Methods Design: double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, cross‐over, randomised clinical trial with an open‐label dose‐optimisation phase
Number of study sites: 5
Country: USA
Setting: outpatients
Statistical methods: per protocol
Participants Sample size: 142 adults with a diagnosis of ADHD, according to DSM‐IV‐TR criteria
Psychiatric comorbid disorders: excluded participants with a comorbid psychiatric diagnosis with significant symptoms, with comorbid substance abuse, or at risk of suicide, or who showed a lack of response to previous amphetamine therapy
Mean age: 30.5 years
Gender: 88 (62%) men
Race: 6 (4.2%) African American; 127 (89.4%) Caucasian; 9 (6.3%) Other
ADHD subtype: 98 (69%) combined; 39 (27.5%) inattentive; 5 (3.5%) hyperactive‐impulsive
Interventions All participants entered a 4‐week, open‐label dose‐optimisation phase, during which they received lisdexamfetamine, which was initiated at 30 mg/d and was upwardly titrated to 70 mg/d depending on efficacy and tolerability. Only participants who tolerated lisdexamfetamine and showed marked improvement in ADHD symptoms (response with ≥ 30% reduction in ADHD‐RS‐IV score and a CGI‐I rating of 1 or 2, very much or much improved) were randomised to 2 groups:
  1. Lisdexamfetamine (n = 63): at the optimised dose during the open‐label phase. Mean maintenance dose: 52.3 mg/d

  2. Placebo (n = 64)


Psychotherapy: not administered
Duration: 1 week
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptom severity, assessed with clinician‐rated ADHD‐RS‐IV

  2. Clinical impression, assessed with CGI‐I scale

  3. Retention to treatment

Notes Author's affiliation: university, healthcare and pharmaceutical industry
Study funding: pharmaceutical industry
Study protocol: available (NCT00697515)
Study start and end dates: July 2008 to December 2008
Declared/potential conflicts of interest: yes
Missing data: we requested additional data on efficacy outcomes but have not yet obtained this information.
Other comments: none
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the sequence generation method is not described.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: the method of allocation concealment is not described.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 Retention to treatment Unclear risk Comment: it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when study medications with powerful behavioural effects (amphetamines) are compared to placebo.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 Subjective outcomes Unclear risk Comment: attrition was low (19%), and the statistical analysis was per protocol.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: study protocol was available, and outcomes stated in the protocol were reported in the article.
Other bias High risk Comment: no washout phase was included. The possibility of a carry‐over effect was not studied. Patients with a history of non‐response to amphetamines were excluded. All participants took lisdexamfetamine (up to 70 mg/d) during a run‐in phase before they were randomised to the study interventions.

ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; ADHD:RS‐AP: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scale With Adult Prompts; ADHD‐RS: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scale; ADHD‐RS‐IV: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scale, Fourth Version; AE: adverse events; AISRS: Adult Attention Deficity Hyperactivity Disorder Investigator Rating Scale; BDI: Beck Depression Inventory; BRIEF: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; BRIEF‐A: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function ‐ Adult Version; CAARS: Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scale; CAARS‐SF: Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scale ‐ Short Form; CBT: cognitive‐behavioural therapy; CGI‐I: Clinical Global Impressions ‐ Improvement scale; CGI‐S: Clinical Global Impressions ‐ Severity scale; CO: carbon monoxide; DSM:Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders;DSM‐IV:Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders ‐ Fourth Edition;DSM‐IV‐TR:Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders ‐ Fourth Edition ‐ Text Revision;DSM‐5:Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition;HAM‐A: Hamilton Anxiety Rating scale; HAM‐D: Hamilton Depression Rating scale;IQ: intelligence quotient; ITT: intention‐to‐treat; MAS: mixed amphetamine salts; MAS‐IR: mixed amphetamine salts ‐ immediate release; MAS‐ER: mixed amphetamine salts ‐ extended release; NR: not reported; QD: once a day; SD: standard deviation.

Characteristics of excluded studies [ordered by study ID]

Study Reason for exclusion
Adler 2011 Not controlled with placebo or active control. RCT comparing MAS‐IR and MAS‐XR
Adler 2014 Not an RCT
Arnold 1972 Not an RCT
Castaneda 2000 Not an RCT
Dodson 2005 Not an RCT
Faraone 2002 Study participants were children with ADHD.
Goodman 2005 Not an RCT
Lasser 2010 Not an RCT
Mattingly 2012 Not an RCT
Paterson 1999 Used subthreshold, DSM‐IV criteria for ADHD
Rostain 2009 Not an RCT
Spencer 2004 Not an RCT
Waxmonsky 2011 Not an RCT
Weisler 2014 Not an RCT
Wiebe 2010 Study participants were children with ADHD.
Wilens 2005 Not an RCT
Young 2015 RCT assessing cognitive‐behavioural therapy, not amphetamines

ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; DSM‐IV:Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition;MAS‐IR: mixed amphetamine salts ‐ immediate release; MAS‐ER: mixed amphetamine salts ‐ extended release; RCT: randomised controlled trial.

Characteristics of ongoing studies [ordered by study ID]

NCT00202605.

Trial name or title A phase II, randomised, double‐blind, multi‐centre, placebo‐controlled, cross‐over study of SPD465 in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
Methods Phase II, randomised, double‐blind, multi‐centre, placebo‐controlled, cross‐over study
Participants Adults with ADHD, using DSM‐IV‐TR criteria, with a baseline ADHD‐RS‐IV score ≥ 24
Interventions
  1. SPD465 (triple‐bead MAS)

  2. Placebo

Outcomes
  1. Time Segment Rating System

  2. Participant self‐report of ADHD

  3. Treatment‐emergent adverse events

  4. Sleep quality

Starting date Status: completed
Study start date: September 2005
Study end date: April 2006
Last updated: November 2007
Contact information Principal investigator: not specified
Contact name(s): not provided
Telephone number(s): not provided
Address: not provided
Email(s): not provided
Sponsor/collaborator: Shire Pharmaceuticals
Notes NCT00202605  

NCT00514202.

Trial name or title Pilot study examining effect for dextroamphetamine to treat cocaine dependence plus attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
Methods Random allocation, double‐blind, 12 weeks' duration, 2 parallel groups, placebo‐controlled, phase II
Participants Patients with ADHD who are cocaine dependent
Interventions
  1. Dextroamphetamine SR 60 mg/d

  2. Placebo


+ CBT
Outcomes
  1. Substance use

  2. ADHD symptoms

  3. Treatment retention

  4. Cocaine craving

Starting date Status: completed
Study start date: August 2007
 Study end date: October 2008
Last updated: February 2012
Contact information Principal investigator: David V Herin
Contact name(s): not provided
Telephone number(s): not provided
Address: The University of Texas Health Science Center, Houston, Texas, USA
Email(s): not provided
Sponsor/collaborator: The University of Texas Health Science Center, Houston, Texas, USA
Notes NCT00514202  

NCT00928148.

Trial name or title A phase II, randomised, double‐blind, multi‐centre, placebo‐ and active‐controlled, cross‐over study of SPD465 in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
Methods Random allocation, double‐blind, 1 week's duration, cross‐over assignment, placebo‐controlled, phase II
Participants Adults with ADHD
Interventions 1. SPD465 (triple‐bead MAS)
2. Immediate‐release amphetamine salt
3. Placebo
Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptoms

  2. Quality of sleep

  3. Performance

Starting date Status: completed
Study start date: March 2004
Study end date: October 2004
Last updated: June 2009
Contact information Principal investigator: not provided but the responsible party is Timothy Whitaker, MD, Shire Pharmaceuticals
Contact name(s): not provided
Telephone number(s): not provided
Address: not provided
Email(s): not provided
Sponsor/collaborator: not provided
Notes NCT00928148

NCT01863459.

Trial name or title Lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in the treatment of adult ADHD with anxiety disorder comorbidity
Methods Random allocation, double‐blind, 18 weeks' duration, cross‐over assignment, placebo‐controlled, phase IV
Participants Adults with ADHD and anxiety disorder
Interventions
  1. Lisdexamfetamine

  2. Placebo

Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptoms

  2. Anxiety symptoms

  3. Depressive symptoms

  4. Quality of life

  5. Quality of sleep

  6. Disability

Starting date Status: completed
Study start date: April 2013
Study end date: March 2017
Last updated: August 2017
Contact information Principal investigator: Stephen Collins
Contact name(s): not provided
 Telephone number(s): not provided
Address: Centre for Anxiety, Attention Deficit and Trauma, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
Email(s): not provided
Sponsor/collaborator: Centre for Anxiety, Attention Deficit and Trauma, Ontario, Canada; Shire
Notes NCT01863459

NCT02635035.

Trial name or title Efficacy of lisdexamfetamine in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and sluggish cognitive tempo
Methods Random allocation, double‐blind, 10 weeks' duration, cross‐over assignment, placebo‐controlled, phase II
Participants Adults with ADHD and sluggish cognitive tempo
Interventions
  1. Lisdexamfetamine

  2. Placebo

Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptoms

  2. Reaction time

  3. Arousal

  4. Motivation

Starting date Status: recruiting
Study start date: November 2015
Estimated study end date: June 2019
Last updated: January 2017
Contact information Principal investigator: Lenard Adler
Contact name(s): Glenn Hirsch, MD; Terry Leon, MD
Telephone number(s): +1 646 754 4837
Address: New York University School of Medicine, New York, USA
Email(s): hirscg01@nyumc.org; guzmat01@nyumc.org
Sponsor/collaborator: New York University School of Medicine; Shire
Notes NCT02635035

NCT02803229.

Trial name or title Treatment of cannabis use disorder among adults with comorbid attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder
Methods Random allocation, double‐blind, 12 weeks' duration, 2 parallel groups, placebo‐controlled, phases II and III
Participants Adults with ADHD and cannabis use disorder
Interventions
  1. Adderall‐XR

  2. Placebo

Outcomes
  1. Cannabis use

  2. ADHD symptoms

  3. Treatment retention

Starting date Status: recruiting
Study start date: July 2016
Estimated study end date: September 2018
Last updated: April 2017
Contact information Principal investigator: Frances R Levin, MD
Contact name(s): Amy Mahony, LMHC; Elizabeth Martinez
Telephone number(s): 646‐774‐8183; 212‐923‐3031
Address: New York Psychiatric Institute, New York, USA
Email(s): Amy.mahony@nyspi.columbia.edu
Sponsor/collaborator: New York State Psychiatric Institute; National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA)
Notes NCT02803229

NCT03153488.

Trial name or title Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) prediction of treatment response
Methods Random allocation, not blinded, 24 weeks' duration, parallel assignment, active control, phase IV
Participants Adults with ADHD
Interventions
  1. MAS XR

  2. Methylphenidate LA

Outcomes
  1. CGI‐I scale

  2. CGI‐S scale

  3. MRI

Starting date Status: not yet recruiting
Anticipated study start date: December 2017
Estimated study end date: December 2019
Last updated: August 2017
Contact information Principal investigator: Joseph Biederman, MD
Contact name(s): Elizabeth Noyes; Alexa P Pulli, BS
Telephone number(s): 617‐724‐2551; 617‐726‐4651
Address: Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
Email(s): enoyes@partners.org; apulli@partners.org
Sponsor/collaborator: Massachusetts General Hospital; Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Notes NCT03153488

ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; ADHD‐RS‐IV: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder ‐ Rating Scale ‐ Fourth Version; CBT: cognitive‐behavioural therapy; CGI‐I: Clinical Global Impression ‐ Improvement;CGI‐S: Clinical Global Impression ‐ Severity; DSM‐IV‐TR:Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders ‐ Fourth Edition ‐ Text Revision; LA: long‐acting; MAS: mixed amphetamine salts; MAS‐XR: mixed amphetamine salts ‐ extended release; MRI: magnetic resonance imaging; XR: extended release.

Differences between protocol and review

See our protocol (Castells 2009a).

  1. Review authors

    1. Josep Antoni Ramos‐Quiroga, Rosa Bosch, Mariana Nogueira, and Miguel Casas left the review author team and were replaced by Lídia Blanco‐Silvente and Ruth Cunill.

  2. Types of outcome measures

    1. The primary study outcome was initially "Change in the severity of ADHD symptoms assessed by a standardised instrument". However, we found that efficacy was studied by means of continuous outcome variables (change score, for example, change in the ADHD symptom severity score from baseline to study completion; and endpoint score, for example, ADHD symptom severity score at study completion), as well as binary ones (for example, proportion of patients achieving a reduction of at least 30% in the severity of ADHD symptoms), and few studies used the same efficacy outcome, making the meta‐analysis of different studies poor. To allow the combination of the highest number of studies, we redefined the primary efficacy outcome to "ADHD symptom severity". This outcome combined studies reporting change scores or endpoint scores.

    2. The first version of this review ‐ Castells 2011a ‐ included only a small number of studies, so we conducted a post hoc analysis by aggregating all available data from studies irrespective of the reported efficacy outcome. We named this outcome "Efficacy for ADHD symptoms" and combined continuous and binary data on efficacy outcomes. We did not perform this analysis in this update because we included a considerably greater number of studies and were able to combine the results on primary study outcomes from several clinical trials.

    3. We changed the outcome "attrition" to "retention".

  3. Electronic searches

    1. To comply with MECIR conduct standards (Higgins 2016), which were introduced after the first version of this review was published (Castells 2011a), we searched a number of additional databases: MEDLINE In‐Process and Other Non‐Indexed Citations; MEDLINE Epub Ahead of Print (both these segments are updated daily); Science Citation Index; and Social Science Citation Index, We also searched for grey literature (via Conference Proceedings Citation Index ‐ Science; Conference Proceedings Citation Index ‐ Social Science & Humanities; and WorldCat) and sources of other reviews (Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews and Database of Abstracts of Reviews of Effects).

  4. Data collection and analysis

    1. When appropriate, we updated our references to the most recent chapters of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011b).

  5. Assessment of risk of bias in included studies

    1. In this update of the review, we also assessed the overall risk of bias within studies (Higgins 2017a), to facilitate our evaluation of the quality of evidence. We added the following paragraph to Appendix 4: "We considered a study to be: 1) at low risk of bias overall if all the key domains were judged at low risk of bias; 2) at unclear risk of bias overall if one or more domains were judged at unclear risk of bias and all other domains were judged at low risk of bias and; 3) at high risk of bias overall if one or more domains were judged at high risk of bias (Higgins 2017a)."

  6. Assessment of heterogeneity

    1. The protocol established that heterogeneity would be defined on the basis of the P value of the heterogeneity test (Castells 2009a). According to this method, heterogeneity is present or absent. It is more suitable to calculate the amount of heterogeneity using the I² statistic, which indicates the percentage of variance in a meta‐analysis that is attributable to study heterogeneity (Higgins 2017b).

  7. Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity

    1. This section was insufficiently described in the protocol (Castells 2009a). In the previous version of this review published in 2011 (Castells 2011a), we broadened this section and explained the reasons for not doing some of our prespecified subgroup analyses. In the previous version of the review (Castells 2011a), we were unable to perform a subgroup analysis for comorbidity because data were insufficient, but as the number of included studies in this update is substantially larger, we were able to perform the analysis. However, we were not able to compare industry‐sponsored trials to independent trials.

  8. Sensitivity analysis

    1. We planned to run a sensitivity analysis excluding any study rated at high or unclear risk of bias on any domain of the Cochrane 'Risk of bias' tool (Higgins 2017a). However, we were unable to perform this analysis as no study met this criterion because it is unclear whether blinding can be achieved when amphetamines are compared to placebo. Instead, we used our assessments for incomplete outcome data and other potential sources of bias, whose scores showed between‐study variability, and conducted a sensitivity analysis that included only studies scoring low risk of bias on these specific domains.

    2. We conducted three post hoc sensitivity analyses: (1) we calculated the effect size of cross‐over studies by borrowing the correlation coefficient from Taylor 2000 (see Unit of analysis issues); (2) we calculated the pooled risk difference for the outcomes "proportion of patients withdrawn due to AE" and "proportion of patients withdrawn due to cardiovascular AE" because this analysis allows for inclusion of studies that had no events for these outcomes; and (3) we excluded from the analysis one cross‐over study (Spencer 2001), which had a carry‐over effect, to determine wether the carry‐over effect may have biased the results of this review. We did not perform this sensitivity analysis for the outcome "retention in treatment" because results from the first study period were available and, therefore, there was no risk of a carry‐over effect‐related bias for this outcome.

  9. Data synthesis

    1. The methods used to aggregate data have been explained in greater detail than in the published protocol (Castells 2009a), which described only the type of statistical model used.

  10. Effects of interventions

    1. In contrast to the previous version of this review (Castells 2011a), we did not conduct a post hoc analysis that pooled together all available studies irrespective of the type of efficacy outcome. Nor did we conduct a second post hoc sensitivity analysis combining "AE‐induced dropouts" with those events described as dropouts due to "loss to follow‐up", "withdrawal of consent", and "unknown reason". In the previous review (Castells 2011a), these analyses had allowed the inclusion of a larger number of studies in the efficacy analysis. However, as the number of included studies is substantially larger in the present update, we think that these analyses are no longer justified.

Contributions of authors

Xavier Castells (XC) and Ruth Cunill (RC) designed the review.
 XC and RC wrote the background.
 XC and RC wrote the methods, results, discussion, and conclusions with input from Lídia Blanco‐Silvente (LB).
 XC and RC selected studies.
 XC, RC, and LB extracted data.
 XC and RC conducted the statistical analyses.

XC is the guarantor for the review.

Sources of support

Internal sources

  • None, Other.

External sources

  • Universitat de Girona, Spain.

    Grants (MPCUdG2016/ref50) to Xavier Casatells and (IFUdG2015/17) Lídia Blanco‐Silvente

Declarations of interest

Xavier Castells received a research grant for 'Improving the scientific productivity' (MPCUdG2016/ref50) from the Universitat de Girona, Spain.
 Ruth Cunill ‐ none known.
 Lídia Blanco‐Silvente has a pre‐doctoral research contract (IFUdG2015/17) with the Universitat de Girona, Spain.

New search for studies and content updated (no change to conclusions)

References

References to studies included in this review

Adler 2008 {published data only}

  1. Adler LA, Goodman D, Weisler R, Hamdani M, Roth T. Effect of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate on sleep in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behavioral and Brain Functions 2009;5:34‐47. [DOI: 10.1186/1744-9081-5-34; NCT00334880; PMC2732626; PUBMED: 19650932] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adler LA, Goodman DW, Kollins SH, Weisler RH, Krishnan S, Zhang Y, et al. Double‐blind, placebo‐controlled study of the efficacy and safety of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2008;69(9):1364‐73. [NCT00334880; PUBMED: 19012818] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Babcock T, Dirks B, Adeyi B, Scheckner B. Efficacy of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder previously treated with amphetamines: analyses from a randomized, double‐blind, multicenter, placebo‐controlled titration study. BMC Pharmacology & Toxicology 2012;13:18‐27. [DOI: 10.1186/2050-6511-13-18; NCT00334880; PMC3554536; PUBMED: 23254273] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Kollins SH, Youcha S, Lasser R, Thase ME. Lisdexamfetamine dimesylate for the treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults with a history of depression or history of substance use disorder. Innovations in Clinical Neuroscience 2011;8(2):28‐32. [PMC3071091; PUBMED: 21468295] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Mattingly GW, Weisler RH, Young J, Adeyi B, Dirks B, Babcock T, et al. Clinical response and symptomatic remission in short‐ and long‐term trials of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. BMC Psychiatry 2013;13:39‐50. [DOI: 10.1186/1471-244X-13-39] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Adler 2013 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}

  1. Adler LA, Dirks B, Deas P, Raychaudhuri A, Dauphin M, Saylor K, et al. Self‐reported quality of life in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder and executive function impairment treated with lisdexamfetamine dimesylate: a randomized, double‐blind, multicenter, placebo‐controlled, parallel‐group study. BMC Psychiatry 2013;13:253. [DOI: 10.1186/1471-244X-13-253; NCT01101022; PMC3854089; PUBMED: 24106804] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adler LA, Dirks B, Deas PF, Raychaudhuri A, Dauphin MR, Lasser RA, et al. Lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder who report clinically significant impairment in executive function: results from a randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2013;74(7):694‐702. [DOI: 10.4088/JCP.12m08144; NCT01101022; PUBMED: 23945447] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Weisler R, Ginsberg L, Dirks B, Deas P, Adeyi B, Adler LA. Treatment with lisdexamfetamine dimesylate improves self‐ and informant‐rated executive function behaviors and clinician‐ and informant‐rated ADHD symptoms in adults: data from a randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled study. Journal of Attention Disorders 2017; Vol. 21, issue 14:1198‐207. [DOI: 10.1177/1087054713518242; PUBMED: 24464328] [DOI] [PubMed]

Biederman 2012 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}

  1. Biederman J, Fried R, Hammerness P, Surman C, Mehler B, Petty CR, et al. The effects of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate on driving behaviors in young adults with ADHD assessed with the Manchester driving behavior questionnaire. Journal of Adolescent Health 2012;51(6):601‐7. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.03.005; NCT00801229; PUBMED: 23174471] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Biederman J, Fried R, Hammerness P, Surman C, Mehler B, Petty CR, et al. The effects of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate on the driving performance of young adults with ADHD: a randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled study using a validated driving simulator paradigm. Journal of Psychiatric Research 2012;46(4):484‐91. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.01.007; PUBMED: 22277301] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Brams 2012 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}

  1. Brams M, Weisler R, Findling RL, Gasior M, Hamdani M, Ferreira‐Cornwell MC, et al. Maintenance of efficacy of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: randomized withdrawal design. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2012;73(7):977‐83. [DOI: 10.4088/JCP.11m07430; NCT00877487; PUBMED: 22780921] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Dupaul 2012 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}

  1. Dupaul GJ, Weyandt LL, Rossi JS, Vilardo BA, O'Dell SM, Carson KM, et al. Double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, crossover study of the efficacy and safety of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in college students with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders 2012;16(3):202‐20. [DOI: 10.1177/1087054711427299; PUBMED: 22166471] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Frick 2017 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}

  1. Frick G, Yan B, Adler LA. Triple‐Bead Mixed Amphetamine Salts (SHP465) in adults with ADHD: results of a phase 3, double‐blind, randomized, forced‐dose trial. Journal of Attention Disorders 2017 Apr 1 [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI: 10.1177/1087054717696771; PUBMED: 28413925] [DOI] [PubMed]

Kay 2009 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}

  1. Kay GG, Michaels MA, Pakull B. Simulated driving changes in young adults with ADHD receiving mixed amphetamine salts extended release and atomoxetine. Journal of Attention Disorders 2009;12(4):316‐29. [DOI: 10.1177/1087054708322986; PUBMED: 18815438] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Kollins 2014 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}

  1. Kollins SH, English JS, Itchon‐Ramos N, Chrisman AK, Dew R, O'Brien B, et al. A pilot study of lis‐dexamfetamine dimesylate (LDX/SPD489) to facilitate smoking cessation in nicotine‐dependent adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders 2014;18(2):158‐68. [DOI: 10.1177/1087054712440320; PMC3421044; PUBMED: 22508760] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Levin 2015 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}

  1. Levin FR, Mariani JJ, Specker S, Mooney M, Mahony A, Brooks DJ, et al. Extended‐release mixed amphetamine salts vs placebo for comorbid adult attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder and cocaine use disorder: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 2015;72(6):593‐602. [DOI: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.41; NCT00553319; PMC4456227; PUBMED: 25887096] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Notzon DP, Mariani JJ, Pavlicova M, Glass A, Mahony AL, Brooks DJ, et al. Mixed‐amphetamine salts increase abstinence from marijuana in patients with co‐occurring attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder and cocaine dependence. American Journal on Addictions 2016;25(8):666‐72. [DOI: 10.1111/ajad.12467; PMC5435118; PUBMED: 28051838] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Martin 2014a {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}

  1. Martin PT, Corcoran M, Zhang P, Katic A. Randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, crossover study of the effects of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate and mixed amphetamine salts on cognition throughout the day in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Drug Investigation 2014;34(2):147‐57. [DOI: 10.1007/s40261-013-0156-z; PMC3899471; PUBMED: 24297663] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Martin 2014b {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}

  1. Martin PT, Corcoran M, Zhang P, Katic A. Randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, crossover study of the effects of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate and mixed amphetamine salts on cognition throughout the day in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Drug Investigation 2014;34(2):147‐57. [DOI: 10.1007/s40261-013-0156-z; PMC3899471; PUBMED: 24297663] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Spencer 2001 {published data only}

  1. Spencer T, Biederman J, Wilens T, Faraone S, Prince J, Gerard K, et al. Efficacy of a mixed amphetamine salts compound in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 2001;58(8):775‐82. [PUBMED: 11483144] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Spencer 2008 {published data only}

  1. Spencer TJ, Adler LA, Weisler RH, Youcha SH. Triple‐bead mixed amphetamine salts (SPD465), a novel, enhanced extended‐release amphetamine formulation for the treatment of adults with ADHD: a randomized, double‐blind, multicenter, placebo‐controlled study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2008;69(9):1437‐48. [NCT00150579; PUBMED: 19012813] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Spencer TJ, Landgraf JM, Adler LA, Weisler RH, Anderson CS, Youcha SH. Attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder‐specific quality of life with triple‐bead mixed amphetamine salts (SPD465) in adults: results of a randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2008;69(11):1766‐75. [NCT00150579; PUBMED: 19026251] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Taylor 2000 {published data only}

  1. Taylor FB, Russo J. Efficacy of modafinil compared to dextroamphetamine for the treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology 2000;10(4):311‐20. [DOI: 10.1089/cap.2000.10.311; PUBMED: 11191692] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Taylor 2001 {published data only}

  1. Taylor FB, Russo J. Comparing guanfacine and dextroamphetamine for the treatment of adult attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 2001;21(2):223‐8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Waxmonsky 2014 {published and unpublished data}

  1. Babinski DE, Waxmonsky JG, Waschbusch DA, Humphery H, Pelham WE Jr. Parent‐reported improvements in family functioning in a randomized controlled trial of lisdexamfetamine for treatment of parental attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacolgy 2017;27(3):250‐7. [DOI: 10.1089/cap.2016.0129; NCT01127607; PUBMED: 27991835] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Waxmonsky JG, Waschbusch DA, Babinski DE, Humphrey HH, Alfonso A, Crum KI, et al. Does pharmacological treatment of ADHD in adults enhance parenting performance? Results of a double‐blind randomized trial. CNS Drugs 2014;28(7):665‐77. [DOI: 10.1007/s40263-014-0165-3; NCT01127607; PUBMED: 24796970] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Weisler 2006 {published data only}

  1. Biederman J, Spencer TJ, Wilens TE, Weisler RH, Read SC, Tulloch SJ, et al. Long‐term safety and effectiveness of mixed amphetamine salts extended release in adults with ADHD. CNS Spectrums 2005;10(12 Suppl 20):16‐25. [PUBMED: 16344837] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Weisler RH, Biederman J, Spencer TJ, Wilens TE. Long‐term cardiovascular effects of mixed amphetamine salts extended release in adults with ADHD. CNS Spectrums 2005;10(12 Suppl 20):35‐43. [PUBMED: 16344839] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Weisler RH, Biederman J, Spencer TJ, Wilens TE, Faraone SV, Chrisman AK, et al. Mixed amphetamine salts extended‐release in the treatment of adult ADHD: a randomized, controlled trial. CNS Spectrums 2006;11(8):625‐39. [PUBMED: 16871129] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Weisler 2017 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}

  1. Weisler RH, Greenbaum M, Arnold V, Yu M, Yan M, Jaffee M, et al. Efficacy and safety of SHP465 mixed amphetamine salts in the treatment of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: results of a randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, forced‐dose clinical study. CNS Drugs 2017;31(8):685–97. [DOI: 10.1007/s40263-017-0455-7; NCT02604407; PMC5533822; PUBMED: 28712074] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Weiss 2006 {published and unpublished data}

  1. Weiss M, Hechtman L, Adult ADHD Research Group. A randomized double‐blind trial of paroxetine and/or dextroamphetamine and problem‐focused therapy for attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2006;67(4):611‐9. [PUBMED: 16669726] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Weiss M, Murray C, Wasdell M, Greenfield B, Giles L, Hechtman L. A randomized controlled trial of CBT therapy for adults with ADHD with and without medication. BMC Psychiatry 2012;12:30‐8. [DOI: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-30] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Wigal 2010 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}

  1. Wigal T, Brams M, Gasior M, Gao J, Giblin J. Effect size of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Postgraduate Medicine 2011;123(2):169‐76. [DOI: 10.3810/pgm.2011.03.2275; NCT00746733; PUBMED: 21474905] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Wigal T, Brams M, Gasior M, Gao J, Squires L, Giblin J. Randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, crossover study of the efficacy and safety of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: novel findings using a simulated adult workplace environment design. Behavioral and Brain Functions 2010;6:34‐48. [DOI: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-34; PMC2908054] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

References to studies excluded from this review

Adler 2011 {published data only}

  1. Adler LA, Lynch LR, Shaw DM, Wallace SP, Ciranni MA, Briggie AM, et al. Medication adherence and symptom reduction in adults treated with mixed amphetamine salts in a randomized crossover study. Postgraduate Medicine 2011;123(5):71‐9. [DOI: 10.3810/pgm.2011.09.2461; NCT00468143; PUBMED: 21904088] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Adler 2014 {published data only}

  1. Adler LA, Alperin S, Leon T, Faraone S. Clinical effects of lisdexamfetamine and mixed amphetamine salts immediate release in adult ADHD: results of a crossover design clinical trial. Postgraduate Medicine 2014;126(5):17‐24. [DOI: 10.3810/pgm.2014.09.2796; NCT01070394; PUBMED: 25295646] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adler LA, Alperin S, Leon T, Faraone, SV. Pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic properties of lisdexamfetamine in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology 2017;27(2):196‐9. [DOI: 10.1089/cap.2016.0121; PUBMED: 27935735] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Arnold 1972 {published data only}

  1. Arnold LE, Strobl D, Weisenberg A. Hyperkinetic adult. Study of the "paradoxical" amphetamine response. JAMA 1972;222(6):693‐4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Castaneda 2000 {published data only}

  1. Castaneda R, Levy R, Hardy M, Trujillo M. Long‐acting stimulants for the treatment of attention‐deficit disorder in cocaine‐dependent adults. Psychiatric Services 2000;51(2):169‐71. [DOI: 10.1176/appi.ps.51.2.169; PUBMED: 10654994] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Dodson 2005 {published data only}

  1. Dodson WW. Pharmacotherapy of adult ADHD. Journal of Clinical Psychology 2005;61(5):589‐606. [DOI: 10.1002/jclp.20122; PUBMED: 15723384] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Faraone 2002 {published data only}

  1. Faraone SV, Short EJ, Biederman J, Findling RL, Roe C, Manos MJ. Efficacy of Adderall and methylphenidate in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a drug‐placebo and drug‐drug response curve analysis of a naturalistic study. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 2002;5(2):121‐9. [DOI: 10.1017/S1461145702002845; PUBMED: 12135536] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Goodman 2005 {published data only}

  1. Goodman DW, Ginsberg L, Weisler RH, Cutler AJ, Hodgkins P. An interim analysis of the Quality of Life, Effectiveness, Safety, and Tolerability (QU.E.S.T.) evaluation of mixed amphetamine salts extended release in adults with ADHD. CNS Spectrums 2005;10(12 Suppl 20):26‐34. [PUBMED: 16344838] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Lasser 2010 {published data only}

  1. Lasser RA, Dirks B, Adeyi B, Babcock T. Comparative efficacy and safety of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate and mixed amphetamine salts extended release in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Primary Psychiatry 2010;17(9):44‐54. [Google Scholar]

Mattingly 2012 {published data only}

  1. Mattingly G, Weisler R, Dirks B, Babcock T, Adeyi B, Scheckner B, et al. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder subtypes and symptom response in adults treated with lisdexamfetamine dimesylate. Innovations in Clinical Neuroscience 2012;9(5‐6):22‐30. [PMC3398683; PUBMED: 22808446] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Paterson 1999 {published data only}

  1. Paterson R, Douglas C, Hallmayer J, Hagan M, Krupenia Z. A randomised, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled trial of dexamphetamine in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 1999;33(4):494‐502. [DOI: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.1999.00590.x; PUBMED: 10483843] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Rostain 2009 {published data only}

  1. Rostain AL. Lisdexamfetamine in the treatment of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults. Current Psychiatry Reports 2009;11(5):341‐2. [PUBMED: 19785973] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Spencer 2004 {published data only}

  1. Spencer T, Biederman J, Wilens T. Stimulant treatment of adult attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America 2004;27(2):361‐72. [DOI: 10.1016/j.psc.2003.12.002; PUBMED: 15064002] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Waxmonsky 2011 {published data only}

  1. Waxmonsky JG, Waschbusch DA, Glatt SJ, Faraone SV. Prediction of placebo response in 2 clinical trials of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate for the treatment of ADHD. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2011;72(10):1366‐75. [DOI: 10.4088/JCP.10m05979pur; NCT00334880; NCT00556296; PUBMED: 21367347] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Weisler 2014 {published data only}

  1. Weisler RH, Adler LA, Kollins SH, Goodman DW, Hamdani M, Dirks B, et al. Analysis of individual items on the attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom rating scale in children and adults: the effects of age and sex in pivotal trials of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 2014;10:1‐12. [DOI: 10.2147/NDT.S47087; PUBMED: PMC3862743] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Wiebe 2010 {published data only}

  1. Wiebe S, Gruber R, Charney El, Aryal S, Waldman I, Newcorn H, et al. Sleep and emotional reactivity to extended release dexmethylphenidate versus mixed amphetamine salts: a double‐blind, placebo controlled study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 2010;19(Suppl 1):S82. [Google Scholar]

Wilens 2005 {published data only}

  1. Wilens TE, Hammerness PG, Biederman J, Kwon A, Spencer TJ, Clark S, et al. Blood pressure changes associated with medication treatment of adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2005;66(2):253‐9. [PUBMED: 15705013] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Young 2015 {published data only}

  1. Young S, Khondoker M, Emilsson B, Sigurdsson JF, Philipp‐Wiegmann F, Baldursson G, et al. Cognitive‐behavioural therapy in medication‐treated adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder and co‐morbid psychopathology: a randomized controlled trial using multi‐level analysis. Psychological Medicine 2015;45(13):2793‐804. [DOI: 10.1017/S0033291715000756; PMC4595859; PUBMED: 26022103] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

References to ongoing studies

NCT00202605 {published data only}

  1. NCT00202605. Safety and efficacy of SPD465 in adults with ADHD [A phase II, randomized, double‐blind, multi‐center, placebo‐controlled, crossover study of SPD465 in adults with attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)]. clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT00202605 (first received 20 September 2005).

NCT00514202 {published data only}

  1. NCT00514202. Pilot study examining effect for dextroamphetamine to treat cocaine dependence plus attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder [Dextroamphetamine treatment for comorbid cocaine dependence and ADHD]. clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT00514202 (first received 9 August 2007).

NCT00928148 {published data only}

  1. NCT00928148. The safety and efficacy of SPD465 in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [A phase 2, randomized, double‐blind, multi‐center, placebo‐ and active‐controlled, crossover study of SPD465 in adults with attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder]. clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT00928148 (first received 25 June 2009).

NCT01863459 {published data only}

  1. NCT01863459. Lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in the treatment of adult ADHD with anxiety disorder comorbidity. clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT01863459 (first received 29 May 2013).

NCT02635035 {published data only}

  1. NCT02635035. Shire SCT: lisdexamfetamine treatment for ADHD and SCT [Efficacy of lisdexamfetamine in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT)]. clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02635035 (first received 18 December 2015).

NCT02803229 {published data only}

  1. NCT02803229. Treatment of cannabis use disorder among adults with comorbid attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (MJ‐ADHD). clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02803229 (first received 16 June 2016).

NCT03153488 {published data only}

  1. NCT03153488. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) prediction of treatment response. Clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT03153488 (first received 15 May 2007).

Additional references

Adler 2009

  1. Adler LA, Weisler RH, Goodman DW, Hamdani M, Niebler GE. Short‐term effects of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate on cardiovascular parameters in a 4‐week clinical trial in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2009;70(12):1652‐61. [DOI: 10.4088/JCP.09m05335pur; NCT00334880; PUBMED: 20141706] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Arnsten 2006

  1. Arnsten AF. Stimulants: therapeutic actions in ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006;31(11):2376‐83. [PUBMED: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301164; PUBMED: 16855530] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Balshem 2011

  1. Balshem H, Helfand M, Schünemann HJ, Oxman AD, Kunz R, Brozek J, et al. GRADE guidelines: 3. Rating the quality of evidence. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(4):401‐6. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.07.015; PUBMED: 21208779] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Barkley 2002

  1. Barkley RA, Murphy KR, Dupaul GI, Bush T. Driving in young adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: knowledge, performance, adverse outcomes, and the role of executive functioning. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society 2002;8(5):655‐72. [PUBMED: 12164675] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Bekelman 2003

  1. Bekelman JE, Li Y, Gross CP. Scope and impact of financial conflicts of interest in biomedical research: a systematic review. JAMA 2003;289(4):454‐65. [PUBMED: 12533125] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Biederman 1993

  1. Biederman J, Faraone SV, Spencer T, Wilens T, Norman D, Lapey KA, et al. Patterns of psychiatric comorbidity, cognition, and psychosocial functioning in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 1993;150(12):1792‐8. [DOI: 10.1176/ajp.150.12.1792; PUBMED: 8238632] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Biederman 2000

  1. Biederman J, Mick E, Faraone SV. Age‐dependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: impact of remission definition and symptom type. American Journal of Psychiatry 2000;157(5):816‐8. [DOI: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.816; PUBMED: 10784477] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Biederman 2006

  1. Biederman J, Faraone SV, Spencer TJ, Mick E, Monuteaux MC, Aleardi M. Functional impairments in adults with self‐reports of diagnosed ADHD: a controlled study of 1001 adults in the community. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2006;67(4):524‐40. [16669717] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Carboni 2004

  1. Carboni E, Silvagni A. Experimental investigations on dopamine transmission can provide clues on the mechanism of the therapeutic effect of amphetamine and methylphenidate in ADHD. Neural Plasticity 2004;11(1‐2):77‐95. [DOI: 10.1155/NP.2004.77; PMC2565436] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Castellanos 2006

  1. Castellanos FX, Sonuga‐Barke EJ, Milham MP, Tannock R. Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2006;10(3):117‐23. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011; PUBMED: 16460990] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Castells 2009b [pers comm]

  1. Castells X. Request of data [personal communication]. Email to: Shire 30 December 2009.

Castells 2011b

  1. Castells X, Ramos‐Quiroga JA, Rigau D, Bosch R, Nogueira M, Vidal X, et al. Efficacy of methylphenidate for adults with attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta‐regression analysis. CNS Drugs 2011;25(2):157‐69. [DOI: 10.2165/11539440-000000000-00000; PUBMED: 21254791] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Castells 2013

  1. Castells X, Cunill R, Capellà D. Treatment discontinuation with methylphenidate in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta‐analysis of randomized clinical trials. European Journal of Clininical Pharmacology 2013;69(3):347‐56. [DOI: 10.1007/s00228-012-1390-7; PUBMED: 22983311] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Castells 2016

  1. Castells X, Cunill R, Pérez‐Mañá C, Vidal X, Capellà D. Psychostimulant drugs for cocaine dependence. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2016, Issue 9. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD007380.pub4] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Caye 2016

  1. Caye A, Spadini AV, Karam RG, Grevet EH, Rovaris DL, Bau CH, et al. Predictors of persistence of ADHD into adulthood: a systematic review of the literature and meta‐analysis. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 2016;25:1151‐9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Childs 2009

  1. Childs E, Wit H. Amphetamine‐induced place preference in humans. Biological Psychiatry 2009;65(10):900‐4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Cohen 1988

  1. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for Behavioural Sciences. New York: Academic Press, 1988. [Google Scholar]

Conners 1999

  1. Conners CK, Erhardt D, Sparrow E. Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS): Technical Manual. North Tonawanda (NY): Multi‐Health Systems Inc, 1999. [Google Scholar]

Corbisiero 2013

  1. Corbisiero S, Stieglitz RD, Retz W, Rösler M. Is emotional dysregulation part of the psychopathology of ADHD in adults?. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorders 2013;5(2):83‐92. [DOI: 10.1007/s12402-012-0097-z; PUBMED: 23208078] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Cunill 2013

  1. Cunill R, Castells X, Tobias A, Capellà D. Atomoxetine for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in the adulthood: a meta‐analysis and meta‐regression. Pharmacoepidemiology and Drug Safety 2013;22:961‐9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Cunill 2015

  1. Cunill R, Castells X, Tobias A, Capellà D. Pharmacological treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with co‐morbid drug dependence. Journal of Psychopharmacology 2015;29:15‐23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Cunill 2016

  1. Cunill R, Castells X, Tobias A, Capellà D. Efficacy, safety and variability in pharmacotherapy for adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta‐analysis and meta‐regression in over 9000 patients. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2016;233:187‐97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Dalsgaard 2015

  1. Dalsgaard S, Østergaard SD, Leckman JF, Mortensen PB, Pedersen MG. Mortality in children, adolescents, and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a nationwide cohort study. Lancet 2015;385(9983):2190‐6. [DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61684-6; PUBMED: 25726514] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Deeks 2017

  1. Deeks JJ, Higgins JPT, Altman DG, editor(s). Chapter 9. Analysing data and undertaking meta‐analyses. In: Higgins JPT, Churchill R, Chandler J, Cumpston MS, editor(s). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 5.2.0 (updated June 2017). Cochrane, 2017. Available from www.training.cochrane.org/handbook.

DSM‐5

  1. American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Fifth Edition (DSM‐5). Washington (DC): APA, 2013. [ISBN‐10: 8123923791] [Google Scholar]

DSM‐III

  1. American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Third Edition (DSM‐III). Washington DC: APA, 1980. [ASIN: B000P1A7CK] [Google Scholar]

DSM‐III‐R

  1. American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Third Edition, Revised (DSM‐III‐R). Washington (DC): APA, 1987. [ASIN: B01K3RVIIM] [Google Scholar]

DSM‐IV

  1. American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Fourth Edition (DSM‐IV). Washington (DC): APA, 1994. [ASIN: B01FIX0LA2] [Google Scholar]

DSM‐IV‐TR

  1. American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM‐IV‐TR). Washington (DC): APA, 2000. [ISBN‐10: 0890420254] [Google Scholar]

DuPaul 1998

  1. DuPaul GJ, Power TJ, Anastopoulos AD, Reid R. ADHD Rating Scale‐IV: Checklists, Norms and Clinical Interpretation. New York (NY): Guildford Press, 1998. [ISBN‐10: 1572304235] [Google Scholar]

Egger 1997

  1. Egger M, Davey‐Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta‐analysis detected by a simple graphical test. BMJ 1997;315(7109):629‐34. [PMC2127453; PUBMED: 9310563] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Elbourne 2002

  1. Elbourne DR, Altman DG, Higgins JP, Curtin F, Worthington HV, Vail A. Meta‐analyses involving cross‐over trials: methodological issues. International Journal of Epidemiology 2002;31(1):140‐9. [PUBMED: 11914310] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Ermer 2016

  1. Ermer JC, Pennick M, Frick G. Lisdexamfetamine dimesylate: prodrug delivery, amphetamine exposure and duration of efficacy. Clinical Drug Investigation 2016;36(5):341‐56. [DOI: 10.1007/s40261-015-0354-y; PMC4823324; PUBMED: 27021968] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Faraone 2004

  1. Faraone SV, Spencer T, Aleardi M, Pagano C, Biederman J. Meta‐analysis of the efficacy of methylphenidate for treating adult attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 2004;24(1):24‐9. [DOI: 10.1097/01.jcp.0000108984.11879.95; PUBMED: 14709943] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Faraone 2006

  1. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Mick E. The age‐dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta‐analysis of follow‐up studies. Psychological Medicine 2006;36(2):159‐65. [DOI: 10.1017/S003329170500471X; PUBMED: 16420712] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

FDA 2007

  1. Dexedrine® (dextroamphetamine sulfate): Spansule® sustained‐release capsules and tablets. www.accessdata.fda.gov/drugsatfda_docs/label/2007/017078s042lbl.pdf (accessed prior to 11 July 2018).

FDA 2015a

  1. Adderall®. www.accessdata.fda.gov/drugsatfda_docs/label/2015/011522s041lbl.pdf (accessed prior to 11 July 2018).

FDA 2015b

  1. Highlights of prescribing information: Vyvanse® (lisdexamfetamine dimesylate) capsules, for oral use, CII Initial U.S. Approval: 2007. www.accessdata.fda.gov/drugsatfda_docs/label/2015/021977s039lbl.pdf (accessed prior to 11 July 2018).

Fleckenstein 2007

  1. Fleckenstein AE, Volz TJ, Riddle EL, Gibb JW, Hanson GR. New insights into the mechanism of action of amphetamines. Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology 2007;47:681‐98. [DOI: 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.47.120505.105140; PUBMED: 17209801] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Grace 2002

  1. Grace A. Psychostimulants actions on dopamine and limbic system function: relevance to the pathophysiology and treatment of ADHD. In: Solanto MV, Arnsten AFT, Castellanos FX editor(s). Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and Clinical Neuroscience. New York (NY): Oxford University Press, 2001:134‐57. [Google Scholar]

GRADEPro GDT 2015 [Computer program]

  1. McMaster University (developed by Evidence Prime). GRADEpro GDT. Version accessed 18 October 2017. Hamilton (ON): McMaster University (developed by Evidence Prime), 2015.

Guy 1976

  1. Guy W. ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology, Revised. Rockville (MD): US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare; National Institute of Mental Health, 1976. [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2011a

  1. Guyatt G, Oxman AD, Akl EA, Kunz R, Vist G, Brozek J, et al. GRADE guidelines: 1. Introduction ‐ GRADE evidence profiles and summary of findings tables. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(4):383‐94. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.04.026; PUBMED: 21195583] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2011b

  1. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Vist G, Kunz R, Brozek J, Alonso‐Coello P, et al. GRADE guidelines: 4. Rating the quality of evidence ‐ study limitations (risk of bias). Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(4):407‐15. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.07.017; PUBMED: 21247734] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2011c

  1. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Kunz R, Brozek J, Alonso‐Coello P, Rind D, et al. GRADE guidelines 6. Rating the quality of evidence ‐ imprecision. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(12):1283‐93. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.01.012; PUBMED: 21839614] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2011d

  1. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Kunz R, Woodcock J, Brozek J, Helfand M, et al. GRADE guidelines: 7. Rating the quality of evidence ‐ inconsistency. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(12):1294‐302. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.03.017; PUBMED: 21803546] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2011e

  1. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Kunz R, Woodcock J, Brozek J, Helfand M, et al. GRADE guidelines: 8. Rating the quality of evidence ‐ indirectness. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(12):1303‐10. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.04.014; PUBMED: 21802903] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2011f

  1. Guyatt G, Oxman AD, Sultan S, Brozek J, Glasziou P, Alonso‐Coello P, et al. GRADE guidelines: 11. Making an overall rating of confidence in effect estimates for a single outcome and for all outcomes. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2013;66(2):151‐7. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2012.01.006; PUBMED: 22542023] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Hardman 2001

  1. Hardman JG, Limbird LE, Gilman AG, editor(s). Goodman & Gilman's The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics. 10th Edition. New York (NY): McGraw‐Hill, 2001. [Google Scholar]

Heal 2013

  1. Heal DJ, Smith SL, Gosden J, Nutt DJ. Amphetamine, past and present ‐ a pharmacological and clinical perspective. Journal of Psychopharmacology 2013;27(6):479‐96. [DOI: 10.1177/0269881113482532; PMC3666194] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Higgins 2011a

  1. Higgins JPT, Deeks JJ, editor(s). Chapter 7. Selecting studies and collecting data. In: Higgins JP, Green S, editor(s). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from handbook.cochrane.org.

Higgins 2011b

  1. Higgins JP, Green S, editor(s). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from handbook.cochrane.org.

Higgins 2016

  1. Higgins JPT, Lasserson T, Chandler J, Tovey D, Churchill R. Standards for the conduct of new Cochrane Intervention reviews. Methodological Expectations of Cochrane Intervention Reviews. London (UK): Cochrane, 2016. [Google Scholar]

Higgins 2017a

  1. Higgins JPT, Altman DG, Sterne JAC, editor(s). Chapter 8. Assessing risk of bias in included studies. In: Higgins JPT, Churchill R, Chandler J, Cumpston MS, editor(s). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.2.0 (updated June 2017). Cochrane, 2017. Available from www.training.cochrane.org/handbook.

Higgins 2017b

  1. Higgins JPT, Altman DG, Sterne JAC, editor(s). Chapter 9.5.2. Indetifyin and measuring heterogeneity. In: Higgins JPT, Churchill R, Chandler J, Cumpston MS, editor(s). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.2.0 (updated June 2017). Cochrane, 2017. Available from www.training.cochrane.org/handbook.

Hill 1996

  1. Hill JC, Schoener EP. Age‐dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 1996;153(9):1143‐6. [DOI: 10.1176/ajp.153.9.1143; PUBMED: 8780416] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

ICD‐10

  1. World Health Organization (WHO). The ICD‐10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic guidelines. Geneva (CH): WHO, 1992. [Google Scholar]

Ioannides‐Demos 2005

  1. Ioannides‐Demos LL, Proietto J, McNeil JJ. Pharmacotherapy for obesity. Drugs 2005;65(10):1391‐418. [PUBMED: 15977970] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Johanson 1980

  1. Johanson CE, Uhlenhuth EH. Drug preference and mood in humans: d‐amphetamine. Psychopharmacology 1980;71(3):275‐9. [PUBMED: 6779335] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Keck 1998

  1. Keck PE Jr, McElroy SL, Strakowski SM, West SA, Sax KW, Hawkins JM, et al. 12‐month outcome of patients with bipolar disorder following hospitalization for a manic or mixed episode. American Journal of Psychiatry 1998;155(5):646‐52. [DOI: 10.1176/ajp.155.5.646; PUBMED: 9585716] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Kessler 2006

  1. Kessler RC, Adler L, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners CK, Demler O, et al. The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. American Journal of Psychiatry 2006;163(4):716‐23. [DOI: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.4.716; PMC2859678; PUBMED: 16585449] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Kessler 2010

  1. Kessler RC, Green JG, Adler LA, Barkley RA, Chatterji S, Faraone SV, et al. Structure and diagnosis of adult attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: analysis of expanded symptom criteria from the Adult ADHD Clinical Diagnosis Scale. Archives of General Psychiatry 2010;67(11):1168–78. [DOI: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.146; PMC3131739; PUBMED: 21041618] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Koesters 2008

  1. Koesters M, Becker T, Kilian R, Fegert JM, Weinmann S. Limits of meta‐analysis: methylphenidate in the treatment of adult attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Psychopharmacology 2009;23(7):733‐44. [DOI: 10.1177/0269881108092338; PUBMED: 18562416] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Kondro 2005

  1. Kondro W. Inconclusive evidence puts Adderall back on the market. Canadian Medical Association Journal 2005;173(8):858. [DOI: 10.1503/cmaj.051145; PMC1247691; PUBMED: 16217102] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Kooij 2009

  1. Kooij SJ, Bejerot S, Blackwell A, Caci H, Casas‐Brugué M, Carpentier PJ, et al. European consensus statement on diagnosis and treatment of adult ADHD: The European Network Adult ADHD. BMC Psychiatry 2010;10:67. [DOI: 10.1186/1471-244X-10-67; PMC2942810; PUBMED: 20815868] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Lara 2009

  1. Lara C, Fayyad J, Graaf R, Kessler RC, Aguilar‐Gaxiola S, Angermeyer M, et al. Childhood predictors of adult ADHD: results from the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative. Biological Psychiatry 2009;65(1):46‐54. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.005; NIHMS83060; PMC2629074] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Levin 1998

  1. Levin FR, Evans SM, McDowell DM, Kleber HD. Methylphenidate treatment for cocaine abusers with attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder: a pilot study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 1998;59(6):300‐5. [PUBMED: 9671342] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Lundh 2017

  1. Lundh A, Lexchin J, Mintzes B, Schroll JB, Bero L. Industry sponsorship and research outcome. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2017, Issue 2. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.MR000033.pub3] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Makris 2004

  1. Makris AP, Rush CR, Frederich RC, Kelly TH. Wake‐promoting agents with different mechanisms of action: comparison of effects of modafinil and amphetamine on food intake and cardiovascular activity. Appetite 2004;42(2):185‐95. [DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2003.11.003; PUBMED: 15010183] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Makris 2007

  1. Makris AP, Rush CR, Frederich RC, Taylor AC, Kelly TH. Behavioral and subjective effects of d‐amphetamine and modafinil in healthy adults. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 2007;15(2):123‐33. [DOI: 10.1037/1064-1297.15.2.123; PUBMED: 17469936] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Markowitz 2017

  1. Markowitz JS, Patrick KS. The clinical pharmacokinetics of amphetamines utilized in the treatment of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology 2017;27(8):678‐89. [DOI: 10.1089/cap.2017.0071; PUBMED: 28910145] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Medori 2008

  1. Medori R, Ramos‐Quiroga JA, Casas M, Kooij JJ, Niemelä A, Trott GE, et al. A randomized, placebo‐controlled trial of three fixed dosages of prolonged‐release OROS methylphenidate in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 2008;63(10):981‐9. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.11.008; 18206857; NCT00246220] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

MHRA 2015

  1. Medicines and Healthcare Products Regulatory Agency. Public Assessment Report. Decentralised Procedure. Elvanse 30 mg, 50 mg and 70 mg capsules, hard: lisdexamfetamine dimesylate, UK/H/3326/001‐03/DC, UK licence no: PL 08081/0050‐2. Shire Pharmaceuticals Contracts Limited. www.mhra.gov.uk/home/groups/par/documents/websiteresources/con261790.pdf (accessed prior to 11 July 2018).

Moher 2009

  1. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta‐analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine 2009;6(7):e1000097. [DOI: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097; PMC2707599; PUBMED: 19621072] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

MTA 2004

  1. MTA Cooperative Group. National Institute of Mental Health Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD follow‐up: changes in effectiveness and growth after the end of treatment. Pediatrics 2004;113(4):762–9. [PUBMED: 15060225] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Nishino 2007

  1. Nishino S. Narcolepsy: pathophysiology and pharmacology. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2007;68(Suppl 13):9‐15. [PUBMED: 18078360] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Otasowie 2014

  1. Otasowie J, Castells X, Ehimare UP, Smith CH. Tricyclic antidepressants for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children and adolescents. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2014, Issue 9. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD006997.pub2] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Peterson 2007

  1. Peterson K, McDonagh MS, Fu R. Comparative benefits and harms of competing medications for adults with attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and indirect comparison meta‐analysis. Psychopharmacology 2008;197(1):1‐11. [DOI: 10.1007/s00213-007-0996-4; PUBMED: 18026719] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Phillips 2014

  1. Phillips KA, Epstein DH, Preston KL. Psychostimulant addiction treatment. Neuropharmacology 2014;87:150‐60. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.04.002; PMC4524548; PUBMED: 24727297] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Punja 2016

  1. Punja S, Shamseer L, Hartling L, Urichuk L, Vandermeer B, Nikles J, et al. Amphetamines for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children and adolescents. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2016, Issue 2. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD009996.pub2] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Retz 2012

  1. Retz W, Stieglitz R‐D, Corbisiero S, Retz‐Junginger P, Rösler M. Emotional dysregulation in adult ADHD: what is the empirical evidence?. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics 2012;12(10):1241‐51. [DOI: 10.1586/ern.12.109; PUBMED: 23082740] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Review Manager 2014 [Computer program]

  1. Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration. Review Manager 5 (RevMan 5). Version 5.3. Copenhagen: Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration, 2014.

Riccio 2005

  1. Riccio CA, Wolfe M, Davis B, Romine C, George C, Lee D. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: manifestation in adulthood. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 2005;20(2):249‐69. [DOI: 10.1016/j.acn.2004.07.005; PUBMED: 15708734] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Riera 2017

  1. Riera M, Castells X, Tobias A, Cunill R, Blanco L, Capellà D. Discontinuation of pharmacological treatment of children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: meta‐analysis of 63 studies enrolling 11,788 patients. Psychopharmacology 2017; Vol. 234, issue 17:2657‐71. [DOI: 10.1007/s00213-017-4662-1; PUBMED: 28631099] [DOI] [PubMed]

Safer 2016

  1. Safer DJ. Recent trends in stimulant usage. Journal of Attention Disorders 2016;20(6):471‐7. [DOI: 10.1177/1087054715605915; PUBMED: 26486603] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Schhneider 2006

  1. Schneider LS, Tariot PN, Dagerman KS, Davis SM, Hsiao JK, Ismail MS, et al. Effectiveness of atypical antipsychotic drugs in patients with Alzheimer's disease. New England Journal of Medicine 2006;355(15):1525–38. [DOI: 10.1056/NEJMoa061240; NCT00015548; PUBMED: 17035647] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Schoechlin 2005

  1. Schoechlin C, Engel RR. Neuropsychological performance in adult attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder: meta‐analysis of empirical data. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 2005;20(6):727‐44. [DOI: 10.1016/j.acn.2005.04.005; PUBMED: 15953706] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Schultz 1995

  1. Schulz KF, Chalmers I, Hayes RJ, Altman DG. Empirical evidence of bias. Dimensions of methodological quality associated with estimates of treatment effects in controlled trials. JAMA 1995;273(5):408‐12. [PUBMED: 7823387] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Shearer 2002

  1. Shearer J, Sherman J, Wodak A, Beek I. Substitution therapy for amphetamine users. Drug and Alcohol Review 2002;21(2):179‐85. [DOI: 10.1080/09595230220139082; PUBMED: 12188997] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Simon 2009

  1. Simon V, Czobor P, Balint S, Meézaros A, Bitter I. Prevalence and correlates of adult attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder: meta‐analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry 2009;194(3):204‐11. [DOI: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.048827; PUBMED: 19252145] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Sonuga‐Barke 2008

  1. Sonuga‐Barke EJ, Sergeant JA, Nigg J, Willcutt E. Executive dysfunction and delay aversion in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: nosologic and diagnostic implications. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America 2008;17(2):367‐84. [DOI: 10.1016/j.chc.2007.11.008; PUBMED: 18295151] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Spencer 2007

  1. Spencer TJ, Adler LA, McGough JJ, Muniz R, Jiang H, Pestreich L, et al. Efficacy and safety of dexmethylphenidate extended‐release capsules in adults with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 2007;61(12):1380‐7. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.032; PUBMED: 17137560] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Storebø 2015

  1. Storebø OJ, Ramstad E, Krogh HB, Nilausen TD, Skoog M, Holmskov M, et al. Methylphenidate for children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2015, Issue 11. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD009885.pub2] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Stroup 2003

  1. Stroup TS, McEvoy JP, Swartz MS, Byerly MJ, Glick ID, Canive JM, et al. The National Institute of Mental Health Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) project: schizophrenia trial design and protocol development. Schizophrenia Bulletin 2003;29(1):15‐31. [PUBMED: 12908658] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Sulzer 2005

  1. Sulzer D, Sonders MS, Poulsen NW, Galli A. Mechanisms of neurotransmitter release by amphetamines: a review. Progress in Neurobiolology 2005;75(6):406‐33. [DOI: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.04.003; PUBMED: 15955613] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Swanson 2007

  1. Swanson JM, Kinsbourne M, Nigg J, Lanphear B, Stefanatos GA, Volkow N, et al. Etiologic subtypes of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: brain imaging, molecular genetic and environmental factors and the dopamine hypothesis. Neuropsychology Review 2007;17(1):39‐59. [DOI: 10.1007/s11065-007-9019-9; PUBMED: 17318414] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Thapar 2016

  1. Thapar A, Cooper M. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet 2016;387(10024):1240‐50. [DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00238-X] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Thomas 2015

  1. Thomas R, Sanders S, Doust J, Beller E, Glasziou P. Prevalence of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Pediatrics 2015;135(4):e994‐1001. [DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-3482; PUBMED: 25733754] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Van Emmerik‐van Oortmerssen 2012

  1. Emmerik‐Van Oortmerssen K, Glind G, Brink W, Smit F, Crunelle CL, Swets M, et al. Prevalence of attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder in substance use disorder patients: a meta‐analysis and meta‐regression analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 2012;122(1‐2):11‐9. [DOI: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.12.007; PUBMED: 22209385] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Wachtel 1992

  1. Wachtel SR, Wit H. Subjective and behavioral effects of repeated d‐amphetamine in humans. Behavioral Pharmacology 1999;10(3):271‐81. [PUBMED: 10780242] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Weyandt 2016

  1. Weyandt LL, Oster DR, Marraccini ME, Gudmundsdottir BG, Munro BA, Rathkey ES, et al. Prescription stimulant medication misuse: where are we and where do we go from here?. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 2016;24(5):400‐14. [DOI: 10.1037/pha0000093; PMC5113141; PUBMED: 27690507] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Wilens 2003

  1. Wilens TE. Drug therapy for adults with attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder. Drugs 2003;63(22):2395‐411. [PUBMED: 14609347] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Willcutt 2012

  1. Willcutt EG. The prevalence of DSM‐IV attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta‐analytic review. Neurotherapeutics 2012;9(3):490‐9. [DOI: 10.1007/s13311-012-0135-8; PMC3441936 ; PUBMED: 22976615] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Young 2005

  1. Young S, Heptinstall E, Sonuga Barke EJ, Chadwick O, Taylor E. The adolescent outcome of hyperactive girls: self‐report of psychosocial  status. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2005;46(3):255‐62. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00350.x; PUBMED: 15755302] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

References to other published versions of this review

Castells 2009a

  1. Castells X, Ramos‐Quiroga JA, Bosch R, Nogueira M, Casas M. Amfetamines for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in adults. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2009, Issue 5. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD007813] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Castells 2011a

  1. Castells X, Ramos‐Quiroga JA, Bosch R, Nogueira M, Casas M. Amphetamines for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in adults. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2011, Issue 6. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD007813.pub2] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES