Abstract
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder is a pervasive developmental disorder characterized by inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity and is 75–90% heritable. Latrophilin-3 (LPHN3; or ADGRL(3) is associated with a subtype of ADHD, but how it translates to symptoms is unknown. LPHN3 is a synaptic adhesion G protein coupled receptor that binds to fibronectin leucine rich transmembrane protein 3 and teneurin-3 (FLRT3 and TEN-3). We created a null mutation of Lphn3 (KO) in Sprague-Dawley rats using CRISPR/Cas9 to delete exon-3. The KO rats had no effects on reproduction or survival but reduced growth. KO females showed catch-up weight gain whereas KO males did not. We tested WT and KO littermates for home-cage activity, anxiety-like behavior, acoustic startle response, and activity after amphetamine challenge. Expression of Lphn3-related genes, monoamines, and receptors were determined. Lphn3 KO rats showed persistent hyperactivity, increased acoustic startle, reduced activity in response to amphetamine relative to baseline, and female-specific reduced anxiety-like behavior. Expression of Lphn1, Lphn2, and Flrt3 by qPCR and their protein products by western-blot analysis showed no compensatory upregulation. Striatal tyrosine hydroxylase, aromatic L-amino acid decarboxylase (AADC), and the dopamine transporter were increased and dopamine D1 receptor (DRD1) and dopamine- and cAMP-regulated neuronal phosphoprotein (DARPP-32) decreased with no changes in DRD2, DRD4, vesicular monoamine transporter-2, N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA)-NR1, -NR2A, or -NR2B. LPHN3 is expressed in many brain regions but its function is largely unknown. Data from human, mouse, zebrafish, Drosophila and our new Lphn3 KO rat data collectively show that its disruption is significantly correlated with hyperactivity and associated striatal changes in dopamine markers.
Keywords: Latrophilin-3 (LPHN3 or ADGRL(3)), ADHD, dopamine D1 receptor (DRD1), tyrosine hydroxylase, aromatic amino acid decarboxylase (AADC), DARPP-32, dopamine transporter (DAT)
Introduction
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Demontis et al., 2019) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by hyperactivity, inattention, and impulsivity (Acosta et al., 2011), often with comorbid conditions, such as anxiety (Steinberg and Drabick, 2015; Tung et al., 2016). ADHD affects 5% of children and adolescents world-wide (Martinez et al., 2011) and 2.5% of adults (Franke et al., 2012; Demontis et al., 2018). Research has implicated dysregulation of monoamines, especially dopamine (DA) as important factors in ADHD (Sagvolden et al., 1998; Russell et al., 2005). Family, twin, and adoption studies indicate that ADHD is heritable, but it is not associated with large-effect gene variants, but rather with multiple small-effect variants (Hwang et al., 2015; van der Voet et al., 2016). ADHDGene lists 398 copy number variants, 1391 SNPs, and 173 gene associations from a genome-wide association study (GWAS), and linkage disequilibrium, pathway analyses, and gene mapping studies (Zhang et al., 2012). A new GWAS further reports a number of novel genetic associations with ADHD symptoms (Demontis et al., 2018).
GWAS and fine mapping studies from a genetic isolate in Colombia (Paisa) identified a heretofore unrecognized haplotype associated with ADHD: latrophilin-3 (Arcos-Burgos and Muenke, 2010; Acosta et al., 2011), also known as adhesion G protein-coupled receptor L(3) or ADGRL(3) [OMIM 616417]. Variants of LPHN3 increase the risk of ADHD by 1.2 fold and are associated with symptom severity and medication response. The association was also found in North American, European, Spanish, Korean, and Chinese patients (Domene et al., 2011a; Franke et al., 2012; Hwang et al., 2015; Huang et al., 2018). Twenty-one LPHN3 SNP variants are associated with ADHD (Domene et al., 2011b) and with response to methylphenidate, a common ADHD medication (Arcos-Burgos et al., 2010a; Ribases et al., 2011; O’Sullivan et al., 2014; Hwang et al., 2015; Ranaivoson et al., 2015). LPHN3 is also associated with substance abuse disorders, another comorbidity with ADHD (Arcos-Burgos et al., 2019).
Latrophilins are adhesion G protein coupled receptors (aGPCRs). There are three isoforms: LPHN1 is expressed in brain and periphery; LPHN2 is primarily peripheral, and LPHN3 is in brain and adrenal (Sugita et al., 1998). LPHN3 is most abundant in caudate-putamen, but is also expressed in prefrontal cortex (PFC), amygdala, hippocampus (HIP), and cerebellum (Cb) (Arcos-Burgos et al., 2010b). LPHN3 is localized synaptically with an extracellular domain that binds to fibronectin leucine rich transmembrane protein (FLRT3) and teneurin-3 (Sando et al., 2019) thereby spanning the synaptic cleft. The LPHN3-FLRT3-teneurin-3 linkage is also associated with adjacent residues forming a synapse spanning linkage composed of a dystroglycan-teneurin complex cross-linked at a TCAP site. This complex is thought to stabilize the synapse (Silva and Ushkaryov, 2010; Ranaivoson et al., 2015) and determine synaptic specificity (Sando et al., 2019).
Knock-down of the lphn3.1 orthologue in zebrafish leads to increased swim distance and speed, and the effects are reversed by methylphenidate (MPH), atomoxetine (ATO), and selective dopaminergic agonists; also the number of DA cells were reduced in the posterior tuberculum (Lange et al., 2012; Lange et al., 2018). Lphn3 knockout (KO) mice are hyperactive in a novel environment and show exaggerated hyperactivity when given cocaine. These mice also have increased brain DA and serotonin (5-HT) (Wallis et al., 2012) and a complex pattern of protein expression changes (Orsini et al., 2016). Knockout of lphn3 in Drosophila similarly causes hyperactivity that is reduced by methylphenidate (van der Voet et al., 2016). However, little is known about LPHN3 beyond the hyperactivity induced by its deletion. To address this, we used CRISPR/Cas9 to delete exon 3 of Lphn3 in Sprague-Dawley rats. We hypothesized that these rats would not only show acute hyperactivity, but persistent hyperactivity in a familiar environment, a key feature of ADHD (Russell et al., 2005). We further hypothesized that LPHN3 deletion would increase startle reactivity and reduce motor activation in response to amphetamine, and these changes would be accompanied by changes in DA markers.
Methods
Animals and Husbandry
Generation of Lphn3 KO rats
Lphn3−/− (knockout) rats on a Sprague-Dawley (SD-IGS, strain 001, Charles River, Charleston, NC) background were generated in the Cincinnati Children’s Transgenic Animal and Genome Editing Core using CRISPR/Cas9 to delete exon 3 (Fig. 1A). Briefly, two sgRNAs targeting the sequences flanking exon 3 (GTCCCTTGCCAGTACATCTC and CCTAGTGTTGTGTTCTGCTA) were selected according to the off-target scores from the CRISPR design web tool (http://genome-engineering.org). The sgRNA and Cas9 mRNA were in vitro transcribed using MEGAshorscript T7 kit (ThermoFisher) and mMESSAGE mMACHINE T7 ULTRA kit (ThermoFisher), respectively, according to the manufacturer’s instructions. The mutant rats were generated by injection of two sgRNAs (50 ng/μL each) and Cas9 mRNA (100 ng/μL), along with two ssDNA donor oligos that contained loxP (50 ng/μL each), into the cytoplasm of fertilized eggs, using a piezo-driven microinjection technique (Yang et al., 2014; Scott and Hu, 2019). Injected embryos were transferred immediately into the oviductal ampulla of pseudopregnant females. Live born pups were genotyped by PCR and further confirmed by Sanger sequencing. We obtained several KO alleles though the loxP-containing alleles were not obtained from this round of microinjection. We bred the founders with wildtype (WT) rats to establish the lines and used Lphn3+/− x Lphn3+/− crossings for generating KO and WT littermates for the experiments (Olincy et al., 2000). Rats were housed in polysulfone cages in a pathogen free vivarium using a Modular Animal Caging System (Alternative Design, Siloam Spring, AR) with HEPA filtered air at 30 air changes/h (Alternative Design, Siloam Spring, AR). Rats were provided ad libitum reverse-osmosis filtered/UV sterilized water (SE Lab Group, Napa, CA) and fed NIH-07 rat chow (LabDiet, Richmond, IN). Cages had woodchip bedding and stainless steel huts as enrichment (Vorhees et al., 2011). Rats were maintained on a 14 h light-10 h dark cycle (lights on at 600 h) at 20 ± 1 °C and 50 ± 10% relative humidity. Multiparous Lphn3+/− x Lphn3+/− pairs were bred, and pregnant dams were moved to individual cages. Birth was designated postnatal day 0 (P0). Ear punches were collected from offspring at P7 for genotyping using three primers: 1. AAAGGGTCATAGCATCCGGC, 2. CTAACGTGGCTTTTTGTCTTCT, and 3. GCTCGACAGACAGTGTGGAT. HotStarTaq Master Mix kit (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany) was used per manufacturer recommendations. Thermocycler parameters were: 1) 95 °C 5 min, 2) 94 °C 1 min, 3) 61.5 °C 1 min, 4) 72 °C 2 min, 5), steps 2–4 were repeated 34 more times followed by 6) 72 °C 10 min, and 7) held at 4 °C until the product was run on a 2% agarose gel with ethidium bromide staining. The WT band occurs at ~320 bp and KO band at ~452 bp. (Fig. 1B).
Dams were removed from litters on P28, and offspring were housed 2/cage/sex. Offspring were tested for 48 h of home-cage activity on P35 and again at P50 followed by tests for anxiety, acoustic startle response, and activity with amphetamine challenge. To control for litter effects, only one rat per genotype per sex was tested per litter and selected using a random number table. Testing was by personnel blinded to the genotype. The vivarium is accredited by AAALAC International. All research followed the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Animals in Research. Protocols were approved by the Cincinnati Children’s Research Foundation Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Behavioral Tests
20 WT male, 20 WT female, 20 KO male, and 20 KO female rats from 20 litters were used. Home-cage activity was assessed on P35–36 and P50–51. On P52 rats were tested in an elevated zero maze (EZM) for 5 min. On P53 rats were tested for acoustic and tactile startle (ASR/TSR). On P60 rats were tested acutely for open-field activity before and after a challenge dose of amphetamine. Rats were later euthanized and brains dissected into hippocampus (HIP), neostriatum (STR), nucleus accumbens (NAcc), prefrontal cortex (PFC), and cerebellum (Cb) over ice and stored at −80 °C. Behavioral equipment was cleaned between subjects with Process NPD cleaner (STERIS Life Sciences, Mentor, OH) an EPA approved, non-toxic denaturing, antibacterial, antiviral solution.
Home-cage activity
Rats were singly housed for 48 h during home-cage activity testing (Tang et al., 2002). Each cage rested within a frame consisting of a 4 × 8 array of photocells spaced 5 cm apart located along the X and Y axes 2 cm above the bottom of the cage (PAS System, San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA).
Elevated Zero Maze
Anxiety-related behavior was measured using an EZM. The maze was a circular track 10 cm wide, 100 cm in diameter, and elevated 50 cm from the floor (Stoelting Co., Wood Dale, IL). The floor is made of gray texturized aluminum. The maze was divided in four quadrants of equal size with two open and two closed quadrants. Open quadrants had 1.3 cm high clear acrylic curbs to prevent falls, and the two opposing closed quadrants had 30 cm high acrylic walls with inner walls of IR transparent acrylic. Testing was performed under 8.2 lux. Rats were placed in the middle of a closed quadrant and allowed to explore for 5 min. A camera mounted above the maze was synchronized to a computer in an adjacent room with Any-maze tracking software (Stoelting Co., Wood Dale, IL).
Startle Response
Acoustic (ASR) and tactile startle responses (TSR) were measured in SR-LAB apparatus (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA). Each rat was placed in an acrylic cylindrical holder mounted on a platform with a piezoelectric accelerometer mounted underneath. The holder was placed inside a sound-attenuated chamber with fan and house light. Each session consisted of a 5 min acclimation period followed by 50 trials of each type. The acoustic pulse was a 20 ms 120 dB sound pressure level mixed frequency white noise burst (rise time 1.5 ms), and the tactile burst was a 20 ms, 50 psi air puff directed at mid-dorsum. The recording window was 100 ms. Maximum and average startle amplitude (Vmax and Vavg (both in mV)) were analyzed in blocks of 10 trials.
Open-field with amphetamine challenge
Rats were placed in 41 × 41 cm activity monitors (PAS System, San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA) with 16 photocells spaced along the X and Y planes positioned 2 cm above the floor. Rats were first habituated for 30 min to the test chamber. They were then removed and injected s.c. with saline (1 mL/kg) and further tested for another 30 min such that they were at basal levels of activity. Following this, they were removed, administered s.c. (+)-amphetamine sulfate (1 mg/kg in 1 mL/kg, expressed as the freebase and > 99% purity, Sigma-Aldrich), and tested for an additional 180 min. Activity was captured in 5-min intervals and analyzed in 10-min intervals.
Monoamines
After a one week washout period, rats were euthanized, brains dissected over ice, frozen on dry ice, and stored at −80 °C. For assay, tissue was weighed and sonicated in 0.1 N perchloric acid and centrifuged at 2100 RCF for 13 min at 4 °C. The supernatant (20 μL) was collected and loaded on a Dionex UltiMate® 3000 analytical autosampler (Thermo Scientific) for injection into a high performance liquid chromatograph (HPLC) with an electrochemical detector (ECD). The HPLC-ECD system consisted of an ESA 5840 pump, an ESA 5020 Guard Cell, a Supelco Supelcosil™ LC-18 column (15 cm × 4.6 mm, 3 μm; Sigma-Aldrich Co.) and a Coulochem III ECD (Thermo Fisher Scientific). The pump flow rate was 0.5 mL/min at 28 °C. Potential for the guard cell was +350 mV and the Coulochem III potential settings were −150 mV for E1 and +250 mV for E2. Commercially available MD-TM mobile phase (Thermo Fisher Scientific) consisting of 89% water, 10% acetonitrile, and 1% sodium phosphate monobasic (monohydrate) with pH = 3 was used. Standards for norepinephrine (NE), DA, 5-HT, 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC), 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), and homovanillic acid (HVA) were prepared in 0.1 N perchloric acid. All neurotransmitter standards were run on a single chromatogram as well as individually for peak verification. Neurotransmitter standards were run with serial dilutions in order to produce a standard curve.
Quantitative PCR
RNA was isolated from STR and PFC from 8 KO and 8 WT male rats using RNAqueous-Micro (ThermoFisher Scientific) following the manufacturer’s instructions. RNA was isolated using 1 mL of TRIzol for every 50–100 mg of tissue. RNA was quantified by Nanodrop (Thermo Scientific). Reverse transcription (RT) reactions were performed using 4 μL of iScript at room temperature with 1 μg-1 pg of RNA template (Bio-Rad) in a final volume of 20 μL. PCR reactions were carried out as follows: 5 min at 25 °C, 20 min at 46 °C, and 1 min at 95 °C. Quantitative PCR (qPCR) samples contained 160 ng of cDNA, 300 nM of each primer (forward and reverse), and 1x SYBR Green Master Mix (BioRad) in a 20 μL volume. Two 20 μL aliquots of the mix were placed in a 96-well plate and the qPCR was performed on an 7500 Real Time PCR System (Applied Biosystems) using the following conditions: 50 °C for 2 min, 95 °C for 10 min, 50 cycles at 95 °C for 15 s, and 60 °C for 1 min. Primers were synthesized by Integrated DNA Technologies (Coralville, IA) and selected based on primer efficiency at 95–100%. Rat primer sequences are shown in Table 1. Ct values were determined using Applied Biosystems 7500 System Sequence detection software (v2.4) with a threshold set at 0.5. The average Ct values from duplicates assayed were calculated. Changes in mRNA were measured with the ΔΔCt method (Livak and Schmittgen, 2001) using actin as the reference and the Lphn3 WT rat samples as calibrator.
Table 1.
Gene | Primer Sequence (5’->3’) |
---|---|
Lphn1 | CTCTGCTACCCAAGGCCTGA |
CCACACACTGGGTTCGGTTA | |
Lphn2 | TCTGGTACACAGAGCCGTA |
GGGGTCGAAGAAGAGGGTTT | |
Lphn3 | ACCCCATTGAGCTACGCTGT |
ATCTTGTCATCTGTCCTCCCG | |
Flrt3 | TCTTCCTGGAGGTGCTCAGTC |
TCATGGTCAGCAGTGTTGAGG |
Western Blots
Western blots were used to confirm LPHN3 deletion and analyze DA markers in STR from 8 WT and 8 KO female rats; actin was used as a reference (Fig. 3D–G). Frozen tissue was homogenized in radioimmuno-precipitation assay buffer (25 mM Tris, 150 mM NaCl, 0.5% sodium deoxychlorate, and 1% Triton X-100 adjusted to 7.2 pH with protease inhibitor (Pierce Biotechnology, Rockford, IL). Protein was quantified using the BCA™ Protein Assay Kit (Pierce Biotechnology, Rockford, IL) and diluted to 3 μg/μL. Western blots were performed using LI-COR Odyssey® (LI-COR Biosciences, Lincoln, NE) procedures. Briefly, 25 μL of sample was mixed with Laemmli buffer (Sigma, USA) and loaded on a 12% gel (Bio-Rad Laboratories, Hercules, CA) and run at 200 V for 35 min in running buffer (25 mM Tris, 192 mM glycine, 0.1% sodium dodecyl sulfate (SDS)). The gel was transferred to Immobilon-FL transfer membrane (Millipore, USA) in 1X rapid transfer buffer (AMRESCO, Solon, OH) at 40 V for 1.5 h. Membranes were soaked in Odyssey phosphate buffered saline blocking buffer for 1 h and incubated overnight at 4 °C with primary antibody in blocking buffer with 0.2% Tween 20. Membranes were incubated with secondary antibody in blocking buffer (0.2% Tween 20 and 0.01% SDS) for 1 h at room temperature. Antibodies were mouse anti-LPHN3 (SC-393576, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Dallas, TX) at 1:500, and 1:15,000 rabbit anti-actin (926–42210, LI-COR Biosciences, Lincoln, NE). Odyssey IRDye 680 secondary antibody was used at a 1:15,000 dilution (LI-COR Biosciences, Lincoln, NE). Rabbit anti-tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) (Ab112, AbCam, Cambridge, MA) at 1:1,000 with Odyssey IRDye 800 secondary body used at a 1:2,000 dilution. Rabbit anti-dopamine transporter (DAT) (Ab184451, AbCam, Cambridge, MA) at 1:2,000 with Odyssey IRDye 800 secondary antibody at 1:20,000 dilution. Rabbit anti-dopamine receptor D1 (DRD1) (Ab40653, AbCam, Cambridge, MA) at 1:1,000 with Odyssey IRDye 800 secondary antibody at 1:3,000 dilution. Rabbit anti-dopamine receptor D2 (DRD2) (Ab85367, AbCam, Cambridge, MA) at 1:500 with Odyssey IRDye 800 secondary antibody at 1:3,000 dilution. Rabbit anti-dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) (SC-33661, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Dallas, TX) at 1:500, and 1:15,000 rabbit anti-actin (926–42210, LI-COR Biosciences, Lincoln, NE) with Odyssey IRDye 680 secondary antibody at 1:5,000 dilution. Rabbit anti-DOPA decarboxylase (aromatic amino acid decarboxylase) antibody (Ab3905, AbCam, Cambridge, MA) at 1:1,000 with Odyssey IRDye 800 secondary antibody at 1:2,000 dilution. Rabbit anti-DARPP32 (Ab40801, AbCam, Cambridge, MA) at 1:2,000 with Odyssey IRDye 800 secondary antibody at 1:4,000 dilution. Mouse anti-actin (Ab3280, AbCam, Cambridge, MA) at 1:2000 with Odyssey IRDye 680 at a 1:15,000 secondary antibody as a loading control. Because of the known interactions between DA and NMDA neurons, western blot analyses were performed for subunits of the NMDA receptors (de Bartolomeis et al., 2014). Rabbit anti-NMDA receptor 1 (NR1) (Ab109182, AbCam, Cambridge, MA) at 1:4000 with Odyssey IRDye 800 secondary antibody at 1:3,000 dilution. Rabbit anti-NMDA receptor 2A (NR2A) (Ab124913, AbCam, Cambridge, MA) at 1:9,000 with Odyssey IRDye 800 secondary antibody at 1:20,000 dilution. Rabbit anti-NMDA receptor 2B (NR2B) (Ab81271, AbCam, Cambridge, MA) at 1:5,000 with Odyssey IRDye 800 secondary antibody at 1:20,000 dilution. Rabbit anti-vesicular monoamine transporter 2 (AB1598P, Millipore, Temecula, CA) at 1:1,000 with Odyssey IRDye 800 secondary antibody at 1:2,000 dilution. Relative protein levels were quantified using the LI-COR Odyssey® scanner and Image Studio software for fluorescent intensity of each sample normalized to actin.
Experimental Design and Statistical Analysis
Data were analyzed using general linear model ANOVAs (SAS, v9.3, SAS Institute, Cary, NC) with p ≤ 0.05 as the threshold for significance. To control for litter effects, only one rat per genotype per sex per litter was used, and litter was used as a random factor in ANOVAs. Two-factor mixed model ANOVAs were used when between-subject factors were genotype and sex with Kenward-Rogers degrees of freedom. Repeated measure (RM)-ANOVAs for activity with time or weight with day as repeated measure factors used autoregressive moving average RM-ANOVA (ARMA(1,1) mixed models with first order Kenward-Roger degrees of freedom. T-tests for independent samples were used for qPCR and western data. Open-field activity was log transformed to normalize the data.
Results
Lphn3 KO characteristics
Lphn3 KO rats showed no apparent differences in gross appearance, survival, grooming, and had no outward signs of differences compared with WT rats. Lphn3 KO male rats had reduced weight gain that was significant by P28 and remained lower through the end of the experiment at P91 (Fig. 2A). KO females had less weight gain than WT females from P21-P84, but showed no differences at P91 [Genotype: (F(1,72)=57.24, p<0.0001); Day: (F(13,832)=658.54, p<0.0001); Genotype x Day: (F(13,832)=5.19, p<0.0001); Sex: (F(1,84.3)=196.21, p<0.0001); Sex x Genotype: (F(1,69.1)=6.53, p<0.0128); Sex x Day: (F(13,832)=46.20, p<0.0001); Genotype x Sex x Day: (13,832)=2.36, p<0.0042)] (Fig. 2B).
mRNA and protein analysis
Deletion of exon 3 resulted in absence of Lphn3 mRNA expression in HIP [t(7.1) = −2.6, p=0.01], STR [t(7.0) = −5.41, p=0.0001], and PFC [t(7.0)= −2.21, p=0.04] (Fig. 3A–C) of KO rats compared with WT littermates. Within the HIP, Lphn1 mRNA was reduced [t(12.5) = −2.56, p=0.02] (Fig. 3A). No changes were found for Lphn2 or Flrt3 in HIP] or for Lphn1, Lphn2, or Flrt3 in STR (Fig. 3B) or PFC (Fig. 3C).
LPHN3 protein was absent in STR of KO rats [t(10)=5.0, p=0.0001] (Fig. 3D), whereas there were no changes in LPNH1 (Fig. 3E), LPHN2 (Fig. 3F) or FLRT3 (Fig. 3G) compared with WT rats.
DA- and NMDA-related proteins were examined in the STR. TH [t(17) = 2.3, p=0.034) (Fig. 4A)], AADC [t(10)=2.39, p=0.0383 (Fig. 4B)], and DAT [t(9.63)=3.14, p<0.01 (Fig. 4C)] were significantly increased, whereas DRD1 was reduced (Fig. 4D) [t(22) = −2.41, p<0.025] in KO rats compared with WT rats. DARPP-32 also showed a significant reduction [t(13.23)= −3.89, p<0.002] (Fig. G) in Lphn3 KO rats compared with WT rats. No differences were seen in DRD2, DRD4, VMAT-2, NMDA-NR1, NMDA-NR2A, or NMDA-NR2B between KO and WT rats (Fig. 4E,F,H,I,J,K).
Home-cage activity
At P35, KO male and female rats exhibited hyperactivity compared with WT rats (Fig. 5A,B) [P35: Genotype: (F(1,340) = 57.42, P=0.0001)]. Other significant outcomes were Sex: (p< 0.0091) females were more active than males; Interval: (p< 0.0001); and Genotype x Interval: (p<0.0001). At P50 a similar pattern was seen: Genotype was significant (F(1,264)= 16.63, p=0.0001). Other significant outcomes were Sex: (p<0.0001) females were more active than males, Interval (p<0.0001); Genotype x Interval (p<0.0001), and Sex x Interval: (p<0.0001). Hyperactivity appeared during the first diurnal period in KO rats, then declined to WT levels before reemerging during the dark cycle. During the second light cycle hyperactivity in KO rats continued but gradually declined to WT levels before the start of the second dark cycle when the hyperactivity emerged again, and this continued throughout the dark period and gradually declined again during the third light period.
Elevated Zero Maze
There was no main effect of genotype, but there was a main effect of Sex: (F(1,69.5)= 8.73, p=0.0043), females spent more time in the open than males. There was an interaction of Genotype x Sex: (F(1,50.9)= 8.21, p= 0.006), and the genotype effect was only in females with the KO females in the open more than the WT females: (p=0.0021); no differences were seen in males (Fig. 6A).
ASR/TSR
The KO rats had an increased ASR compared with the WT rats [Genotype: (F(1,62.1)=30.46, p= 0.0001)] (Fig. 6B); Block was also significant (p<0.0001).
There was no genotype main effect for TSR but there was a significant Genotype x Block: (F(4,295)=2.74, p=0.0291). KO rats were more reactive on all blocks compared with WT rats, but only block-5 was significant (Fig. 6C).
Locomotor with Amphetamine Challenge
During habituation, KO rats showed increased central and peripheral open-field activity compared with WT littermates, i.e., for central activity the Genotype main effect was significant (F(1,32.8)=11.83, p<0.01). There was also a Genotype x Sex interaction (F(1,32.8)=8.39, p<0.01). Slice-effect ANOVAs by sex showed that the effects were significant for females (p<0.0001 (Fig. 7A)) but not for males (Fig. 7C). Female KO rats were more active than WT females. For peripheral activity the main effect of genotype was not significant but the genotype x sex interaction was (F(1,32.4) = 4.83, p<0.05; Fig. 7B & D). However, slice-effect ANOVAs on each sex both fell short of significance (females p=0.11; males p=0.15).
For central activity after saline injection, Genotype (F(1,32.2)=13.49, p<0.001), Genotype x Sex (F(1,32.2)=7.11, p<0.02), and Genotype x Interval (F(2,44.2)=5.25, p<0.01) were significant. Slice-effect ANOVAs on each sex showed the effect was significant in females (p<0.0001) but not in males. Slice-effect ANOVAs on each interval showed effects in females at all intervals but no difference between males (Fig. 7A,C). For peripheral activity, the main effect of genotype was not significant but there was a genotype x sex interaction trend (F(1,31.2)=3.54, p=0.069; Fig. 7B, D).
After amphetamine all groups showed an increase in activity. For central activity, there was a main effect trend (F(1,40.6)=2.69, p=0.10) and a significant genotype x sex interaction (F(1,40.6)=5.65, p<0.03). Slice-effect ANOVAs by sex showed a significant effect in females (p<0.01; Fig. 7A) but not males (Fig. 7C). Female KO rats were more active than WT females, a difference not seen in males. For peripheral activity, the main effect of genotype was not significant but there was a genotype x sex interaction (F(1,41.7)=9.18, p<0.01). Slice-effect ANOVAs by sex showed a significant effect in females (F(1,41.7)=10.34, p<0.01; Fig. 7B) but not in males (Fig. 7D). Female KO rats were more active than female WT rats.
When activity was analyzed as a percent of pre-drug baseline, there was a significant main effect of genotype (F(1,35.6)=4.98, p<0.05). The effect is shown in Fig. 8 by sex for consistency with the data in Fig. 7. The main effect is shown by the bracket connecting males and females (Fig. 8A,C). KO rats had less central activity relative to their higher pre-drug baseline compared with WT rats that had more central activity relative to their lower pre-drug baseline. For peripheral activity percent of pre-drug baseline, the main effect of genotype was not significant however the genotype x sex interaction was (F(1,36.5)=4.08, p=0.05). Slice-effect ANOVAs by sex showed a significant effect in females (p<0.05; Fig. 8B) but not in males (Fig. 8D). Interestingly, while the KO female central activity was less than WT females relative to their baselines, the KO females had more peripheral activity than WT females relative to their baselines (cf., Fig. 8A vs. 8B). For males, KO male post-drug relative change in activity was less in both the central and peripheral regions compared with WT males. This unusual pattern will be the subject of future experiments.
Monoamines
Monoamine and major metabolites were assayed in PFC, STR, NAcc, HIP, and Cb. No genotype-related differences were seen for NE, DOPAC, DA, 5-HIAA, HVA, or 5-HT in any region (Table 2).
Table 2.
PFC | ||
Monoamine | WT | KO |
NE | 317.6 (24.3) | 282.8 (23.8) |
5-HIAA | 1368.8 (95.9) | 1378.7 (97.5) |
5-HT | 1510 (22.5) | 1408.8 (22.6) |
STR | ||
WT | KO | |
NE | 109.6 (14.0) | 114.9 (13.7) |
DOPAC | 1404.2 (181.1) | 1503.5 (173.9) |
DA | 4706.1 (625.4) | 4185.7 (614.1) |
5-HIAA | 1103.6 (78.4) | 989.2 (77.3) |
HVA | 439.2 (51.1) | 526.9 (53.2) |
5-HT | 803.0 (76.3) | 688.5 (72.6) |
NAcc | ||
WT | KO | |
NE | 1140.8 (168.6) | 1159.5 (157.7) |
DOPAC | 951.1 (172.8) | 1038.0 (164.7) |
DA | 2318.8 (412.8) | 2520.3 (412.4) |
5-HIAA | 1604.7 (82.0) | 1544.4 (75.8) |
HVA | 262.7 (44.3) | 238.9 (38.6) |
5-HT | 1509.8 (160.4) | 1408.8 (157.4) |
HIP | ||
Monoamine | WT | KO |
NE | 312.14 (21.9) | 353.7 (20.0) |
5-HIAA | 866.2 (38.4) | 886.1 (36.8) |
5-HT | 464.1 (71.7) | 441.3 (95.8) |
Cb | ||
WT | KO | |
NE | 210.67 (9.7) | 180.02 (9.2) |
DOPAC | 60.4 (6.2) | 51.3 (5.6) |
5-HIAA | 179.5 (10.5) | 157.9 (10.5) |
5-HT | 113.8 (12.9) | 116.0 (12.5) |
Values are Mean (SEM) (pg/mg tissue)
WT: n=16, KO: n=16.
Discussion
Latrophilin orthologues are found in all animals with at least three homologues present in chordates (Fredriksson and Schioth, 2005; Nordstrom et al., 2009). They are also conserved in invertebrates with expression in the nervous system (Rohou et al., 2007). LPHN3 is found in insects, invertebrates, and vertebrates, yet it remains poorly understood. LPHN3 is a synaptic adhesion GPCR with an extracellular domain that binds to FLRT3 and tenurin-3 to span the synapse and has a Gq/11 intracellular domain hypothesized to regulate neurotransmitter release (Davletov et al., 1998; Sugita et al., 1998; Ichtchenko et al., 1999). There is evidence that it plays a role in brain development; its concentrations are highest early in development and gradually decline to stable adult levels (Arcos-Burgos and Muenke, 2010). There are 21 LPHN3 variants linked with ADHD, but how each contributes to symptoms is unknown. Transgenic models of LPHN3 disruption in Drosophila, zebrafish, and mouse have been reported (Lange et al., 2012; Wallis et al., 2012; Orsini et al., 2016; van der Voet et al., 2016; Lange et al., 2018). All are consistent in demonstrating that LPHN3 disruption results in hypermobility. We created a rat model to investigate this poorly understood protein and its relationship to hyperactivity. Rats have several advantages over these other species that may be useful for understanding what this protein does under normal and abnormal conditions. Rats have a greater range of behavioral capacities than mice and may ultimately be used to investigate other attributes of ADHD, such as inattention, impulsivity, and cognitive effects. As the first report of the rat KO of LPHN3, the objective here was first to establish the hyperactive phenotype and the rat KO shows several features not seen in the most closely related model, the Lphn3 KO mouse. For example, the KO rat was hyperactive in two different familiar environments, home-cage and after habituation to an open-field. This was not shown for the KO mouse. The KO rat is hyper-reactive as seen in tests of acoustic startle, the mouse KO has no report on acoustic startle. The rat KO, mostly in females, shows a reduction in activity after challenged with a sympathomimetic stimulant, amphetamine, relative to its pre-drug activity level, unlike WT controls that do not show such a pattern. By contrast, the KO mouse shows exaggerated hyperactivity in response to the sympathomimetic stimulant, cocaine. Female KO rats also showed increased time-in-open in the elevated zero-maze test of anxiety-like behavior without an increase in number of quadrant entries suggesting that the effect was not a byproduct of being hyperactive.
Additionally, KO rats showed changes in striatal markers of dopamine regulation. These included increased TH, AADC, and DAT accompanied by decreased DRD1 and DARPP32 by western analysis, with no changes in related gene expression or protein levels for Lphn1/LPHN1, Lphn2/LPHN2, or Flrt3/FLRT3 in striatum. Nor was gene expression of these factors changed in PFC or for Lphn2 or Flrt3 in hippocampus with a minor reduction in Lphn1 in hippocampus, demonstrating the selectivity of the rat KO while not affecting related gene or proteins.
We also obtained all rats for testing using heterozygotes crossing to avoid issues from null x null vs. WT x WT crosses. We controlled for litter effects by selecting only one male and one female of each genotype from each litter (Crusio, 2004; Crusio et al., 2009) and also used litter as a random factor in ANOVA mixed linear models to ensure litter contributions were accounted for in the statistical analyses (Holson and Pearce, 1992; Lazic and Essioux, 2013; Williams et al., 2017).
The Lphn3 KO rat is not a model of ADHD per se because in humans LPHN3 variants lead to reduced gene expression, not a null genotype (Domene et al., 2011a; Martinez et al., 2016). Our objective was to use a loss-of-function model to better understand the function of LPHN3. Given that LPHN3 was suggested to have effects on activity, we assessed locomotor activity in familiar environments (hyperactivity is a characteristic of ADHD in familiar environments) and after a challenge dose with amphetamine (a drug used to treat ADHD), and we found effects consistent with ADHD. Because ADHD is associated with changes in DA signaling and because there is cross-talk between DA and NMDA-Rs we also examined NMDA-NR1, -NR2A, and –NR2B in striatum and found no differences in KO rats. But, we saw no changes in levels of DA, NE, 5-HT or their major metabolites by HPLC-ECD in relevant brain regions (STR, HIP, or PFC).
Lphn3 KO rats of both sexes were hyperactive. This was most pronounced during the dark phase in the home-cage test. The hyperactivity was present at P35 and at P50. In an open-field even after an hour of no-treatment and saline-treatment habituation, KO rats remained hyperactive. After amphetamine, however, complex changes were seen in males versus females. The basis of these differential responses are not yet known and require further investigation. Interestingly, children with ADHD also show a decline in hyperactivity relative to their level before treatment (Labbe et al., 2012; Briars and Todd, 2016) as do the KO rats.
Sex differences in ADHD prevalence in humans are well documented. ADHD affects 5–7% of the school age children, with a male to female ratio of ~3:1 (Willcutt, 2012; Arnett et al., 2015). Males are more often diagnosed with the combined or impulsive subtype of ADHD, whereas females are more often diagnosed with the inattentive subtype (Gaub and Carlson, 1997). One hypothesis to explain these differences is that females internalize and become more easily distracted whereas males with ADHD are hyperactive and impulsive because they tend toward externalized behavior (Arnold, 1996; Biederman et al., 1999; Quinn, 2008).
Combining the behavioral and neurochemical data suggest that increased DA synthesis may result in DA overflow leading to DA-mediated hyperlocomotion. If DA release in STR is increased, this would be consistent with the altered response to amphetamine. Hence, the data point toward LPHN3 having a role in striatal DA regulation. These effects are in general agreement with the mouse, Drosophila, and zebrafish models of disrupted brain LPHN3 and with the human reduction in LPHN3 expression in some children with ADHD.
While both the rat and mouse KO models show hyperactivity, they also differ. The Lphn3 KO mouse shows increased levels of brain DA, NE, and 5-HT whereas the KO rat does not. The KO mouse had no working memory or discrimination deficits in an operant task, but showed higher lever press rates on a high FR schedule (40:1); we have not tested the KO rat for schedule controlled behavior. KO mice show less immobility in a forced swim test, a behavior we did not test in KO rats but may also reflect the KO hyperactivity effect. In primary neuronal culture, KO mice showed increased neurite outgrowth, reduced transcripts for cell adhesion and calcium signaling proteins, consistent with LPHN3 being an adhesion protein, we have not examined these factors.
The largest effect in LPHN3 KO rats was nocturnal hyperactivity; this was seen at both adolescent and adult ages, demonstrating persistence. ADHD patients show hyperactivity in familiar settings (Sagvolden and Sergeant, 1998) and as KO rats habituated to the home-cage environment their hyperactivity dissipated at first, then re-emerged with a delayed onset of about 6 h during the dark cycle. Rodent activity typically peaks 5–6 h into the night phase (Zoratto et al., 2013) and this peak was heightened in KO rats. Once it emerged, hyperactivity continued for the remainder of the dark cycle in KO rats, then dissipated again during the next light period and reemerged 6 h into the second dark period.
Lphn3 KO rats showed facilitated acoustic and tactile startle. Appropriate filtering of stimuli is a critical component of attention. To further assess this, we plan to assess prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle, a measure of sensorimotor gating in follow-up experiments. Patients with ADHD report being flooded with sensory stimuli, an effect that can cause increased reactivity (Faraone et al., 2000), and there are data indicating sensory processing deficits in those with ADHD (Durukan et al., 2008; Holstein et al., 2013; Micoulaud-Franchi et al., 2015).
Dysregulated monoaminergic systems are seen in ADHD and treatment of ADHD with psychostimulants that increase or prolong catecholamine release provide clinical benefit (Zhu and Reith, 2008; dela Pena et al., 2015). Abnormal monoamine synthesis is also reported in patients with ADHD (Ernst et al., 1998; Ernst et al., 1999), consistent with our KO rat finding of increased TH and AADC levels (Cook et al., 1995; LaHoste et al., 1996; Gill et al., 1997).
ADHD is a polygenic disorder, with multiple small effect genes contributing in yet to be defined combinations along with unknown environmental contributions. Moreover, ADHD is a set of traits along a continuum (Akutagava-Martins et al., 2016) and cannot be modeled by any single-gene effect. However, genes that confer ADHD risk need to be disentangled and this can be done by dissecting the effects of each genetic contribution. LPHN3 is not a dominant variant but contributes to a subset of ADHD cases in conjunction with other gene variants. Hence, Lphn3 variants confer incremental risk but are not a cause of ADHD. Nevertheless, LPHN3 may be a potential target for drug development for those that carry an LPHN3 risk allele.
HIGHLIGHTS.
We knocked-out Lphn3 in rats to study its function in the brain
Human studies link LPHN3 variants to Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
KO rats are hyperactive, hyper-reactive, and reduce activity after amphetamine
Lphn3 KO rats show no mRNA or protein compensation of Lphn1, Lphn2, or Flrt3
KO rats show higher striatal TH, DAT, and AADC, and lower DRD1 and DARPP32 than WT
Acknowledgement
This research was supported by a grant from the L.I.F.E. Foundation and NIH T32 ES007051 (E.M.P.) and these institutional shared services: Transgenic Animal and Genome Editing Core and Animal Behavior Core, Cincinnati Children’s Research Foundation.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Acosta MT, Velez JI, Bustamante ML, Balog JZ, Arcos-Burgos M, Muenke M (2011) A two-locus genetic interaction between LPHN3 and 11q predicts ADHD severity and long-term outcome. Transl Psychiatry 1:e17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Akutagava-Martins GC, Rohde LA, Hutz MH (2016) Genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: an update. Expert review of neurotherapeutics 16:145–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arcos-Burgos M, Muenke M (2010) Toward a better understanding of ADHD: LPHN3 gene variants and the susceptibility to develop ADHD. Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorders 2:139–147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arcos-Burgos M et al. (2019) ADGRL3 (LPHN3) variants predict substance use disorder. Translational Psychiatry 9:42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arcos-Burgos M et al. (2010a) A common variant of the latrophilin 3 gene, LPHN3, confers susceptibility to ADHD and predicts effectiveness of stimulant medication. Molecular psychiatry 15:1053–1066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arcos-Burgos M et al. (2010b) A common variant of the latrophilin 3 gene, LPHN3, confers susceptibility to ADHD and predicts effectiveness of stimulant medication. Mol Psychiatry 15:1053–1066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnett AB, Pennington BF, Willcutt EG, DeFries JC, Olson RK (2015) Sex differences in ADHD symptom severity. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines 56:632–639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnold LE (1996) Sex differences in ADHD: conference summary. Journal of abnormal child psychology 24:555–569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biederman J, Faraone SV, Mick E, Williamson S, Wilens TE, Spencer TJ, Weber W, Jetton J, Kraus I, Pert J, Zallen B (1999) Clinical correlates of ADHD in females: findings from a large group of girls ascertained from pediatric and psychiatric referral sources. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 38:966–975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briars L, Todd T (2016) A Review of Pharmacological Management of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. The journal of pediatric pharmacology and therapeutics : JPPT : the official journal of PPAG 21:192–206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook EH Jr., Stein MA, Krasowski MD, Cox NJ, Olkon DM, Kieffer JE, Leventhal BL (1995) Association of attention-deficit disorder and the dopamine transporter gene. American journal of human genetics 56:993–998. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crusio WE (2004) Flanking gene and genetic background problems in genetically manipulated mice. Biol Psychiatry 56:381–385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crusio WE, Goldowitz D, Holmes A, Wolfer D (2009) Standards for the publication of mouse mutant studies. Genes Brain Behav 8:1–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davletov BA, Meunier FA, Ashton AC, Matsushita H, Hirst WD, Lelianova VG, Wilkin GP, Dolly JO, Ushkaryov YA (1998) Vesicle exocytosis stimulated by alpha-latrotoxin is mediated by latrophilin and requires both external and stored Ca2+. The EMBO journal 17:3909–3920. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Bartolomeis A, Buonaguro EF, Iasevoli F, Tomasetti C (2014) The emerging role of dopamine-glutamate interaction and of the postsynaptic density in bipolar disorder pathophysiology: Implications for treatment. J Psychopharmacol 28:505–526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- dela Pena I, Gevorkiana R, Shi WX (2015) Psychostimulants affect dopamine transmission through both dopamine transporter-dependent and independent mechanisms. European journal of pharmacology 764:562–570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demontis D et al. (2018) Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat Genet. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demontis D et al. (2019) Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat Genet 51:63–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domene S, Stanescu H, Wallis D, Tinloy B, Pineda DE, Kleta R, Arcos-Burgos M, Roessler E, Muenke M (2011a) Screening of human LPHN3 for variants with a potential impact on ADHD susceptibility. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 156b:11–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domene S, Stanescu H, Wallis D, Tinloy B, Pineda DE, Kleta R, Arcos-Burgos M, Roessler E, Muenke M (2011b) Screening of human LPHN3 for variants with a potential impact on ADHD susceptibility. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 156B:11–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durukan I, Turkbay T, Congologlu A (2008) [The effects of methylphenidate on various components of visual attention in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder]. Turk psikiyatri dergisi = Turkish journal of psychiatry 19:358–364. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ernst M, Zametkin AJ, Matochik JA, Jons PH, Cohen RM (1998) DOPA decarboxylase activity in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder adults. A [fluorine-18]fluorodopa positron emission tomographic study. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 18:5901–5907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ernst M, Zametkin AJ, Matochik JA, Pascualvaca D, Jons PH, Cohen RM (1999) High midbrain [18F]DOPA accumulation in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The American journal of psychiatry 156:1209–1215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faraone SV, Biederman J, Spencer T, Wilens T, Seidman LJ, Mick E, Doyle AE (2000) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: an overview. Biological psychiatry 48:9–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franke B, Faraone SV, Asherson P, Buitelaar J, Bau CH, Ramos-Quiroga JA, Mick E, Grevet EH, Johansson S, Haavik J, Lesch KP, Cormand B, Reif A (2012) The genetics of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults, a review. Molecular psychiatry 17:960–987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredriksson R, Schioth HB (2005) The repertoire of G-protein-coupled receptors in fully sequenced genomes. Mol Pharmacol 67:1414–1425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaub M, Carlson CL (1997) Gender differences in ADHD: a meta-analysis and critical review. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 36:1036–1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gill M, Daly G, Heron S, Hawi Z, Fitzgerald M (1997) Confirmation of association between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and a dopamine transporter polymorphism. Molecular psychiatry 2:311–313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holson RR, Pearce B (1992) Principles and pitfalls in the analysis of prenatal treatment effects in multiparous species. Neurotoxicol Teratol 14:221–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holstein DH, Vollenweider FX, Geyer MA, Csomor PA, Belser N, Eich D (2013) Sensory and sensorimotor gating in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Psychiatry research 205:117–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang X, Zhang Q, Gu X, Hou Y, Wang M, Chen X, Wu J (2018) LPHN3 gene variations and susceptibility to ADHD in Chinese Han population: a two-stage case-control association study and gene-environment interactions. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Hwang IW, Lim MH, Kwon HJ, Jin HJ (2015) Association of LPHN3 rs6551665 A/G polymorphism with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder in Korean children. Gene 566:68–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ichtchenko K, Bittner MA, Krasnoperov V, Little AR, Chepurny O, Holz RW, Petrenko AG (1999) A novel ubiquitously expressed alpha-latrotoxin receptor is a member of the CIRL family of G-protein-coupled receptors. The Journal of biological chemistry 274:5491–5498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Labbe A, Liu A, Atherton J, Gizenko N, Fortier ME, Sengupta SM, Ridha J (2012) Refining psychiatric phenotypes for response to treatment: contribution of LPHN3 in ADHD. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 159B:776–785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaHoste GJ, Swanson JM, Wigal SB, Glabe C, Wigal T, King N, Kennedy JL (1996) Dopamine D4 receptor gene polymorphism is associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Molecular psychiatry 1:121–124. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lange M, Froc C, Grunwald H, Norton WHJ, Bally-Cuif L (2018) Pharmacological analysis of zebrafish lphn3.1 morphant larvae suggests that saturated dopaminergic signaling could underlie the ADHD-like locomotor hyperactivity. Progress in neuro-psychopharmacology & biological psychiatry 84:181–189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lange M, Norton W, Coolen M, Chaminade M, Merker S, Proft F, Schmitt A, Vernier P, Lesch KP, Bally-Cuif L (2012) The ADHD-susceptibility gene lphn3.1 modulates dopaminergic neuron formation and locomotor activity during zebrafish development. Mol Psychiatry 17:946–954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lazic SE, Essioux L (2013) Improving basic and translational science by accounting for litter-to-litter variation in animal models. BMC Neurosci 14:37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Livak KJ, Schmittgen TD (2001) Analysis of relative gene expression data using real-time quantitative PCR and the 2(-Delta Delta C(T)) Method. Methods (San Diego, Calif) 25:402–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez AF, Muenke M, Arcos-Burgos M (2011) From the black widow spider to human behavior: Latrophilins, a relatively unknown class of G protein-coupled receptors, are implicated in psychiatric disorders. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 156B:1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez AF, Abe Y, Hong S, Molyneux K, Yarnell D, Löhr H, Driever W, Acosta MT, Arcos-Burgos M, Muenke M (2016) An Ultraconserved Brain-Specific Enhancer Within ADGRL3 (LPHN3) Underpins Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Susceptibility. Biological psychiatry 80:943–954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Micoulaud-Franchi JA, Lopez R, Vaillant F, Richieri R, El-Kaim A, Bioulac S, Philip P, Boyer L, Lancon C (2015) Perceptual abnormalities related to sensory gating deficit are core symptoms in adults with ADHD. Psychiatry research 230:357–363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nordstrom KJ, Lagerstrom MC, Waller LM, Fredriksson R, Schioth HB (2009) The Secretin GPCRs descended from the family of Adhesion GPCRs. Mol Biol Evol 26:71–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Sullivan ML, Martini F, von Daake S, Comoletti D, Ghosh A (2014) LPHN3, a presynaptic adhesion-GPCR implicated in ADHD, regulates the strength of neocortical layer 2/3 synaptic input to layer 5. Neural Dev 9:7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olincy A, Ross RG, Harris JG, Young DA, McAndrews MA, Cawthra E, McRae KA, Sullivan B, Adler LE, Freedman R (2000) The P50 auditory event-evoked potential in adult attention-deficit disorder: comparison with schizophrenia. Biological psychiatry 47:969–977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orsini CA, Setlow B, DeJesus M, Galaviz S, Loesch K, Ioerger T, Wallis D (2016) Behavioral and transcriptomic profiling of mice null for Lphn3, a gene implicated in ADHD and addiction. Mol Genet Genomic Med 4:322–343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quinn PO (2008) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and its comorbidities in women and girls: an evolving picture. Curr Psychiatry Rep 10:419–423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ranaivoson FM, Liu Q, Martini F, Bergami F, von Daake S, Li S, Lee D, Demeler B, Hendrickson WA, Comoletti D (2015) Structural and Mechanistic Insights into the Latrophilin3-FLRT3 Complex that Mediates Glutamatergic Synapse Development. Structure 23:1665–1677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ribases M, Ramos-Quiroga JA, Sanchez-Mora C, Bosch R, Richarte V, Palomar G, Gastaminza X, Bielsa A, Arcos-Burgos M, Muenke M, Castellanos FX, Cormand B, Bayes M, Casas M (2011) Contribution of LPHN3 to the genetic susceptibility to ADHD in adulthood: a replication study. Genes Brain Behav 10:149–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rohou A, Nield J, Ushkaryov YA (2007) Insecticidal toxins from black widow spider venom. Toxicon 49:531–549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell VA, Sagvolden T, Johansen EB (2005) Animal models of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behav Brain Funct 1:9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sagvolden T, Sergeant JA (1998) Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder--from brain dysfunctions to behaviour. Behav Brain Res 94:1–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sagvolden T, Aase H, Zeiner P, Berger D (1998) Altered reinforcement mechanisms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behavioural brain research 94:61–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sando R, Jiang X, Sudhof TC (2019) Latrophilin GPCRs direct synapse specificity by coincident binding of FLRTs and teneurins. Science 363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott MA, Hu YC (2019) Generation of CRISPR-Edited Rodents Using a Piezo-Driven Zygote Injection Technique. Methods in molecular biology (Clifton, NJ) 1874:169–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silva JP, Ushkaryov YA (2010) The latrophilins, “split-personality” receptors. Adv Exp Med Biol 706:59–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg EA, Drabick DA (2015) A Developmental Psychopathology Perspective on ADHD and Comorbid Conditions: The Role of Emotion Regulation. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 46:951–966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sugita S, Ichtchenko K, Khvotchev M, Sudhof TC (1998) alpha-Latrotoxin receptor CIRL/latrophilin 1 (CL1) defines an unusual family of ubiquitous G-protein-linked receptors. G-protein coupling not required for triggering exocytosis. The Journal of biological chemistry 273:32715–32724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang X, Orchard SM, Sanford LD (2002) Home cage activity and behavioral performance in inbred and hybrid mice. Behavioural brain research 136:555–569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tung I, Li JJ, Meza JI, Jezior KL, Kianmahd JS, Hentschel PG, O’Neil PM, Lee SS (2016) Patterns of Comorbidity Among Girls With ADHD: A Meta-analysis. Pediatrics 138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Voet M, Harich B, Franke B, Schenck A (2016) ADHD-associated dopamine transporter, latrophilin and neurofibromin share a dopamine-related locomotor signature in Drosophila. Mol Psychiatry 21:565–573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vorhees CV, He E, Skelton MR, Graham DL, Schaefer TL, Grace CE, Braun AA, Amos-Kroohs R, Williams MT (2011) Comparison of (+)-methamphetamine, +/−-methylenedioxymethamphetamine, (+)-amphetamine and +/−-fenfluramine in rats on egocentric learning in the Cincinnati water maze. Synapse (New York, NY) 65:368–378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallis D, Hill DS, Mendez IA, Abbott LC, Finnell RH, Wellman PJ, Setlow B (2012) Initial characterization of mice null for Lphn3, a gene implicated in ADHD and addiction. Brain research 1463:85–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willcutt EG (2012) The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics : the journal of the American Society for Experimental NeuroTherapeutics 9:490–499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams DR, Carlsson R, Burkner PC (2017) Between-litter variation in developmental studies of hormones and behavior: Inflated false positives and diminished power. Front Neuroendocrinol 47:154–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang H, Wang H, Jaenisch R (2014) Generating genetically modified mice using CRISPR/Cas-mediated genome engineering. Nat Protoc 9:1956–1968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang L, Chang S, Li Z, Zhang K, Du Y, Ott J, Wang J (2012) ADHDgene: a genetic database for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Nucleic acids research 40:D1003–1009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu J, Reith ME (2008) Role of the dopamine transporter in the action of psychostimulants, nicotine, and other drugs of abuse. CNS & neurological disorders drug targets 7:393–409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zoratto F, Tringle AL, Bellenchi G, Speranza L, Travaglini D, di Porzio U, Perrone-Capano C, Laviola G, Dreyer JL, Adriani W (2013) Impulsivity and home-cage activity are decreased by lentivirus-mediated silencing of serotonin transporter in the rat hippocampus. Neurosci Lett 548:38–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]