Abstract
Sensory-guided behaviors require the transformation of sensory information into task-specific motor commands. Prior research on sensorimotor integration has emphasized visuomotor processes in the context of simplified orienting movements in controlled laboratory tasks rather than an animal's more complete, natural behavioral repertoire. Here, we conducted a series of neural recording experiments in the midbrain superior colliculus (SC) of echolocating bats engaged in a sonar target-tracking task that invoked dynamic active sensing behaviors. We hypothesized that SC activity in freely behaving animals would reveal dynamic shifts in neural firing patterns within and across sensory, sensorimotor, and premotor layers. We recorded neural activity in the SC of freely echolocating bats (three females and one male) and replicated the general trends reported in other species with sensory responses in the dorsal divisions and premotor activity in ventral divisions of the SC. However, within this coarse functional organization, we discovered that sensory and motor neurons are comingled within layers throughout the volume of the bat SC. In addition, as the bat increased pulse rate adaptively to increase resolution of the target location with closing distance, the activity of sensory and vocal premotor neurons changed such that auditory response times decreased, and vocal premotor lead times shortened. This finding demonstrates that SC activity can be modified dynamically in concert with adaptive behaviors and suggests that an integrated functional organization within SC laminae supports rapid and local integration of sensory and motor signals for natural, adaptive behaviors.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Natural sensory-guided behaviors involve the rapid integration of information from the environment to direct flexible motor actions. The vast majority of research on sensorimotor integration has used artificial stimuli and simplified behaviors, leaving open questions about nervous system function in the context of natural tasks. Our work investigated mechanisms of dynamic sensorimotor feedback control by analyzing patterns of neural activity in the midbrain superior colliculus (SC) of an echolocating bat tracking and intercepting moving prey. Recordings revealed that sensory and motor neurons comingle within laminae of the SC to support rapid sensorimotor integration. Further, we discovered that neural activity in the bat SC changes with dynamic adaptations in the animal's echolocation behavior.
Keywords: adaptive behavior, echolocation, natural vocalizations, neuroethology, sensorimotor, superior colliculus
Introduction
As animals operate in the natural world, sensory information guides behaviors on both short and long time-scales. Sensorimotor integration supports a variety of goal-directed actions, including saccades to a visual target (Wurtz and Goldberg, 1971; McIlwain et al., 1991) or reaching for an object (Werner et al., 1997; Stuphorn et al., 1999, 2000), as well as more complex sequences of adaptive behaviors such as tracking and intercepting prey (Furigo et al., 2010; Moss and Surlykke, 2010; Wagner et al., 2013) and avoiding predators (Brandão et al., 1994; Fanselow, 1994; Kavaliers and Choleris, 2001; Comoli et al., 2012; Maior et al., 2012). To investigate the neural underpinnings of sensorimotor integration in a dynamic and natural behavioral context, we recorded neural activity chronically in the superior colliculus (SC) of an echolocating bat engaged in a sonar target-tracking task.
A wealth of data suggests that the midbrain SC plays an important role in sensorimotor integration. Across species, the functional organization of the SC reflects the importance of a particular sensory modality to an animal's natural behaviors. Early work stressed the representation of retinotopic visual space for eye movements as a model for collicular function (Schiller and Koerner, 1971; Cynader and Berman, 1972; Goldberg and Wurtz, 1972). Comparative data have documented the role of the SC in the saccadic eye movement system of primates (Sparks, 1986), in gaze-control orientation behaviors in cats and barn owls (Knudsen, 1982; Middlebrooks and Knudsen, 1984; du Lac and Knudsen, 1990; Munoz et al., 1991a), and in prey capture behaviors in pit vipers and frogs (Hartline et al., 1978; Grobstein, 1988). Neurophysiological studies of the primate and bat midbrain SC implicate this structure in auditory localization (Jay and Sparks, 1987; Valentine and Moss, 1997; Lee and Groh, 2012). Additional work in the bat has demonstrated premotor commands for sonar vocalizations, head movements, and adjustments of the pinnae (Valentine et al., 2002; Sinha and Moss, 2007) that are consistent with the notion that the SC supports species-specific orienting behaviors.
Biological sonar operates through tight coupling between sensing and action. Bats produce sounds that return echoes from objects in the environment, yielding acoustic cues for 3D localization (Griffin, 1958) The bat computes target azimuth from spectrotemporal differences of echoes at the two ears, elevation from echo spectrum, and distance from the time delay between sonar call and echo arrival (for review, see Wohlgemuth et al., 2016a). The spatial information that a bat extracts from echo features, then drives adaptive adjustments in vocal parameters, and changes in sonar vocalizations influence echo features available to the bat's sonar imaging system (Moss and Surlykke, 2010). The millisecond temporal precision of sensory and motor events for echolocation, coupled with the bat's adaptive sonar behaviors, provides a powerful system with which to investigate sensorimotor signaling in natural tasks.
In broad strokes, the canonical SC functional organization shows that neurons in dorsal layers respond largely to visual stimuli (Cynader and Berman, 1972; McIlwain, 1983; Berson and Stein, 1995), whereas intermediate layers show unimodal and multimodal sensory responses, and sensorimotor activity (May, 2006). In the more ventral layers, neurons are active before orienting movements of the body (Wurtz and Goldberg, 1971; Roucoux and Crommelinck, 1976; Ma et al., 1990; Peck, 1990). These results were primarily identified in visually guided primates and cats, and comparative studies in other species have reported more mixing of sensory and motor neurons across the dorsal–ventral axis of the SC (May, 2006). Does this organizational plan also hold for animals that rely largely on hearing to guide natural behaviors? How do natural adaptive motor behaviors influence sensorimotor signaling? We hypothesize that dynamic orienting, in which an animal must execute a complex sequence of sensorimotor behaviors (e.g., target tracking and interception), reveals a more integrated functional organization within and across laminae of the SC, in which local pools of neurons facilitate rapid sensorimotor integration.
Here, we exploited bat sonar orientation to characterize auditory and vocal premotor activity across the SC laminae in animals actively engaged in dynamic sensorimotor behaviors for prey tracking and interception. We discovered that activity patterns of sensory and vocal premotor neurons in the SC were modulated by the natural echolocation behaviors of the bat. Furthermore, both sensory and vocal premotor neurons could be characterized throughout all SC layers, providing evidence for an integrated functional organization, which supports rapid sensorimotor signaling for adaptive orienting behaviors.
Materials and Methods
Animals and behavioral training.
Four wild-caught big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus, three females and one male) served as subjects in this study. The bats were collected in the state of Maryland under a permit issued by the Department of Natural Resources and were housed in animal vivaria at the University of Maryland–College Park and Johns Hopkins University. All procedures used were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees at the University of Maryland and Johns Hopkins University, where this research was conducted.
The behavioral paradigm in the current experiment was described in detail in a previous report (Wohlgemuth et al., 2016b). Briefly, the bats were initially trained to associate a sound stimulus with the presentation of a food reward (mealworms). Once this contingency was acquired, the presentation of the food reward gradually involved greater distances traveled by the target until the bats learned to track the target from a distance of 4 m. The target's motion was controlled via a computer that interfaced with a rotary servo-motor (Aerotech Ensemble MP10 motor controller connected to a BMS60 servo motor). A loop of monofilament line was suspended on a set of four pulleys and wound around the rotary motor (Fig. 1A). A bundle of mealworms was then tethered to the monofilament line and, by driving the rotary motor, the velocity, acceleration, and direction of target travel were controlled experimentally.
The lighting in the room prevented the bats from using vision so that they instead had to rely on sonar to track the movement of the target (Hope and Bhatnagar, 1979). The target moved along the range axis, with no change in azimuth or elevation (see Fig. 1B, top, for example target motion and example vocalizations). The bats made adaptive modifications to their sonar vocalizations that were typical of a free-flying bat tracking a target closing in distance (Moss and Surlykke, 2010; Kothari et al., 2014; Wohlgemuth et al., 2016a), demonstrating decreases in pulse interval and pulse duration with decreasing target distance (Fig. 1C).
Surgical methods and physiological recordings.
Once the bats learned the behavioral task, a 16-channel silicon probe affixed to a microdrive was chronically implanted into the SC. The silicon probe had 4 shanks with a 4 × 4 grid of recording sites. Recording sites were separated by 100 μm on each shank and 100 μm between shanks. The microdrive was advanced manually through the layers of the SC during the experiment at 25 μm intervals, allowing us to finely sample SC activity at different locations along the dorsal–ventral axis. Data were collected from the SC of the four bats at 56 unique recording locations. With 16 available sites and 56 different recording locations, there were 896 unique recording locations sampled (i.e., 56 × 16 = 896), but not every site yielded neural activity. Neural data were collected with a Plexon Omniplex acquisition system; recordings of the bat's sonar vocalizations were made with a National Instruments M-series A/D board. The target motion was controlled using custom MATLAB (The MathWorks) software routines that additionally generated a TTL pulse for time synchrony across hardware systems.
Histological verification of recording sites.
At the conclusion of each experiment, the bats were perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde. The brains were then removed from the skull and submerged in a solution of 4% paraformaldehyde with 30% sucrose for cryoprotection. Once the brains were cryoprotected, they were embedded in Optimal cutting temperature compound, frozen, and 50 μm coronal sections were cut on a cryostat. The sections were then mounted on slides and stained with cresyl violet to image the location of the silicon probe tracts. Delineation of the SC layers was confirmed by cross-referencing with previous histological reconstructions of the midbrain (Covey et al., 1987; May, 2006; Big Brown Bat Stereotaxis Brain Atlas, courtesy of E. Covey, University of Washington).
Experimental design and statistical analysis.
In the four bats, neural activity was sampled from multiple locations along the dorsal–ventral axis (Bat A, 21 recording locations; Bat B, 18 recording locations; Bat C, 11 recording locations; and Bat D, six recording locations). While chronic recordings were taken in the SC (digitized at 40 kHz), the bat's sonar vocalizations were recorded with an ultrasonic microphone sampled at 250 kHz and the position of the target was sampled at 1 kHz.
Once neural and behavioral data were collected, analysis was performed to relate the animal's adaptive sonar call production and echo reception to changes in the activity of SC neurons. First, the wideband neural traces were filtered between 600 and 3000 Hz to identify and sort action potentials from single neurons. This was performed through a wavelet-based clustering algorithm (Quiroga et al., 2004). The clustering was run on each channel from the silicon probe (i.e., across 16 channels), identifying 268 neurons for Bat A, 176 neurons for Bat B, 51 neurons for Bat C, and 31 neurons for Bat D, totaling 526 neurons. A concurrent step in the analysis quantified the bat's echolocation behavior in the target tracking task. Recordings of the bat's vocalizations were analyzed to identify the onsets and offsets of each sonar pulse, corrected for the distance the microphone was placed in front of the bat. Across all recording sessions, there were between 15 and 54 target-tracking trials for each of the bats, totaling 34–1662 individual sonar vocalizations per recording session.
Once the onsets of sonar vocalizations were identified, time-aligned windows of SC activity were extracted from the recorded neural data for all neurons held for at least 100 vocalizations. We then determined whether SC activity increased around the time of sonar vocalizations by first measuring the baseline firing rate for each neuron when the bat was not actively tracking the target (i.e., during the intertrial periods). The neural activity was summed across 2 ms bins for this analysis. Our definition of an increase in neural activity was a firing rate >3 SDs above the baseline firing rate for at least 10 ms. This criterion was based upon past studies of SC activity in the bat in which a threshold of 2 SDs above baseline firing rates was used (Sinha and Moss, 2007). We chose 3 SDs to be more conservative in our determination of reliable increases in SC activity. Of the 526 total neurons recorded, 242 showed an increase in firing rate >3 SDs above the mean baseline firing rate in a window starting 50 ms before sonar vocal onset and extending until 50 ms after sonar vocal onset. We then determined when the SC activity peaked with respect to sonar vocal onset. If the peak occurred before sonar vocal onset, then the neuron was classified as vocal premotor; if the peak in activity occurred after sonar vocal onset, then the neuron was classified as sensory (auditory); and if there were peaks before and after sonar vocal onset, then the neuron was classified as sensorimotor (Fig. 2).
After each active neuron was classified as sensory, sensorimotor, or vocal premotor, we determined the corresponding depth of recording site. We then constructed histograms of the range of depths for each class of neuron and ran a two-tailed t test to determine whether the average recording depths of sensory, sensorimotor, and vocal premotor neurons were significantly different. Next, to more closely analyze the relationship between the depth of a recording site and the timing of neural activity with respect to sonar vocalizations/echoes, we converted spike raster plots into heat maps, the color of which indicates the firing rate of the neuron. This was performed by determining instantaneous firing rates over 2 ms bins for SC activity occurring in a 100 ms window centered at sonar vocal onset. This firing rate was then mapped onto a color scale in which blue indicated the lowest firing rate and gold indicated the highest firing rate. Heat maps for each neuron (within a bat) were then arranged in order of increasing recording depth to examine changes in sensory and vocal premotor activity as a function of the dorsal–ventral position of the recording.
Next, we combined data from recorded neurons across bats to provide a more comprehensive analysis of changes in sensory and vocal premotor activity across the dorsal–ventral axis of the SC. To summarize these data in a figure, we first determined the spiking probability in 2 ms bins across a 100 ms window centered at sonar vocal onset. Spike probability was used for this analysis instead of spike rate to provide a normalized measure of activity across neurons. The spike probabilities of all 242 neurons were then arranged in order of increasing recording depth. Because there was some overlap in the recording depths across bats, we binned the range of recording depths (from 0–1200 μm) into 25 μm bins and averaged the activity of neurons within each bin. This analysis resulted in a matrix of data in which the X-dimension represented the time axis and the Y-dimension represented the recording depth for all neurons. These data were then smoothed with a 2D Gaussian. The X-dimension of the Gaussian was 5 ms in width with a 2 ms SD. The Y-dimension of the Gaussian was 50 μm in width with a 10 μm SD. The smoothed data were then plotted as a surface heat plot.
To analyze how the bat's adaptive behaviors were tied to the timing of sensory responses (i.e., echo responses), and the lead time of vocal premotor activity, we first measured the pulse interval (PI) of all sonar vocalizations. The PI is defined as the time from the onset of one vocalization to the onset of the next vocalization. The entire range of vocalization intervals across bats approximately fell into three categories, short PIs of <25 ms, middle PIs of 30–60 ms, and long PIs of 75–250 ms.
We next calculated the latencies of auditory responses to echoes and vocal premotor lead times to sonar vocal onset. The time of echo arrival was calculated by first determining the target distance at the time of each sonar vocal onset, multiplying the target distance by 2 (the sonar signal travels from the bat to the target, and then back to the bat from the target), and calculating when the echo arrived at the bat, assuming a speed of sound in air of 344 m/s. The latency of auditory responses was then defined as the time from echo arrival to the first spike. This latency was calculated for all sensory neurons that were categorized through the methods described above. The lead time of vocal premotor activity was the time interval from sonar vocal onset to the first spike backwards in time (premotor spikes occur before sonar vocalizations). These lead times were calculated for all vocal premotor neurons that were categorized as described previously. We then sorted all lead times by the PI of the sonar vocalizations. We only included activity from neurons with at least 50 vocalizations in each of the short, middle, and long PI categories. Using a two-tailed test, we then tested whether the average sensory latencies and vocal premotor lead times were significantly different across short, middle, and long PI vocalizations.
Results
Behavior and SC recordings
Four big brown bats (E. fuscus) were trained under low-level, long-wavelength lighting to track a moving prey item from a stationary position by echolocation (see Fig. 1A for the experiment setup). A 16-channel silicon probe mounted on a microdrive was then used to record chronically from the SC while the bats performed this target-tracking task. This resulted in recordings at 56 different sites in the four bats, totaling 526 neurons at recording depths ranging from 100–1200 μm (Covey et al., 1987; Wohlgemuth and Moss, 2016).
Echolocation is an active sensing system that permits discrete and selective sampling of acoustic information from the environment (Griffin, 1958; Ulanovsky and Moss, 2008; Moss and Surlykke, 2010). Therefore, we analyzed changes in SC activity with respect to the sonar vocal behavior of the bat. Among the 526 neurons we studied, 242 showed a significant increase in activity before and/or after the onset of the bat's sonar vocalizations (see Materials and Methods for selection criteria) and the other 284 neurons showed no significant change in activity around sonar vocal onset or echo arrival.
Functional classification of SC neuron activity
Neurons with sensory-related activity in the bat SC show an increase in activity related to sonar echo arrival, and responses therefore occur after the bat vocalizes when echoes return to the ears. Two example neurons exhibiting sensory (auditory) evoked activity are shown in Figure 2A. The top two panels of Figure 2A display a raster and average firing rate plot of vocal aligned activity for a single neuron. In this example neuron, an increase in activity occurs at ∼18 ms after the onset of the sonar vocalization, when sonar echoes are returning to the bat. A similar response property is presented for a second example auditory neuron in the bottom two panels of Figure 2A, where there is a peak in activity ∼22 ms after sonar vocal onset at the time of echo arrival.
Vocal premotor signals in the bat SC are implicated in the production of sonar vocalizations and thus occur before vocal onset. Two example neurons demonstrating vocal premotor activity are shown in Figure 2B. In the top two panels (raster and firing rate plot), a vocal premotor neuron shows a burst of activity ∼20 ms before sonar vocal onset is shown. The bottom two panels of Figure 2B display a vocal premotor neuron with a shorter vocal premotor lead time (∼10 ms).
The third category of SC neurons are the sensorimotor neurons—those displaying both sensory and vocal premotor activity. Two example sensorimotor neurons are presented in Figure 2C (top and bottom), each showing a burst of activity before and after sonar vocal onset. In total, 79 neurons were sensory and responded to sonar echoes, 83 neurons fired a vocal premotor burst before sonar vocalizations, and 80 neurons showed both sensory and vocal premotor activity and were therefore classified as sensorimotor. As is shown in Figure 2, sensory activity in the SC was broadly responsive to returning echoes and premotor neurons generally active before sonar vocalizations, in agreement with the broadly tuned SC neurons found in past work in visual mammals (McIlwain and Buser, 1968).
Functional changes in SC activity across the dorsal–ventral axis
Our recording technique allowed us to collect single neuron activity across the laminae of the SC throughout the course of the experiment. In doing so, we reconstructed the locations of sensory, sensorimotor, and motor neurons along the dorsal–ventral axis of the bat SC. Shown in Figure 3 is the distribution of recording depths for each category of neuron (sensory in blue, premotor in green, and sensorimotor in red). We found that, in general, sensory neurons are located at the most dorsal positions of the bat SC, whereas sensorimotor and motor neurons are found at the deepest locations (all pairwise comparisons of mean depths are significantly different; see details in Fig. 3 legend). However, we also identified auditory and vocal premotor neurons throughout the dorsal–ventral axis (Fig. 3, sensory neurons in blue, vocal premotor neurons in green). Sensorimotor neurons also span a large range of recording depths across the dorsal–ventral extent of the SC (Fig. 3, sensorimotor neurons in red).
To analyze more precisely the distribution of sensorimotor activity across the dorsal–ventral extent of the SC, we prepared histological sections of the bat brain to determine where along the dorsal–ventral axis the recordings were collected. Shown in Figure 3B are three serial coronal sections from the SC of Bat A. Indicated in red are the locations of the lesions from the silicon probe tract and in black are the boundaries of the SC. We attempted to identify laminar subdivisions by referring to previous histological reconstructions in the bat and other species (Covey et al., 1987; May, 2006; Big Brown Bat Stereotaxis Brain Atlas, courtesy of E. Covey, University of Washington). From this histological reconstruction, we suggest that recordings made at depths <300 μm were in the stratum griseum superficiale (SGS) layers of the SC; recordings between 300 and 800 μm were in the stratum griseum intermediale (SGI) layers; and recordings made at locations >800 μm were either in the stratum album intermediale (SAI) or stratum griseum profundum (SGP). We would like to note that comparative functional homologies of neuroanatomical divisions are not straightforward (Butler and Hodos, 2005) and our use of the nomenclature SGS, SGI, and SGP is intended to aid readers who are familiar with SC studies of visually dominant mammals.
Because our findings show that neurons with sensory and/or vocal premotor activity are distributed across layers in the in the bat SC, we analyzed neural activity carefully along a dorsal–ventral recording column within an experiment. An example of this analysis is shown in Figure 4, which displays recordings from one specific channel: channel 3 of the 16-channel silicon probe tracked through different recording depths. This analysis first involved identifying neural activity tied to the bat's echolocation behaviors. As in previous analyses and again shown in Figure 4A, we extracted windows of SC activity aligned to the onsets of the bat's sonar vocalizations and created raster plots (Fig. 4A, left) and firing rate plots (Fig. 4A, middle) of neural activity aligned to sonar vocal onset. The vocal-aligned mean firing rate (50 ms before sonar vocal onset and 50 ms after sonar vocal onset) was then converted into a heat plot for each isolated neuron (Fig. 4A, right, yellow is maximum rate, blue is minimum rate, white line is vocal onset). Shown in Figure 4A is the spiking activity of one neuron on one recording channel (channel 3). As the silicon probe was advanced to a new location at the conclusion of each recording session, many different single neurons were isolated on channel 3 over days (Fig. 4B). The vocal-aligned activity patterns of all neurons recorded on channel 3 are plotted with respect to their recording depth in Figure 4B (left, diagram of channel 3 through depths of the SC; middle, recording depths; right, vocal-aligned activity heat plot; the example neuron in Fig. 4A is identified with an asterisk). By displaying sonar vocal-aligned neural activity in this way, patterns of activity throughout the dorsal–ventral axis are visible. In the example shown in Figure 4, SC activity from channel 3 at all depths was mostly auditory, showing an increase in spiking activity in response to the returning target echo (Fig. 4B, right, increase in activity after white line indicating vocal onset), with a few sites showing vocal premotor activity before vocal onset along this columnar penetration throughout the SC. Note that there are also changes in the timing of neural activity with respect to sonar vocal onset across SC recording depths, with sharper temporal responses at intermediate sites.
Because the silicon probe used in our experiments has a consistent geometry (Fig. 4B, left, 100 μm spacing between individual sites on a shank and 100 μm spacing between shanks), it is possible to construct a more comprehensive overview of SC activity by analyzing activity across the channels of a single shank at different recording depths. Shown in Figure 5 are two different reconstructions of all SC neurons recorded on single shanks of the silicon probe during separate experiments. Figure 5A displays all recorded SC neurons recorded on shank 1 from Bat A that have an increase in activity tied to sonar vocal onset (see Materials and Methods for the statistical criteria used). In this single shank penetration, SC activity is mostly auditory in dorsal locations (Fig. 5A, right, increase in activity after sonar vocal onset at more superficial recording sites), whereas neurons at ventral locations are largely vocal premotor (Fig. 5A, right, shows an increase in activity before sonar vocal onset at deeper recording sites). However, this figure also shows that auditory responses are found in neurons at deeper locations in the SC and vocal premotor activity in neurons at more superficial locations, suggesting that the mapping of sensory and vocal premotor activity across the layers of the bat SC is more distributed than reported in previous studies of other mammalian species (Sparks and Hartwich-Young, 1989; May, 2006). This same trend is seen in the example shown in Figure 5B, which displays neural activity collected on shank 2 from Bat B tracked through different recording depths. Data recorded from shank 2 show more vocal premotor activity at dorsal sites than is seen in Figure 5A, but, similar to the example in Figure 5A, sensory and vocal premotor neurons are found throughout most recording depths.
The data shown in Figure 4 illustrate that vocal-aligned SC activity changes as a function of recording depth at one site as the probe was advanced, whereas Figure 5 shows changes in SC activity across dorsal–ventral sites on one probe shank; that is, four sites on one shank throughout recording depths. By combining data across all isolated neural activity at all recording depths, we constructed a summary of SC functional activity along the dorsal–ventral axis (n = 164 unique recording sites at 41 different depths). Figure 6 reconstructs the mapping of sensory, vocal premotor, and sensorimotor activity through all dorsal–ventral SC recording sites by combining data across all silicon probe channels over the course of experiments in four animals. To construct this surface plot, spiking probability for each SC neuron aligned to sonar vocal onset was arranged according to recording depth and then convolved with a 2D Gaussian. The height of the peaks in the surface plot represents the probability of spiking at any given depth location in the SC with respect to sonar vocal onset (indicated with a white line). As in Figures 4 and 5, auditory activity occurs after sonar vocal onset (indicated in time with a blue rear wall of the figure) and vocal premotor activity precedes sonar vocalizations (indicated in time with a green rear wall in the figure). There is a general trend of sensory evoked activity at dorsal and intermediate locations and vocal premotor activity in ventral layers. This is illustrated by yellow peaks in the auditory region of the surface plot at shallow depths and yellow peaks in the vocal premotor region at deeper locations. Interestingly and importantly, auditory evoked activity appears at ventral locations, as indicated by the increased surface plot elevation after sonar vocal onset, as well as vocal premotor activity at more dorsal recording sites, as indicated by the increased surface plot elevation before sonar vocal onset at more superficial recording locations. These analyses show that there is a comingling of sensory, sensorimotor, and premotor signals across the dorsal–ventral extent of the bat SC. The local clusters of sensory, sensorimotor, and premotor neurons within laminae of the bat SC suggest a functional organization to support signaling between different classes of neurons for sensory-guided orientation.
Dynamic changes in sensory and motor activity with adaptive behaviors
For the bat, sensorimotor integration must be rapid to keep pace with the millisecond precision of echo processing and distance-dependent adjustments in call timing. We hypothesize that, when sensorimotor processing demands are high, that is, when sonar calls and echoes occur in rapid succession, the activity of sensory and premotor neurons in the SC is modulated to accommodate processing demands of rapid sensory and motor events. We therefore investigated whether changes in the rate of the bat's vocal production alter the timing of sensory and vocal premotor neuron activity. As illustrated in Figure 1, the bat decreases PI (the time between the onset of one pulse to the onset of the next pulse) as the target approaches. Shown in Figure 7A is the PI distribution of calls produced by the bats in the current study. This distribution is trimodal, so we categorized vocalization intervals into short PIs (<25 ms), middle PIs (30–60 ms), and long PIs (75–250 ms) and examined changes in sensory and vocal premotor activity in single neurons across these three PI categories. For the sensory neurons shown in Figure 7B, there was a significant decrease in the latency between echo arrival and spike timing when bats produced calls in the shortest PI category (i.e., <25 ms), compared with middle and long PI categories. Figure 7B, left and right, show distributions of spike latencies with respect to echo arrival for two example sensory neurons; short PI shown in blue, middle PI in gray, and long PI in black. There is a significant decrease in spike latency for short PI vocalizations (p = 0.001 for left example, p = 0.002 for right example, permutation test).
We extended this analysis to the entire population of neurons studied and tested whether auditory neurons show changes in echo-evoked spike latencies across the three PI categories in which at least 50 vocalizations were produced in each PI category (n = 32 neurons). Across sensory neurons, we found a significant decrease in latency from echo arrival time to spike time for echoes returned after short PI vocalizations (Fig. 7C, p = 0.0002, two-tailed t test). We performed a similar analysis for vocal premotor neurons and shown in Figure 7D are the lead times from spike to pulse onset for two example vocal premotor neurons. Similar to sensory neurons, vocal premotor neurons showed a significant decrease in spike lead time of single neurons to pulse onset for short PI vocalizations. Figure 7D, left and right, show distributions of spike lead times with respect to pulse onset for two example vocal premotor neurons (short PI shown in green, middle PI in gray, and long PI in black; significant decrease in spike latencies for short PI vocalizations, p = 0.001 for left example neuron, p = 0.02 right example, permutation test). Across all vocal premotor neurons recorded, there was a significant decrease in lead time from spike to pulse onset for the vocalizations produced at short PIs (Fig. 7E, p = 0.0005, two-tailed t test, n = 29 neurons). These results demonstrate that the timing of both sensory and vocal premotor activity is influenced by the rate at which bats produce echolocation calls and process information carried by echo returns.
Discussion
Sensorimotor processing is integral to a wide range of natural behaviors,yet research to date has emphasized the neural underpinnings of simplified visuomotor behaviors under controlled conditions (Sparks and Hartwich-Young, 1989; Gandhi and Katnani, 2011; Krauzlis et al., 2013). To extend this line of investigation to dynamic audiomotor integration in adaptive orienting behaviors, we characterized neural activity in the midbrain SC of echolocating bats engaged in a natural target-tracking task. We hypothesized that the SC would exhibit integrated laminar activity of sensory, sensorimotor, and vocal premotor neurons to support rapid audiomotor integration.
Although the functional organization of the SC is broadly conserved across mammals, there are differences in the number and size of layers, which are influenced by the dominant sensory modality used by an animal for spatial orientation (May, 2006). The upper layer in primates, the SGS is devoted to visual processing (May, 2006), reflecting this animal's reliance on visual orienting. The SGS is further divided into upper and lower sublaminae (i.e. upper stratum griseum superficiale and lower stratum griseum superficiale, or uSGS and lSGS, respectively) in the monkey and into three divisions in the cat, with a layer below the uSGS and lSGS, stratum opticum containing mostly fibers originating from the retina (May, 2006). In this study, we refer to the most dorsal region of the bat SC as the SGS based on anatomical location, but recognize that this nomenclature deviates from the canonical functional characterization of superficial layer neurons as visual. Past work in bats shows species differences in visual input to superficial layers. In the omnivorous pale spear-nosed bat, Phyllostomus discoler, neurons in the superficial SC responded to visual stimulation, were topographically organized, and in register with an auditory space map in deeper layers (Hoffmann et al., 2016). A study of the insectivorous bat Pteronotus parnellii revealed a very sparse retinal input layer and, indeed, it may be that intermediate layers of the bat SC invade the most superficial regions in species that rely primarily on hearing to orient themselves (Covey et al., 1987). Consistent with this view are neurophysiological recordings from the SC of insectivorous echolocating bats, which show robust auditory activity in the most dorsal layers (Jen et al., 1984; Poussin and Schlegel, 1984; Shimozawa et al., 1984; Wong, 1984; Valentine and Moss, 1997). However, neuroanatomical comparisons across species warrant caution (Butler and Hodos, 2005).
The intermediate layers of the SC or SGI are unimodal, multimodal, and sensorimotor in primate, cat, and rodent (May, 2006). The SGI is further divided into two or three subdivisions, depending upon the convention. The upper SGI (uSGI) contains cell bodies, whereas the lower division(s) contains fewer cell bodies and fibers of passage running along the rostral–caudal dimension. Sensory representations in SGI include visual, auditory, and somatosensory neurons (Meredith and Stein, 1986; May, 2006) and visual SGI neurons show both sensory and premotor activity (Mays and Sparks, 1980), with some variations across species. In addition, neurons in the SGI display facilitated responses to multimodal stimuli (Meredith and Stein, 1986), suggesting a potential interaction between sensory neurons in these laminae of the SC. Our data show auditory, audiomotor, and vocal premotor neurons in intermediate layers of the bat SC, implying that interactions may occur among neurons of different functional classes as the bat engages in adaptive spatial orienting behaviors.
Below the SGI is the SAI, which contains fibers of passage and is considered to be the most ventral portion of the intermediate zone. The adjacent ventral layer contains more cell bodies and is termed SGP layer (May, 2006). Recordings from the SGP in the primate SC show neurons with premotor fields for saccadic eye movements (Sparks, 1986; Sparks and Hartwich-Young, 1989; Munoz et al., 1991b). Previous work also reports visual, auditory, somatosensory, and multisensory neurons in ventral SC across species (Wise and Irvine, 1983, 1985; Jen et al., 1984; Shimozawa et al., 1984; Wallace et al., 1996; Hoffmann et al., 2016). At ventral SC locations in our study, vocal premotor neurons were active before sonar vocalizations, consistent with earlier studies of the bat (Sinha and Moss, 2007).
The majority of our recordings were taken at depths >400 μm, which we identify as intermediate and deep layers. At intermediate depths, in the SGI layers, we found a mix of sensory, sensorimotor, and vocal premotor neurons, which is in agreement with previous reports in other animals (May, 2006). The most ventral recording sites, which may be homologous to the SAI and SGP layers, contained a preponderance of sensorimotor and vocal premotor neurons. The largest increase in vocal premotor activity was at depths >800 μm (Fig. 7), within the traditionally defined motor layers of the SC (Ma et al., 1990; May, 2006).
Previous multiunit recordings from the SC of big brown bats revealed two bursts of activity preceding each sonar vocalization: one burst at short lead time and locked to vocal onset and a second burst at a longer and variable lead time (Sinha and Moss, 2007). These prior results raise the question of whether activity before vocalizations in the bat SC influences sonar call parameters or serves as efference motor signals. Sinha and Moss (2007) propose that the long lead time vocal premotor activity is implicated in shaping sonar call features, whereas the precisely timed short-lead premotor burst marks the time of sonar emission for echo delay measurement. In our single unit recordings, we mostly identified increased activity before sonar vocalizations at variable lead times (Fig. 7). We therefore cannot conclude that the single unit vocal premotor activity in our study represents an efference signal of call onset time.
What is important, and currently unknown, is the connectivity between sensory and premotor neurons in the bat SC. Prior anatomical work in visual mammals demonstrated that intralaminar connectivity patterns differ across SC layers (Isa, 2002; May, 2006; Isa and Hall, 2009; Vokoun et al., 2011; Bayguinov et al., 2015). Superficial layers have larger inhibitory networks that enable surround suppression (Mize, 1988; Behan and Appell, 1992). More ventral layers, however, show a mixture of inhibitory and excitatory connections within layers (McIlwain, 1982; Munoz and Istvan, 1998). Intralaminar organization may differ in the bat, but we hypothesize that neurons in the same layers are also functionally connected and that the colocalization of sensory and premotor neurons within layers facilitates local interactions. Testing this hypothesis in bats and other animals would contribute to broader comparative analyses that would help to differentiate between species-specific specializations and general mechanisms.
Ultimately, an animal must integrate sensory information from the environment to select appropriate actions. Prior work on the SC demonstrates interactions within the same subregions, that alter activity in a variety of ways (Meredith and Stein, 1986; Wallace et al., 1996; Stein, 1998). For instance, studies of the intermediate layers of the SC have shown that a neuron can be either inhibited or facilitated by multimodal stimulation depending on the timing of sensory inputs through each channel (Meredith and Stein, 1983; King and Palmer, 1985). This suggests a mechanism by which streams of information can activate SC neurons differentially. In addition, auditory neurons in the primate SC show shifts in spatial tuning with eye position (Jay and Sparks, 1984; Groh et al., 2001), revealing multimodal–sensorimotor interactions. We posit that interactions between pools of local neurons with different functional properties in the bat SC enable the dynamic audiomotor integration intrinsic to echolocation.
How might comingling of sensory and premotor neurons within laminae enable sensorimotor processing for natural behaviors? To provide a possible answer, we analyzed natural variations in echolocation call parameters with respect to changes in SC sensory and vocal premotor signaling. As a bat tracks prey, vocal rate increases with decreasing target distance (Griffin, 1958). This adaptive vocal behavior increases echo sample rate and can therefore increase sensory resolution (Ulanovsky and Moss, 2008). We analyzed the activity of SC sensory and vocal premotor neurons with respect to changes in sonar temporal patterning and found significant changes in SC activity when the bat produced vocalizations at the highest rates (i.e., the shortest PIs). When the bat produced vocalizations at rates >40 calls/s, response latencies of sensory neurons and spike lead time to vocal onset of vocal premotor neurons were significantly shorter. These results show that both sensory and vocal premotor neurons change activity patterns when the bat increases its sonar sample rate before prey capture.
There are several mechanisms that could be responsible for the shortened echo response latencies and vocal premotor lead times during high call rates. For example, sensory latencies could be reduced through mechanisms similar to forward suppression, as described previously in both the cortex (Beetz et al., 2016) and midbrain (Voytenko and Galazyuk, 2010; Tang et al., 2015) of bats. In this scheme, global inhibition shapes responses such that the brain responds only to a salient stimulus. In the inferior colliculus and auditory cortex of bats, forward suppression could sharpen the selectivity of auditory responses to the temporal features of natural echolocation stimuli (Sanderson and Simmons, 2002, 2005). In our data, forward suppression would allow for faster and more precise response times in auditory neurons.
In vocal premotor neurons, it is possible that another mechanism drives the decrease in premotor lead times with increasing call rate. Prior studies in the SC have demonstrated decreases in saccadic reaction times in primates by “priming” the system through subthreshold microstimulation (Carello and Krauzlis, 2004; Müller et al., 2005). Our results complement this finding, showing that a natural source of SC priming, high call rates and increased echo returns, shortens the lead time of vocal premotor neurons.
When bats produce echolocation calls at relatively high rates, the activity levels of both sensory and vocal premotor neurons often increase. This could provide a functional substrate for shortened spike latencies; i.e. increases in sensory activity can drive more global suppression and/or lateral inhibition leading to sharpened responses, while increases in baseline motor activity can act to prime the system for shorter lead time premotor signals. It is possible that both of these mechanisms are at play for sensory and motor neurons, but, importantly, our findings show that local pools of sensory and premotor neurons have the spatial proximity to communicate quickly and keep pace with sensorimotor processing demands. These findings lead us to propose that the functional organization of the bat SC supports local and rapid integration of behaviorally modulated sensory and premotor signals for acoustic orientation by sonar.
In conclusion, we have characterized neural activity throughout the dorsal–ventral axis of the SC in an echolocating bat engaged in a dynamic and natural target-tracking task. Our results revealed a distribution of sensory, sensorimotor, and premotor neurons within laminae, which leads us to hypothesize that local pools of sensory and premotor neurons interact to mediate adaptive sensorimotor behaviors. Furthermore, we discovered that adaptive echolocation behavior evokes changes in sensory and premotor signaling in the bat SC, which may arise through local loops of sensorimotor signaling. Future comparative studies of SC activity in other species engaged in natural and dynamic orienting tasks will provide important data to test this hypothesis.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Human Frontiers Science Program (Grants RGP0040, LT000279/2016-L; ONR, N00014-12-1-0339; AFOSR FA9550-14-1-0398); the National Science Foundation (Collaborative Research in Computational Neuroscience Grant IOS1460149 to C.F.M.); the Center for the Evolutionary Biology of Hearing (Training Grant T32-DC00046 to M.J.W.). C.F.M. worked on this manuscript while participating at the 2017 Kavli Institute for Theoretical Physics Workshop the Physics of Hearing: From Neurobiology to Information Theory and Back and was supported in part by the National Science Foundation(Grant PHY11-25915). We thank E. Covey for providing a brain atlas for the big brown bat (Eptesicus fuscus) and the members of the Johns Hopkins Batlab for their advice and input throughout the development of this manuscript.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Bayguinov PO, Ghitani N, Jackson MB, Basso MA (2015) A hard-wired priority map in the superior colliculus shaped by asymmetric inhibitory circuitry. J Neurophysiol 114:662–676. 10.1152/jn.00144.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beetz MJ, Hechavarría JC, Kössl M (2016) Temporal tuning in the bat auditory cortex is sharper when studied with natural echolocation sequences. Sci Rep 6:29102. 10.1038/srep29102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behan M, Appell PP (1992) Intrinsic circuitry in the cat superior colliculus: projections from the superficial layers. J Comp Neurol 315:230–243. 10.1002/cne.903150209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berson DM, Stein JJ (1995) Retinotopic organization of the superior colliculus in relation to the retinal distribution of afferent ganglion cells. Vis Neurosci 12:671–686. 10.1017/S0952523800008968 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brandão ML, Cardoso SH, Melo LL, Motta V, Coimbra NC (1994) Neural substrate of defensive behavior in the midbrain tectum. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 18:339–346. 10.1016/0149-7634(94)90047-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler AB, Hodos W (2005) Comparative vertebrate neuroanatomy: evolution and adaptation. New York: Wiley Interscience. [Google Scholar]
- Carello CD, Krauzlis RJ (2004) Manipulating intent: Evidence for a causal role of the superior colliculus in target selection. Neuron 43:575–583. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.07.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Comoli E, Das Neves Favaro P, Vautrelle N, Leriche M, Overton PG, Redgrave P (2012) Segregated anatomical input to sub-regions of the rodent superior colliculus associated with approach and defense. Front Neuroanat 6:9. 10.3389/fnana.2012.00009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Covey E, Hall WC, Kobler JB (1987) Subcortical connections of the superior colliculus in the mustache bat, Pteronotus parnellii. J Comp Neurol 263:179–197. 10.1002/cne.902630203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cynader M, Berman N (1972) Receptive-field organization of monkey superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 35:187–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- du Lac S, Knudsen EI (1990) Neural maps of head movement vector and speed in the optic tectum of the barn owl. J Neurophysiol 63:131–146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fanselow MS. (1994) Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear. Psychon Bull Rev 1:429–438. 10.3758/BF03210947 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furigo IC, de Oliveira WF, de Oliveira AR, Comoli E, Baldo MV, Mota-Ortiz SR, Canteras NS (2010) The role of the superior colliculus in predatory hunting. Neuroscience 165:1–15. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gandhi NJ, Katnani HA (2011) Motor functions of the superior colliculus. Annu Rev Neurosci 34:205–231. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113728 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg ME, Wurtz RH (1972) Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. I. Visual receptive fields of single neurons. J Neurophysiol 35:542–559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffin D. (1958) Listening in the dark: the acoustic orientation of bats and men. New Haven, CT: Yale University. [Google Scholar]
- Grobstein P. (1988) Between the retinotectal projection and directed movement: topography of a sensorimotor interface. Brain Behav Evol 31:34–48. 10.1159/000116574 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groh JM, Trause AS, Underhill AM, Clark KR, Inati S (2001) Eye position influences auditory responses in primate inferior colliculus. Neuron 29:509–518. 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00222-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartline PH, Kass L, Loop MS (1978) Merging of modalities in the optic tectum: infrared and visual integration in rattlesnakes. Science 199:1225–1229. 10.1126/science.628839 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffmann S, Vega-Zuniga T, Greiter W, Krabichler Q, Bley A, Matthes M, Zimmer C, Firzlaff U, Luksch H (2016) Congruent representation of visual and acoustic space in the superior colliculus of the echolocating bat Phyllostomus discolor. Eur J Neurosci 44:2685–2697. 10.1111/ejn.13394 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hope GM, Bhatnagar KP (1979) Electrical response of bat retina to spectral stimulation: comparison of four microchiropteran species. Experientia 35:1189–1191. 10.1007/BF01963279 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isa T. (2002) Intrinsic processing in the mammalian superior colliculus. Curr Opin Neurobiol 12:668–677. 10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00387-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isa T, Hall WC (2009) Exploring the superior colliculus in vitro. J Neurophysiol 102:2581–2593. 10.1152/jn.00498.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jay MF, Sparks DL (1984) Auditory receptive fields in primate superior colliculus shift with changes in eye position. Nature 309:345–347. 10.1038/309345a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jay MF, Sparks DL (1987) Sensorimotor integration in the primate superior colliculus. II. Coordinates of auditory signals. J Neurophysiol 57:35–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jen PH, Sun X, Kamada T, Zhang S, Shimozawa T (1984) Auditory response properties and spatial response areas of superior collicular neurons of the FM bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 154:407–413. 10.1007/BF00605239 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kavaliers M, Choleris E (2001) Antipredator responses and defensive behavior: ecological and ethological approaches for the neurosciences. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25:577–586. 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00042-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King AJ, Palmer AR (1985) Integration of visual and auditory information in bimodal neurones in the guinea-pig superior colliculus. Exp Brain Res 60:492–500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knudsen EI. (1982) Auditory and visual maps of space in the optic tectum of the owl. J Neurosci 2:1177–1194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kothari NB, Wohlgemuth MJ, Hulgard K, Surlykke A, Moss CF (2014) Timing matters: sonar call groups facilitate target localization in bats. Front Physiol 5:168. 10.3389/fphys.2014.00168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krauzlis RJ, Lovejoy LP, Zénon A (2013) Superior colliculus and visual spatial attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 36:165–182. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170249 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J, Groh JM (2012) Auditory signals evolve from hybrid- to eye-centered coordinates in the primate superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 108:227–242. 10.1152/jn.00706.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma TP, Cheng HW, Czech JA, Rafols JA (1990) Intermediate and deep layers of the macaque superior colliculus: a Golgi study. J Comp Neurol 295:92–110. 10.1002/cne.902950109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maior RS, Hori E, Uribe CE, Saletti PG, Ono T, Nishijo H, Tomaz C (2012) A role for the superior colliculus in the modulation of threat responsiveness in primates: toward the ontogenesis of the social brain. Rev Neurosci 23:697–706. 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- May PJ. (2006) The mammalian superior colliculus: laminar structure and connections. Prog Brain Res 151:321–378. 10.1016/S0079-6123(05)51011-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mays LE, Sparks DL (1980) Dissociation of visual and saccade-related responses in superior colliculus neurons. J Neurophysiol 43:207–232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIlwain JT. (1982) Lateral spread of neural excitation during microstimulation in intermediate gray layer of cat's superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 47:167–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIlwain JT. (1983) Representation of the visual streak in visuotopic maps of the cat's superior colliculus: influence of the mapping variable. Vision Res 23:507–516. 10.1016/0042-6989(83)90125-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIlwain JT, Buser P (1968) Receptive fields of single cells in the cat's superior colliculus. Exp Brain Res 5:314–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIlwain JT. (1991) Distributed spatial coding in the superior colliculus: a review. Vis Neurosci 6:3–13. 10.1017/S0952523800000857 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meredith MA, Stein BE (1983) Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the superior colliculus. Science 221:389–391. 10.1126/science.6867718 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meredith MA, Stein BE (1986) Visual, auditory, and somatosensory convergence on cells in superior colliculus results in multisensory integration. J Neurophysiol 56:640–662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middlebrooks JC, Knudsen EI (1984) A neural code for auditory space in the cat's superior colliculus. J Neurosci 4:2621–2634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mize RR. (1988) Immunocytochemical localization of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) in the cat superior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 276:169–187. 10.1002/cne.902760203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moss CF, Surlykke A (2010) Probing the natural scene by echolocation in bats. Front Behav Neurosci 4: pii: 33. 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller JR, Philiastides MG, Newsome WT (2005) Microstimulation of the superior colliculus focuses attention without moving the eyes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:524–529 10.1073/pnas.0408311101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Munoz DP, Istvan PJ (1998) Lateral inhibitory interactions in the intermediate layers of the monkey superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 79:1193–1209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Munoz DP, Guitton D, Pélisson D (1991a) Control of orienting gaze shifts by the tectoreticulospinal system in the head-free cat. III. Spatiotemporal characteristics of phasic motor discharges. J Neurophysiol 66:1642–1666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Munoz DP, Pélisson D, Guitton D (1991b) Movement of neural activity on the superior colliculus motor map during gaze shifts. Science 251:1358–1360. 10.1126/science.2003221 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peck CK. (1990) Neuronal activity related to head and eye movements in cat superior colliculus. J Physiol 421:79–104. 10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp017934 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poussin C, Schlegel P (1984) Directional sensitivity of auditory neurons in the superior colliculus of the bat, Eptesicus fuscus, using free field sound stimulation. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 154:253–261. 10.1007/BF00604991 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Quiroga RQ, Nadasdy Z, Ben-Shaul Y (2004) Unsupervised spike detection and sorting with wavelets and superparamagnetic clustering. Neural computation 16:1661–1687. 10.1162/089976604774201631 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roucoux A, Crommelinck M (1976) Eye movements evoked by superior colliculus stimulation in the alert cat. Brain Res 106:349–363. 10.1016/0006-8993(76)91030-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanderson MI, Simmons JA (2002) Selectivity for echo spectral interference and delay in the auditory cortex of the big brown bat Eptesicus fuscus. J Neurophysiol 87:2823–2834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanderson MI, Simmons JA (2005) Target representation of naturalistic echolocation sequences in single unit responses from the inferior colliculus of big brown bats. J Acoust Soc Am 118:3352–3361. 10.1121/1.2041227 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiller PH, Koerner F (1971) Discharge characteristics of single units in superior colliculus of the alert rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 34:920–936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shimozawa T, Sun X, Jen PH (1984) Auditory space representation in the superior colliculus of the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. Brain Res 311:289–296. 10.1016/0006-8993(84)90091-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinha SR, Moss CF (2007) Vocal premotor activity in the superior colliculus. J Neurosci 27:98–110. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2683-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparks DL. (1986) Translation of sensory signals into commands for control of saccadic eye movements: role of primate superior colliculus. Physiol Rev 66:118–171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparks DL, Hartwich-Young R (1989) The deep layers of the superior colliculus. Rev Oculomot Res 3:213–255. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein BE. (1998) Neural mechanisms for synthesizing sensory information and producing adaptive behaviors. Exp Brain Res 123:124–135. 10.1007/s002210050553 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuphorn V, Hoffmann KP, Miller LE (1999) Correlation of primate superior colliculus and reticular formation discharge with proximal limb muscle activity. J Neurophysiol 81:1978–1982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuphorn V, Bauswein E, Hoffmann KP (2000) Neurons in the primate superior colliculus coding for arm movements in gaze-related coordinates. J Neurophysiol 83:1283–1299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang J, Fu ZY, Wei CX, Chen QC (2015) Effect of echolocation behavior-related constant frequency-frequency modulation sound on the frequency tuning of inferior collicular neurons in Hipposideros armiger. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 201:783–794. 10.1007/s00359-015-1018-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ulanovsky N, Moss CF (2008) What the bat's voice tells the bat's brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:8491–8498. 10.1073/pnas.0703550105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valentine DE, Moss CF (1997) Spatially selective auditory responses in the superior colliculus of the echolocating bat. J Neurosci 17:1720–1733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valentine DE, Sinha SR, Moss CF (2002) Orienting responses and vocalizations produced by microstimulation in the superior colliculus of the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 188:89–108. 10.1007/s00359-001-0275-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vokoun CR, Jackson MB, Basso MA (2011) Circuit dynamics of the superior colliculus revealed by in vitro voltage imaging. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1233:41–47. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06166.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voytenko SV, Galazyuk AV (2010) Suppression of spontaneous firing in inferior colliculus neurons during sound processing. Neuroscience 165:1490–1500. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.11.070 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner H, Kettler L, Orlowski J, Tellers P (2013) Neuroethology of prey capture in the barn owl (Tyto alba L.). J Physiol Paris 107:51–61. 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2012.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace MT, Wilkinson LK, Stein BE (1996) Representation and integration of multiple sensory inputs in primate superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 76:1246–1266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werner W, Dannenberg S, Hoffmann KP (1997) Arm-movement-related neurons in the primate superior colliculus and underlying reticular formation: comparison of neuronal activity with EMGs of muscles of the shoulder, arm and trunk during reaching. Exp Brain Res 115:191–205. 10.1007/PL00005690 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise LZ, Irvine DR (1983) Auditory response properties of neurons in deep layers of cat superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 49:674–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise LZ, Irvine DR (1985) Topographic organization of interaural intensity difference sensitivity in deep layers of cat superior colliculus: implications for auditory spatial representation. J Neurophysiol 54:185–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wohlgemuth MJ, Kothari NB, Moss CF (2016a) Action enhances acoustic cues for 3-D target localization by echolocating bats. PLoS Biol 14:e1002544. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002544 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wohlgemuth MJ, Moss CF (2016) Midbrain auditory selectivity to natural sounds. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113:2508–2513. 10.1073/pnas.1517451113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wohlgemuth MJ, Luo J, Moss CF (2016b) Three-dimensional auditory localization in the echolocating bat. Curr Opin Neurobiol 41:78–86. 10.1016/j.conb.2016.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong D. (1984) Spatial tuning of auditory neurons in the superior colliculus of the echolocating bat, Myotis lucifugus. Hear Res 16:261–270. 10.1016/0378-5955(84)90115-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wurtz RH, Goldberg ME (1971) Superior colliculus cell responses related to eye movements in awake monkeys. Science 171:82–84. 10.1126/science.171.3966.82 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]