Abstract
Coordination in living systems—from cells to people—must be understood at multiple levels of description. Analyses and modelling of empirically observed patterns of biological coordination often focus either on ensemble-level statistics in large-scale systems with many components, or on detailed dynamics in small-scale systems with few components. The two approaches have proceeded largely independent of each other. To bridge this gap between levels and scales, we have recently conducted a human experiment of mid-scale social coordination specifically designed to reveal coordination at multiple levels (ensemble, subgroups and dyads) simultaneously. Based on this experiment, the present work shows that, surprisingly, a single system of equations captures key observations at all relevant levels. It also connects empirically validated models of large- and small-scale biological coordination—the Kuramoto and extended Haken–Kelso–Bunz (HKB) models—and the hallmark phenomena that each is known to capture. For example, it exhibits both multistability and metastability observed in small-scale empirical research (via the second-order coupling and symmetry breaking in extended HKB) and the growth of biological complexity as a function of scale (via the scalability of the Kuramoto model). Only by incorporating both of these features simultaneously can we reproduce the essential coordination behaviour observed in our experiment.
Keywords: nonlinear dynamics, statistical mechanics, coordination dynamics, complex systems, social, complexity
1. Introduction
Coordination is central to living systems and biological complexity at large, where the whole can be more than and different from the sum of its parts. Rhythmic coordination [1–3] is of particular interest for understanding the formation and change of spatio-temporal patterns in living systems, including, e.g. slime mould [4,5], fireflies [6,7], social groups [8,9] and the brain [10–14]. Theoretical descriptions of biological coordination are often in terms of coupled oscillators, whose behaviour is constrained by their phase relations with each other [2,15–18]. Existing studies of phase coordination often focus on systems of either very few (small-scale, mostly N = 2) [13,19,20], or very many oscillators (large-scale, N → ∞) [21–23]. Here we inquire how the two might be connected and applied to midscale systems with neither too many nor too few components. The present work answers this question by modelling empirically observed coordinative behaviour in midscale systems (N = 8), based on data collected in a specially designed human experiment [24]. The resultant model that captures all key experimental observations happens to also connect previous theories of small- and large-scale biological coordination in a single mathematical formulation.
But first, how are small- and large-scale models different? Small-scale models were usually developed to capture empirically observed coordination patterns, as in animal gaits [25–27], bimanual movement coordination [28,29], neuronal coordination [30], interpersonal coordination [31,32], human–animal coordination [33] and human–machine coordination [34,35]. They describe multiple stable coordination patterns (multistability) and the transitions between them (order-to-order transitions), e.g. from a trot to a gallop for a horse [36]. In humans, dyadic coordination patterns like inphase and antiphase (synchronization, syncopation) were found across neural, sensorimotor and social levels (see [13,14] for reviews), well captured by the extended Haken–Kelso–Bunz (HKB) model [29,37,38]. However, the extended HKB was restricted to describing coordination phenomena at N = 2 (i.e. not directly applicable to higher-dimensional coordination phenomena). By contrast, large-scale models are concerned more about statistical features like the overall level of synchrony and disorder-to-order transitions, but not so much about patterns at finer levels. As a representative, the classical Kuramoto model [2] is applicable to describing a wide range of large-scale coordination between, e.g. people [23,39], fish [40] and neural processes [22], and is often studied analytically for its incoherence-to-coherence transition (at the statistical level, for N → ∞; see [41,42] for reviews).
Although the extended HKB and the classical Kuramoto model emerged separately, they connect to each other by an interesting difference: the Kuramoto model with N = 2 is almost the extended HKB model except that the former lacks the term responsible for antiphase coordination in the latter (more accurately, the bistability of inphase and antiphase). Bistability of inphase and antiphase coordination, with associated order-to-order transitions and hysteresis, happens to be a key observation in small-scale human experiments [28,43]. This begs the question of whether there is a fundamental difference between large- and small-scale coordination phenomena. Does the existence of antiphase, multistability, and order-to-order transitions depend on scale N? With these questions in mind, we recently conducted a human experiment [24] at an intermediate scale (N = 8), such that the system is large enough for studying its macro-level properties, yet small enough for examining patterns at finer levels, ideal for theories and empirical data to meet at multiple levels of description. In the following sections, we demonstrate how the marriage between the two models (not either one alone) is sufficient for capturing empirical observations at multiple levels of description and we discuss its empirical and theoretical implications for biological coordination.
2. Results
2.1. Human coordination at intermediate scales
Before getting into the model, we briefly review the mid-scale experiment and key results [24]. In the experiment (dubbed the ‘Human Firefly’ experiment), ensembles of eight people (N = 8, total 120 subjects) spontaneously coordinated rhythmic movements in an all-to-all network (via eight touchpads and eight ring-shaped arrays of eight LEDs as in figure 1; see Material and methods for details), even though they were not explicitly instructed to coordinate with each other. To induce different grouping behaviour, subjects were paced with different metronomes prior to interaction such that each ensemble was split into two frequency groups of equal size with intergroup difference δf = 0, 0.3 or 0.6 Hz (referred to as levels of ‘diversity’), and were asked to maintain that frequency during interaction after the metronome was turned off. Subjects’ instantaneous tapping frequencies from three example trials (figure 2a–c) show intuitively the consequences of frequency manipulations: from (a) to (c) a supergroup of eight gradually split into two frequency groups of four as diversity increased from δf = 0 to 0.6 Hz.
Key results involve multiple levels of description, in terms of intergroup, intragroup and interpersonal relations. The level of intergroup integration is defined as the relationship between intragroup and intergroup coordination (β1, slope of regression lines in figure 3a. Here intragroup coordination is measured by the average pair-wise phase-locking value over all intragroup dyads, and likewise, intergroup coordination over all intergroup dyads. Phase-locking value per se is a measure of stability of a relative phase pattern within a period of time, which equals to one minus the circular variance. See section ‘Phase-locking value and level of integration’ for technical details). Intuitively, we say that two groups are integrated if intragroup and intergroup coordination facilitate each other (positive relation between respective phase-locking values, β1 > 0), and segregated if intragroup and intergroup coordination undermine each other (negative relation between respective phase-locking values, β1 < 0. We will see later in figure 4 how this measure meaningfully captures coordination dynamics). In the experimental result, two frequency groups were integrated when diversity is low or moderate (δf = 0, 0.3 Hz, blue and red lines, slope β1 > 0) and segregated when diversity is high (δf = 0.6 Hz, yellow line, slope β1 < 0). A critical level of diversity demarcating the regime of intergroup integration and segregation was estimated to be δf* = 0.5 Hz. Within the frequency groups, coordination was also reduced by the presence of intergroup difference (figure 3b, left, red and yellow bars shorter than blue bar). At the interpersonal level, inphase and antiphase were preferred phase relations (inphase much stronger than antiphase; distributions in figure 2d–f), especially when the diversity was very low (figure 2d, peaks around ϕ = 0, π, in radians throughout this paper), but both were weakened by increasing diversity (figure 2e,f; in episodes of strong coordination, antiphase is greatly amplified and much more susceptible to diversity than inphase, see [24]). Notice that subjects did not remain locked into these phase relations but rather engaged and disengaged intermittently (two persons dwell near and escape from preferred phase relations recurrently, a sign of metastability [13]; see figure 6a red trajectory for example), reflected also as ‘kissing’ and ‘splitting’ of frequency trajectories (e.g. in figure 2b). In the following sections, we present a model that captures these key experimental observations at their respective levels of description.
2.2. A minimal experiment-based model of multiagent coordination
Our model of coordination is based on a family of N oscillators, each represented by a single phase angle φi. We will show that a pair-wise phase coupling [2,25,29] of the form
2.1 |
suffices to model the key features of the experimental data identified above. The left side of this equation is the time derivative of φi, while the constant ωi > 0 on the right is the natural (i.e. uncoupled) frequency of the ith oscillator. The coefficients aij > 0 and bij > 0 are parameters that govern the coupling.
Equations (2.1) include a number of well-studied models as special cases. For instance, setting ϕ := φ1 − φ2, δω := ω1 − ω2, and for N = 2, the difference of the two resulting equations (2.1) yields the relative phase equation
2.2 |
of the extended HKB model [37]. The HKB model [29] was originally designed to describe the dynamics of human bimanual coordination, corresponding to equation (2.2) with δω = 0 (i.e. describing the coordination between two identical components). The extended HKB introduces the symmetry breaking term δω to capture empirically observed coordinative behaviour between asymmetric as well as symmetric components (i.e. the HKB model is included in the extended HKB model, which is further included in equations (2.1)). It has since been shown to apply to a broad variety of dyadic coordination phenomena in living systems, e.g. [13,14,19,43,44]. Equations (2.1) can be considered a generalization of the extended HKB model from 2 to N oscillators. It is remarkable that such a direct generalization can reproduce key features of the collective rhythmic coordination in ensembles of human subjects at multiple levels of description.
Another well-studied special case of equations (2.1) is the Kuramoto model [2], which has bij = 0 (and typically aij = a, independent of i and j). We will see below, however, that the Kuramoto model cannot exhibit at least one feature of the experimental data. Namely, the data show a secondary peak in the pairwise relative phase of experimental subjects at antiphase (see figure 2d–f). Simulations using the Kuramoto model do not reproduce this effect, while simulations of equations (2.1) model do (compare figure 5d–f and g–i). We give additional analytical support for this point by studying relevant fixed points of both models in the electronic supplementary materials (section ‘Multistability of the present model’).
2.3. Weak coupling captures human behaviour
Given the spatially symmetric set-up of the ‘Human Firefly’ experiment (all-to-all network, visual presentation at equal distance to fixation point), it is reasonable to further simplify equations (2.1) by letting aij = a and bij = b (a, b > 0),
2.3 |
where ϕij = φi − φj is the relative phase between oscillators i and j (henceforth we use the notation ϕij instead of the subtraction, since relative phase is the crucial variable for coordination [28,29]).
The behaviour of the model itself clearly depends on the coupling strength (a, b) and frequency diversity (distribution of ωi’s). While the latter was explicitly manipulated in the human experiment [24], the former was unknown. A qualitative look at simulated dynamics (see examples figure 4a–c for δf = 0.6 Hz) indicates that weak coupling better captures human behaviour (members of the same group do not collapse to a single trajectory in figure 4a as in figure 2). By contrast, stronger coupling (figure 4b,c) deprives the system of much of the metastability. Quantitatively, we fitted the coupling strength (assuming a = b) to the human data based on the level of intergroup integration (β1) (see distribution of model β1 in figure 4d) particularly for diversity condition δf = 0.3 Hz (i.e. using only one-third of the data to prevent overfitting). We show below how the model captures human behaviour across all diversity conditions and levels of description under the best-fit coupling strength (a = b = 0.105; see section ‘Choosing the appropriate coupling strength’ in the electronic supplementary materials for more details).
At the level of intergroup relations, model behaviour (figure 3c) successfully captures human behaviour (figure 3a) at all levels of diversity. Similar to the human experiment, low diversity (δf = 0 Hz) results in a high level of integration in the model (blue line in figure 3c slope close to 1; β1 = 0.972, t199 = 66.6, p < 0.001); high diversity (δf = 0.6 Hz) comes with segregation (yellow line slope negative; β1 = −0.113, t199 = −3.56, p < 0.001); and in between, moderate diversity (δf = 0.3 Hz) is associated with partial integration (red line positive slope far less than 1; β1 = 0.318, t199 = 4.23, p < 0.001). Here we did not estimate the critical diversity δf* the same way as for the human data (by linear interpolation), since we found theoretically that the level of integration depends nonlinearly on diversity δf, and as a result the theoretical δf* is 0.4 Hz (figure 4d). This prediction can be tested in future experiments by making finer divisions between δf = 0.3 and 0.6 Hz.
In the human experiment, not only did we uncover the effect of diversity on intergroup relations, but also, non-trivially, on intragroup coordination (outside affects within, a sign of complexity). Statistically, this is shown in figure 3b (three bars on the left): with the presence of intergroup difference (δf > 0), intragroup coordination was reduced (red, yellow bars significantly shorter than blue bar). This is well captured by the model as shown in figure 3d (two-way ANOVA interaction effect, F2,19194 = 3416, p < 0.001; the simulated data also capture the rapid decline of intergroup coordination with increasing δf in human data, shown in figure 3b,d, right). In addition to capturing this statistical reduction of intragroup coordination due to intergroup difference, the model, more importantly, provides a window to the dynamical mechanism underlying such statistical phenomena. For example, comparing three simulated trials with identical intragroup properties but different levels of intergroup difference (figure 5a–c), we see that the presence of intergroup difference (figure 5b,c for δf = 0.3, 0.6 Hz) dramatically elevates metastability in the system (compare intermittently converging–diverging dynamics in figure 5b,c to the rather constant behaviour in figure 5a for δf = 0). This suggests that the decrease of intragroup coordination in a statistical sense reflects the increase of metastability in a dynamical sense (see section ‘Examples of dynamics with intergroup coupling removed’ in the electronic supplementary materials for baseline dynamics when intergroup coupling is removed). Indeed, if we remove intragroup metastability from all simulations (by reducing intragroup frequency variability), they no longer capture the empirically observed statistical result (see section ‘Effect of reduced intragroup variability in natural frequency’ in the electronic supplementary materials).
At the interpersonal level, human subjects tended to coordinate with each other around inphase and antiphase, especially when the diversity is low (δf = 0 Hz; figure 2d, peaks around ϕ = 0, π separated by a trough near ϕ = π/2); and the preference for inphase and antiphase both diminishes as diversity increases (δf = 0.3, 0.6, figure 2e,f ). Both aspects are well reproduced in simulations of the model (figure 5d–f). Note that these model-based distributions are overall less dispersed than the more variable human-produced distributions (figure 2d–f), likely due to the deterministic nature of the model (i.e. no stochastic terms). Yet as demonstrated above, a deterministic model is sufficient for capturing key empirical results at all three levels of description, i.e. coexistence of inphase and antiphase tendencies and their reduction with diversity, reduction of intragroup coordination with the presence of intergroup difference, and intergroup integration∼segregation at different levels of diversity. Thus, the deterministic version of the model is preferred for simplicity.
2.4. The necessity of second-order coupling
Equation (2.3) becomes the classical Kuramoto model [2] when b = 0. We follow the same analyses as in the previous section but now for a = 0.154 and b = 0 (see section ‘Intergroup relation without second order coupling’ in the electronic supplementary material on parameter choices). The relationship between intragroup and intergroup coordination (electronic supplementary material, figure S8A; β1(0 Hz) = 0.974, t199 = 53.2, p < 0.001; β1(0.3 Hz) = 0.292, t199 = 4.52, p < 0.001; β1(0.6 Hz) = −0.011, t199 = −0.41, p > 0.05) resembles the case of b ≠ 0 (a = b = 0.105, figure 3c). A difference remains that for b = 0, β1(0.6 Hz) is not significantly less than zero (p = 0.68; electronic supplementary material, figure S8A yellow). The average level of intragroup and intergroup coordination also varies with diversity in the same way as the case of b ≠ 0 (electronic supplementary material, figure S8b for b = 0, interaction effect F2,19194 = 3737, p < 0.001, compared to figure 3d for b ≠ 0). In short, group-level statistical features can be mostly preserved without second-order coupling (i.e. b = 0).
However, this is no longer the case when it comes to interpersonal relations. The distributions of dyadic relative phases are shown in figure 5g–i. Without second-order coupling, the model does not show a preference for antiphase in any of the three diversity conditions, thereby missing an important feature of human social coordination (for additional comparisons between human and model behaviour, see section ‘Additional analyses on the coexistence of inphase and antiphase preference’ in the electronic supplementary materials). Analytically, we find that the coupling ratio κ = 2b/a determines whether antiphase is preferred (for the simple case of identical oscillators, see section ‘Multistability of the present model’ in the electronic supplementary materials). A critical coupling ratio κc = 1 demarcates the regimes of monostability (only all-inphase is stable for κ < 1) and multistability (any combination of inphase and antiphase is stable for κ > 1). This critical ratio (for equation (2.3)) is identical to the critical coupling of the HKB model [29], where the transition between monostability (inphase) and multistability (inphase and antiphase) occurs (equation (2.2); parameters in the two equations map to each other by and ). This shows how equation (2.3) is a natural N-dimensional generalization of the extended HKB model, in terms of multistability and order-to-order transitions.
2.5. The effect of non-uniform coupling
So far, our model has captured very well experimental observations with the simple assumption of uniform coupling. However, loosening this assumption is necessary for understanding detailed dynamics. Here is an example from [24] (figure 6a), where coordination among three agents (1, 3 and 4, labels of locations on LED arrays) is visualized as the dynamics of two relative phases (ϕ13 red, ϕ34 yellow). Agents 3 and 4 coordinated inphase persistently (10–40 s yellow trajectory flat at ϕ34 ≈ 0), while agents 3 and 1 coordinated intermittently every time they passed by inphase (red trajectory ϕ13 becames flat, i.e. dwells, near inphase around 10, 20 and 35 s). Curiously, every dwell in ϕ13 (red) was accompanied by a little bump in ϕ34, suggesting ϕ34 was periodically influenced by ϕ13. In the framework of our model, we can approximate the dynamics of ϕ34 from equation (2.1) by assuming ϕ34 = 0 (thus ϕ13 = ϕ14),
2.4 |
where f(ϕ34) is the influence of ϕ34 on itself, K(ϕ13) the influence of ϕ13 on ϕ34. From K(ϕ13), we see that ϕ13 has no influence on ϕ34 if the coupling is completely uniform (i.e. K(ϕ13) ≡ 0 if a31 = a41 and b31 = b41), making it impossible to capture the empirical observation (red relation influencing yellow relation, figure 6a). To break the symmetry between agent 3 and 4, we ‘upgrade’ equation (2.3) to the system
2.5 |
where each oscillator can have its own coupling style (oscillator specific coupling strength ai and bi). In the present case, we are interested in what happens when a3 ≠ a4 for i ∈ {1, 3, 4}. Two simulated trials are shown in figure 6b and c with non-uniform versus uniform coupling (same initial conditions and natural frequencies across trials, estimated from the human data). The bumps in ϕ34, accompanying dwells in ϕ13, are reproduced when a3 ≫ a4 (figure 6b) but not when a3 = a4 (figure 6c; see section ‘Additional triadic dynamics’ in the electronic supplementary materials for more analyses). This example shows that to understand interesting dynamic patterns in specific trials, non-uniform coupling strength is important.
3. Discussion
The present model successfully captures key features of multiagent coordination in mid-scale ensembles at multiple levels of description [24]. Similar to the HKB model [29], second-order coupling is demanded by the experimental observation of antiphase (and associated multistability) but now in eight-person coordination; and similar to the extended HKB [37], the model captures how increasing frequency difference δf weakens inphase and antiphase patterns, leading to segregation but now between two groups instead of two persons. This cross-scale consistency of experimental observations may be explained by the scale-invariant nature of the critical coupling ratio κc = 1, the transition point between monostability (only an all-inphase state) and multistability (states containing any number of antiphase relations). The scale invariance suggests that experimental methods and conclusions for small-scale coordination dynamics have implications for multistability, phase transitions and metastability at larger scales, and enables a unified approach to biological coordination that meshes statistical mechanics and nonlinear dynamics.
Another generalization of the classical Kuramoto model by Hong & Strogatz [45] also allows for antiphase-containing patterns (π-state) by letting the sign of the first-order coupling (a) be positive for some oscillators (the conformists) and negative for others (the contrarians). However, in contrast to our model, antiphase induced this way does not come with multistability, nor the associated order-to-order transitions observed in human rhythmic coordination [13,46]. The second-order coupling in our model allows each individual to be both a conformist and a contrarian but possibly to different degrees [47]. The simple addition of a second stable state may not seem like a big plus at N = 2 (2 stable states), but it rapidly expands the system’s behavioural repertoire as the system becomes larger (2N−1 stable states for N oscillators; with only first-order coupling, the system always has 1N−1 = 1 stable state, and therefore does not benefit from scaling up). This benefit of scale may be how micro-level multistability contributes to the functional complexity of biological systems [43,48].
Outside of the mathematical context of stability analysis, we have to recall that spontaneous social coordination is highly metastable (e.g. figure 2a) [24], captured by the model when frequency diversity is combined with weak coupling (e.g. figure 4a, in contrast to b,c under stronger coupling). Individuals did not become phase-locked in the long term, but coordinated temporarily when passing by a preferred state (inphase and antiphase) [14,43] (e.g. red trajectory in figure 6a). For N > 2, an ensemble can visit different spatial organizations sequentially (see examples in [24]), forming patterns that extend in both space and time (electronic supplementary material, figure S4 for intragroup patterns), which further expands the repertoire of coordinative behaviour (see section ‘A note on metastability’ in the electronic supplementary materials). By allowing complex patterns to be elaborated over time, metastability makes a viable mechanism for encoding complex information as real-world complex living systems do (e.g. the brain) [13,14,22,49–52]. By contrast, highly coherent patterns like collective synchronization can be less functional and even pathological [53,54]. Our results call for more attention to these not-quite coherent but empirically relevant patterns of coordination.
Besides the multistability or multi-clustering in micro patterns (a general feature endowed by higher-order coupling, e.g. [55–57]), existing mathematical studies suggest that the presence of second-order coupling should also manifest at the macro level in large-scale coordination. Naturally, second-order coupling induces multistability of the order parameter in the thermodynamic limit [58–61]. It also alters the critical scaling of macroscopic order (see [41] for a summary), i.e. for coupling strength K > Kc near Kc, the order parameter ∥H∥ (norm of the order function [62]) is proportional to , with β = 1/2 for the classical Kuramoto model and β = 1 when second-order coupling is added [63,64]. For complex biological systems like the brain which appears to operate near criticality [65], these two types of scaling behaviour may have very different functional implications. When modelling empirical data of biological coordination, one may want to have a closer examination or re-examination of the data for multistability and critical scaling of the order parameter, especially if finer level details are not available.
Key experimental observations are captured by our model under the assumptions of uniform coupling (everyone coordinates with others in the same way) and constant natural frequency, but these assumptions may be loosened to reflect detailed dynamics. For example, introducing individual differences in coupling style (equation (2.5)) gives more room to explain how one metastable phase relation may exert strong influence on another (figure 6a). Long timescale dynamics observed in the experiment (see section ‘Additional triadic dynamics’ in the electronic supplementary materials) may also be explained by frequency adaptation, which has been observed in dyadic social coordination [66]. A systematic study of the consequences of asymmetric coupling and frequency adaptation on coordination among multiple agents seems worthy of further experimental and theoretical exploration.
To conclude, we proposed a model that captured key features of human social coordination in mid-sized ensembles [24], and at the same time connects empirically validated large-scale and small-scale models of biological coordination. The model provides mechanistic explanations of the statistics and dynamics already observed, as well as a road map for future empirical exploration. As an experimental–theoretical platform for understanding biological coordination, the value of the middle scale should not be underestimated, nor the importance of examining coordination phenomena at multiple levels of description.
4. Material and methods
4.1. Methods of the human experiment
A complete description of the methods of the ‘Human Firefly’ experiment can be found in [24]. Here we provide as many details as necessary for understanding the present paper. A total of 120 subjects participated in the experiment, making up 15 independent ensembles of eight people. The protocol was approved by Florida Atlantic University Institutional Review Board and is in agreement with the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to the experiment.
For an ensemble of eight people, each subject was equipped with a touchpad that recorded his/her tapping behaviour as a series of zeros and ones at 250 Hz (1 = touch, 0 = detach; green rectangles in figure 1), and an array of eight LEDs arranged in a ring (yellow in figure 1), each of which flashed when a particular subject tapped. For each trial, subjects were first paced with metronomes for 10 s, later interacting with each other for 50 s (instructed to maintain metronome frequency while looking at others’ taps as flashes of the LEDs). Between the pacing and interaction period, there was a 3 s transient, during which subjects tapped by themselves. Participants were instructed to match their own tapping frequency to the metronome frequency during the 10 s pacing period, and remain tapping at that frequency throughout the rest of the trial even after the metronome disappeared.
During pacing, four subjects received the same metronome (same frequency, random initial phase), and the other four another metronome. The metronome assignments created two frequency groups (say, group A and B) with intergroup difference δf = |fA − fB| = 0, 0.3 or 0.6 Hz (same average (fA + fB)/2 = 1.5 Hz). This gives rise to three conditions: (1) 1.5 Hz versus 1.5 Hz, (2) 1.65 Hz versus 1.35 Hz, and (3) 1.8 Hz versus 1.2 Hz. Each ensemble completed six trials per condition (a total of 18 trials in random order). From a single subject’s perspective, the LED array looks like the legend of figure 2a (all LEDs emit white light; colour-coding only for labelling locations): a subject always saw his/her own taps as the flashes of LED 1, members of his/her own frequency group LED 2–4, and members of the other group LED 5–8 (members from two groups were interleaved to preserve spatial symmetry).
From the tapping data (rectangular waves of zeros and ones), we obtained the onset of each tap, from which we calculated instantaneous frequency and phase. Instantaneous frequency is the reciprocal of the interval between two consecutive taps. Phase (φ) is calculated by assigning the onset of the nth tap phase 2π(n − 1), then interpolating the phase between onsets with a cubic spline. The relative phase between the ith and jth subject at time t is ϕij(t) = φi(t) − φj(t).
4.2. Estimating the distribution of natural frequencies
Human subjects have variable capability to match the metronome frequency and maintain it, which in turn affects how they coordinate. To reflect this kind of variability in the simulations, the oscillators’ natural frequencies were drawn from a probability distribution around the ‘metronome frequency’ (central frequencies fA and fB for groups A and B). To estimate this distribution from human data, we first approximated the ‘natural frequency’ of each subject in each trial with the average tapping frequency during the transient between pacing and interaction periods (see Methods of the human experiment), and subtracted from it the metronome frequency (see blue histogram in electronic supplementary material, figure S3 from the ‘Human Firefly’ experiment [24]). We then estimated the distribution non-parametrically, with a kernel density estimator in the form of
4.1 |
where the Kernel Smoothing Function is Normal, . Here n = 2072 (259 trials × 8 subjects) from the experiment. We choose the bandwidth h = 0.0219, which is optimal for a normal density function according to [67],
4.2 |
where σ is the measure of dispersion, estimated by
4.3 |
where yi’s are samples [68]. The result of the estimation is shown in electronic supplementary material, figure S3 (red curve).
4.3. Phase-locking value and level of integration
The (short-windowed) phase-locking value (PLV) between two oscillators (say x and y) during a trial is defined as
4.4 |
where ϕxy = φx − φy, W is the number of windows which each ϕ trajectory is split into, and M is number of samples in each window (in the present study, W = 16 and M = 750, same as [24]).
Intragroup PLV (PLVintra) is defined as
4.5 |
where A and B are two frequency groups of four oscillators, corresponding to the design of the ‘Human Firefly’ experiment [24], A = {1, 2, 3, 4}, B = {5, 6, 7, 8} and |A| = |B| = 4.
Intergroup PLV (PLVinter) is defined as
4.6 |
In both the human and simulated data, comparisons of PLVintra and PLVinter for different levels of δf were done using two-way ANOVA with Type III sums of squares, and Tukey honest significant difference tests for post-hoc comparisons (shown in figure 3b,d).
The level of integration between two frequency groups is defined based on the relationship between intragroup coordination (measured by PLVintra) and intergroup coordination (measured by PLVinter). The groups are said to be integrated if intragroup coordination is positively related to intergroup coordination, and segregated if negatively related. Quantitatively, for each combination of intergroup difference δf and coupling strength a (assuming a = b for our model, assuming b = 0 for the classical Kuramoto model), we use linear regression
4.7 |
where is the inter/intra-group PLV for the kth trial simulated with the parameter pair (δf, a), and the slope of the regression line is defined as the measure of the level of integration between two frequency groups. If β1 > 0, the groups may be said to be integrated; if β1 < 0, segregated. The set is the critical boundary between the domains of intergroup integration and segregation. Theoretical analyses (section ‘Choosing the appropriate coupling strength’ in the electronic supplementary materials) show that this measure is meaningful (i.e. reflecting qualitative differences between dynamics; figure 4a–c).
4.4. Method of simulation
All simulations were done using the Runge–Kutta 4th-order integration scheme, with a fixed time step Δt = 0.004 for duration T = 50 (matching the sampling interval and the duration of interaction period of the human experiment [24]; second may be used as unit), i.e. for system , with initial condition X(0) = X0, the (n + 1)th sample of the numeric solution can be solved recursively
4.8 |
where
4.9 |
4.10 |
4.11 |
4.12 |
The solver was implemented in CUDA C++, ran on a NVIDIA graphics processing unit, solving every 200 trials in parallel for each parameter pair (δf, a). For each trial, initial phases (of eight oscillators) were drawn randomly from a uniform distribution between 0 and 2π, and natural frequencies from distributions defined by equation (4.2) (reflecting the design of and variability observed in the human experiment [24]). Here 200 trials are used per condition, greater than that of the human experiment (see [24] and section ‘Design of the human experiment’ in the electronic supplementary materials for details) to obtain a more accurate estimate of the mean.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Dr Roxana Stefanescu for assisting in data collection of the human experiment.
Ethics
The protocol of the human experiment was approved by Florida Atlantic University Institutional Review Board and is in agreement with the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to the experiment.
Data accessibility
Data of the human experiment are available online at doi:10.17605/OSF.IO/SC9P6. Simulated data are available at doi:10.17605/OSF.IO/PB7DH.
Authors' contributions
M.Z. designed and conducted the human experiment, developed the theoretical model, performed simulations and mathematical analysis of the model, analysed human behavioural data and simulated data, and drafted the manuscript; C.B. participated in the design of the human experiment, participated in the development and mathematical analysis of the model and critically revised the manuscript; J.A.S.K. participated in the design of the human experiment, participated in the development of the model and critically revised the manuscript; E.T. participated in the design of the human experiment, participated in the development of the model and critically revised the manuscript.
Funding
The authors of this work were supported by NIMH grant no. MH080838, NIBIB grant no. EB025819, the FAU Foundation, FAU I-SENSE and FAU Brain Institute.
References
- 1.Winfree AT. 1987. The timing of biological clocks. New York, NY: Scientific American Books. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Kuramoto Y. 1984. Chemical oscillations, waves, and turbulence, vol. 19 of Springer Series in Synergetics. Berlin, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Glass L. 2001. Synchronization and rhythmic processes in physiology. Nature 410, 277–284. ( 10.1038/35065745) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Nakagaki T, Yamada H, Ueda T. 2000. Interaction between cell shape and contraction pattern in the Physarum plasmodium. Biophys. Chem. 84, 195–204. ( 10.1016/S0301-4622(00)00108-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kobayashi R, Tero A, Nakagaki T. 2006. Mathematical model for rhythmic protoplasmic movement in the true slime mold. J. Math. Biol. 53, 273–286. ( 10.1007/s00285-006-0007-0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Buck J, Buck E. 1966. Biology of synchronous flashing of fireflies. Nature 211, 562–564. ( 10.1038/211562a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Mirollo RE, Strogatz SH. 1990. Synchronization of pulse-coupled biological oscillators. SIAM J. Appl. Math. 50, 1645–1662. ( 10.1137/0150098) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Néda Z, Ravasz E, Brechet Y, Vicsek T, Barabási AL. 2000. The sound of many hands clapping. Nature 403, 849–850. ( 10.1038/35002660) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Richardson MJ, Marsh KL, Isenhower RW, Goodman JRL, Schmidt RCC. 2007. Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Hum. Mov. Sci. 26, 867–891. ( 10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Grillner S. 1985. Neurobiological bases of rhythmic motor acts in vertebrates. Science 228, 143–149. ( 10.1126/science.3975635) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Marder E, Calabrese RL. 1996. Principles of rhythmic motor pattern generation. Physiol. Rev. 76, 687–717. ( 10.1152/physrev.1996.76.3.687) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bressler SL, Kelso JAS. 2001. Cortical coordination dynamics and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 26–36. ( 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01564-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kelso JAS. 1995. Dynamic patterns: the self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Tognoli E, Kelso JAS. 2014. The metastable brain. Neuron 81, 35–48. ( 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.022) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Winfree AT. 2001. The geometry of biological time, vol. 12 of interdisciplinary applied mathematics. New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Schöner G, Kelso JAS. 1988. Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. Science 239, 1513–1520. ( 10.1126/science.3281253) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rand RH, Cohen AH, Holmes PJ. 1988. Systems of coupled oscillators as models of central pattern generators. In Neural control of rhythmic movements in vertebrates (ed. AH Cohen), pp. 333–367. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
- 18.Kopell N, Ermentrout GB. 1988. Coupled oscillators and the design of central pattern generators. Math. Biosci. 90, 87–109. ( 10.1016/0025-5564(88)90059-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Jeka JJ, Kelso JAS, Kiemel T. 1993. Pattern switching in human multilimb coordination dynamics. Bull. Math. Biol. 55, 829–845. ( 10.1007/BF02460675) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Yokoyama K, Yamamoto Y. 2011. Three people can synchronize as coupled oscillators during sports activities. PLoS Comput. Biol. 7, e1002181 ( 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002181) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Acebrón JA, Bonilla LL, Pérez Vicente CJ, Ritort F, Spigler R. 2005. The Kuramoto model: a simple paradigm for synchronization phenomena. Rev. Mod. Phys. 77, 137–185. ( 10.1103/RevModPhys.77.137) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Breakspear M, Heitmann S, Daffertshofer A. 2010. Generative models of cortical oscillations: neurobiological implications of the Kuramoto model. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4, 1–14. ( 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00190) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Castellano C, Fortunato S, Loreto V. 2009. Statistical physics of social dynamics. Rev. Mod. Phys. 81, 591–646. ( 10.1103/RevModPhys.81.591) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Zhang M, Kelso JAS, Tognoli E. 2018. Critical diversity: divided or united states of social coordination. PLoS ONE 13, e0193843 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0193843) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Schöner G, Jiang WY, Kelso JAS. 1990. A synergetic theory of quadrupedal gaits and gait transitions. J. Theoret. Biol. 142, 359–391. ( 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80558-2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Collins JJ, Stewart I. 1993. Coupled nonlinear oscillators and the symmetries of animal gaits. J. Nonlinear Sci. 3, 349–392. ( 10.1007/BF02429870) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Golubitsky M, Stewart I, Buono PLL, Collins JJ. 1999. Symmetry in locomotor central pattern generators and animal gaits. Nature 401, 693–695. ( 10.1038/44416) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kelso JAS. 1984. Phase transitions and critical behaviour in human bimanual coordination. Amer. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 246, R1000–R1004. ( 10.1152/ajpregu.1984.246.6.r1000) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Haken H, Kelso JAS, Bunz H. 1985. A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biol. Cybern 51, 347–356. ( 10.1007/BF00336922) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Marder E, Bucher D. 2001. Central pattern generators and the control of rhythmic movements. Curr. Biol. 11, R986–R996. ( 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00581-4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Tognoli E, Lagarde J, DeGuzman GC, Kelso JAS. 2007. The phi complex as a neuromarker of human social coordination. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 8190–8195. ( 10.1073/pnas.0611453104) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Schmidt RC, Richardson MJ. 2008. Dynamics of interpersonal coordination. In Coordination: neural, behavioral and social dynamics, vol. 2008 (eds A Fuchs, VK Jirsa), pp. 281–308. Berlin, Germany: Springer.
- 33.Lagarde J, Peham C, Licka T, Kelso JAS. 2005. Coordination dynamics of the horse–rider system. J. Mot. Behav. 37, 418–424. ( 10.3200/JMBR.37.6.418-424) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kelso JAS, de Guzman GC, Reveley C, Tognoli E. 2009. Virtual partner interaction (VPI): exploring novel behaviors via coordination dynamics. PLoS ONE 4, e5749 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0005749) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Dumas G, de Guzman GC, Tognoli E, Kelso JAS. 2014. The human dynamic clamp as a paradigm for social interaction. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, E3726–E3734. ( 10.1073/pnas.1407486111) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hoyt DF, Taylor CR. 1981. Gait and the energetics of locomotion in horses. Nature 292, 239–240. ( 10.1038/292239a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kelso JAS, Del Colle JD, Schöner G. 1990. Action-perception as a pattern formation process. In Attention and performance 13: motor representation and control, vol. 45 (ed. M Jeannerod), pp. 139–169. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
- 38.Fuchs A, Jirsa VK, Haken H, Kelso JAS. 1996. Extending the HKB model of coordinated movement to oscillators with different eigenfrequencies. Biol. Cybern 74, 21–30. ( 10.1007/BF00199134) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Néda Z, Ravasz E, Vicsek T, Brechet Y, Barabási ALL. 2000. Physics of the rhythmic applause. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Phys. Plasmas Fluids Related Interdiscip. Top. 61, 6987–6992. ( 10.1103/physreve.61.6987) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Leonard NE, Shen T, Nabet B, Scardovi L, Couzin ID, Levin Sa. 2012. Decision versus compromise for animal groups in motion. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 227–232. ( 10.1073/pnas.1118318108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Strogatz SH. 2000. From Kuramoto to Crawford: exploring the onset of synchronization in populations of coupled oscillators. Phys. D Nonlinear Phenom. 143, 1–20. ( 10.1016/S0167-2789(00)00094-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Acebrón JA, Bonilla LL, De Leo S, Spigler R. 1998. Breaking the symmetry in bimodal frequency distributions of globally coupled oscillators. Phys. Rev. E 57, 5287–5290. ( 10.1103/PhysRevE.57.5287) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kelso JAS. 2012. Multistability and metastability: understanding dynamic coordination in the brain. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367, 906–918. ( 10.1098/rstb.2011.0351) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Schmidt RC, Turvey MT. 1994. Phase-entrainment dynamics of visually coupled rhythmic movements. Biol. Cybern 70, 369–376. ( 10.1007/BF00200334) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hong H, Strogatz SH. 2011. Kuramoto model of coupled oscillators with positive and negative coupling parameters: an example of conformist and contrarian oscillators. Phys. Rev. Lett. 106, 054102 ( 10.1103/PhysRevLett.106.054102) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Fuchs A, Kelso JAS. 1994. A theoretical note on models of interlimb coordination. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 20, 1088–1097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kelso JAS, Engstrom DA. 2006. The complementary nature. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Laurent M, Kellershohn N. 1999. Multistability: a major means of differentiation and evolution in biological systems. Trends Biochem. Sci. 24, 418–422. ( 10.1016/S0968-0004(99)01473-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Friston KJ. 1997. Transients, metastability, and neuronal dynamics. NeuroImage 5, 164–171. ( 10.1006/nimg.1997.0259) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Tognoli E, Kelso JAS. 2014. Enlarging the scope: grasping brain complexity. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8, 122 ( 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00122) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Ashwin P, Orosz G, Wordsworth J, Townley S. 2007. Dynamics on networks of cluster states for globally coupled phase oscillators. SIAM J. Appl. Dyn. Syst. 6, 728–758. ( 10.1137/070683969) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Rabinovich MI, Huerta R, Varona P, Afraimovich VS. 2008. Transient cognitive dynamics, metastability, and decision making. PLoS Comput. Biol. 4, e1000072 ( 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000072) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Jirsa VK, Stacey WC, Quilichini PP, Ivanov AI, Bernard C. 2014. On the nature of seizure dynamics. Brain 137, 2210–2230. ( 10.1093/brain/awu133) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Tang E. et al. 2017. Developmental increases in white matter network controllability support a growing diversity of brain dynamics. Nat. Commun. 8, 1252 ( 10.1038/s41467-017-01254-4) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Okuda K. 1993. Variety and generality of clustering in globally coupled oscillators. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenom. 63, 424–436. ( 10.1016/0167-2789(93)90121-G) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hansel D, Mato G. 1993. Patterns of synchrony in a heterogeneous Hodgkin–Huxley neural network with weak coupling. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Appl. 200, 662–669. ( 10.1016/0378-4371(93)90573-M) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Hansel D, Mato G, Meunier C. 1993. Clustering and slow switching in globally coupled phase oscillators. Phys. Rev. E 48, 3470–3477. ( 10.1103/PhysRevE.48.3470) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Komarov M, Pikovsky A. 2013. Multiplicity of singular synchronous states in the Kuramoto model of coupled oscillators. Phys. Rev. Lett. 111, 204101 ( 10.1103/PhysRevLett.111.204101) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Komarov M, Pikovsky A. 2014. The Kuramoto model of coupled oscillators with a bi-harmonic coupling function. Phys. D Nonlinear Phenom. 289, 18–31. ( 10.1016/j.physd.2014.09.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Vlasov V, Komarov M, Pikovsky A. 2015. Synchronization transitions in ensembles of noisy oscillators with bi-harmonic coupling. J. Phys. A: Math. Theor. 48, 105101 ( 10.1088/1751-8113/48/10/105101) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Clusella P, Politi A, Rosenblum M. 2016. A minimal model of self-consistent partial synchrony. New J. Phys. 18, 093037 ( 10.1088/1367-2630/18/9/093037) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Daido H. 1992. Order function and macroscopic mutual entrainment in uniformly coupled limit-cycle oscillators. Progress Theoret. Phys. 88, 1213–1218. ( 10.1143/ptp/88.6.1213) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Daido H. 1994. Generic scaling at the onset of macroscopic mutual entrainment in limit-cycle oscillators with uniform all-to-all coupling. Phys. Rev. Lett. 73, 760–763. ( 10.1103/PhysRevLett.73.760) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Crawford JD, Davies KTR. 1999. Synchronization of globally-coupled phase oscillators: singularities and scaling for general couplings. Phys. D Nonlinear Phenom. 125, 62 ( 10.1016/S0167-2789(98)00235-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Chialvo DR. 2010. Emergent complex neural dynamics. Nat. Phys. 6, 744 ( 10.1038/nphys1803) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Nordham CA, Tognoli E, Fuchs A, Kelso JAS. 2018. How interpersonal coordination affects individual behavior (and vice versa): experimental analysis and adaptive HKB model of social memory. Ecol. Psychol. 7413, 1–26. ( 10.1080/10407413.2018.1438196) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Bowman AW, Azzalini A. 1997. Applied smoothing techniques for data analysis. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Hogg RV. 1979. Statistical robustness: one view of its use in applications today. Amer. Stat. 33, 108–115. ( 10.2307/2683810) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data of the human experiment are available online at doi:10.17605/OSF.IO/SC9P6. Simulated data are available at doi:10.17605/OSF.IO/PB7DH.