Abstract
Identifying key neural substrates in addiction disorders for targeted drug development remains a major challenge for clinical neuroscience. One emerging target is the opioid system, where substance‐dependent populations demonstrate prefrontal opioid dysregulation that predicts impulsivity and relapse. This may suggest that disturbances to the prefrontal opioid system could confer a risk for relapse in addiction due to weakened ‘top‐down’ control over impulsive behaviour. Naltrexone is currently licensed for alcohol dependence and is also used clinically for impulse control disorders. Using a go/no‐go (GNG) task, we examined the effects of acute naltrexone on the neural correlates of successful motor impulse control in abstinent alcoholics (AUD), abstinent polysubstance‐dependent (poly‐SUD) individuals and controls during a randomised double blind placebo controlled fMRI study. In the absence of any differences on GNG task performance, the AUD group showed a significantly greater BOLD response compared to the control group in lateral and medial prefrontal regions during both placebo and naltrexone treatments; effects that were positively correlated with alcohol abstinence. There was also a dissociation in the positive modulating effects of naltrexone in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and anterior insula cortex (AIC) of the AUD and poly‐SUD groups respectively. Self‐reported trait impulsivity in the poly‐SUD group also predicted the effect of naltrexone in the AIC. These results suggest that acute naltrexone differentially amplifies neural responses within two distinct regions of a salience network during successful motor impulse control in abstinent AUD and poly‐SUD groups, which are predicted by trait impulsivity in the poly‐SUD group.
Keywords: addiction, functional MRI, impulsivity, naltrexone
Introduction
Impulsive behaviour has been implicated in the development and maintenance of addiction disorders (Verdejo‐Garcia et al., 2008; Volkow & Baler, 2012). Impulsivity likely arises due to a breakdown in mechanisms underlying prefrontal‐mediated inhibitory control (Goldstein & Volkow, 2002; Kaufman et al., 2003; Lubman et al., 2004) that may affect the ability to restrain actions related to continued substance use. Inhibitory control deficits have been commonly observed in various addiction populations (Fillmore & Rush, 2002; Fu et al., 2008; Rubio et al., 2008; Luijten et al., 2011), suggesting that the ability to inhibit impulsive actions is a core feature of addiction disorders, and a possible predictor of substance relapse (Moeller et al., 2001; Bowden‐Jones et al., 2005; Krishnan‐Sarin et al., 2007). Therefore, identifying neural substrates of impulse control in addiction disorders that could act as therapeutic targets for relapse prevention during abstinence remains a major challenge in human clinical neuroscience.
Pre‐clinical animal models show that the mu opioid receptor (MOR) plays a significant role in addiction (Giuliano et al., 2013), particularly with respect to alcohol‐seeking and consumption (Boyle et al., 1998; Dayas et al., 2007; Giuliano et al., 2015). Importantly, appetitive motivation for addictive substances may also occur due to disturbances in the prefrontal opioid system that could conceivably result in the recruitment of diverse and functionally opposed pathways (Baldo, 2016) that lead to a breakdown in mechanisms of inhibitory control, which promote substance relapse. Indeed, animal models have shown that the MOR promotes impulsive behaviours that are actually dissociated from alcohol or drug consumption (Olmstead et al., 2009; Pattij et al., 2009; Mahoney et al., 2013; Sanchez‐Roige et al., 2015), suggesting that opioid disturbances within prefrontal brain networks could be targets for diminishing impulsivity in addiction disorders. There is evidence for opioid dysregulation in the prefrontal cortex in human addiction disorders (Gorelick et al., 2005, 2008; Williams et al., 2007; Ghitza et al., 2010), with evidence that endogenous opioids may promote impulsivity in humans (Love et al., 2009). Clinical evidence also suggests that MOR blockade with naltrexone is effective in some impulse control disorders (Kim et al., 2001; Grant, 2005; Lahti et al., 2010; Grant et al., 2014), promotes abstinence in substance addiction populations (Krystal et al., 2001; Srisurapanont & Jarusuraisin, 2005; Grassi et al., 2007), possibly through the promotion of impulse control (Sanchez‐Roige et al., 2015). There is evidence that naltrexone can also ameliorate neural disturbances in addiction (Savulich et al., 2017; Morris et al., 2018), including those related to impulsive choice (Boettiger et al., 2009), pointing to its potential efficacy as a neuromodulator of impulse control. Therefore, evidence of disturbances to prefrontal endogenous opioid functioning, together with the clinical efficacy of naltrexone in treating impulse control and addiction disorders, suggests that this system may be a viable target for relapse prevention, where there are deficits related to impulsivity.
Comorbid alcohol and drug dependence may be particularly detrimental to health. Individuals who have been dependent on multiple substances report the consumption of more alcohol have a greater incidence and severity of psychiatric illness, than individuals who are exclusively alcohol‐dependent (Moss et al., 2015). This divergence in abuse and dependence, could conceivably, induce disparate neural responses that are a product of dependence ‘phenotype’, and that differentially respond to medications that modulate endogenous opioid functioning. Therefore, the current study investigated the effects of acute MOR blockade with naltrexone on the neural correlates of motor impulse control using a go/no‐go task (GNG) in alcoholic and polysubstance‐dependent individuals who were in extended abstinence. The GNG task is sensitive to the neural correlates of impulse control during heightened performance monitoring, exploiting the same prefrontal networks that are targets of the endogenous opioid system and MOR blockade with naltrexone. Therefore, we hypothesised that (i) compared to the control group, the alcoholic and polysubstance‐dependent groups would show poorer motor impulse control, (ii) compared to the control group, the alcoholic and polysubstance‐dependent groups would demonstrate disparate functional differences within prefrontal networks due to dependence ‘phenotype’, and (iii) that the alcoholic and polysubstance‐dependent groups would demonstrate disparate remediating effects on these functional differences following acute MOR blockade with naltrexone.
Material and methods
Participants
This was a randomised, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled multi‐centre study involving three study sites in the United Kingdom (Imperial College London, University of Cambridge and University of Manchester). For a more detailed description of the ICCAM Platform, see Paterson et al. (Paterson et al., 2015). Inclusion criteria were individuals who met DSM‐IV criteria for current or prior alcohol dependence (AUD), or alcohol plus (poly‐SUD) another substance of dependence (e.g., amphetamines, benzodiazepines, cocaine, opiates) and who were abstinent for at least 4 weeks prior to the experimental sessions. There was no upper limit for abstinence length. All participants were aged 21–64 years of age. For a full description of the cohort used in the current study, please see Nestor et al. (2017), which examined the same participant groups using a monetary incentive delay task (Nestor et al., 2017). In the current study, the AUD group was made up of 21 abstinent alcoholics, with the poly‐SUD group comprised of 25 abstinent alcoholic polysubstance‐dependent individuals (having met criteria for dependence to alcohol plus one or more other substances of dependence). The healthy control group was made up of 35 participants with no previous history of substance abuse, as assessed using the Alcohol, Smoking and Substance Involvement Screening Test (ASSIST) (WHO ASSIST Working Group, 2002) and timeline follow‐back. All participants were required to provide a negative breath alcohol test and a negative urine sample for various drugs of abuse on both experimental days (screening for the presence of amphetamines, benzodiazepines, cannabinoids, cocaine and opiates). The Mini‐International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) (Sheehan et al., 1998) was administered to all participants by a trained psychiatrist to screen for the presence of Axis I psychiatric disorders that were part of the study exclusion criteria.
Exclusion criteria included (i) current use of regular prescription or non‐prescription medications that could not be stopped for the study duration, or would interfere with study integrity or subject safety (including but not limited to antipsychotics, anticonvulsants, antidepressants, disulfiram, acamprosate, naltrexone, varenicline); (ii) current primary axis I diagnosis, past history of psychosis (unless drug‐induced); (iii) current or past history of enduring severe mental illness (e.g., schizophrenia, bipolar affective disorder); (iv) other current or past psychiatric history that, in the opinion of a psychiatrist, contraindicated participation; (v) history or presence of a significant neurological diagnosis that may have influenced the outcome or analysis of the results (including but not limited to stroke, epilepsy, space occupying lesions, multiple sclerosis, Parkinson's disease, vascular dementia, transient ischaemic attack, clinically significant head injury); (vi) claustrophobia or unable to lie still in the MRI scanner for up to 90 min; (vii) presence of a cardiac pacemaker, other electronic device or other MRI contraindication, including pregnancy, as assessed by a standard pre‐MRI questionnaire. Secondary or lifetime history of depression or anxiety was permitted in both substance abusers and healthy controls as these are very common psychiatric disorders.
All participants provided written informed consent. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical approval was obtained from West London and Gene Therapy Advisory Committee National Research Ethics Service committee (11/H0707/9) and relevant research governance and Participant Identification Centre (PIC) approvals obtained.
Experimental visits
At the randomised placebo and naltrexone experimental visits (visits 2 or 3), an eligibility check was performed. Participants’ intervening drug use and concomitant medication were checked and participants completed alcohol breath, pregnancy and urine drugs of abuse screening tests. Cigarette smokers in all groups smoked ad lib approximately 60 min prior to scanning in order to avoid the potential confounds of withdrawal and/or craving during scanning.
Medications
Drug preparation, labelling and packaging were performed by UCLH Pharmacy Manufacturing Unit. Placebo was Vitamin C (100 mg, supplier: Sigma, manufacturer: Norbrook) and naltrexone (50 mg Nalorex® ‐ manufacturer ‐ Bristol‐Myers Squibb) were prepared and packaged according to Investigational Medicinal Product guidelines. The maximum naltrexone plasma concentration after an acute 50 mg dose occurs between 0.5 and 3 h (Meyer et al., 1984). Therefore, participants were dosed 2 h prior to each experimental scan session to ensure high MOR occupancy during testing. Naltrexone and placebo medications were supplied in identical white opaque bottles and administered by independent nursing staff, such that both researcher and participant remained blinded.
Go/no‐go (GNG) task
The GNG task (Garavan et al., 2003) consisted of alternating target stimuli (the letters X and Y), each of which were presented for 900 milliseconds (ms), immediately followed by a 100 ms inter‐stimulus interval. Participants were required to make a response (on a hand‐held key pad) as quickly as possible to each stimulus (‘go’ trials). Participants were additionally required to inhibit their response (‘stop’ trials) when the target stimuli did not alternate (i.e. the second of two identical, successively presented target stimuli – e.g., respond to all stimuli except the fifth in the sequence XYXYYX). Participants were required to immediately recommence responding to the alternating stimuli following the presentation of a ‘stop’ stimulus. There were a total of 250 stimuli presented in each run of the task, of which 30 were ‘stop’ trials. Participants completed a total of two runs of the task, with each run lasting 262 s. Dependent measures for the task were the mean ‘go’ and ‘stop’ percentage accuracy, together with mean ‘go’ and mean ‘error’ response times (ms). The task was programmed using E‐Prime (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, USA).
Functional MRI (fMRI) data acquisition
For a more comprehensive description of data acquisition across the three sites, please see McGonigle et al. (McGonigle et al., 2017). Briefly, all centres operated MRI machines with a main magnetic field of 3 tesla (T). Centres in London and Cambridge operated nominally identical 3T Siemens Tim Trio systems running the syngo MR B17 software with a Siemens 32 channel receive‐only phased‐array head coil. The Manchester centre operated a 3T Philips Achieva running version 2.6.3.5 software and an 8 element SENSE head coil. For anatomical images, 160 high‐resolution T1‐weighted anatomic MPRAGE axial images (FOV 256 mm, thickness 1.0 mm, voxel size 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0) were acquired (total duration 303 s). Functional data were acquired using a T2* weighted echo‐planar imaging sequence collecting 36 non‐contiguous (0% gap) 3.0 mm axial slices covering the entire brain (TE = 31 ms, TR = 2000 ms, FOV 225 mm, 64 × 64 mm matrix size in Fourier space). Each run of the GNG task produced a total of 161 volumes of functional MRI data.
GNG fMRI data analyses
Data pre‐processing and statistical analysis were conducted using FEAT (fMRI Expert Analysis Tool) from the FMRIB Software Library (www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Pre‐statistical processing was as follows: motion correction utilising FMRIB's Linear Image Registration Tool (MCFLIRT; non‐brain matter removal using Brain Extraction Tool (BET); spatial smoothing with a 5‐mm full‐width half maximum Gaussian kernel; mean‐based intensity normalisation; non‐linear high‐pass temporal filtering (Gaussian‐weighted least squares straight line fit, with sigma = 25.0 s). The six rigid body movement parameters were also included as regressors in the model in FSL FEAT.
For each participant, first level whole brain mixed‐effects analyses were performed by separately modelling the ‘stop’ and ‘error’ trials (stick function regressors were convolved with the haemodynamic response function). The ‘go’ trials acted as the baseline for both the ‘stop’ and ‘error’ measures, as this period reflects tonic task‐related processes of the GNG task. Therefore, all reported differences are for activation changes vs. the baseline. Registration was conducted through a two‐step procedure, whereby EPI images were first registered to the high‐resolution T1 structural image, then into standard (Montreal Neurological Institute, MNI avg152 template) space, with 12‐parameter affine transformations.
Three (Group: AUD vs. poly‐SUD vs. control) by two (Drug: placebo vs. naltrexone) whole brain cluster‐based repeated measures ANOVA analyses were conducted as part of a higher‐level mixed‐effects analysis on the ‘stop’ and ‘error’ activation measures. These higher‐level analyses were conducted using FLAME (FMRIB's Local Analysis of Mixed Effects). Cluster (Gaussianised F) statistical images were determined by Z > 2.3 with a corrected cluster significance threshold of P < 0.05. This ANOVA analysis produced a total of three (i.e. drug effect, group effect, drug × group interaction) zf statistical images for the ‘stop’ and ‘error’ measures. Owing to scanner differences across the three sites, we used site as a covariate in the cluster‐based repeated measures ANOVA analyses.
Other statistics
Between‐groups demographics (see Table S1.) were examined using Kruskal–Wallis (gender distribution and drug order) or one‐way ANOVA analyses. For analyses conducted on the GNG behavioural measures, three (Group: AUD vs. poly‐SUD vs. control) by two (Drug: placebo vs. naltrexone) repeated measures ANOVA analyses were conducted. We also conducted a three (Group: AUD vs. poly‐SUD vs. control) by two (Drug: placebo vs. naltrexone) repeated measures ANOVA on an index of the relative ‘impulsivity’ value (RIV). This value is based on the ratio of mean reaction times on ‘go’ trials compared to that on ‘error’ trials – i.e. (“go” rt/“error” rt. Here a value >1 reflects a higher relative value of impulsive responding when a participant commits an ‘error’ on a no‐go trial. The mean BOLD signal change for each repeated measures zf statistical cluster was also extracted in order to conduct follow‐up post hoc analyses. These follow‐up analyses used either Bonferroni pair‐wise or independent t‐test analyses to examine group and interaction effects respectively. Correlations between demographics or baseline questionnaire measures and the mean BOLD signal change from the zf statistical clusters were assessed using Pearson's correlations. Owing to the variability in alcohol abstinence in both the AUD and poly‐SUD groups, data were Log (10) transformed prior to correlation analyses, to eliminate the influence of positive skew. These analyses were all conducted using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS Inc., Chicago).
Baseline questionnaires
We assessed self‐reported impulsivity at baseline (visit 1) using both the Barratt Impulsivity scale (BIS‐11) (Patton et al., 1995) and the UPPS‐P scale (Whiteside & Lynam, 2001). These measures were acquired to examine if reported baseline impulsivity could predict the delta (naltrexone minus placebo) mean BOLD signal change in brain regions of the AUD and poly‐SUD groups during ‘stop’ trials of the GNG task. Only the composite score on both these questionnaires was used when conducting the correlations.
Results
Demographics
The groups significantly differed on most of the measures reported (see Table S1 in supporting information for a more detailed description of demographic group differences). The AUD group was significantly older than the poly‐SUD group; the poly‐SUD group reported significantly less years of education, and had a lower estimated IQ, than the control group; the poly‐SUD group had significantly more cigarette use than the control group, and reported significantly more cannabis use than both controls and the poly‐SUD group. The AUD and poly‐SUD groups, however, did not significantly differ on cigarette use (pack years) or alcohol abstinence, but the AUD group did have significantly more alcohol exposure than the poly‐SUD group. Importantly, the three groups did not differ significantly on drug treatment order (χ2 = 0.48, df = 2, P = 0.78) during the study.
GNG performance
Figure 1A below shows the mean ‘go’ accuracy (%) for the AUD, poly‐SUD and control groups during the placebo and naltrexone sessions. A three (Group: AUD vs. poly‐SUD vs. control) by two (Drug: placebo vs. naltrexone) repeated measures ANOVA revealed no effect of drug (F = 2.52; df = 1, 78; P = 0.11), group (F = 1.76; df = 2, 78; P = 0.17) or a drug × group interaction (F = 0.31; df = 2, 78; P = 0.73). The same analysis for mean ‘go’ reaction time (ms) revealed no effect of drug (F = 0.03; df = 1, 78; P = 0.85), but did reveal trends for a group effect (F = 2.52; df = 2, 78; P = 0.087), and a drug × group interaction (F = 2.88; df = 2, 78; P = 0.062 – Fig. 1B). For mean ‘stop’ accuracy (%), there was no drug (F = 0.039; df = 1, 78; P = 0.84), or group effect (F = 0.27; df = 2, 78; P = 0.75), but there was a trend for a drug × group interaction (F = 2.64; df = 2, 78; P = 0.078 – Fig. 1C). For mean ‘error’ reaction time (ms), there was no effect of drug (F = 0.62; df = 1, 78; P = 0.43), group (F = 1.73; df = 2, 78; P = 0.18) or a drug × group interaction (F = 0.63; df = 2, 78; P = 0.53 – Fig. 1D). Finally, for the RIV index, there was no effect of drug (F = 1.06; df = 1, 78; P = 0.31), group (F = 1.93; df = 2, 78; P = 0.15) or a drug × group interaction (F = 0.44; df = 2, 78; P = 0.64 – Fig. 1E).
Functional MRI
All three groups demonstrated statistically significant activation patterns across lateral and midline frontal regions during the placebo and naltrexone challenges for ‘stops’ at a whole brain level – although these activation changes did appear more robust in the AUD group (see Figs S1 and S2). A three (Group: AUD vs. poly‐SUD vs. control) by two (Drug: placebo vs. naltrexone) whole brain cluster‐based repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of group in the right anterior cingulate cortex (280 voxels; x = 6; y = 52; z = 8; zfstat = 3.55; P = 0.003; AUD > control, P = 0.041Bonferroni ‐ Fig. 2A); left frontal pole/inferior frontal gyrus (366 voxels; x = −46; y = 42; z = 2; zfstat = 4.42; P = 0.003; AUD > control, P = 0.005Bonferroni – Fig. 2B); and right inferior frontal gyrus (491 voxels; x = 46; y = 30; z = 4; zfstat = 3.48; P = 0.002; AUD > control, P = 0.049Bonferroni).
The same analysis also revealed a significant drug × group interaction in the left orbitofrontal cortex (127 voxels; x = −42; y = 22; z = −12; zfstat = 4.1; P = 0.006 – Fig. 3A) and left anterior insula cortex (162 voxels; x = −38; y = 16; z = −2; zfstat = 4.04; P = 0.005 – Fig. 3B). Follow‐up independent t‐test analyses on the left orbitofrontal cortex cluster revealed that the AUD group demonstrated a significantly greater activation change compared to the poly‐SUD group on naltrexone (t(44) = 2.2, P = 0.033), with a trend for a greater activation change compared with the control group (t(54) = 1.7, P = 0.096). Analyses for the left anterior insula cluster revealed that the poly‐SUD group demonstrated a significantly greater activation change compared with the AUD group (t(54) = 2.9, P = 0.005) on naltrexone, with a trend for a greater activation change compared to the control group (t(58) = 1.9, P = 0.05). There was no effect of drug for ‘stops’, and there was no effect of drug, group or a drug × group interaction for ‘errors’ for the same whole brain cluster‐based repeated measures ANOVA.
Correlations
There were two significant correlations observed. Alcohol abstinence (months) was significantly positively correlated with the mean (of placebo and naltrexone) left frontal pole/inferior frontal gyrus ‘stop’ BOLD signal change (r(19) = 0.58, P = 0.005) in the AUD group (Fig. 4A) – the same cluster where this group was significantly higher than the control group across drug visits. The total baseline UPPS‐P score was also found to be significantly positively correlated with the delta (naltrexone minus placebo) ‘stop’ BOLD signal change in the anterior insula cortex (r(23) = 0.40, P = 0.027) of the poly‐SUD group (Fig. 4B) – the same cluster showing the drug × group interaction effect in the poly‐SUD group.
Discussion
The current study compared the effects of naltrexone on the neural correlates of motor impulse control in AUD, poly‐SUD and healthy controls using a GNG task. In the absence of any drug or group effects on GNG task performance, the AUD group demonstrated a significantly greater neural response, during both placebo and naltrexone treatments, in lateral and medial prefrontal regions compared to controls during response inhibition. Activation differences were significantly correlated with alcohol abstinence in the AUD group. We further report an apparent disparity in the positive modulating effects of acute naltrexone in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and anterior insula cortex (AIC) of the AUD and poly‐SUD groups respectively, possibly pointing to a differential neural effect in these populations during motor impulse control. The AIC naltrexone effect during motor impulse control was also positively correlated with reported trait impulsivity in the poly‐SUD group.
Greater neural effects in the AUD group during motor impulse control
The monitoring of one's behaviour during abstinence may be especially important when there is a need to override impulses, particularly those that might induce relapse (Garavan & Stout, 2005). Furthermore, treatment adherence may rely upon addicted individuals exercising greater prefrontal control over drug‐seeking behaviours (Goldstein & Volkow, 2002; Everitt et al., 2008). There is evidence that substance‐dependent groups in long‐term abstinence demonstrate increased prefrontal activation during tasks that exploit cognitive control (Nestor et al., 2011; Connolly et al., 2012; Kroenke et al., 2015). The current study showed that the AUD group, who were long‐term abstinent alcoholics, demonstrate significantly greater activation, compared to controls, in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and bilateral frontal pole/inferior frontal gyrus (FP/IFG) during motor impulse control.
The ACC has been implicated in the execution of urgent inhibitions, reflecting heightened performance monitoring (Garavan et al., 2002; Botvinick et al., 2004; Ridderinkhof et al., 2004); with functional disturbances in addiction populations in this region having previously been reported (Goldstein & Volkow, 2002; Peoples, 2002; Volkow et al., 2002). Similarly, the FP/IFG are involved in higher order cognition (Burgess et al., 2007), particularly response inhibition (Swick et al., 2008), and are co‐activated with regions such as the ACC during cognitive control (Gilbert et al., 2010). The amplified response in our group of abstinent alcoholics, therefore, may lend further credence to the supposition of emerging neurocognitive adaptations within prefrontal circuitry that promote long‐term abstinence, and possibly, protect against relapse in this population. Another possibility is that the greater activation patterns observed in the AUD group represent deficits in cortical grey matter volume (GMV) in the medial and lateral prefrontal regions. There is evidence that patients with AUD have density (Thayer et al., 2016) and volume deficits (Medina et al., 2008; Trick et al., 2014; Xiao et al., 2015; Gropper et al., 2016) in the prefrontal cortex, which may also be correlated with impulse control (Wiers et al., 2015; Gropper et al., 2016; Grodin et al., 2017), and predict future relapse (Cardenas et al., 2011). This raises the possibility that the effects observed in the AUD group represent some compensatory mechanism by which prefrontal networks expend excessive energy to support cognitive control processes in abstinence due to structural brain deficits caused by alcohol abuse. The correlation between alcohol abstinence and FP/IFG activation in the AUD group, however, may discount this possibility.
Addiction disorders, such as AUD, are generally associated with impairments in impulse control (Smith et al., 2014). There is a mixed literature regarding the neural correlates of impulse control in AUD, however. The reported activation pattern observed in the FP of the AUD group in the present study, for example, may concur with previous results in abstinent alcoholics during impulse control (Li et al., 2009), who similarly found increased activation in the bilateral prefrontal cortex using a stop signal task (SST). Another study has shown greater cortical activation patterns in AUD (Hu et al., 2015), but not in the prefrontal cortex, and in the presence of poorer impulse control. Other studies, however, have not reported group differences on the neural substrates of impulse control in AUD, instead showing associations between diminished prefrontal responses and harmful alcohol use (Hu et al., 2016) or no differences at all (Taylor et al., 2016), despite high self‐report measures of impulsivity. These differences in impulse control and its neural correlates in AUD across studies may reflect contrasting durations of abstinence at the time of testing that represent differences in the remission of impulsivity in AUD (Sinha, 2011; Naim‐Feil et al., 2014).
Disparate neural effects of naltrexone in the AUD and poly‐SUD groups during motor impulse control
The current study observed a divergent modulating effect of naltrexone in the OFC and AIC of the AUD and poly‐SUD groups respectively. The effects of naltrexone on the neural correlates of motor impulse control appeared to be confined to the left OFC in the AUD group, who showed a significantly greater response compared to the poly‐SUD group during naltrexone treatment. The OFC is considered to be a critical frontal region in the suppression of behaviours (Horn et al., 2003; Chikazoe et al., 2009), possibly through the updating of ‘salient’ events (Torregrossa et al., 2008). The OFC also contains a high number of MORs (Gorelick et al., 2005), possibly making it a target for the modulating effects of naltrexone in this region under conditions of motor impulse control. Naltrexone, which is currently licensed for alcohol dependence and impulse control disorders such as gambling, may remediate neural disturbances by restoring some balance within prefrontal circuitry that is critical for the suppression of impulsive responding in addiction disorders. Acute naltrexone has been shown to enhance activation in the lateral OFC of abstinent alcoholics during a delay discounting task (Boettiger et al., 2009), for example, providing some evidence for its neural effects during impulse control. The observed acute effects of naltrexone in the current study may further represent an enhancement of OFC functioning, possibly through its updating of ‘salient’ (no‐go) events that promotes behavioural inhibition.
The effects of acute naltrexone in the poly‐SUD group, by contrast, were observed in the left AIC, where they showed a significantly greater response compared to the AUD group during naltrexone treatment. The AIC responds under conditions of inhibitory control (Garavan et al., 1999), possibly through the detection (rather than inhibition) of ‘salient’ (no‐go) events (Uddin, 2015) involving behavioural risk (Preuschoff et al., 2008) and the requisite of avoidance behaviour (Krawitz et al., 2010). The insula cortex contains a high number of MORs (Zubieta et al., 2005), also making it a target for the modulating effects of naltrexone. We previously reported that the same poly substance group showed a diminished response in the left AIC during acute naltrexone in response to missed rewards on a monetary incentive delay task (Nestor et al., 2017), suggesting a blunting of error‐related signalling by naltrexone. The current study, however, suggests an opposing effect of naltrexone in the AIC, possibly by amplifying the detection of no‐go events under the demands of heightened performance monitoring. We additionally found that self‐reported trait impulsivity in the poly‐SUD group was significantly correlated with the delta signal in the AIC. This appears to suggest that the greatest neural effects of naltrexone in the AIC, under conditions requiring motor impulse control, were in those with highest reported levels of trait impulsivity prior to naltrexone treatment.
The apparent disparate modulating effects of naltrexone in the alcohol and poly substance groups, within two distinct regions of a salience network during the exploits of successful motor impulse control, however, remains unclear. These differences may have arisen due to the ‘compounding’ toxic effects of chronic alcohol and drug dependence in the poly‐SUD group. The poly‐SUD group was made up a heterogeneous sample, predominantly reporting previous dependence on amphetamines, cocaine and opiates in addition to alcohol. These long‐term toxic effects, therefore, may have conferred a response to MOR blockade in a region that is involved in the detection (rather than inhibition) of ‘salient’ events, possibly due to acute changes in the awareness of interoceptive (i.e. bodily) states (Critchley et al., 2004) during the demands of cognitive control. The OFC, conversely, is considered to be a frontal region critical in the suppression of behaviours, perhaps suggesting that acute MOR blockade with naltrexone in the AUD group remediates functioning in a region typically exploited during executive functioning. While speculative, these results may point, tentatively, towards a poly substance effect in the AIC that needs to be addressed in a larger sample.
There were a number of limitations of the current study, which included a lack of complete matching of groups with respect to age, cannabis and cigarette use, anxiety and mood measures. We did not use these metrics as covariates for any of our fMRI analyses, and therefore, cannot unequivocally dismiss their potential influence on the group and interaction effects reported herein. Moreover, we did not thoroughly assess alcohol and drug craving at either session in the AUD and poly‐SUD groups. This may have conceivably influenced the observed effects of naltrexone with respect to the neural correlates of cognition.
Conclusion
In summary, the current study set out to examine the acute modulating effects of MOR blockade upon the behavioural and neural correlates of motor impulse control in abstinent alcoholic and polysubstance‐dependent individuals. Here we have provided evidence that naltrexone differentially amplifies neural responses within two distinct regions of a salience network during the demands of successful motor impulse control, in two independent addiction populations who are in extended abstinence. Exaggerated responses in abstinent alcoholics may further support the notion of emerging neurocognitive adaptations within prefrontal networks that promote long‐term abstinence, and possibly, protect against relapse.
Conflict of interests
The authors declared the following potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: David J Nutt is an advisor to British National Formulary, MRC, General Medical Council, Department of Health, is President of the European Brain Council, past President of the British Neuroscience Association and European College of Neuropsychopharmacology, chair of the Independent Scientific Committee on Drugs (UK), is a member of the International Centre for Science in Drug Policy, advisor to Swedish government on drug, alcohol and tobacco research, editor of the Journal of Psychopharmacology, sits on advisory Boards at Lundbeck, MSD, Nalpharm, Orexigen, Shire, has received speaking honoraria (in addition to above) from BMS/Otsuka, GSK, Lilly, Janssen, Servier, is a member of the Lundbeck International Neuroscience Foundation, has received grants or clinical trial payments from P1vital, MRC, NHS, Lundbeck, has share options with P1vital, has been expert witness in a number of legal cases relating to psychotropic drugs, and has edited/written 27 books, some purchased by pharmaceutical companies. Trevor W Robbins has research grants with Eli Lilly and Lundbeck, has received royalties from Cambridge Cognition (CANTAB), has received editorial honoraria from Springer Verlag, Elsevier, Society for Neuroscience; has performed educational lectures for Merck, Sharpe and Dohme and does consultancy work for Cambridge Cognition, Eli Lilly, Lundbeck, Teva and Shire Pharmaceuticals. Barbara J Sahakian consults for Cambridge Cognition, PEAK and Mundipharma. William Deakin currently advises or carries out research funded by Autifony, Sunovion, Lundbeck, AstraZeneca and Servier. All payment is to the University of Manchester. Edward T Bullmore was employed half‐time by the University of Cambridge and half‐time by GSK during some of this work, and is a shareholder in GSK. Liam J Nestor was employed by GSK during some of this work. Eugenii Rabiner worked for GSK until 2011 and is a shareholder in GSK. He is a consultant to GSK, TEVA, Lightlake Therapeutics, AbbVie, and Roche.
Author contributions
Liam J Nestor, Anna Murphy, John McGonigle, Csaba Orban, Eleanor Taylor, Remy Flechais, Louise M Paterson, Dana Smith and Karen D Ersche helped set up the study and collected the data; John McGonigle organised the study database; John McGonigle and Liam J Nestor conducted statistical analyses; Liam J Nestor wrote the first draft of the manuscript and all other authors subsequently contributed to the manuscript.
Abbreviations
- 3T
3 tesla
- ACC
anterior cingulate cortex
- AIC
anterior insula cortex
- ANOVA
analysis of variance
- ASSIST
alcohol, smoking and substance involvement screening test
- AUD
alcohol use disorder
- BET
brain extraction tool
- BIS‐11
barratt impulsivity scale
- BOLD
blood oxygen level dependent
- FEAT
fMRI expert analysis tool
- FLAME
FMRIB's local analysis of mixed effects
- FMRIB
functional MRI of the brain
- fMRI
functional magnetic resonance imaging
- FP
frontal pole
- FSL
functional MRI of the brain software library
- GNG
go/no‐go
- IFG
inferior frontal gyrus
- IQ
intelligence quotient
- Log
logarithm
- MCFLIRT
motion correction functional MRI of the brain linear registration tool
- MNI
montreal neurological institute
- MOR
mu opioid receptor
- OFC
orbitofrontal cortex
- PIC
participant identification centre
- RIV
relative ‘impulsivity’ value
- SPSS
statistical package for the social sciences
- SST
stop signal task
- SUD
substance use disorder
- UCLH
university college London hospital
- UPPS‐P
urgency, premeditation, perseverance, sensation seeking, positive urgency, impulsive behaviour scale
Funding
The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship and/or publication of this article: This article presents independent research funded by the MRC as part of their addiction initiative (grant number G1000018). GSK kindly funded the functional and structural MRI scans that took place at the London site for this study.
Supporting information
Acknowledgements
ICCAM platform collaborators: David J Nutt, Anne Lingford‐Hughes, Louise M Paterson, John McGonigle, Remy Flechais, Csaba Orban, William Deakin, Rebecca Elliott, Anna Murphy, Eleanor Taylor, Trevor W Robbins, Karen D Ersche, Edward T Bullmore, John Suckling, Dana Smith, Laurence Reed, Filippo Passetti, Luca Faravelli, David Erritzøe, Inge Mick, Nicola Kalk, Adam Waldman, Liam J Nestor, Shankar Kuchibatla, Venkataramana Boyapati, Antonio Metastasio, Yetunde Faluyi, Emilio Fernandez‐Egea, Sanja Abbott, Barbara J Sahakian, Valerie Voon, Ilan Rabiner. The research was carried out at the NIHR/Wellcome Trust Imperial Clinical Research Facility, the NIHR/Wellcome Trust Cambridge Research Facility and Clinical Trials Unit at Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust, and is supported by the North West London, Eastern and Greater Manchester NIHR Clinical Research Networks. This article presents independent research funded by the Medical Research Council and supported by the NIHR CRF at Imperial College Healthcare NHS Trust. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the Medical Research Council, the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. The authors wish to thank research assistants Claire Whitelock, Heather Agyepong, Rania Christoforou and Natalie Cuzen for their help with data collection; Dr Sharon Morein‐Zamir for her help with data extraction; MR physicist Rex Newbould and MR technician, Jonathan Howard for their assistance with MR acquisition and task set‐up. The authors also thank their recruitment partners; Imperial College Healthcare NHS Trust, Central and North West London NHS Trust, Camden and Islington NHS Trust, Cambridge University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Norfolk and Suffolk NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, South Staffordshire and Shropshire NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester Mental Health NHS and Social Care Trust, Greater Manchester West NHS Foundation Trust, Pennine Care NHS Foundation Trust, Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust, Addaction, Foundation 66 and CRI (Crime Reduction Initiative). Trevor W Robbins, Barbara J Sahakian, John Suckling, Edward T Bullmore and Karen D Ersche also acknowledge the NIHR Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre (Mental Health Theme).
Edited by Rita Goldstein. Reviewed by Chiang‐Shan and Corinde Wiers.
All peer review communications can be found with the online version of the article.
Contributor Information
Liam J. Nestor, Email: liam.nestor@imperial.ac.uk.
ICCAM Consortium:
Filippo Passetti, Luca Faravelli, David Erritzoe, Inge Mick, Nicola Kalk, Adam Waldman, Shankar Kuchibatla, Venkataramana Boyapati, Antonio Metastasio, Yetunde Faluyi, Emilio Fernandez‐Egea, Sanja Abbott, and Valerie Voon
Data accessibility
All relevant data are available upon request from the ICCAM consortium.
References
- Baldo, B.A. (2016) Prefrontal cortical opioids and dysregulated motivation: a network hypothesis. Trends Neurosci., 39, 366–377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boettiger, C.A. , Kelley, E.A. , Mitchell, J.M. , D'Esposito, M. & Fields, H.L. (2009) Now or Later? An fMRI study of the effects of endogenous opioid blockade on a decision‐making network. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav., 93, 291–299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick, M.M. , Cohen, J.D. & Carter, C.S. (2004) Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn Sci, 8, 539–546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowden‐Jones, H. , McPhillips, M. , Rogers, R. , Hutton, S. & Joyce, E. (2005) Risk‐taking on tests sensitive to ventromedial prefrontal cortex dysfunction predicts early relapse in alcohol dependency: a pilot study. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci., 17, 417–420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyle, A.E.L. , Stewart, R.B. , Macenski, M.J. , Spiga, R. , Johnson, B.A. & Meisch, R.A. (1998) Effects of acute and chronic doses of naltrexone on ethanol self‐administration in Rhesus monkeys. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res., 22, 359–366. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess, P.W. , Gilbert, S.J. & Dumontheil, I. (2007) Function and localization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10). Philos T Roy Soc B, 362, 887–899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardenas, V.A. , Durazzo, T.C. , Gazdzinski, S. , Mon, A. , Studholme, C. & Meyerhoff, D.J. (2011) Brain Morphology at Entry into Treatment for Alcohol Dependence Is Related to Relapse Propensity. Biol. Psychiat., 70, 561–567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chikazoe, J. , Jimura, K. , Hirose, S. , Yamashita, K. , Miyashita, Y. & Konishi, S. (2009) Preparation to inhibit a response complements response inhibition during performance of a stop‐signal task. J. Neurosci., 29, 15870–15877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connolly, C.G. , Foxe, J.J. , Nierenberg, J. , Shpaner, M. & Garavan, H. (2012) The neurobiology of cognitive control in successful cocaine abstinence. Drug Alcohol Depend., 121, 45–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley, H.D. , Wiens, S. , Rotshtein, P. , Ohman, A. & Dolan, R.J. (2004) Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci., 7, 189–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayas, C.V. , Liu, X. , Simms, J.A. & Weiss, F. (2007) Distinct patterns of neural activation associated with ethanol seeking: effects of naltrexone. Biol. Psychiat., 61, 979–989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt, B.J. , Belin, D. , Economidou, D. , Pelloux, Y. , Dalley, J.W. & Robbins, T.W. (2008) Review. Neural mechanisms underlying the vulnerability to develop compulsive drug‐seeking habits and addiction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 363, 3125–3135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fillmore, M.T. & Rush, C.R. (2002) Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend., 66, 265–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fu, L.P. , Bi, G.H. , Zou, Z.T. , Wang, Y. , Ye, E.M. , Ma, L. , Ming, F. & Yang, Z. (2008) Impaired response inhibition function in abstinent heroin dependents: an fMRI study. Neurosci. Lett., 438, 322–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garavan, H. & Stout, J.C. (2005) Neurocognitive insights into substance abuse. Trends Cogn Sci, 9, 195–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garavan, H. , Ross, T.J. & Stein, E.A. (1999) Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event‐related functional MRI study. Proc Natl. Acad Sci U S A, 96, 8301–8306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garavan, H. , Ross, T.J. , Murphy, K. , Roche, R.A. & Stein, E.A. (2002) Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: inhibition, error detection, and correction. NeuroImage, 17, 1820–1829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garavan, H. , Ross, T.J. , Kaufman, J. & Stein, E.A. (2003) A midline dissociation between error‐processing and response‐conflict monitoring. NeuroImage, 20, 1132–1139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghitza, U.E. , Preston, K.L. , Epstein, D.H. , Kuwabara, H. , Endres, C.J. , Bencherif, B. , Boyd, S.J. , Copersino, M.L. et al (2010) Brain mu‐opioid receptor binding predicts treatment outcome in cocaine‐abusing outpatients. Biol. Psychiatry, 68, 697–703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert, S.J. , Gonen‐Yaacovi, G. , Benoit, R.G. , Volle, E. & Burgess, P.W. (2010) Distinct functional connectivity associated with lateral versus medial rostral prefrontal cortex: a meta‐analysis. NeuroImage, 53, 1359–1367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giuliano, C. , Robbins, T.W. , Wille, D.R. , Bullmore, E.T. & Everitt, B.J. (2013) Attenuation of cocaine and heroin seeking by mu‐opioid receptor antagonism. Psychopharmacology, 227, 137–147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giuliano, C. , Goodlett, C.R. , Economidou, D. , Garcia‐Pardo, M.P. , Belin, D. , Robbins, T.W. , Bullmore, E.T. & Everitt, B.J. (2015) The Novel mu‐opioid receptor antagonist GSK1521498 decreases both alcohol seeking and drinking: evidence from a new preclinical model of alcohol seeking. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40, 2981–2992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein, R.Z. & Volkow, N.D. (2002) Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am. J. Psychiatry, 159, 1642–1652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gorelick, D.A. , Kim, Y.K. , Bencherif, B. , Boyd, S.J. , Nelson, R. , Copersino, M. , Endres, C.J. , Dannals, R.F. et al (2005) Imaging brain mu‐opioid receptors in abstinent cocaine users: time course and relation to cocaine craving. Biol. Psychiatry, 57, 1573–1582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gorelick, D.A. , Kim, Y.K. , Bencherif, B. , Boyd, S.J. , Nelson, R. , Copersino, M.L. , Dannals, R.F. & Frost, J.J. (2008) Brain mu‐opioid receptor binding: relationship to relapse to cocaine use after monitored abstinence. Psychopharmacology, 200, 475–486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant, J.E. (2005) Outcome study of kleptomania patients treated with naltrexone: a chart review. Clin. Neuropharmacol., 28, 11–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant, J.E. , Odlaug, B.L. , Schreiber, L.R. & Kim, S.W. (2014) The opiate antagonist, naltrexone, in the treatment of trichotillomania: results of a double‐blind, placebo‐controlled study. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol., 34, 134–138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grassi, M.C. , Cioce, A.M. , Giudici, F.D. , Antonilli, L. & Nencini, P. (2007) Short‐term efficacy of Disulfiram or Naltrexone in reducing positive urinalysis for both cocaine and cocaethylene in cocaine abusers: a pilot study. Pharmacol. Res., 55, 117–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grodin, E.N. , Cortes, C.R. , Spagnolo, P.A. & Momenan, R. (2017) Structural deficits in salience network regions are associated with increased impulsivity and compulsivity in alcohol dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend., 179, 100–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gropper, S. , Spengler, S. , Stuke, H. , Gawron, C.K. , Parnack, J. , Gutwinski, S. , Wiers, C.E. & Bermpohl, F. (2016) Behavioral impulsivity mediates the relationship between decreased frontal gray matter volume and harmful alcohol drinking: a voxel‐based morphometry study. J. Psychiatr. Res., 83, 16–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horn, N.R. , Dolan, M. , Elliott, R. , Deakin, J.F. & Woodruff, P.W. (2003) Response inhibition and impulsivity: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1959–1966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu, S. , Ide, J.S. , Zhang, S. , Sinha, R. & Li, C.S.R. (2015) Conflict anticipation in alcohol dependence ‐ A model‐based fMRI study of stop signal task. Neuroimage‐Clin, 8, 39–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu, S. , Zhang, S. , Chao, H.H. , Krystal, J.H. & Li, C.S.R. (2016) Association of drinking problems and duration of alcohol use to inhibitory control in nondependent young adult social drinkers. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res., 40, 319–328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman, J.N. , Ross, T.J. , Stein, E.A. & Garavan, H. (2003) Cingulate hypoactivity in cocaine users during a GO‐NOGO task as revealed by event‐related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci., 23, 7839–7843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim, S.W. , Grant, J.E. , Adson, D.E. & Shin, Y.C. (2001) Double‐blind naltrexone and placebo comparison study in the treatment of pathological gambling. Biol. Psychiatry, 49, 914–921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krawitz, A. , Fukunaga, R. & Brown, J.W. (2010) Anterior insula activity predicts the influence of positively framed messages on decision making. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci., 10, 392–405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krishnan‐Sarin, S. , Reynolds, B. , Duhig, A.M. , Smith, A. , Liss, T. , McFetridge, A. , Cavallo, D.A. , Carroll, K.M. et al (2007) Behavioral impulsivity predicts treatment outcome in a smoking cessation program for adolescent smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend., 88, 79–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kroenke, K.M. , Wolff, M. , Benz, A. & Goschke, T. (2015) Successful smoking cessation is associated with prefrontal cortical function during a Stroop task: a preliminary study. Psychiat Res‐Neuroim, 234, 52–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krystal, J. H. , Cramer, J. A. , Krol, W. F. , Kirk, G. F. & Rosenheck, R. A. , & Veterans Affairs Naltrexone Cooperative Study, G . (2001). Naltrexone in the treatment of alcohol dependence. N. Engl. J. Med., 345, 1734–1739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahti, T. , Halme, J.T. , Pankakoski, M. , Sinclair, D. & Alho, H. (2010) Treatment of pathological gambling with naltrexone pharmacotherapy and brief intervention: a pilot study. Psychopharmacol. Bull., 43, 35–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li, C.S.R. , Luo, X. , Yan, P. , Bergquist, K. & Sinha, R. (2009) Altered impulse control in alcohol dependence: neural measures of stop signal performance. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res., 33, 740–750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Love, T.M. , Stohler, C.S. & Zubieta, J.K. (2009) Positron emission tomography measures of endogenous opioid neurotransmission and impulsiveness traits in humans. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry, 66, 1124–1134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lubman, D.I. , Yucel, M. & Pantelis, C. (2004) Addiction, a condition of compulsive behaviour? Neuroimaging and neuropsychological evidence of inhibitory dysregulation. Addiction, 99, 1491–1502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luijten, M. , Littel, M. & Franken, I.H.A. (2011) Deficits in inhibitory control in smokers during a Go/NoGo task: an investigation using event‐related brain potentials. PLoS ONE, 6, e18898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mahoney, M.K. , Silveira, M.M. & Olmstead, M.C. (2013) Increased impulsive action in rats: effects of morphine in a short and long fixed‐delay response inhibition task. Psychopharmacology, 230, 569–577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGonigle, J. , Murphy, A. , Paterson, L.M. , Reed, L.J. , Nestor, L. , Nash, J. , Elliott, R. , Ersche, K.D. et al (2017) The ICCAM platform study: an experimental medicine platform for evaluating new drugs for relapse prevention in addiction. Part B: fMRI description. J Psychopharmacol, 31, 3–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Medina, K.L. , McQueeny, T. , Nagel, B.J. , Hanson, K.L. , Schweinsburg, A.D. & Tapert, S.F. (2008) Prefrontal cortex volumes in adolescents with alcohol use disorders: unique gender effects. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res., 32, 386–394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer, M.C. , Straughn, A.B. , Lo, M.W. , Schary, W.L. & Whitney, C.C. (1984) Bioequivalence, dose‐proportionality, and pharmacokinetics of naltrexone after oral administration. J. Clin. Psychiatry, 45, 15–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moeller, F.G. , Dougherty, D.M. , Barratt, E.S. , Schmitz, J.M. , Swann, A.C. & Grabowski, J. (2001) The impact of impulsivity on cocaine use and retention in treatment. J. Subst. Abuse Treat., 21, 193–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris, L.S. , Baek, K. , Tait, R. , Elliott, R. , Ersche, K.D. , Flechais, R. , McGonigle, J. , Murphy, A. et al (2018) Naltrexone ameliorates functional network abnormalities in alcohol‐dependent individuals. Addict Biol, 23, 425–436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moss, H.B. , Goldstein, R.B. , Chen, C.M. & Yi, H.Y. (2015) Patterns of use of other drugs among those with alcohol dependence: associations with drinking behavior and psychopathology. Addict. Behav., 50, 192–198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naim‐Feil, J. , Fitzgerald, P.B. , Bradshaw, J.L. , Lubman, D.I. & Sheppard, D. (2014) Neurocognitive Deficits, Craving, and Abstinence among Alcohol‐Dependent Individuals Following Detoxification. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol., 29, 26–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nestor, L. , McCabe, E. , Jones, J. , Clancy, L. & Garavan, H. (2011) Differences in “bottom‐up” and “top‐down” neural activity in current and former cigarette smokers: evidence for neural substrates which may promote nicotine abstinence through increased cognitive control. NeuroImage, 56, 2258–2275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nestor, L.J. , Murphy, A. , McGonigle, J. , Orban, C. , Reed, L. , Taylor, E. , Flechais, R. , Paterson, L.M. et al (2017) Acute naltrexone does not remediate fronto‐striatal disturbances in alcoholic and alcoholic polysubstance‐dependent populations during a monetary incentive delay task. Addict Biol, 22, 1576–1589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olmstead, M.C. , Ouagazzal, A.M. & Kieffer, B.L. (2009) Mu and delta opioid receptors oppositely regulate motor impulsivity in the signaled nose poke task. PLoS ONE, 4, e4410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paterson, L.M. , Flechais, R.S. , Murphy, A. , Reed, L.J. , Abbott, S. , Boyapati, V. , Elliott, R. , Erritzoe, D. et al (2015) The Imperial College Cambridge Manchester (ICCAM) platform study: an experimental medicine platform for evaluating new drugs for relapse prevention in addiction. Part A: study Description. J Psychopharmacol, 29, 943–960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pattij, T. , Schetters, D. , Janssen, M.C.W. , Wiskerke, J. & Schoffelmeer, A.N.M. (2009) Acute effects of morphine on distinct forms of impulsive behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology, 205, 489–502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patton, J.H. , Stanford, M.S. & Barratt, E.S. (1995) Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J. Clin. Psychol., 51, 768–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peoples, L. L (2002) Will, anterior cingulate cortex, and addiction. Science, 296, 1623–1624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preuschoff, K. , Quartz, S.R. & Bossaerts, P. (2008) Human insula activation reflects risk prediction errors as well as risk. J. Neurosci., 28, 2745–2752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof, K.R. , Ullsperger, M. , Crone, E.A. & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004) The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubio, G. , Jimenez, M. , Rodriguez‐Jimenez, R. , Martinez, I. , Avila, C. , Ferre, F. , Jimenez‐Arriero, M.A. , Ponce, G. et al (2008) The role of behavioral impulsivity in the development of alcohol dependence: a 4‐year follow‐up study. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res., 32, 1681–1687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez‐Roige, S. , Ripley, T.L. & Stephens, D.N. (2015) Alleviating waiting impulsivity and perseverative responding by mu‐opioid receptor antagonism in two inbred mouse strains. Psychopharmacology, 232, 1483–1492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savulich, G. , Riccelli, R. , Passamonti, L. , Correia, M. , Deakin, J.F.W. , Elliott, R. , Flechais, R.S.A. , Lingford‐Hughes, A.R. et al (2017) Effects of naltrexone are influenced by childhood adversity during negative emotional processing in addiction recovery. Transl. Psychiat., 7, e1054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan, D.V. , Lecrubier, Y. , Sheehan, K.H. , Amorim, P. , Janavs, J. , Weiller, E. , Hergueta, T. , Baker, R. et al (1998) The Mini‐International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM‐IV and ICD‐10. J. Clin. Psychiatry, 59(Suppl. 20), 22–33.;quiz 34‐57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinha, R. (2011) New Findings on Biological Factors Predicting Addiction Relapse Vulnerability. Curr Psychiat Rep, 13, 398–405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, J.L. , Mattick, R.P. , Jamadar, S.D. & Iredale, J.M. (2014) Deficits in behavioural inhibition in substance abuse and addiction: a meta‐analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend., 145, 1–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Srisurapanont, M. & Jarusuraisin, N. (2005) Naltrexone for the treatment of alcoholism: a meta‐analysis of randomized controlled trials. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol., 8, 267–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swick, D. , Ashley, V. & Turken, A.U. (2008) Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neurosci., 9, 102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, E.M. , Murphy, A. , Boyapati, V. , Ersche, K.D. , Flechais, R. , Kuchibatla, S. , McGonigle, J. , Metastasio, A. et al (2016) Impulsivity in abstinent alcohol and polydrug dependence: a multidimensional approach. Psychopharmacology, 233, 1487–1499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thayer, R.E. , Hagerty, S.L. , Sabbineni, A. , Claus, E.D. , Hutchison, K.E. & Weiland, B.J. (2016) Negative and Interactive Effects of Sex, Aging, and Alcohol Abuse on Gray Matter Morphometry. Hum. Brain Mapp., 37, 2276–2292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torregrossa, M.M. , Quinn, J.J. & Taylor, J.R. (2008) Impulsivity, compulsivity, and habit: the role of orbitofrontal cortex revisited. Biol. Psychiat., 63, 253–255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trick, L. , Kempton, M.J. , Williams, S.C.R. & Duka, T. (2014) Impaired fear recognition and attentional set‐shifting is associated with brain structural changes in alcoholic patients. Addict Biol, 19, 1041–1054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uddin, L.Q. (2015) Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 16, 55–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verdejo‐Garcia, A. , Lawrence, A.J. & Clark, L. (2008) Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance‐use disorders: review of findings from high‐risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 32, 777–810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow, N.D. & Baler, R.D. (2012) Neuroscience. To stop or not to stop? Science, 335, 546–548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow, N. , Fowler, J. , Wang, G. & Goldstein, R. (2002) Role of dopamine, the frontal cortex and memory circuits in drug addiction: insight from imaging studies. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem., 78, 610–624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whiteside, S.P. & Lynam, D.R. (2001) The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality Individ. Differ., 30, 669–689. [Google Scholar]
- WHO Assist Working Group (2002) The Alcohol, Smoking and Substance Involvement Screening Test (ASSIST): development, reliability and feasibility. Addiction, 97, 1183–1194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiers, C.E. , Gawron, C.K. , Gropper, S. , Spengler, S. , Stuke, H. , Lindenmeyer, J. , Walter, H. & Bermpohl, F. (2015) Decreased gray matter volume in inferior frontal gyrus is related to stop‐signal task performance in alcohol‐dependent patients. Psychiat Res‐Neuroim, 233, 125–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams, T.M. , Daglish, M.R. , Lingford‐Hughes, A. , Taylor, L.G. , Hammers, A. , Brooks, D.J. , Grasby, P. , Myles, J.S. et al (2007) Brain opioid receptor binding in early abstinence from opioid dependence: positron emission tomography study. Br. J. Psychiatry, 191, 63–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xiao, P.R. , Dai, Z.Y. , Zhong, J.G. , Zhu, Y.L. , Shi, H.C. & Pan, P.L. (2015) Regional gray matter deficits in alcohol dependence: a meta‐analysis of voxel‐based morphometry studies. Drug Alcohol Depend., 153, 22–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zubieta, J.K. , Bueller, J.A. , Jackson, L.R. , Scott, D.J. , Xu, Y.J. , Koeppe, R.A. , Nichols, T.E. & Stohler, C.S. (2005) Placebo effects mediated by endogenous opioid activity on mu‐opioid receptors. J. Neurosci., 25, 7754–7762. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are available upon request from the ICCAM consortium.