Skip to main content
American Journal of Epidemiology logoLink to American Journal of Epidemiology
. 2019 Jul 5;188(10):1838–1848. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwz160

The Effects of Reverse Causality and Selective Attrition on the Relationship Between Body Mass Index and Mortality in Postmenopausal Women

Hailey R Banack , Jennifer W Bea, Jay S Kaufman, Andrew Stokes, Candyce H Kroenke, Marcia L Stefanick, Shirley A Beresford, Chloe E Bird, Lorena Garcia, Robert Wallace, Robert A Wild, Bette Caan, Jean Wactawski-Wende
PMCID: PMC6768808  PMID: 31274146

Abstract

Concerns about reverse causality and selection bias complicate the interpretation of studies of body mass index (BMI, calculated as weight (kg)/height (m)2) and mortality in older adults. The objective of this study was to investigate methodological explanations for the apparent attenuation of obesity-related risks in older adults. We used data from 68,132 participants in the Women’s Health Initiative (WHI) clinical trial for this analysis. All of the participants were postmenopausal women aged 50–79 years at baseline (1993–1998). To examine reverse causality and selective attrition, we compared rate ratios from inverse probability of treatment– and censoring–weighted Poisson marginal structural models with results from an unweighted adjusted Poisson regression model. The estimated mortality rate ratios and 95% confidence intervals for BMIs of 30.0–34.9, 35.0–39.9 and ≥40.0 were 0.86 (95% confidence interval (CI): 0.77, 0.96), 0.85 (95% CI: 0.72, 0.99), and 0.88 (95% CI: 0.72, 1.07), respectively, in the unweighted model. The corresponding mortality rate ratios were 0.96 (95% CI: 0.86, 1.07), 1.12 (95% CI: 0.97, 1.29), and 1.31 95% CI: (1.08, 1.57), respectively, in the marginal structural model. Results from the inverse probability of treatment– and censoring–weighted marginal structural model were attenuated in low BMI categories and increased in high BMI categories. The results demonstrate the importance of accounting for reverse causality and selective attrition in studies of older adults.

Keywords: aging, body mass index, reverse causality, selection bias, selective attrition


The nature and shape of the relationship between body mass index (BMI) and mortality in older adults is a frequently debated topic in the epidemiologic and clinical literature. Several papers have demonstrated decreasing relative risks of high BMI in older adults, suggesting that the relationship between high BMI and mortality is either null or slightly protective (13). Numerous hypotheses have been suggested to explain the altered BMI-mortality relationship in older adults, including both physiological and methodological explanations (4, 5). Questions about the relationship between BMI and mortality are particularly salient in older women because the proportion of individuals with BMI over 30 (calculated as weight (kg) divided by height (m) squared) is higher in women than in men (40% vs. 35%) and life expectancy is greater for women than men (6, 7). We examined the influence of reverse causality and selection bias on the relationship between BMI and mortality, using data from a large cohort of postmenopausal women in the United States, the Women’s Health Initiative (WHI).

REVERSE CAUSALITY

Reverse causality is defined as a phenomenon in which the outcome precedes and causes the exposure (810). Technically, using a strict definition of reverse causality, it is not possible to have reverse causality in studies with all-cause mortality as the endpoint. Experiencing the outcome (mortality) prior to exposure measurement, or having mortality cause exposure, is impossible. However, in the context of obesity-mortality research, the term reverse causality is often used to refer to a situation in which disease status affects both exposure and outcome, because disease often causes weight loss and disease increases mortality risk. Despite being called reverse causality, this is actually a concept that fits the standard definition of confounding in epidemiology (see Figure 1A–C) (10, 11). This is why, in the context of obesity-mortality studies, the phrase reverse causality is often used interchangeably with the terms “confounding by preexisting disease” or “confounding by illness-related weight loss” (8, 10, 12). Throughout this discussion, we use the term reverse causality to refer to a concept that most epidemiologists would call confounding, but we are referring to it as reverse causality for consistency with the obesity literature.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Causal diagrams depicting a generic confounding scenario (A), confounding due to preexisting disease (B), and confounding due to illness-related weight loss (C). In all these situations, the confounding variable is associated with the exposure and a risk factor for the outcome. BMI, body mass index.

There are 2 common techniques to assess the possible impact of reverse causation in obesity research: 1) excluding individuals with prevalent disease at baseline and 2) excluding deaths that occur early in the follow-up period. The rationale for the first technique comes from a long-standing tradition in observational epidemiology, that individuals must be free of disease at baseline. It is now recognized that such exclusions can, in certain cases, introduce bias (1315). It is well-known that obesity leads to cardiovascular disease and many types of cancer; thus, excluding individuals with these prevalent conditions at baseline amounts to conditioning on a variable affected by exposure (16). Therefore, while some authors seek to use exclusions to explore reverse causality, it is likely inducing a different form of bias (17).

There is a slightly different rationale for the second type of exclusion, deleting deaths that occur early in the follow-up period. This type of exclusion is intended to mitigate the concern that subclinical illness leads to weight loss and then death early in follow-up. If subclinical and then clinical disease cause weight loss, lower BMI categories will be composed of a disproportionate number of individuals with disease at cohort entry (10). Excluding early deaths is an attempt to control for the influence of subclinical disease; because we cannot control for undiagnosed illness using statistical techniques, early deaths are used as a proxy measure (18). A number of studies have demonstrated that excluding early deaths fails to account for illness-related weight loss, or reverse causality (10, 18, 19). As Allison et al. concluded, “If excluding early deaths resulted in elimination of all diseased subjects, and no nondiseased subjects, the bias would be eliminated. However, this outcome seems quite unlikely to be achieved . . .” (18, p. 673). Excluding early deaths is unlikely to control for bias as intended and results in substantial reductions in sample size.

Instead of relying on restrictions or exclusions, statistical methods have been developed to control for time-varying confounding affected by prior exposure, such as marginal structural models (8, 20, 21). In this context, the time-varying confounder is disease status: Obesity is a cause of disease, being ill affects body weight, and both obesity and disease increase mortality risk (Figure 2) (22). Disease status is on the causal pathway from obesity to mortality; thus, using conventional adjustment techniques, such as regression adjustment or restriction, might bias the estimated effect of obesity on mortality (13, 23). It is not possible to estimate the obesity-mortality relationship using standard statistical approaches because failing to account for disease status will result in a biased effect estimate, but adjusting for disease status will also result in a biased estimate (13).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Directed acyclic graph for the effect of body mass index (BMI) on mortality. Vk is a vector of baseline covariates and Lk is a vector of time-varying covariates of BMI and mortality. Subscript k represents the baseline the Women’s Health Initiative clinic visit, k + 1 represents visit 1, and k + 2 represents visit 2. C represents censoring and Z represents a vector of time-varying covariates that influence both censoring and mortality. This directed acyclic graph is a simplified representation of these relationship for 2 Women’s Health Initiative (United States, 1993–2005) clinic visits, but similar relationships exist for the entire range of follow-up visits.

SELECTIVE ATTRITION

Selective attrition, a form of selection bias, is a second hypothesized explanation for the attenuated relationship between obesity and mortality in older adults (13, 24). Selective attrition produces selection bias when the losses to follow-up are influenced by the exposure of interest and the outcome, or by determinants of the exposure and the outcome. This is a particular concern in studies of obesity in older adults because obesity predicts disease risk, which is strongly associated with attrition and mortality (24). This is depicted in Figure 2. To account for differential attrition, we use inverse probability of censoring weights (IPCW) (13, 20, 24). IPCW were developed to correct for selection bias using measured determinants of attrition and the outcome of interest (25). The goal of using IPCW is to recreate the study population that we would have observed had there been no censoring, upweighting existing observations to account for those who are missing.

Our objective was to understand the influence of reverse causality and selection bias on the relationship between BMI and mortality in postmenopausal women. We compared the results from an inverse probability–weighted marginal structural model with results from a conventionally adjusted model to add to our understanding of the magnitude and direction of these sources of bias in aging research.

METHODS

Study population

We used data from 68,132 participants in the Women’s Health Initiative (WHI) clinical trials in this analysis. The WHI is a cohort of postmenopausal women recruited between ages 50 and 79 years from 40 clinical centers across the United States (2628). The WHI clinical trials (1993–2005) were focused on the effects of hormone therapy, dietary modification, and vitamin supplementation on health outcomes in postmenopausal women. Detailed information about recruitment has been published elsewhere (26, 27, 29). During the active trial phase, clinic visits were required semiannually and consisted of questionnaires and interviews focused on symptom reporting and management, compliance, and safety. Once per year, participants’ anthropometric measurements were recorded, including height and weight (26). We chose to study the participants in the WHI clinical trials, as opposed to the entire WHI cohort, which includes an additional 93,676 women in an observational study, because the annual anthropometric measures provide valuable information about weight change and disease status. Women in the WHI observational study did not have annual in-person follow-up visits. The study protocol was reviewed by institutional review boards at each clinical center as well as the WHI coordinating center (30).

Assessment of endpoints

The primary outcome of interest in this analysis is all-cause mortality up until the end of the core WHI study follow-up (March 31, 2005). Mortality was ascertained through annual mail and telephone contacts, searches of medical record and death certificates, and the National Death Index (30). Follow-up for primary clinical endpoints (cardiovascular events, cancer, fracture) occurred every 6 months during the WHI trials and were adjudicated by trained physicians (27). Diagnosis of cardiovascular disease included angina, coronary revascularization, coronary heart disease, carotid endarterectomy/angioplasty, stroke, transient ischemic attack, heart failure, and myocardial infarction. Diagnosed fracture included any adjudicated fracture of hip, leg (including knee), pelvis, foot/ankle, tailbone, spine, arm (including elbow), and shoulder. Diagnosed cancer included all cancer types except nonmelanoma skin cancer.

Assessment of exposure and covariates

BMI was measured annually during the active study phase for all WHI clinical trial participants. BMI is an index of weight for height, calculated as weight in kilograms (kg) divided by height in meters (m) squared. For this analysis, participants were categorized into standard BMI groups: ≤18.5, 18.5–24.9, 25.0–29.9, 30.0–34.9, 35.0–39.9, and ≥40.0. Information on relevant confounding variables was collected at baseline and annual study visits, either through questionnaires or in-clinic measurements. Baseline confounders of the BMI-mortality relationship include age, trial participation (hormone therapy, dietary modification), trial arm (intervention or not), race/ethnicity, education, marital status, income, health insurance, and smoking history (pack-years). Time-varying confounders were measured throughout the follow-up period. Time-varying confounders in this analysis include disease status (cardiovascular disease, cancer, fracture), hormone therapy use, smoking status, self-rated health status, physical activity level, alcohol intake, physical functioning, hypertension, and diabetes.

Statistical analysis

Our primary analysis focused on understanding the influence of reverse causality and selection bias on the BMI-mortality relationship. We first used generalized linear models with a log link and a Poisson distribution to obtain unweighted adjusted rate ratios for each BMI category compared with the reference category, BMI 25.0–29.9. BMI 25.0–29.9 was used as the reference group because it is the group associated with the lowest risk of mortality and less likely to be affected by illness-related weight loss than BMI 18.5–24.9 (3133). BMI 25.0–29.9 is also the largest group, and using it as the reference provides the most stable results. The Poisson parameter is a collapsible effect estimate and thus can be compared with results from a marginal structural model (34). The number of days between WHI clinic visits was included as an offset term. All analyses were performed using Stata, version 14 (StataCorp LLC, College Station, Texas).

We then fit the parameters of a marginal structural model in a 2-step process: estimating inverse probability weights and then using the weights to approximate the parameters of a marginal structural model. Estimating inverse probability of treatment weights (IPTW) accounts for time-varying confounding by variables affected by prior exposure and estimating IPCW accounts for differential attrition. We estimated the weights using pooled logistic regression and used stabilized weights to improve statistical efficiency (20, 35, 36). Due to the large range of the IPTW for BMI, we truncated the weights at the 1st and 99th percentile. This decision was made understanding the bias-variance tradeoff associated with weight truncation (20). For each individual, IPTW are the inverse of the probability of being in each BMI category at each follow-up visit, conditional on observed time-fixed and time-varying variables:

SWijX=k=0jff[xik|x¯ik1,Vi0,Cik=0]f[xik|x¯ik1,L¯ik1,Cik=0]

X ik represents individual i’s BMI category measured at visit k, with the start of follow-up at visit k = 0; Xik1 represents individual i’s BMI measured at the previous cohort visit. Overbars represent covariate history up to and including visit k. L¯ik1 is a vector of baseline and time-varying covariates measured for individual i at visit k − 1. The variables included in L are age, cohort, smoking status, physical activity, alcohol consumption, physical functioning, cardiovascular disease, hypertension, diabetes, cancer, and any fracture. Values of time-varying covariates included in L were from the prior cohort visit. Implicit in this weighting model is the Markov assumption that the individual’s probability of exposure at visit k depends on their history of the time-varying covariates included in L¯ik1. Following the assumptions encoded in our directed acyclic graph, probability of exposure at visit k depends on the history of the time-varying covariates included in L¯ik1 only through the most recent value Lik1 (20, 24, 37). Vi0 is a subset of Li, a vector of variables measured at baseline (visit k = 0), including trial participation and arm, race/ethnicity, education, marital status, health insurance status, self-rated health, pack-years smoked, and prior hormone therapy use. Cik = 0 indicates that individual i was not censored by visit k (i.e., to be included in the IPTW model for visit k, individual i still had to be part of the study cohort). We explored numerous additional functional forms for the pooled logistic model used to estimate the IPTW, including product terms for interaction between BMI and disease status variables, restricted cubic splines for BMI and age with 4 knots, and a broader set of covariates (including the use of a continuous variable for systolic and diastolic blood pressure, number of years since menopause, age at menarche, age at menopause, hysterectomy status, specific fracture types, differing types of hormone use, and more detailed information about smoking history, such as pack-years). However, the alternative specifications did not appreciably alter study results.

IPCW are the inverse of the probability of remaining in the study cohort (i.e., uncensored) at each follow-up visit:

CWijX=k=0jfPr[Cik=0|C¯ik1=0,Xik,Vi0]Pr[Cik=0|C¯ik1=0,Xik,Z¯ik1]

Where the denominator is the probability that participant i is not censored by visit k, conditional on individual i not being censored at a previous clinic visit, BMI category at visit k, and a defined set of baseline and time-varying covariates that influence censoring and mortality (Zik−1). Accounting for selective attrition using IPCW requires that we identify a complete set of confounders of censoring and mortality (38). For individual i, the numerator of the IPCW is the conditional probability of remaining alive and uncensored up to visit k, given that they had not been censored between visits k − 1 and k, their exposure status at visit k (Xik), and a vector of baseline covariates (Vi0) that are a subset of Zik.

Finally, using the product of the IPTW and IPCW as an analytical weight, we fit marginal structural models with a robust variance estimator to estimate the adjusted effects of BMI on mortality. Baseline confounders of the BMI-mortality relationship (V) were included in the final marginal structural model, given that they had been previously included in the numerator of the stabilized weight models (39). The final Poisson marginal structural model, including the offset term log Ti, was:

log(E(Y|X)=β0+β1X+BkV+logTi

As a secondary analysis, we investigated the influence of baseline age on the relationship between BMI and mortality. In this analysis, we included a product term for interaction between baseline age cohort (A; 50–59 years, 60–69 years, or 70–79 years) and BMI (X) in the marginal structural model (40, 41):

log(Pr(Y|X)=β0+β1X+β2A+β3XA+B4V

The results from this supplemental analysis allowed us to calculate stratum-specific effects and examine whether the relationship between BMI and mortality differed among women who were younger versus older at cohort entry.

RESULTS

Demographic characteristics of the study population at baseline are presented in Table 1. There were 5,310 women in the estrogen-alone trial, 8,506 women in the estrogen-and progestin trial, and 19,541 women in the dietary-modification trial. Mean BMI was consistent across age strata, but there were interesting differences in the proportions of women in specific BMI categories according to age at baseline. The proportion of women in the lower 3 BMI groups (<18.5, 18.5–24.9, 25.0–29.9) increased across age strata, while the proportion of women in the higher 3 BMI groups (30.0–34.9, 35.0–39.9, ≥40.0) decreased across baseline age strata. Of the 68,132 women in the WHI clinical trials, there were 3,769 deaths (5.5%) up to the end of the core WHI trial follow-up in 2005, an average of 8.6 years of follow-up. As expected, a greater proportion of women who died were older at baseline: By the end of follow-up, 720 women (2.6%) who were aged 50–59 years had died, 1,750 women (5.8%) who were 60–69 years had died, and 1,299 women (12.3%) who were 70–79 years had died.

Table 1.

Baseline Demographic Characteristics of Participants in the Women’s Health Initiative Clinical Trials, United States, 1993–1998

Characteristic Total Cohort, % (n = 68,132) Age Group, years
50–59 (n = 27,408), % 60–69 (n = 30,193), % 70–79 (n = 10,531), %
Age, yearsa 62.1 (7.0) 55.2 (3.0) 64.7 (2.8) 73.1 (2.3)
Race/ethnicity
 Non-Hispanic white 81.7 76.9 83.8 87.8
 Non-Hispanic black 10.3 12.7 9.2 6.9
 Hispanic 4.2 6.1 3.4 1.7
 Other 3.8 4.3 3.5 3.6
Education
 Some high school 5.6 4.76 5.8 7.2
 High-school diploma or equivalent 18.5 15.4 20.9 19.6
 Postsecondary school 50.0 50.9 49.2 50.3
 Postgraduate school 25.9 29.0 24.1 22.9
Smoking status
 Current smoker 7.9 10.4 7.1 4.1
 Former smoker 41.0 40.6 42.0 39.1
 Never smoker 51.1 49.0 51.0 56.8
Marital status
 Married 60.9 65.5 61.7 47.2
 Widowed 17.1 6.9 18.8 38.5
 Divorced 16.3 20.5 14.9 9.7
Had health insurance 93.8 89.7 95.6 99.5
Employed 39.4 65.0 27.4 10.4
History of CVD 19.0 13.6 20.6 28.6
BMI categoriesb
 <18.5 0.43 0.36 0.42 0.62
 18.5–24.9 26.9 27.2 25.6 30.2
 25.0–29.9 35.7 33.7 36.5 38.6
 30.0–34.9 22.3 22.2 22.9 21.3
 35.0–39.9 9.9 10.8 10.1 7.2
 ≥40.0 4.7 5.8 4.5 2.1
BMIa,b 28.9 (5.9) 29.2 (6.2) 29.0 (5.8) 27.9 (5.2)
Years since menopausea 14.4 (9.0) 8.1 (6.4) 16.4 (7.3) 24.7 (7.0)

Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; CVD, cardiovascular disease.

a Values are expressed as mean (standard deviation).

b Weight (kg)/height (m)2.

The primary study results are illustrated Figure 3, comparing results from an unweighted Poisson regression model and 2 marginal structural models, one weighted by IPTW and the second weighted by the product of the IPTW and IPCW. The mean of the weights was 0.99 (SD, 0.07) and ranged from 0.61 to 1.41. At low BMI levels (<18.5 and 18.5–24.9), IPTW and IPCW attenuated the mortality rate ratio compared with the unweighted regression results. Adding IPCW to the IPTW-weighted marginal structural model resulted in no change in estimates, indicating no influence of selective attrition at low BMI levels. At high BMI levels (30.0–34.9, 35.0–39.9, and ≥40.0), including IPTW and IPCW resulted in increased rate ratios. Resulting values from an IPTW-weighted marginal structural model were higher than from the unweighted model, and adding IPCW to the model further increased estimates.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Graphical comparison of log mortality rate ratio results from unweighted, inverse probability of treatment–weighted (IPTW) and IPTW × inverse probability of censoring–weighted (IPCW) marginal structural model, Women’s Health Initiative cohort, United States, 1993–2005.

Numeric results comparing the mortality rate ratios and 95% confidence intervals from unweighted adjusted and marginal structural models are presented in Table 2. Compared with those for BMI 25.0–29.9, rate ratios from the multivariate adjusted unweighted model were 4.20 (95% confidence interval (CI): 3.22, 5.48) for BMI <18.5, 1.46 (95% CI: 1.32, 1.62) for BMI 18.5–24.9, 0.86 (95% CI: 0.77, 0.96) for BMI 30.0–34.9, 0.85 (95% CI: 0.72, 0.99) for BMI 35.0–39.9, and 0.88 (95% CI: 0.72, 1.07) for BMI ≥40.0. Corresponding estimates from the marginal structural model weighted by IPTW and IPCW were: 3.51 (95% CI: 2.57, 4.44) for BMI <18.5, 1.39 (95% CI: 1.26, 1.54) for BMI 18.5–24.9, 0.96 (95% CI: 0.86, 1.07) for BMI 30.0–34.9, 1.12 (95% CI: 0.97, 1.29) for BMI 35.0–39.9, and 1.31 (95% CI: 1.08, 1.57) for BMI ≥40.0.

Table 2.

Incidence Rate Ratios for the Effect of Body Mass Index on Mortality Among Postmenopausal Women in the Women’s Health Initiative Clinical Trials, United States, 1993–2005

BMIa Adjusted, Unweighted Resultsb IPTW-Weighted Marginal Structural Modelc IPTW × IPCW Weighted Marginal Structural Modelc
IRR 95% CI IRR 95% CI IRR 95% CI
<18.5 4.20 3.22, 5.48 3.34 2.61, 4.25 3.51 2.57, 4.44
18.5–24.9 1.46 1.32, 1.62 1.33 1.21, 1.46 1.39 1.26, 1.54
25.0–29.9 1.0 Referent 1.0 Referent 1.0 Referent
30.0–34.9 0.86 0.77, 0.96 0.94 0.85, 1.04 0.96 0.86, 1.07
35.0–39.9 0.85 0.72, 0.99 1.05 0.92, 1.20 1.12 0.97, 1.29
≥40.0 0.88 0.72, 1.07 1.20 1.01, 1.43 1.31 1.08, 1.57

Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; BMI, body mass index; IPCW, inverse probability of censoring weights; IPTW, inverse probability of treatment weights; IRR, incidence rate ratio.

a Weight (kg)/height (m)2.

b The unweighted model adjusted for baseline covariates plus time-varying confounders (age, trial arm, race/ethnicity, education, marital status, income, health insurance status, smoking status and history, disease status (cardiovascular disease, cancer, fracture), hormone therapy use, self-rated health, physical activity level, alcohol intake, physical functioning, hypertension, and diabetes).

c Adjusted only for baseline covariates (age, trial participation and assignment arm, race/ethnicity, education, marital status, income, health insurance status, and smoking status and history).

Results from secondary analyses stratified by baseline age cohort are presented in Table 3 and Figure 4, comparing mortality rate ratio estimates for an unweighted model and a marginal structural model with IPTW and IPCW. In women who were 50–59 years of age at baseline, results from the marginal structural model were greater than results from the unweighted adjusted model in all of the BMI categories. In the youngest age group, results from the marginal structural model indicated an elevated risk at low BMI (<18.5 and 18.5–24.9) and high BMI (35.0–39.9 and ≥40.0) compared with BMI 25.0–29.9. In the oldest age group, 70–79 years, comparing results from the unweighted adjusted and marginal structural model indicated differing effects in the low and high BMI categories. In the low BMI categories, the results from the marginal structural model were lower than the results from the unweighted adjusted model, but in the high BMI categories, results from the marginal structural model were higher than results from the unweighted adjusted models.

Table 3.

Incidence Rate Ratios, Stratified by Age Group at Baseline, for the Effect of Body Mass Index on Mortality in Postmenopausal Women in the Women’s Health Initiative Clinical Trials, United States, 1993–2005

Age Group and BMIa Adjusted, Unweighted Resultsb IPTW × IPCW Weighted Marginal Structural Modelc
IRR 95% CI IRR 95% CI
50–59 years
 <18.5 5.17 2.07, 8.28 5.97 3.27, 10.91
 18.5–24.9 1.44 1.08, 1.81 1.52 1.18, 1.97
 25.0–29.9 1.0 Referent 1.0 Referent
 30.0–34.9 0.96 0.71, 1.21 1.05 0.80, 1.37
 35.0–39.9 1.17 0.84, 1.52 1.53 1.13, 2.06
 ≥40.0 0.91 0.59, 1.24 1.43 1.00, 2.05
60–69 years
 <18.5 3.42 1.91, 4.93 2.83 1.80, 4.44
 18.5–24.9 1.53 1.30, 1.76 1.44 1.24, 1.68
 25.0–29.9 1.0 Referent 1.0 Referent
 30.0–34.9 0.85 0.71, 0.99 0.96 0.82, 1.14
 35.0–39.9 0.88 0.71, 1.07 1.03 0.83, 1.26
 ≥40.0 0.80 0.58, 1.02 1.11 0.85, 1.46
70–79 years
 <18.5 4.80 2.92, 6.67 3.46 2.29, 5.22
 18.5–24.9 1.41 1.17, 1.65 1.29 1.10, 1.51
 25.0–29.9 1.0 Referent 1.0 Referent
 30.0–34.9 0.83 0.66, 1.00 0.86 0.73, 1.07
 35.0–39.9 0.59 0.40, 0.77 0.84 0.62, 1.12
 ≥40.0 1.27 0.78, 1.77 1.35 0.89, 2.05

Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; BMI, body mass index; IPCW, inverse probability of censoring weights; IPTW, inverse probability of treatment weights; IRR, incidence rate ratio.

a Weight (kg)/ height (m)2.

b The unweighted model adjusted for baseline covariates plus time-varying confounders (age, trial arm, race/ethnicity, education, marital status, income, health insurance status, smoking status and history, disease status (cardiovascular disease, cancer, fracture), hormone therapy use, self-rated health, physical activity level, alcohol intake, physical functioning, hypertension, and diabetes).

c Adjusted only for baseline covariates (age, trial participation and assignment arm, race/ethnicity, education, marital status, income, health insurance status, and smoking status and history).

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Comparison of results from an unweighted model with adjustment and inverse probability of treatment–weighted (IPTW) and inverse probability of censoring–weighted (IPCW) marginal structural models for women who were 50–59 years old (A), 60–69 years old (B), and 70–79 years old (C) at baseline, Women’s Health Initiative cohort, United States, 1993–2005.

DISCUSSION

Using prospectively collected data from the WHI clinical trials, we fitted inverse probability–weighted marginal structural models to examine the influence of reverse causality and selective attrition on the BMI-mortality relationship in postmenopausal women. The final weighted model demonstrated the mortality risks associated with being either in the low (<18.5 and 18.5–24.9) or high (35.0–39.9 and ≥40.0) BMI categories compared with having a BMI of 25.0–29.9. There was no strong evidence of an increased mortality risk for women with BMI 30.0–34.9.

Results highlight that the influence of reverse causality and selection bias differs in low and high BMI categories. In the total cohort, in the low BMI categories, using an IPTW marginal structural model resulted in attenuated effect estimates, relative to an unweighted model with adjustment, and minimal change when IPCW were included in the model. These results support the hypothesis that the lower BMI categories are composed of individuals who have experienced disease-related weight loss. Accounting for the confounding by disease status using an IPTW marginal structural model resulted in a lower mortality rate than a standard model with adjustment. We did not find evidence that selective attrition influenced the BMI-mortality relationship in the low BMI categories.

In high BMI categories, using an IPTW marginal structural model resulted in greater effect estimates than in the unweighted model with adjustment, and adding IPCW to the model further increased the mortality rate. These findings support our 2 main hypotheses regarding the influence of reverse causality and selective attrition: Illness-related weight loss biases the relative risks downward in high BMI categories, because individuals who have experienced disease-related weight loss are less likely to be in the high BMI categories, thus leaving an artificially “healthy” group in the high BMI categories (10). Also, individuals with high BMI are more likely to drop out of a study, and also have a higher mortality risk, artificially attenuating effect estimates (42). After accounting for these 2 sources of downward bias, we saw the mortality rate ratio increase, which is consistent with prior research on reverse causality and selection bias (24, 25, 43, 44). It is important to note that the confidence intervals for the unweighted and weighted models were largely overlapping. This makes it difficult to draw a definitive conclusion about the potential influence of reverse causality in this analysis.

We also presented results from analyses stratified by baseline age cohort. The stratified results demonstrated notable differences in the BMI-mortality relationship in women who were older versus younger at cohort entry. The unweighted, stratified results are consistent with previous research in the WHI, demonstrating attenuated risks among women who were 70–79 years old at cohort entry compared with those who were 50–59 or 60–69 years old (1, 3). There are several plausible explanations for these findings, including selective survival, birth cohort effects, and effect measure modification, or, in all likelihood, a combination of these potential explanations. In research on older adults, there is a concern that obese individuals might experience premature mortality, dying prior to cohort entry and leaving a selected study sample of survivors to be enrolled into the analytical cohort. Survival up to cohort entry is recognized as an important form of selection bias known as selective survival (13, 45, 46). Selection bias due to selective survival is a challenging methodological concern when studying obesity in older adults (47).

Weuve et al. (48) report that this form of selective survival could be responsible for apparent “muting” of certain risk factors in older ages, whereby harmful risk factors appear less detrimental. Our results are consistent with this hypothesis but do not preclude other plausible explanations. This bias is structurally similar to the type of selection bias discussed by authors examining the relationship between cigarette smoking and dementia risk, a situation in which it is nearly impossible to claim any real benefit of exposure (24, 47). Unlike cigarette smoking, biological explanations for beneficial effects of BMI in old age might be plausible, but selection bias is also a reasonable explanation. Stratifying on age at cohort entry is one approach that can be used to understand and compare the obesity-related mortality risk among individuals who are different ages at cohort entry (baseline age). In this context, stratification does not eliminate the influence of selective survival, but could help us to understand it better. Simulation methods exist (44, 49) to quantify the magnitude of bias under scenarios that mimic the real-life survival experience of a cohort of older adults, and it is possible to use an estimand such as the “survivor average causal effect” that theoretically applies to individuals who would have survived to old age, regardless of exposure status (46). However, when seeking to understand the effect of obesity in a real-life cohort of older adults, neither approach fully answers the question of interest. It is difficult to provide a single explanation for why the BMI-mortality relationship differs across age strata because the reasons for these differences are complex and multifactorial, including both biological and methodological factors.

In addition to the use of marginal structural models, other methods have been suggested to control for reverse causality or confounding by disease status, including using weight histories, lifetime maximum BMI, instrumental variables, or Mendelian randomization studies (43, 5054). Although the rationale and assumptions for each approach vary, the underlying theory is that these methods are techniques for understanding the effect of BMI on mortality in later life, unconfounded by illness related weight loss. For instance, in the case of instrumental variables, offspring BMI has been used as an instrument for an individual’s own BMI, because offspring BMI is highly correlated with parental BMI, and an illness (i.e., cancer) that might affect parental BMI would not simultaneously affect offspring BMI (51). Mendelian randomization is an extension of this concept, whereby genetic variants are used as an instrumental variable to estimate the effect of BMI on mortality because disease status would have no influence on an individual’s genetic makeup (53). Using lifetime maximum BMI is a different approach, where individuals are asked to recall their highest adult weight, and that is used as an exposure variable in analyses in place of current weight. This approach is appealing because it identifies an individual’s BMI status in adulthood prior to any illness-related weight loss (43). Finally, other approaches leveraging weight histories include studies with repeated measures of BMI starting in early adulthood, ostensibly capturing the direct influence of changes in BMI, and potentially illness-related weight loss, on mortality (54).

There are several assumptions required to estimate causal effects from observational data. We assumed the model was correctly specified, that there was no unmeasured confounding (exchangeability assumption), no measurement error, and that the positivity assumption was satisfied (20). A limitation of our results is a potential violation of the consistency assumption (55, 56). One of the key identifiability assumptions for causal inference requires that interventions be well-defined; BMI is not a well-defined intervention because there are numerous ways for an individual to be assigned to their actual BMI (57, 58). It is possible to imagine several interventions that could affect an individual’s BMI (e.g., a diet and/or exercise program, medication), and each of these interventions might be associated with a different counterfactual outcome (57, 59). Cole and Frangakis (56) expand on the standard version of consistency to allow for multiple routes for a particular treatment to be assigned, where consistency is defined as Yjobs=Yj(x,k)ifx=Xj, no matter the value of k. This definition assumes that for any individual the exposure is only from the route observed for that individual (56). Other authors, such as VanderWeele and Hernán, have also discussed estimating causal effects when there are multiple versions of treatment (58, 60). An interesting direction for future work would be a simulation study and sensitivity analysis examining the effect of a series of different specifications for BMI as an exposure. If the results demonstrated that the effect of BMI on mortality is consistent across specifications, it would be evidence to support Cole and Frangakis’s definition of consistency (56).

The goal of this paper was to examine reverse causality and selection bias in the context of BMI-mortality studies in postmenopausal women. The strengths of the present work include a large, well-characterized cohort with detailed follow-up information and outcome ascertainment. Using methods from the causal inference literature, we avoided excluding individuals at baseline, deaths that occur early in the follow-up period, or making any unrealistic assumptions that censoring is ignorable. Given that the WHI cohort includes only women, further research is needed on this topic to explore differences by sex, as well as race or ethnicity (61). Another limitation is that the use of BMI to accurately characterize adiposity in postmenopausal women has been questioned (5, 62). BMI might be a less reliable indicator of adiposity in older women due to physical changes that occur after menopause and loss of lean muscle mass (63). This might have introduced some nondifferential exposure misclassification into our analysis (63). Despite these limitations, the conclusions from this study provide insight into common methodological concerns for researchers studying the effects of BMI in older adults. Our study highlights the mortality risk associated with having a very low or high BMI in postmenopausal women, which runs counter to the prevailing conclusion that BMI is a “less important” mortality risk factor in older adults.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Author affiliations: Department of Epidemiology and Environmental Health, School of Public Health and Health Professions, University at Buffalo, the State University of New York, Buffalo, New York (Hailey R. Banack, Jean Wactawski-Wende); Department of Nutritional Sciences, College of Agriculture and Life Sciences, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona (Jennifer W. Bea); Department of Epidemiology, Biostatistics, and Occupational Health, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec (Jay S. Kaufman); Department of Global Health, School of Public Health, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts (Andrew Stokes); Division of Research, Kaiser Permanente Northern California, Oakland, California (Candyce H. Kroenke, Bette Caan); Division of Cardiovascular Medicine, Department of Medicine, School of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, California, (Marcia L. Stefanick); Department of Epidemiology, School of Public Health, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington (Shirley A. Beresford); RAND Corporation, Santa Monica, California (Chloe E. Bird); Department of Public Health Sciences, School of Medicine, University of California Davis, Sacramento, California (Lorena Garcia); Department of Epidemiology, College of Public Health, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa (Robert Wallace); and Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, Oklahoma (Robert A. Wild).

This work was supported by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute of the National Institutes of Health (grants HHSN268201600018C, HHSN268201600001C, HHSN268201600002C, HHSN268201600003C, and HHSN268201600004C) and the Banting Postdoctoral Fellowship Program from the Canadian Institute of Health Research.

WHI Program Office: National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, Bethesda, Maryland—Jacques Rossouw, Shari Ludlam, Joan McGowan, Leslie Ford, and Nancy Geller. WHI Clinical Coordinating Center: Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, Seattle, Washington—Garnet Anderson, Ross Prentice, Andrea LaCroix, and Charles Kooperberg.

WHI Investigators and Academic Centers: Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts—JoAnn E. Manson; MedStar Health Research Institute/Howard University, Washington, DC—Barbara V. Howard; Stanford Prevention Research Center, Stanford, California—M.L.S.; The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio—Rebecca Jackson; University of Arizona, Tucson/Phoenix, Arizona—Cynthia A. Thomson; University at Buffalo, Buffalo, New York—J.W.-W.; University of Florida, Gainesville/Jacksonville, Florida—Marian Limacher; University of Iowa, Iowa City/Davenport, Iowa—Jennifer Robinson; University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania—Lewis Kuller; Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, North Carolina—Sally Shumaker; University of Nevada, Reno, Nevada—Robert Brunner; University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota—Karen L. Margolis. WHI Memory Study: Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, North Carolina—Mark Espeland.

Conflict of interest: none declared.

Abbreviations

BMI

body mass index

IPCW

inverse probability of censoring weights

IPTW

inverse probability of treatment weights

WHI

Women’s Health Initiative

REFERENCES

  • 1. Bea JW, Thomson CA, Wertheim BC, et al. . Risk of mortality according to body mass index and body composition among postmenopausal women. Am J Epidemiol. 2015;182(7):585–596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Flegal KM, Kit BK, Orpana H, et al. . Association of all-cause mortality with overweight and obesity using standard body mass index categories: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA. 2013;309(1):71–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Chen Z, Klimentidis YC, Bea JW, et al. . Body mass index, waist circumference, and mortality in a large multiethnic postmenopausal cohort—results from the Women’s Health Initiative. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2017;65(9):1907–1915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Kaplan GA, Haan MN, Wallace RB. Understanding changing risk factor associations with increasing age in adults. Annu Rev Public Health. 1999;20(1):89–108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Manson JE, Bassuk SS, Hu FB, et al. . Estimating the number of deaths due to obesity: can the divergent findings be reconciled? J Women’s Health (Larchmt). 2007;16(2):168–176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Ortman J, Velkoff V, Hogan H An aging nation: The older population in the United States. US Census Bureau; 2014. http://www.census.gov/prod/2014pubs/p25-1140.pdf. Accessed January 14, 2019.
  • 7. Flegal KM, Kruszon-Moran D, Carroll MD, et al. . Trends in obesity among adults in the United States, 2005 to 2014. JAMA. 2016;315(21):2284–2291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Flanders WD, Augestad LB. Adjusting for reverse causality in the relationship between obesity and mortality. Int J Obes (Lond). 2008;32(suppl 3):S42–S46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Sattar N, Preiss D. Reverse causality in cardiovascular epidemiological research: more common than imagined?. Circulation. 2017;135(24):2369–2372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Flegal KM, Graubard BI, Williamson DF, et al. . Reverse causation and illness-related weight loss in observational studies of body weight and mortality. Am J Epidemiol. 2011;173(1):1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Robins JM. Causal models for estimating the effects of weight gain on mortality. Int J Obes (Lond). 2008;32(suppl 3):S15–S41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Shakiba M, Soori H, Mansournia MA, et al. . Adjusting for reverse causation to estimate the effect of obesity on mortality after incident heart failure in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Epidemiol Health. 2016;38:e2016025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Hernán MA, Hernández-Díaz S, Robins JM. A structural approach to selection bias. Epidemiology. 2004;15(5):615–625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Hernán MA, Clayton D, Keiding N. The Simpson’s paradox unraveled. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;40(3):780–785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Hernán MA. Counterpoint: epidemiology to guide decision-making: moving away from practice-free research. Am J Epidemiol. 2015;182(10):834–839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Lajous M, Banack HR, Kaufman JS, et al. . Should patients with chronic disease be told to gain weight? The obesity paradox and selection bias. Am J Med. 2015;128(4):334–336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Flegal KM, Graubard BI, Yi SW. Comparative effects of the restriction method in two large observational studies of body mass index and mortality among adults. Eur J Clin Invest. 2017;47(6):415–421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Allison DB, Heo M, Flanders DW, et al. . Examination of “early mortality exclusion” as an approach to control for confounding by occult disease in epidemiologic studies of mortality risk factors. Am J Epidemiol. 1997;146(8):672–680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Stevens J, Juhaeri, Cai J. Changes in body mass index prior to baseline among participants who are ill or who die during the early years of follow-up. Am J Epidemiol. 2001;153(10):946–953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Cole SR, Hernán MA. Constructing inverse probability weights for marginal structural models. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;168(6):656–664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Cao B. Estimating the effects of obesity and weight change on mortality using a dynamic causal model. PLoS One. 2015;10(6):e0129946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Arnold M, Jiang L, Stefanick ML, et al. . Duration of adulthood overweight, obesity, and cancer risk in the Women’s Health Initiative: a longitudinal study from the United States. PLoS One. 2016;13(8):e1002081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Shakiba M, Mansournia MA, Salari A, et al. . Accounting for time-varying confounding in the relationship between obesity and coronary heart disease: analysis with G-estimation: the ARIC Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2018;187(6):1319–1326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Weuve J, Tchetgen Tchetgen EJ, Glymour MM, et al. . Accounting for bias due to selective attrition: the example of smoking and cognitive decline. Epidemiology. 2012;23(1):119–128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Banack HR, Harper S, Kaufman JS. Accounting for selection bias in studies of acute cardiac events. Can J Cardiol. 2018;34(6):709–716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Design of the Women’s Health Initiative clinical trial and observational study. The Women’s Health Initiative Study Group. Control Clin Trials. 1998;19(1):61–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Rossouw JE, Anderson GL, Prentice RL, et al. . Risks and benefits of estrogen plus progestin in healthy postmenopausal women: principal results from the Women’s Health Initiative randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2002;288(3):321–333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Manson JE, Chlebowski RT, Stefanick ML, et al. . Menopausal hormone therapy and health outcomes during the intervention and extended poststopping phases of the Women’s Health Initiative randomized trials. JAMA. 2013;310(13):1353–1368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Ritenbaugh C, Patterson RE, Chlebowski RT, et al. . The Women’s Health Initiative Dietary Modification Trial: overview and baseline characteristics of participants. Ann Epidemiol. 2003;13(9 suppl):S87–S97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Neuhouser ML, Aragaki AK, Prentice RL, et al. . Overweight, obesity, and postmenopausal invasive breast cancer risk: a secondary analysis of the Women’s Health Initiative randomized clinical trials. JAMA Oncol. 2015;1(5):611–621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Durazo-Arvizu R, McGee D, Li Z, et al. . Establishing the nadir of the body mass index-mortality relationship: a case study. J Am Stat Assoc. 1997;92(440):1312–1319. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Wong ES, Wang BC, Garrison LP, et al. . Examining the BMI-mortality relationship using fractional polynomials. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2011;11:175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Winter JE, MacInnis RJ, Wattanapenpaiboon N, et al. . BMI and all-cause mortality in older adults: a meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;99(4):875–890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Ritz J, Spiegelman D. Equivalence of conditional and marginal regression models for clustered and longitudinal data. Stat Methods Med Res. 2004;13(4):309–323. [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Robins JM, Hernán MÁ, Brumback B. Marginal structural models and causal inference in epidemiology. Epidemiology. 2000;11(5):550–560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Naimi AI, Moodie EE, Auger N, et al. . Constructing inverse probability weights for continuous exposures: a comparison of methods. Epidemiology. 2014;25(2):292–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Mansournia MA, Naimi AI, Greenland S. The implications of using lagged and baseline exposure terms in longitudinal causal and regression models. Am J Epidemiol. 2019;188(4):753–759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Howe CJ, Cole SR, Lau B, et al. . Selection bias due to loss to follow up in cohort studies. Epidemiology. 2016;27(1):91–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Howe CJ, Cole SR, Mehta SH, et al. . Estimating the effects of multiple time-varying exposures using joint marginal structural models: alcohol consumption, injection drug use, and HIV acquisition. Epidemiology. 2012;23(4):574–582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Cole SR, Hernán MA, Robins JM, et al. . Effect of highly active antiretroviral therapy on time to acquired immunodeficiency syndrome or death using marginal structural models. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;158(7):687–694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Delaney JA, Platt RW, Suissa S. The impact of unmeasured baseline effect modification on estimates from an inverse probability of treatment weighted logistic model. Eur J Epidemiol. 2009;24(7):343–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Simons-Morton DG, Obarzanek E, Cutler JA. Obesity research—limitations of methods, measurements, and medications. JAMA. 2006;295(7):826–828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Yu E, Ley SH, Manson JE, et al. . Weight history, all-cause and cause-specific mortality in three prospective cohort studies. Ann Intern Med. 2017;166(9):613–620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Mayeda ER, Banack HR, Bibbins-Domingo K, et al. . Can survival bias explain the age attenuation of racial inequalities in stroke incidence? A simulation study. Epidemiology. 2018;29(4):525–532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Flanders DW, Eldridge RC, McClellan W. A nearly unavoidable mechanism for collider bias with index-event studies. Epidemiology. 2014;25(5):762–764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Tchetgen Tchetgen EJ, Phiri K, Shapiro R. A simple regression-based approach to account for survival bias in birth outcomes research. Epidemiology. 2015;26(4):473–480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Hernán MA, Alonso A, Logroscino G. Cigarette Smoking and dementia: potential selection bias in the elderly. Epidemiology. 2008;19(3):448–450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Weuve J, Proust-Lima C, Power MC, et al. . Guidelines for reporting methodological challenges and evaluating potential bias in dementia research. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11(9):1098–1109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Mayeda ER, Tchetgen Tchetgen EJ, Power MC, et al. . A simulation platform for quantifying survival bias: an application to research on determinants of cognitive decline. Am J Epidemiol. 2016;184(5):378–387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Kjøllesdal MKR, Smith GD, Ariansen I, et al. . The association between BMI and mortality using early adulthood BMI as an instrumental variable for midlife BMI. Sci Rep. 2018;8:11499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Carslake D, Davey Smith G, Gunnell D, et al. . Confounding by ill health in the observed association between BMI and mortality: evidence from the HUNT Study using offspring BMI as an instrument. Int J Epidemiol. 2018;47(3):760–770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Stokes A. Using maximum weight to redefine body mass index categories in studies of the mortality risks of obesity. Popul Health Metr. 2014;12:6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Hägg S, Fall T, Ploner A, et al. . Adiposity as a cause of cardiovascular disease: a Mendelian randomization study. Int J Epidemiol. 2015;44(2):578–586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Zheng H, Tumin D, Qian Z. Obesity and mortality risk: new findings from body mass index trajectories. Am J Epidemiol. 2013;178(11):1591–1599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Hernán MA, Robins JM. Causal Inference. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Cole SR, Frangakis CE. The consistency statement in causal inference: a definition or an assumption? Epidemiology. 2009;20(1):3–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Hernán MA, Taubman SL. Does obesity shorten life? the importance of well-defined interventions to answer causal questions. Int J Obes (Lond). 2008;32(suppl 3):S8–S14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. VanderWeele TJ. On well-defined hypothetical interventions in the potential outcomes framework. Epidemiology. 2018;29(4):e24–e25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Shakiba M, Mansournia MA, Kaufman JS. Estimating effect of obesity on stroke using G-estimation: the ARIC study. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2019;27(2):304–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. VanderWeele TJ, Hernán MA. Causal inference under multiple versions of treatment. J Causal Inference. 2013;1:1–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Corrada MM, Kawas CH, Mozaffar F, et al. . Association of body mass index and weight change with all-cause mortality in the elderly. Am J Epidemiol. 2006;163(10):938–949. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Batsis JA, Mackenzie TA, Bartels SJ, et al. . Diagnostic accuracy of body mass index to identify obesity in older adults: NHANES 1999–2004. Int J Obes (Lond). 2016;40(5):761–767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Banack HR, Stokes A, Fox MP, et al. . Stratified probabilistic bias analysis for body mass index-related exposure misclassification in postmenopausal women. Epidemiology. 2018;29(5):604–613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from American Journal of Epidemiology are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES