TABLE 3.
References | Group (N) | Neuropsychological Tests | Strengths | Limitations | Conclusions |
Goldstein et al., 1996 | 29 GWV, 39 non-veterans | WAIS-R Information WAIS-R Similarities WAIS-R Digit Span WAIS-R Arithmetic WAIS-R Digit Symbol WAIS-R Picture Completion WAIS-R Block Design COWAT Incidental Memory Verbal Associative Learning Short-Term Memory Symbol Digit Learning Trail Making Test CPT Grooved Pegboard | – GWVs compared to demographically matched controls on neuropsychological tests – Test battery included some measurements sensitive to GWI (i.e., Block Design) |
– No case designation based on GWI – Limited measurement of mood |
– Found that GWVs performed worse on impairment index in compared to controls – However, the level of impairments (1 SD) was not consistent with subjective cognitive complaints – Effect size Cohen’s d calculation∗ show small effect for Trails B (d = 0.25) and pegboard dominant (d = 0.18). |
Axelrod and Milner, 1997 | 44 male GWV from Army Guard unit | Reitan-Indiana Aphasia Screening Test WAIS-R AVLT Stroop∗ Trail Making Test WMS-R Finger Tapping Grooved Pegboard∗ Grip Strength COWAT Category Fluency PIAT-R WCST | – Veterans compared via normative data and grouped by both objective (i.e., Grooved Pegboard, Stroop) and subjective (i.e., health complaint) measures | – Small sample size – Lack of correction for Type 1 error – Volunteer sample – No hypotheses |
– No evidence of deficits – Differences in subjective complaints in psychological measures – Effect size Cohen’s d calculation∗ show large effect for Trails B (d = 1.28), and small/medium effect for motor tests (d = −0.63 to −0.48) |
Hom et al., 1997 | 26 GWV with Haley Syndromes, 10 GWV and 10 non-deployed veteran controls | WAIS∗ Halstead Category Test∗ Tactual Performance Test Seashore Rhythm Test Speech-Sounds Perception Test Finger Oscillation Test Trail Making Test∗ Reitan-Indiana Aphasia Screening Examination, Reitan-Klove Sensory Perceptual Examination Reitean-Klove Lateral Dominance Examination Reitan Word Finding Test WMS-R∗ WRAT3∗ | – Matched GWV group – GWI criteria used with a factor derived technique |
– Small sample size – Limitations of those with fitting factor criteria – Multiple hypothesis testing Initial differences between control group and cases |
– Differences between GWI and GWVs in global neurocognitive functioning – Cohen’s d calculation∗ showed a large effect size for Block Design (d = −1.57) and a medium effect size for Trail Making Test- Trail B (d = 0.69) – Psychological responding was consistent with other medical patients |
Sillanpaa et al., 1997 | 49 GWV from a single Army reserve military police unit | NES-2 CPT∗ Grip Strength Grooved Pegboard∗ Neurological Screen Fingertip Number Writing perception∗ WCST∗ AVLT WAIS-R ∗ | – Examiner blind to participant’s medical history – Exposure to toxins measured via self-report – Models tested to mimic GWI and psychological functioning |
– Small sample size – Low variance and range in scores – Multicollinearity problems present |
– Psychological model accounted for neuropsychological performance with a R2 of at least a 0.03 (at or above a small effect) for all domains – At least a small effect for exposure and symptoms seen in nearly all domains – Cohen’s d calculation∗ showed a medium effect for motor functioning (d = 0.76) |
Vasterling et al., 1998 | 43 GWVs: 19 with PTSD and 24 without | Letter Cancelation Stroop CPT∗ WCST WAIS-R Digit Span WAIS-R Arithmetic∗ Rey-AVLT∗ CVMT∗ | – Investigated GWVs with PTSD | – Small sample size – Lack of comparison sample of participants with differing mental disorders – Unable to manipulate trauma exposure |
– Veterans with PTSD had deficiencies in sustained attention, mental manipulation, information acquisition, and retroactive interference |
Anger et al., 1999 | 66 GWV with unexplained symptoms from WA and OR, 35 GWV controls | Behavioral Assessment and Research System (BARS): Simple Reaction Time∗ Selective Attention Test Digit Span∗ Symbol Digit∗ Serial Digit Learning ODTP∗ | – Compared those with GWI with controls – Physician blind to participant status |
– Volunteer sample Self-selection bias | – Specific problems demonstrated in processing speed – Individuals compared based on processing speed differences also found that those with slower processing speed had deficits in memory and attention |
Binder et al., 1999 | 100 GWV with unexplained symptoms | CPT ODTP∗ | – Investigated self-report of cognitive ability and affective distress in conjunction with objective cognitive performance | – Cognitive measures may lack sensitivity | – Subjective complaints associated with psychological distress over objective cognitive performance |
Bunegin et al., 2001 | 8 symptomatic GWV, 8 GWV controls | NES-2: Hand-Eye Coordination Simple Reaction Time Visual Digit Span Forward and Backward∗ Horizontal Addition Pattern Memory∗ Switching Attention∗ | – Compared symptomatic GWVs with non-symptomatic GWVs – Investigated blood flow |
– Small sample size – Less sensitive measures used |
– Symptomatic GWVs had worse performance in memory and executive function tasks – Exposure to acetone also impacted cognitive performance in GWVs – Cohen’s d calculation∗ showed a small effect size for CPT, reaction time (d = −0.14) |
Lange et al., 2001 | 48 symptomatic GWV, 39 GWV controls | NES∗ PASAT∗ WAIS-R Digit Span∗ CVLT RCFT Trails Making Test Category Test∗ Judgment of Line Orientation Test WAIS-R Block Design Grooved Pegboard | – Compared those with GWI with matched controls | – Volunteer sample of health-care seeking veterans – Small sample size CFS sample – Unequal cells comparisons |
– Impairment found in attention (R2 = 0.12– 0.19) and executive functioning tasks (R2 = 0.07) even after controlling for mood – Cohen’s d calculation∗ showed a large effect size for CPT reaction time (d = 0.85). |
White et al., 2001 | 193 GWV, 47 Germany deployed veterans | WAIS-R CPT Trail Making Test PASAT WCST Digit Span CVLT∗ WMS-R∗ Finger Tapping Purdue Pegboard POMS∗ TOMM | – Compared deployed and non-deployed veterans – Detailed account of toxin exposure – Stratified Random sample |
– TOMM scores evidenced possible poor effort in some participants – Multiple comparisons |
– Initially, mood was only significant with adjustment for multiple comparisons – Comparing those with and without exposure, had worse performance in short term memory (R2 = 0.315– 0.399), attention (R2 = 0.381), and mood ((R2 = 0.202– 0.315) – Cohen’s d calculation∗ showed small effect sizes for all neuropsych tests (d = −0.47 to 0.22 |
David et al., 2002 | 209 British GWV, 132 non-deployed era veterans | WAIS-R NART WAIS-III Letter Sequencing PASAT SART Stroop Trail Making Test WMS-R Purdue Pegboard | – Compared Gulf War deployment and medical status against other deployments (Bosnia) and controls – Stratified Random sample Blind raters Statistical analyses |
– Cross-over effects Self-report symptoms | – Significance only found in PTSD measure – Cohen’s d calculation∗ showed a large effect size for Block Design (d = −2.53) |
Lindem et al., 2003a | 193 GWV, 47 Germany deployed veterans | WAIS-R Information subscale∗ WAIS-R Digit Span∗ WMS-R Digit span CPT∗ Trail Making Test WCST PASAT∗ Finger Tapping∗ Purdue Pegboard∗ WAIS-R Block Design WMS-R Verbal Paired Associate Learning CVLT∗ Visual Reproduction∗ POMS∗ | – Investigated PTSD in relation to exposure to chemical agents – Investigated symptom severity – Better generalization with use of overall cohorts in Gulf War – Large sample size |
– Correlational analyses – Lack of baseline performance or known preexisting conditions |
– PTSD symptoms severity correlated with greater deficits in a wide array of neuropsychological measures in GW deployed veterans (Partial R2 = 0.02– 0.10) – CBW exposure and PTSD severity in GWVs associated with deficits in sustained attention (Partial R2 = 0.0004– 0.0015), motor speed/motor coordination (0.0000–0.0007) |
Lindem et al., 2003b | 193 GWV, 47 Germany deployed veterans | WAIS-R Information subscale WAIS-R Digit Span WMS-R Digit span CPT Trail Making Test A Trail Making Test B WCST PASAT Finger Tapping Purdue Pegboard WAIS-R Block Design WMS-R Verbal Paired Associate Learning CVLT Visual Reproduction POMS∗ | – Investigated GWVs discrepancy between subject complaints and objective performance | – Multiple comparisons | – Subjective complaints more associated with mood symptoms |
Lindem et al., 2003c | 58 GWV and 19 Germany-deployed veterans | WAIS-R Information subscale WAIS-R Digit Span WMS-R Digit span CPT Trail Making Test∗ WCST∗ PASAT Finger Tapping Purdue Pegboard WAIS-R Block Design WMS-R Verbal Paired Associate Learning∗ CVLT∗ Visual Reproduction∗ TOMM∗ POMS | – Investigated motivation in GWVs | – Small sample size – Difficult to ascertain the reason behind lower TOMM scores – Low amount of those with low TOMM scores |
– Variability was seen in those with lower TOMM scores particularly in attention, executive functioning, and memory |
Proctor et al., 2003 | Danish GWVs (215), comparing deployed (143) and non-deployed veterans (72) | WAIS-R Information CPT Trail-making Test Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Purdue Pegboard WAIS-R Block Designs California Verbal Learning Test WMS Visual Reproductions POMS∗ TOMM | – Blind to categorization of “higher or lower” symptom status during all phases of recruitment, testing, and interviewing – Differences in deployment missions between Danish and American groups |
– Significant mean age difference between deployed (38.8 years) and non-deployed (34.8 years) – Self-report of exposure |
– Evidence of increased mood complaints related to GW service – no significant domain-specific evidence of CNS dysfunction was found – No associations between reported GW Environmental exposures related to the Danish GW deployment mission and objective measures of cognitive functioning were observed |
Sullivan et al., 2003 | 207 treatment seeking GWV (120 referred for neuropsych evaluation), 53 treatment seeking non-deployed veterans | WAIS-R Information WAIS-R Digit Span∗ Trail Making Test NES CPT Stroop Test Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test Wisconsin Card Sort Test∗ CVLT WMS-R Paired Associate Learning WMS-R Visual Reproductions∗ Hooper Visual Organization Test WAIS-R Block Design∗ Finger tapping Purdue Pegboard RCFT∗ POMS∗ TOMM | – Investigated deployment, treatment seeking, use of pyridostigmine bromide (PB) and PTSD on cognitive functioning – Matched by control group that was also treatment seeking |
– Self-report of exposure – Sample size small for comparisons |
– GW deployed worse than controls on attention, visuospatial skills, visual memory, and mood – PB use in GWVs worse in executive system tasks – GWVs with PTSD versus those without PTSD showed no differences – Cohen’s d calculation∗ showed a large effect sizes for block design and digit span forward (d = −2.43 to −1.00), small effect sizes for Trails A and B, digit span backward, CVLT, WMS, immediate recall, and finger tapping (d = −0.090 to 0.43), and a medium effect size for WMS, delay recall (d = −0.55). |
Vasterling et al., 2003 | 72 GWVs deployed and 33 non-deployed GWVs | WAIS-R Digit Span WCST AVLT CVMT Purdue Pegboard WAIS-R Information | – Selection of a non-treatment seeking group of GWVs – Comparison of deployed GWVs to a group of GWVs mobilized but no deployed – Use of olfactory and neurocognitive measures with demonstrated sensitivity to neurotoxic exposures |
– Sample was regionally recruited | – No evidence that performance on olfactory or neurocognitive measures were related to war-zone duty or to self-reported exposure to GW toxicants – Symptoms of emotional distress were positively correlated with self-report of health and cognitive complaints |
Proctor et al., 2006 | 140 Army GWV with modeled estimates of nerve agent exposure | CPT Trail Making Test WAIS-R Digit Span WCST Finger Tapping Purdue Pegboard∗ WAIS-R Block Design∗ CVLT WMS-R verbal paired associate learning WMS visual reproduction | – Stratified random sampling – Examined performance by exposure to sarin and cyclosarin – Sample was unaware of sarin and cyclosarin components, analyses were conducted a prior to exposure knowledge |
– Etiology undetermined given the risk of another illness between exposure and measurement (i.e., no baseline health information) – Limited objective information about exposures |
– Exposure associated with poor fine psychomotor dexterity (d = 0.44) and visuospatial abilities (d = 0.43) |
Barrash, 2007 | 301 GWV, 99 era veterans deployed elsewhere | WAIS-III Similarities∗ Block Design∗ Digit Symbol Digit Span North American Reading Test – Revised Starry Night Test COWAT AVLT∗ Benton Visual Retention Test∗ RMT-Words and Faces∗ Stroop Grooved Pegboard∗ | – Study of effort and neurocognitive performance in GWVs –Grouped by credible or non-credible impairment |
– Small sample of non-credible group –Decreased statistical power – Lack of measures investigating reason behind low effort |
– Non-credible impairment associated with more variability in tests and worse emotional/cognitive functioning |
Wallin et al., 2009 | 41 GWVs: 25 with GWI and 16 controls | WRAT reading Block Design Trail Making Test CVLT Pegboard | – Stratified random sampling – Used GWI criteria to divide groups |
– Small sample size – Gap between deployment and time of study – Multiple analyses |
– Differences only seen in mood and health measures – Cohen’s d calculation∗ showed a medium effect size for block design, Trails B, and CVLT long delay, (d = −0.73 to 0.51), and a small effect size for WRAT reading, Trails A, and Pegboard (d = −0.13 to 0.39). |
Toomey et al., 2009 | 1061 deployed GWV and 1128 non-deployed GWV | WAIS-III Digit Span Trail Making Test∗ PASAT CPT∗ WCST CVLT∗ RCFT∗ Finger Tapping∗ Purdue Pegboard∗ TOMM WRAT-III | – Investigated differences in deployment, toxin exposure, and GWI status – Large sample size, stratified random sampling method – Use of factor analysis – Use of Khamisiyah exposure data |
– Low study participation rates – Cross-sectional design – Neuropsychology raters were not blind to condition |
– Deployed veterans had worse performance on motor speed (OR = 2.35) and sustained attention (OR = 2.64) – Those with Khamisiyah exposure showed poor motor speed after controlling for mood – Cohen’s d calculation∗ showed small effect sizes for all neuropsych tests (d = −0.09 to 0.06). |
Chao et al., 2010 | 40 GWV with a history of DOD notified sarin cyclosarin exposure risk and 40 non-exposed matched GW veteran controls | CPT Trail Making Test WAIS-III Digit Span Short Category Test COWAT Grooved Pegboard WAIS-III Digit Symbol, matching WAIS-III Block Design WAIS-III Verbal Comprehension Index CVLT-II WMS-III Logical Memory BVMT-R TOMM∗ | – Used matched cohort sample – Use of Khamisiyah exposure data |
– Lack of information regarding the unit and rank of veterans – Lack of information regarding symptom severity (i.e., CMI, smoking status, head injuries) – Lack of cumulative exposure for all GW veterans – Plume estimates only by unit |
– No differences in cognitive measures after controlling for poor effort (i.e., failure of TOMM). – Cohen’s d calculation∗ showed a small effect sizes for all neuropsych tests (d = 0.22 to 0.26). |
Chao et al., 2011 | 64 sarin and cyclosarin exposed GWVs and 64 “matched” unexposed GWVs | CPT∗ WAIS-III Digit Span∗ Trail Making Test Short Category Test CVLT-II∗ Grooved Pegboard TOMM | – Used matched controls to compare structural and functional differences in veterans with suspected neurotoxicant exposure – Use of more sensitive MRI (4T) – Use of some sensitive tests for neuropsychological and mood outcomes |
– Lack of information regarding veteran’s unit, severity of GWI symptoms, smoking status, or history of head injury – Neurotoxicant exposure measured at unit over individual level |
– Reduced gray matter and white matter in exposed veterans which was linked to neurotoxicant exposure – Exposed veterans made more omission errors and had slower responses times; omission errors was also linked to neurotoxicant exposure – Cohen’s d calculations∗ showed a medium effect size for Trails A (d = −0.64), and small effect sizes for CPT, Trails B, CVLT, and pegboard (d = −0.36 to 0.38). |
Chao et al., 2016 | 136 GWVs: 106 who reported hearing chemical alarms sound | WAIS III Block Design∗ Digit Span∗ CVLT | – Had to rely on self-reports of deployment-related exposures – Lack of pre-GW measurements of brain structure and function – Small sample size – Lack of a non-deployed GW-era veteran control group |
– Self-reported frequency of hearing chemical alarms was inversely associated with and significantly predicted performance on the Block Design visuospatial task. – This effect was partially mediated by the relationship between hearing chemical alarms and lateral occipital cortex volume. |
|
Chao, 2017 | 428 deployed GWVs: 272 which met CDC criteria for CMI | CVLT-II∗ | – Tested verbal memory with GWVs presenting with subjective memory complaints – Large sample size |
– Only measured one domain to control for Type 1 error | – Worse performance on verbal memory associated subjective complaints over and above mood, however, there was higher endorsement of PTSD symptoms |
Sullivan et al., 2018 | 159 GW-deployed preventative medicine personnel who had varying levels of pesticide exposure | WAIS-III information subtest Boston Naming Test Trail Making Test∗ CPT∗ WCST Finger Tapping Grooved Pegboard HVOT RCFT∗ Stanford-Binet Copying Test CVLT II POMS∗ TOMM | – Grouped veterans by exposure (low/high) to PB and pesticides – Sample had sophisticated knowledge of exposure as they were part of the medical team |
– Multiple analyses – Exposures of PB and pesticide may be correlated – Classifications of groups based on self-report |
– High pesticide/high PB had worse information processing speed, attention (i.e., errors), visual memory, and increased mood complaints |
See original journal articles in first column for test references. ∗Denotes significance of p < 0.05.