Summary
Introduction
Intuitive eating (IE) has emerged as a weight‐neutral approach to health promotion for those with overweight/obesity. This weight‐neutral paradigm has some support, although research thus far has often neglected to control for potential confounds (i.e. objective weight status and demographics) and foundational studies are lacking. The objective of the current study was to observe the unique association of IE with physical health indicators in a sample of adults, independent of objective weight status.
Methods
Participants were 248 adults (32 ± 14 years old, 73% female, 64% White) of all weight categories (18.2–55.3 kg m−2), with an average body mass index (BMI) of 30 ± 8 kg m−2. IE was measured with the Intuitive Eating Scale‐2 (IES‐2). BMI was objectively measured in‐lab. Health indicators included blood pressure (BP) and fasting glucose.
Results
A series of hierarchical linear regressions revealed no significant associations between IE and systolic BP (β = −0.076, P = 0.256), diastolic BP (DBP; β = −0.122, P = 0.073) or fasting glucose (β = 0.047, P = 0.500) after controlling for BMI. All effects sizes were small or below (f 2 = 0.00 to −0.04). Sensitivity analyses revealed significantly lower DBP in high intuitive eaters versus low when analysed with a t‐test, t(111.651) = 3.602, P < 0.001, Levene corrected; however, after controlling for relevant covariates (i.e. BMI and demographics), analysis of covariance revealed no difference in DBP between groups, F(1, 116) = 0.330, P = 0.567. No significant differences in systolic BP or fasting glucose were observed between low and high intuitive eaters before or after considering covariates.
Conclusions
In sum, this study investigated associations between IE and common indicators of physical health after controlling for objective weight status. Findings revealed no unique relationship between IE and physical health, and any IE–physical health relationships that were observed were accounted for BMI and/or demographic factors.
Keywords: Blood pressure, eating behaviours, glucose, weight control
Introduction
Current treatment approaches for obesity most often consist of behavioural weight loss (BWL) interventions, typically characterized by lifestyle intervention focused on decreasing calorie intake and increasing physical activity.1 Although the current obesity treatment approach often leads to initial losses of weight, approximately 46% of lost weight is regained.2 The general consensus is that weight loss maintenance from BWL treatments is poor, and individuals typically regain most, if not all, of the weight that they lose.3, 4, 5, 6 Further, although the primary target of BWL interventions is physical health, the treatment can affect many other aspects of individuals' lives. A review of the literature revealed that dietary restraint – a tendency to consciously restrict food intake that is often promoted in BWL calorie reduction plans – is associated with negative psychosocial factors such as excessive body and shape concerns, problematic food‐related attitudes and behaviours and impairments in general psychological functioning in some individuals.7
As a response to these weaknesses in current obesity treatments, new paradigms that are less focused on weight are being considered,8, 9 particularly in light of evidence suggesting that improvements in physiological markers of health (e.g. blood pressure [BP] and cholesterol) can be found independent of weight loss.10, 11, 12, 13 One such approach that has received substantial attention in research, clinical and popular culture domains is intuitive eating (IE).11, 14 As first defined by Tribole and Resch,15 IE is an adaptive approach to eating that is based on trusting the body to guide eating decisions rather than adhering to external rules of dietary restraint. It is currently conceptualized as four separate domains, which together refer to eating based on physiological need, choosing foods that provide optimal fuel and removing the restrictions of when and what to eat that are common in traditional diets.16
Intuitive eating shares some similarities with traditional non‐diet programmes, such as Health at Every Size (HAES).17 The HAES philosophy includes a weight‐neutral approach to health that ‘promotes feeling good about oneself; eating well in a natural, relaxed way; and being comfortably active’ (p. 13, emphasis added).17 As seen, HAES does include IE‐like principles (see italics), but it also captures acceptance of the self and body and aspects of physical activity. IE may also be compared with emerging weight loss paradigms, including acceptance‐based treatment (ABT) for weight loss.18 Compared with IE, ABT approaches to weight loss are more closely aligned with traditional BWL treatments.18 While IE encourages the release of dietary restrictions and acceptance of internal eating regulation, ABT utilizes acceptance to increase adherence to traditional dietary prescriptions and achieve weight loss through skills such as mindfulness and cognitive defusion.18 Additionally, both HAES and ABT are larger treatment programmes, while IE is a specific construct. Although there are a variety of interventions/treatment programmes utilizing the weight‐neutral/non‐diet approach present in the literature, all of these approaches are similar in that they include a focus on eating based on internal cues rather than dietary prescriptions, which is the construct objectively defined as IE.11, 15, 18
Intuitive eating has been positively associated with a broad range of psychological and behavioural factors.7, 19, 20, 21 Specifically, IE has been found to have a positive relationship with general psychological well‐being, body image and self‐esteem, and pleasure from eating. Additionally, IE has displayed a negative relationship with preoccupation with food and disordered eating. Despite these psychological benefits, it may be premature to promote IE as a health‐promoting alternative to dieting if there are limited benefits to physical health.
Unfortunately, the research on IE and physical health indicators is less developed. To begin, IE is consistently negatively associated with body mass index (BMI) in cross‐sectional studies,20, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27 and weight‐neutral interventions are often associated with either a maintenance of or a decrease in weight.10, 13, 28, 29, 30, 31, 32 Fewer studies include biomarkers other than BMI or weight, and available results are mixed. For example, Hawks and colleagues found that individuals high in IE displayed lower BMI, higher high‐density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol, lower triglycerides and lower cardiovascular risk than individuals low in IE in a cross‐sectional study.22 Other investigations in this area have been conducted to evaluate non‐dieting interventions. Recent reviews of these studies have suggested that these interventions may positively impact BP and blood lipids.12, 33, 34 Specific findings across the literature include improvements in the levels of total cholesterol,10, 13, 31, 32 HDL cholesterol,28 LDL cholesterol,10, 13, 31, 32 triglycerides,13 systolic BP (SBP)10, 13, 30, 32, 35 and diastolic BP (DBP).28, 30, 32, 35 However, there is also at least one published study that did not observe an effect for each one of these variables,10, 13, 28, 31, 32, 36, 37 and none of these studies considered the impact of BMI on the IE–physical health relationships.
Taken together, the aforementioned findings suggest that IE and the weight‐neutral paradigm may contribute to both psychological and physical benefits. However, several gaps and methodological limitations are present in the current literature (e.g. non‐representative samples, inconsistent study designs and a lack of foundational research) that limit conclusions about the physical health benefits of IE and whether they are independent of weight status. Thus, the objective of the current study was to observe the unique baseline association of IE with physical health indicators in a sample of adults, independent of weight status. The primary study aims were (a) to determine whether IE is associated with better overall physical health (as measured using BP and glucose measurements) and (b) to evaluate whether these relationships remain after adjusting for BMI. Due to lack of consilience in the extant literature, we made no a priori hypotheses.
Method
Overview
Data used in the current analyses came from compiling baseline data from multiple studies that included the Intuitive Eating Scale (IES‐2), measured BMI, BP and fasting glucose measurements. Baseline data from two larger trials of BWL in adults with obesity – Cognitive and Self‐Regulatory Mechanisms of Obesity Study (COSMOS)18 and Pilot of Weight Reduction in Underserved Populations (POWER‐UP) – were used as well as data from community and laboratory conducted studies. Compilation of data from these sources allowed for a diverse sample of individuals from all weight statuses, from underweight to obese.
Participants
Participants included adults with obesity from the community enrolled in a weight loss trial (i.e. COSMOS and POWER‐UP), community members of all weight statuses and college students enrolled at a large public university. Inclusion criteria for this study were (a) aged ≥21 years and ≤65 years, (b) speak English fluently, (c) completed the IES‐2 and (d) completed BMI, BP and glucose assessments. Exclusion criteria were as follows: (a) individuals out of the stated age range, (b) those who were currently pregnant, (c) history of a neurological disorder and/or (d) non‐English speaking. Using G*Power version 3,38 we estimated that a sample size of at least 156 participants would be needed to have an 80% chance to detect a significant small effect (f 2 = 0.051) at the 5% level (one‐tailed).
Measures
Intuitive eating (IES‐2)
The IES‐216 is a 23‐item self‐report instrument that measures an individual's tendency to eat based on his or her body's internal cues. Responses to each question range from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree) and are averaged to provide the total IE score. Higher scores represent higher levels of IE. The IES‐2 has previously displayed good reliability and validity in both women and men.16 Specifically, Cronbach's coefficient alphas for internal consistency were 0.87 and 0.89 for women and men, respectively. In the current study, the IES‐2 total score displayed good reliability as well (women α = 0.84; men α = 0.79).
Health indicators
Multiple measures that have been shown to be related to physical health were examined in this study. Specifically, these measures are predictive of negative health outcomes commonly associated with obesity (e.g. hypertension, type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular disease).
Body mass index
Body mass index was measured continuously as a function of participants' height and weight. Participants' height (cm) and weight (kg) were measured directly by research personnel using research‐grade scales: Tanita scale (TANITA Body Fat Analyzer Model TBF‐105 K930599) or seca scale (Model 813).
Blood pressure
Systolic blood pressure and DBP (mmHg) were measured with an electronic sphygmomanometer by a research personnel. When multiple readings were available, the average of the first three readings was taken.
Fasting glucose
Fasting glucose levels were obtained via clinic blood draw (COSMOS sample) or via fingerstick and a glucometer (for all other participants). All glucose measurements were taken following a fast of at least 8 h.
Demographic factors and covariates
Participants completed a questionnaire assessing demographic variables, including gender, age, race and education level. These demographic variables were included as covariates in the data analysis.
Procedure
All data were collected from the baseline visit of the larger, ongoing studies across multiple settings. All participants signed informed consent documents approved by the university's Institutional Review Board and were adequately compensated. IE was measured during the baseline visit via self‐report using the IES‐2.16 Demographic factors and covariates (i.e. age, gender, race and education level) were self‐reported. BMI and BP were objectively measured by a trained research personnel. Fasting glucose levels were measured via glucometer or clinic blood draw.
Data analysis
All data were reviewed prior to analysis to assure completion and adequacy based on the assumptions for statistical normality, and tests for homogeneity of variance were conducted (i.e. Levene's). Missing data were imputed via within‐person within‐scale mean imputation when ≤20% of scale responses were missing. Outliers were retained unless they impacted the normality of the data.
A series of hierarchical linear regression analyses were performed to evaluate whether IES‐2 total scores predicted health indicators when adjusting for BMI. A separate analysis was conducted to evaluate IES‐2 total scores on each of the outcome variables (i.e. SBP, DBP and fasting glucose). For all analyses, covariates included age, gender, race and education level. All covariates were entered in Step 1, IES‐2 total was entered in Step 2 and BMI was entered in Step 3. A partial Bonferroni correction was performed according to SISA guidelines,39 which revealed a corrected alpha level of 0.028 to account for multiple testing of interrelated outcomes. Therefore, this alpha level (i.e. 0.028) was used as the criteria for statistical significance for all primary outcome variables. Of note, given that study participants came from several settings, stratified analyses were performed to assure that there were no different patterns of results among the treatment‐seeking and non‐treatment‐seeking samples.
To further probe the relationship between IE and health indicators and pursue a clinical application of IE scores, sensitivity analyses were performed. First, t‐tests were performed to analyse differences in SBP, DBP and fasting glucose between individuals high and low in IE. Second, analyses of covariance (ancovas) were performed to analyse these differences after controlling for the previously stated covariates (i.e. BMI, sex, race/ethnicity and education). Based on previous literature,22 individuals were categorized as high and low IE based on quartile; high IE was defined as a score ≥75th percentile in the sample, and low IE was defined as a score ≤25th percentile in the sample.
Results
Participants
Participants were included in the final sample if they met all eligibility criteria and had complete data for all demographic and IES‐2 measures. Three individuals were excluded due to ineligibility (i.e. age >65), and 31 individuals were excluded due to missing demographic or IES‐2 values. One individual was excluded only from fasting glucose analysis due to an out‐of‐range fasting glucose value. The final sample consisted of 248 adults who were 32.2 ± 14.3 years old, 73% female, and 64% White. Because of missing data on outcome variables, the analysed samples for BP and fasting glucose analyses included 243 and 212 participants, respectively. Participants had a mean BMI of 30.4 ± 7.6 kg m−2, and all weight categories were represented (BMI range 18.2–55.3 kg m−2). Mean values for other collected biomarkers (i.e. BP and glucose) were within the normal range on average. At the group level, participants displayed IE scores of 3.3 ± 0.5 on average for the 1–5 scale. Total IE displayed a moderate‐to‐large effect size in association with BMI (r = −0.448). Detailed demographic and descriptive data can be found in Table 1. Stratified analyses confirmed no differences between study samples (i.e. treatment‐seeking versus non‐treatment‐seeking); therefore, all results presented below are from the aggregate sample.
Table 1.
Max (N = 248) | |
---|---|
M (SD) or N (%) | |
Demographics and history | |
Age | 32.18 (14.29) |
Gender (female) | 182 (73.4) |
Education level | |
Some high school | 7 (2.8) |
High school | 31 (12.5) |
Some college | 76 (30.6) |
Bachelor's degree | 70 (28.2) |
Graduate or professional degree | 64 (25.8) |
Race/ethnicity | |
African American | 13 (5.2) |
American Indian/Alaska Native | 37 (14.9) |
Asian/Pacific Islander | 7 (2.8) |
Caucasian | 159 (64.1) |
Hispanic | 7 (2.8) |
Other | 5 (2.0) |
Multiple | 20 (8.1) |
Biomarkers and obesity indicators | |
BMI (kg/m−2) | 30.41 (7.56) |
Blood pressure (mmHg) | |
SBP | 116.06 (13.15) |
DBP | 74.64 (10.14) |
Fasting glucose (mg/dL−1) | 95.68 (26.83) |
Intuitive eating (IES‐2) | 3.27 (0.51) |
BMI, body mass index; DBP, diastolic blood pressure; IES‐2, Intuitive Eating Scale‐2; SBP, systolic blood pressure; SD, standard deviation.
Primary results
Intuitive eating and health indicators
Associations of IES‐2 total scores with SBP, DBP and fasting glucose – following the inclusion of covariates and BMI – were examined. Although there was a significant association between IE and DBP in Step 2 (β = −0.191, P = 0.003), IE showed a non‐significant relationship with BP levels (SBP β = −0.076, P = 0.256; DBP β = −0.122, P = 0.073) after the inclusion of BMI. IE was not significantly related to levels of fasting glucose (β = 0.047, P = 0.500). In line with these results, effect sizes of IES‐2 with health indicators were in the range of small effects or below (f 2 = 0.00 to 0.04). A detailed review of these results can be seen in Table 2.
Table 2.
Systolic blood pressure (n = 243) | Diastolic blood pressure (n = 243) | Fasting glucose (n = 212) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Step 1 | R 2 | ΔR 2 | ΔF | R 2 | ΔR 2 | ΔF | R 2 | ΔR 2 | ΔF |
0.210 | – | 15.808* | 0.138 | – | 9.493* | 0.242 | – | 16.527* | |
β | P | β | P | β | P | ||||
Age | 0.267 | <0.001* | 0.361 | <0.001* | 0.474 | <0.001* | |||
Sex | −0.379 | <0.001* | −0.101 | 0.104 | 0.085 | 0.168 | |||
Race/ethnicity | −0.060 | 0.312 | −0.002 | 0.980 | 0.059 | 0.348 | |||
Education | −0.108 | 0.079 | −0.104 | 0.106 | −0.109 | 0.090 | |||
Step 2 | R 2 | ΔR 2 | ΔF | R 2 | ΔR 2 | ΔF | R 2 | ΔR 2 | ΔF |
0.217 | 0.007 | 2.119 | 0.168 | 0.031 | 8.749* | 0.243 | 0.001 | 0.298 | |
β | P | β | P | β | P | ||||
Age | .237 | <0.001* | 0.297 | <0.001* | 0.487 | <0.001* | |||
Sex | −0.399 | <0.001* | −0.143 | 0.023* | 0.093 | 0.143 | |||
Race/ethnicity | −0.060 | 0.317 | 0.000 | 0.999 | 0.061 | 0.331 | |||
Education | −0.111 | 0.069 | −0.111 | 0.078 | −0.108 | 0.093 | |||
TOT IE | −0.091 | 0.147 | −0.191 | 0.003** | 0.036 | 0.585 | |||
Step 3 | R 2 | ΔR 2 | ΔF | R 2 | ΔR 2 | ΔF | R 2 | ΔR 2 | ΔF |
0.218 | 0.001 | 0.430 | 0.198 | 0.030 | 8.773* | 0.244 | 0.001 | 0.269 | |
β | P | β | P | β | P | ||||
Age | 0.216 | 0.002* | 0.201 | 0.005* | 0.468 | <0.001* | |||
Sex | −0.397 | <0.001* | −0.132 | 0.033* | 0.094 | 0.140 | |||
Race/ethnicity | −0.062 | 0.301 | −0.010 | 0.870 | 0.057 | 0.370 | |||
Education | −0.102 | 0.102 | −0.071 | 0.263 | −0.100 | 0.133 | |||
BMI | 0.048 | 0.512 | f 2 | 0.220 | 0.003* | f 2 | 0.041 | 0.605 | f 2 |
TOT IE | −0.076 | 0.256 | 0.00 (no effect) | −0.122 | 0.073 | 0.04 (small effect) | 0.047 | 0.500 | 0.00 (no effect) |
*Significant at P < 0.05 for omnibus statistics and covariates.
**Significant at P < 0.028 for primary outcomes (bolded).
BMI, body mass index; TOT IE, Intuitive Eating Scale‐2 total score.
Sensitivity analyses
In order to provide clinical information for certain thresholds of IE, additional analyses were performed. As observed by the t‐test, no differences in SBP between high and low IE were found, t(109.788) = 0.974, P = 0.332, Levene corrected. This was supported by the ancova, which controlled for the effects of BMI, sex, race/ethnicity and education, F(1, 116) = 0.015, P = 0.902, partial η 2 = 0.000. Significant differences in DBP between high IE and low IE were observed, t(111.651) = 3.602, P < 0.001, Levene corrected. However, after controlling for relevant covariates, the ancova revealed non‐significant effects, F(1, 116) = 0.330, P = 0.567, partial η 2 = 0.003. These contrasting results are pictured in Figure 1. Lastly, no differences in fasting glucose between high IE and low IE were found by the t‐test, t(103) = 1.302, P = 0.196, or the ancova, F(1, 98) = 2.414, P = 0.123, partial η 2 = 0.024.
Discussion
The objective of the current study was to observe the unique association of IE with physical health indicators, independent of weight status. Overall, the observed results suggest that IE does not have a unique cross‐sectional relationship with specific physical health indicators after adjusting for BMI. Specifically, no unique associations between IE and BP (i.e. SBP or DBP) or fasting glucose were observed. Although the relationship between IE and DBP was significant prior to considering BMI, the relationship did not persist after the inclusion of BMI into the model. This pattern suggests that differences in objective weight status likely explained the observed relationship between IE and DBP.
Further, additional analyses were performed to evaluate differences between high and low intuitive eaters. Once again, no differences in SBP or fasting glucose were observed between individuals displaying low and high levels of IE. Individuals high in IE did display lower levels of DBP, suggesting better resting BP than low intuitive eaters, but this was only the case when covariates were not considered. After controlling for BMI, sex, race/ethnicity and education, there were no differences in DBP between IE groups. In sum, it appears that significant, cross‐sectional IE–physical health relationships are only observed when other relevant factors (i.e. BMI and demographics) are not considered – at least for BP and fasting glucose health indicators.
These results are somewhat consistent with previous literature. While there is some evidence for the association of adaptive eating patterns with BP and fasting glucose, there are also multiple studies that have not found support for the IE–physical health relationship.11, 12, 34, 40 Of note, Hawks and colleagues performed a similar study in which they evaluated associations between IE and health in college women.22 That study found that IE was negatively correlated with BMI, triglycerides and cardiovascular risk and was positively correlated with HDL cholesterol. Further, significant differences were observed between high and low intuitive eaters on all of these indicators, with individuals high in IE displaying better health. However, Hawks et al. did not control for covariates or potential confounds, and they also measured IE with a different self‐report instrument than the one used in the present study. Although the current study did not evaluate the same indicators of physical health or the exact same IE scale, a similar pattern of results emerged. When using similar methodology to Hawks et al. (e.g. t‐test), a relationship between IE and health (i.e. DBP) was observed; however, the current study suggests this relationship is explained by the third variable of objective body weight and/or other demographic variables. The strong influence of relevant covariates on observed results is highlighted in Figure 1.
The overarching aim of this study was to increase knowledge of IE's basic and unique cross‐sectional relationships with physical health indicators, in order to serve as a foundation for continuing to refine longitudinal IE studies that could justify IE's utility as an alternative/adjunctive obesity treatment. Unfortunately, the results of this study did not support the presence of a cross‐sectional IE–physical health relationship that is independent of the effects of BMI. Additionally, the current study contained greater participant diversity in terms of age, race/ethnicity and weight status than many of the previous studies, which allowed for the observation that weight status and demographic characteristics are important potential confounds to consider in the IE–physical health relationship. Recent work has also suggested that BMI may serve as a moderator of IE–physical health associations, such that relationships between IE and body image function differently within individuals of different weight statuses.21 It is imperative for future studies to continue to work to disentangle the complex relationships between eating behaviours, body weight and health.
Given that some researchers and clinicians have called for a paradigm shift in obesity treatment, in which the IE/weight‐neutral approach is utilized over traditional BWL consisting of diet and exercise,8, 11, 17 the results of this study have a variety of clinical and research implications. We suggest that – in order for IE to be recommended as a viable physical health promotion tool among those with excess adiposity – it should improve other physical health indicators even if it does not lead to a decrease in weight.34 However, our study did not reveal that higher IE was associated with better health using basic biomarkers commonly associated with negative consequences of obesity. This finding, in conjunction with the absence of consistent support of a unique IE–physical health relationship in previous literature, does not support IE's potential as a stand‐alone obesity treatment if the goal is improvement in physical health indicators.
Despite the current findings, there are still multiple ways in which IE might serve as an effective health promotion/weight management technique. First, IE displayed a moderate‐large negative correlation with BMI in the present sample, which has previously been found in numerous studies.12, 34 This suggests that – even if IE does not help to improve physical health status independent of weight loss – having an intuitive approach to eating is associated with having a lower baseline weight. Therefore, IE may be well suited as an obesity prevention technique. Focusing on encouraging young people to attend to their body's physical needs over emotional, social or environmental cues for eating may help to decrease the incidence of obesity development. Future research on the utility of an IE intervention for obesity prevention is warranted. Second, IE may still have psychological benefit for individuals who have experienced psychological or behavioural detriments of dieting in the past (e.g. chronic dieting and disordered eating), particularly because IE is consistently linked to positive psychological indices (e.g. self‐esteem).7, 19 Thus, future research might investigate whether IE could serve as protective pre‐treatment to standard obesity treatments, especially for those with risk for eating pathology or history of unhealthy restricting. IE may help these individuals establish healthier relationships with food and foster positive feelings about the self, which may increase the ability to adhere to a healthful eating plan and to be successful at losing and maintaining weight. Lastly, IE may be able to play a role in improving the maintenance of weight loss. Many studies of IE‐based interventions have revealed an attenuation of weight gain within populations with overweight/obesity.12, 34 This finding, along with the previously mentioned psychological and behavioural benefits of IE, supports additional research on the impact of IE for individuals who have successfully improved their health via weight loss and are attempting to establish a sustainable pattern of adaptive eating.
In discussing the implications of these findings on IE utility, various limitations of the current study must be considered. Firstly, there are no validated clinical cut‐offs available to separate high and low intuitive eaters. Developing these cut‐offs may be a useful next step; however, it is not possible in this study because of the lack of a clinical sample. With the addition of these clinical cut‐offs, future research can investigate the psychological and physical consequences of being both high and low in IE. Additionally, based on the cross‐sectional study design, conclusions regarding causality or the study variables' relationships over time may not be made. Furthermore, as a self‐report instrument, the IES‐2 may be prone to some error because of difficulty accurately reporting typical eating behaviours or social desirability. Lastly, this study examined BMI as a covariate or confound in the IE–physical health relationship; future studies may consider alternative ways to conceptualize the role of BMI, such as a potential mediator between IE and physical health outcomes.
In brief summary, this study investigated baseline associations between IE and common indicators of physical health after controlling for objective weight status. Findings revealed no unique relationship between IE and physical health, and any IE–physical health relationships that were observed were accounted for BMI and/or demographic factors. These results display the importance of considering relevant contextual factors, such as objective weight status, in research on IE, and they do not support the recent motion for an IE‐based approach to obesity treatment if physical health indicators are the intervention targets. However, IE may be beneficial for health promotion in terms of obesity prevention, weight loss pre‐treatment to promote or protect psychological health and/or weight loss maintenance. Further, additional research is imperative to disentangle the complex relationships between IE, body weight and physical/mental health.
Funding
Dr Hawkins was supported by an Oklahoma Shared Clinical Translation Resources Pilot Grant from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health (U5GM104938) and by the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Disease (K23DK103941) during the conduct of this study.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declared no conflict of interest.
Keirns N. G., and Hawkins M. A. W. (2019) Intuitive eating, objective weight status and physical indicators of health, Obesity Science & Practice. 5:408–415. doi: 10.1002/osp4.359.
Clinical trial registry: NCT02786238; https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02786238
References
- 1. Jensen MD, Ryan DH, Apovian CM, et al. 2013 AHA/ACC/TOS guideline for the management of overweight and obesity in adults. Circulation 2014; 129: S102–S138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Barte J, Ter Bogt N, Bogers R, et al. Maintenance of weight loss after lifestyle interventions for overweight and obesity, a systematic review. Obes Rev 2010; 11: 899–906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Miller WC. How effective are traditional dietary and exercise interventions for weight loss? Med Sci Sports Exerc 1999; 31: 1129–1134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Fletcher SW, Buring JE, Goodman SN, et al. Methods for voluntary weight loss and control. NIH Technology Assessment Conference Panel. Ann Intern Med 1992; 116: 942–949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Goodrick GK, Poston WS, Foreyt JP. Methods for voluntary weight loss and control: update 1996. Nutrition 1996; 12: 672–676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Mann T, Tomiyama AJ, Westling E, Lew A‐M, Samuels B, Chatman J. Medicare's search for effective obesity treatments: diets are not the answer. Am Psychol 2007; 62: 220–233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Hawks SR, Madanat HN, Christley HS. Psychosocial associations of dietary restraint: implications for healthy weight promotion. Ecol Food Nutr 2008; 47: 450–483. [Google Scholar]
- 8. Bacon L, Aphramor L. Weight science: evaluating the evidence for a paradigm shift. Nutr J 2011; 10: 1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Bombak A. Obesity, health at every size, and public health policy. J Inf 2014; 104: e60–e67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Bacon L, Stern JS, Van Loan MD, Keim NL. Size acceptance and intuitive eating improve health for obese, female chronic dieters. J Am Diet Assoc 2005; 105: 929–936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Cadena‐Schlam L, López‐Guimerà G. Intuitive eating: an emerging approach to eating behavior. Nutr Hosp 2014; 31: 995–1002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Schaefer JT, Magnuson AB. A review of interventions that promote eating by internal cues. J Acad Nutr Diet 2014; 114: 734–760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Bacon L, Keim NL, Van Loan M, et al. Evaluating a “non‐diet” wellness intervention for improvement of metabolic fitness, psychological well‐being and eating and activity behaviors. Int J Obes (Lond) 2002; 26: 854–865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Mull A. The latest diet trend is not dieting. Health 2019; n/a: n/a–n/a. https://www.theatlantic.com/health/archive/2019/02/intuitive‐eating/583357/ Accessed 30 June, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 15. Tribole E, Resch E. Intuitive Eating: A Recovery Book for the Chronic Dieter: Rediscover the Pleasures of Eating and Rebuild Your Body Image . 1995.
- 16. Tylka TL, Kroon Van Diest AM. The Intuitive Eating Scale–2: item refinement and psychometric evaluation with college women and men. J Couns Psychol 2013; 60: 137–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Robison J. Health at every size: toward a new paradigm of weight and health. Medscape Gen Med 2005; 7: 13. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Hawkins MA, Colaizzi J, Gunstad J, et al. Cognitive and Self‐regulatory Mechanisms of Obesity Study (COSMOS): study protocol for a randomized controlled weight loss trial examining change in biomarkers, cognition, and self‐regulation across two behavioral treatments. Contemp Clin Trials 2018; 66: 20–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Bruce LJ, Ricciardelli LA. A systematic review of the psychosocial correlates of intuitive eating among adult women. Appetite 2016; 96: 454–472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Denny KN, Loth K, Eisenberg ME, Neumark‐Sztainer D. Intuitive eating in young adults. Who is doing it, and how is it related to disordered eating behaviors? Appetite 2013; 60: 13–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Keirns N. G., Hawkins M. A. (2019). The relationship between intuitive eating and body image is moderated by measured body mass index. Eating Behaviors, 33, 91–96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Hawks S, Madanat H, Hawks J, Harris A. The relationship between intuitive eating and health indicators among college women. J Health Educ 2005; 36: 331–336. [Google Scholar]
- 23. Madden CE, Leong SL, Gray A, Horwath CC. Eating in response to hunger and satiety signals is related to BMI in a nationwide sample of 1601 mid‐age New Zealand women. Public Health Nutr 2012; 15: 2272–2279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Schoenefeld SJ, Webb JB. Self‐compassion and intuitive eating in college women: examining the contributions of distress tolerance and body image acceptance and action. Eat Behav 2013; 14: 493–496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Smith T, Hawks SR. Intuitive eating, diet composition, and the meaning of food in healthy weight promotion. Am J Health Educ 2006; 37: 130–136. [Google Scholar]
- 26. Tanco S, Linden W, Earle T. Well‐being and morbid obesity in women: a controlled therapy evaluation. Int J Eat Disord 1998; 23: 325–339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Tylka TL, Calogero RM, Daníelsdóttir S. Is intuitive eating the same as flexible dietary control? Their links to each other and well‐being could provide an answer. Appetite 2015; 95: 166–175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Carroll S, Borkoles E, Polman R. Short‐term effects of a non‐dieting lifestyle intervention program on weight management, fitness, metabolic risk, and psychological well‐being in obese premenopausal females with the metabolic syndrome. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 2007; 32: 125–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Leblanc V, Provencher V, Bégin C, Corneau L, Tremblay A, Lemieux S. Impact of a Health‐At‐Every‐Size intervention on changes in dietary intakes and eating patterns in premenopausal overweight women: results of a randomized trial. Clin Nutr 2012; 31: 481–488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Mellin L, Croughan‐Minihane M, Dickey L. The solution method: 2‐year trends in weight, blood pressure, exercise, depression, and functioning of adults trained in development skills. J Am Diet Assoc 1997; 97: 1133–1138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Mensinger JL, Calogero RM, Stranges S, Tylka TL. A weight‐neutral versus weight‐loss approach for health promotion in women with high BMI: a randomized‐controlled trial. Appetite 2016; 105: 364–374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Rapoport L, Clark M, Wardle J. Evaluation of a modified cognitive‐behavioural programme for weight management. Int J Obes (Lond) 2000; 24: 1726–1737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Clifford D, Ozier A, Bundros J, Moore J, Kreiser A, Morris MN. Impact of non‐diet approaches on attitudes, behaviors, and health outcomes: a systematic review. J Nutr Educ Behav 2015; 47: 143–155. e141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Van Dyke N, Drinkwater EJ. Review article relationships between intuitive eating and health indicators: literature review. Public Health Nutr 2014; 17: 1757–1766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Hawley G, Horwath C, Gray A, et al. Sustainability of health and lifestyle improvements following a non‐dieting randomised trial in overweight women. Prev Med 2008; 47: 593–599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Ciliska D. Evaluation of two nondieting interventions for obese women. West J Nurs Res 1998; 20: 119–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Steinhardt MA, Bezner JR, Adams TB. Outcomes of a traditional weight control program and a nondiet alternative: a one‐year comparison. J Psychol 1999; 133: 495–513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G* Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 2007; 39: 175–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Utienbroek DG. SISA Binomial. DG Utienbroek: Southhampton, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 40. Sproesser G, Klusmann V, Ruby MB, et al. The positive eating scale: relationship with objective health parameters and validity in Germany, the USA and India. Psychol Health 2018; 33: 313–339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]