Table 2.
References | Type of intervention | No of subjects and trial period | Cognitive domain focused | Findings | Limitations |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
McAvinue et al. (2013) | •Computerized training task •Control group |
36 healthy older subjects Age range: 64–79 years old 5-week training period + a 6-month follow up |
•Short-term memory •Working memory |
•Improvement in short-term memory, together with transfer of training gains to long-term episode memory tasks •No significant improvement in working memory |
•A small sample size •Lack of inclusion of a measure of visuo-spatial short-term or working memory •Non-adaptive version of the training program for control group |
Yeo et al. (2018) | •Cognitive training system, BRAINMEM | 240 healthy participants Age range: 60–80 years old 24 sessions over 8 weeks and three-monthly booster sessions |
•Attention •Working memory •Delayed recall |
•No significant differences in overall cognitive performance post-intervention between subjects | •Lack of a sham control •Unbiased testing of effect sustainability of the training not done •Lack of generalizability |
Bozoki et al. (2013) | •Online games designed for the program •Active group only |
60 Healthy older subjects Age range: 60–80 years old 6 weeks |
•Visual attention •Working memory •Processing Speed •Reasoning |
•No effects, only improvements on games | •A small sample size; a short-term trial •No control group •Low program intensity |
Corbett et al. (2015) | •Problem-solving cognitive training (ReaCT) •General Cognitive Training (GCT) •A control treatment •Group |
2,192 healthy older subjects; Age mean: 65 years old 6 months |
•Reasoning •Problem solving •Attention •Memory •Visuospatial ability |
•Improved cognition, particularly the reasoning skills, evident from week 6 | •Only people who could access computer were included into the trial •Only people with higher levels of education; retention strategies need to be improved |
Rose et al. (2012) | •Virtual Week Training •Program •Active Control •Group (ACG) |
59 healthy older subjects Age mean: 67.4 years old 1 month 12 sessions, each 1 h long |
•Prospective memory | •Improved prospective memory •Transfer to real-world settings, reflected in participants' daily activities |
•A small sample size •A short-term trial period •A lack of effective strategies used by participants |
Nouchi et al. (2016) | •Processing Speed Training Game (PSTG) •Knowledge and Quiz Training Game (KQTG) •Active control group |
72 healthy older adults Age range: 60 years old or more 4 weeks |
•Processing speed •Reasoning •Short term memory •Working memory •Episodic memory |
•PSTG had a small improvement in processing speed, inhibition and depressive mood •No improved performance in reasoning, shifting, short term/working memory, and episodic memory |
•Short-term training period •No follow-up assessment •A small effect size |
Requena and Rebok (2019) | •Experimental control group •G1—Training with Lumosity •G2—Training with paper and pencil |
54 healthy older adults •Age range: 65 years and older 32 sessions held weekly during the months of October to May during the years 2015–2017 |
•Attention •Memory •Psychological well-being |
•No differences in the psychological well-being in either groups •Significant difference in attention, everyday memory and brain activity •CCT outperformed paper-and-pencil training |
•Difference in age and educational level |
Zhang et al. (2019) | •Multi-domain cognitive training via tablet | 27 older adults with MCI Age range: 55 years and above Twice a week/12 weeks |
•Reasoning •Memory •Visuospatial skills •Language •Calculation •Attention |
•Improvement in immediate memory and visuospatial memory abilities •No significant difference in neuropsychological test scores observed from baseline |
•A small sample size •Inadequate training duration •Lack of control group |
Barnes et al. (2009) | •Computer-based cognitive training (CCT) program developed by Posit Science Corporation (San Francisco, CA) | 47 subjects with mild cognitive impairment Age mean: 74 years old 100 min/day, 5 days/week for 6 weeks |
•Processing speed •Accuracy •Primary Memory •Working auditory memory |
•Primary outcome of global cognitive function between the intervention and control groups not statistically significant •Effect sizes for measures of verbal learning and memory consistently favored the intervention |
•Small sample size •Stimulating cognitive and physical lifestyle activities outside of intervention not controlled |
Marusic et al. (2016) | •Computerized spatial navigation training (CSNT) protocol •Experiment-control groups |
16 healthy men Age range: 55–65 years old 14 days training, 28-day recovery program |
•Executive function •Attention •Processing speed |
•Improved spatial navigation •Improved performance (fidelity), but visible also across other cognitive domains known to be associated with brain areas sub served by those that involve spatial navigation |
•A small sample size •A short duration |
Herrera et al. (2012) | •Programmed training exercises (visual recognition task) •Attention training task |
22 older adults with amnestic MCI Age range: 65–90 years old 12 weeks |
•Memory •Attention |
•Improved episodic memory •Transfer effect between recognition vs. recall •Attention training with the visual focused attentional tasks improved information processing |
•Parameters very frequently manipulated so that training tasks would continue to challenge each patient's abilities throughout training |
Gooding et al. (2015) | •Randomized clinical trial •Computerized Cognitive Training (CCT) •Cognitive Vitality Training (CVT) •An Active Control Group (ACG) |
96 male participants Age mean: ~76 years old 30 h of training/16-weeks |
•Memory •Attention •Executive Function |
•CVT showed significant improvement relative to ACG •No significant difference between participants of CCT and CVT |
•Restricted demographics sample •Did not include measures to assess everyday functioning |