Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2019 Nov 5;9:16073. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-52610-x

Fear expression is suppressed by tyrosine administration

Alessandro Soranzo 1, Luca Aquili 1,
PMCID: PMC6831598  PMID: 31690824

Abstract

Animal studies have demonstrated that catecholamines regulate several aspects of fear conditioning. In humans, however, pharmacological manipulations of the catecholaminergic system have been scarce, and their primary focus has been to interfering with catecholaminergic activity after fear acquisition or expression had taken place, using L-Dopa, primarily, as catecholaminergic precursor. Here, we sought to determine if putative increases in presynaptic dopamine and norepinephrine by tyrosine administered before conditioning could affect fear expression. Electrodermal activity (EDA) of 46 healthy participants (24 placebo, 22 tyrosine) was measured in an instructed fear task. Results showed that tyrosine abolished fear expression compared to placebo. Importantly, tyrosine did not affect EDA responses to the aversive stimulus (UCS) or alter participants’ mood. Therefore, the effect of tyrosine on fear expression cannot be attributed to these factors. Taken together, these findings provide evidence that the catecholaminergic system influences fear expression in humans.

Subject terms: Fear conditioning, Human behaviour

Introduction

Pavlovian fear conditioning is a valuable behavioural paradigm suitable for neurobiological and physiological analyses1 in both animals and humans. Typical fear conditioning protocols in animals consist of presenting a conditioned stimulus (CS) followed by an aversive stimulus (UCS), in a process described as fear acquisition. In humans, fear conditioning protocols can also be “instructed” where explicit instructions about CS-UCS contingencies are provided prior to testing. To distinguish between uninstructed and instructed protocols, some authors have suggested to refer to the former as measuring fear acquisition and to the latter as fear expression2, in that learning can already take place in the instruction phase. In the animal literature, however, the term fear expression is used to describe conditioned responses (CR) that have developed following fear acquisition when the CS is presented in the absence of the UCS.

Using the Pavlovian paradigm, a growing body of literature has indicated the catecholaminergic system as playing a significant role in the acquisition, expression and extinction of fear responses. Animal studies have demonstrated the involvement of dopaminergic D1, D2, D3 and D4 receptors in the amygdala in fear conditioning. D1 and D2 antagonists block the acquisition of fear memories37 whilst the administration of D1 or D2 agonists increase fear expression8,9. Blockade of D3 receptors has, surprisingly, shown to enhance fear acquisition10, whilst D4 activation potentiates acquisition11. In humans, the effects of a dopamine reuptake blocker (Ritalin) or a dopamine precursor (L-Dopa) have only been investigated with respect to fear extinction and spontaneous recovery12,13, but not fear acquisition.

Animal studies have also revealed an important role of norepinephrine (NE) in fear conditioning (for an extensive review, please see14). Administration of the non-selective β-adrenergic receptor antagonist propanol prior to training impairs fear acquisition15,16. Similarly, administration of the selective α2-adrenergic agonist dexmedetomidine (which decreases activity of NE neurons), weakened cued conditioning17. Genetic studies in which NE was reduced, enhanced, or eliminated as in knockout investigations have revealed a more complex picture with respect to the role of NE in fear acquisition1618.

Overall, despite some complex interaction effects, there is robust evidence to suggest that dopamine and norepinephrine are involved in the acquisition/expression of fear responses.

In humans, investigations of dopamine and norepinephrine have been scarce, and have largely focused on the study of extinction, extinction consolidation and reconsolidation processes1928, or in those with posttraumatic stress disorder29,30. In these studies, catecholaminergic activity was manipulated after fear conditioning had taken place. However, given the existing animal literature, catecholaminergic activity may also be important in regulating fear acquisition and/or expression. To understand whether fear expression is affected, one would need to manipulate dopamine and norepinephrine tone before conditioning. Moreover, previous studies were limited to testing the effects of L-Dopa as a catecholaminergic precursor.

In this research, we set out to answer: (i) whether alterations in catecholaminergic tone before conditioning would affect fear expression and (ii) whether a different catecholaminergic precursor to L-Dopa namely tyrosine would modulate this effect.

There are good theoretical reasons for the use of tyrosine rather than L-Dopa. The conversion mechanism of tyrosine to dopamine and norepinephrine is restricted to by competition from other endogenous amino acids and by the rate-limiting tyrosine-hydroxylase enzyme whilst L-Dopa is not31. As a result, these restrictions would limit the overall enhancement in dopamine and norepinephrine levels from tyrosine. This is important given that low or high doses of drugs that alter dopamine and norepinephrine levels enhance or impair fear conditioning respectively32,33.

Here, in a randomized, double-blind study, we tested the effects of tyrosine (placebo controlled) administered before fear conditioning. To test the experimental hypothesis that administration of tyrosine would enhance fear expression, skin conductance responses (SCR) were measured in 46 healthy volunteers while undertaking a fear conditioning task.

Results

SCR magnitudes in fear expression

Skin conductance responses (SCR) have been used to measure the effects of tyrosine and of placebo (cellulose) administration in a fear conditioning task. Drug administration occurred 60 minutes before fear conditioning. The task had a fear expression phase during which a conditioned stimulus (CS+) predicted the occurrence of an unconditioned stimulus (UCS: 75 db loud beep played through a pair of earphones) whereas another conditioned stimulus predicted the occurrence of a neutral event (CS−, which was a fixation dot, Fig. 3A). CS+ and CS− were paired six times. Responses were analysed on a trial by trial basis.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

(A) Schematic representation of fear conditioning task. Stimuli were presented in a pseudorandomized order. In brackets (x6 or x8) indicate the number of CS+ or CS− pairings during expression and extinction. Time is shown in seconds. (B) Summary of experimental procedure. Q = questionnaire to assess the double-blind efficacy of placebo/tyrosine administration. VAS = visual analogue scale. T1 = time 1. T2 = time 2. Time is shown in minutes.

A 2 × 2 × 6 factorial mixed analysis of variance was conducted on SCR magnitudes during the fear expression phase. The between group variable, drugs, having two levels (placebo, tyrosine) the first within subject variable, stimulus, having two levels (CS+, CS−), and the second within subject variable, trial, having six levels (trial 1–6). There was neither a significant main effect of drugs, F (1, 44) = 0.721, p = 0.400, η2 = 0.016, nor a main effect of stimuli, F (1, 44) = 1.678, p = 0.202, η2 = 0.037, nor a trial × drugs, F (5, 44) = 1.781, p = 0.118, η2 = 0.039, nor a trial × drugs × stimuli significant interaction, F (5, 44) = 0.664, p = 0.651, η2 = 0.015.

There was, however, a significant main effect of trial, F (1, 44) = 9.342, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.175, a significant trial × stimulus interaction, F (3.8, 167) = 3.719, p = 0.017, η2 = 0.067, and most importantly, a significant drugs × stimuli significant interaction, F (5, 44) = 4.306, p = 0.044, η2 = 0.089 (see Fig. 1A).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

(A) Line chart representing skin conductance responses measured in magnitudes across trials (1–6), based on SCR responses to CS+ and CS− in the placebo and tyrosine groups during the fear expression phase. (B) Bar chart highlighting the stimuli × drugs significant interaction. Vertical lines represent standard error of the mean. *p = <0.05, **p =  < 0.01, ***p = 0.001.

To break down this significant interaction, planned comparisons demonstrated that in the placebo group, there were significantly higher SCRs for CS+ than CS−, t (23) = 3.612, p = 0.001, d = 0.73, however this was not the case in the tyrosine group, t (23) = 0.427, p = 0.674, d = 0.13. When comparing SCRs for CS+ between the placebo and tyrosine group, significantly higher fear responses occurred for the placebo than tyrosine group, t (44) = 2.914, p = 0.006, d = 0.92, whilst no significant differences were reported for CS−, t (44) = 0.470, p = 0.641, d = 0.13 (see Fig. 1B). These findings therefore demonstrate that tyrosine impairs fear expression.

Control measures: SCR magnitudes during UCS

We checked whether tyrosine effects on fear conditioning could be ascribed to an association between CS+ and UCS, as opposed to unspecific systemic effects on UCS alone. We therefore conducted a drugs × time mixed ANOVA of the responses to the UCS.

There was a significant main effect of time (F (1, 44) = 16.89, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.277). This indicates that SCRs to UCS decreased over time regardless of the drug (see Fig. 2A). Importantly, there was neither a main effect of drugs (F (1, 44) = 0.001, p = 0.985, η2 = 0.000) nor a time × drugs significant interaction (F (1, 44) = 0.632, p = 0.431, η2 = 0.014).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

(A) Line chart representing skin conductance responses to UCS measured in magnitudes during early and late trials in the placebo and tyrosine groups. (B) Fear ratings to CS+/CS− measured using a Visual Analogue Scale during expression in a second experiment.

Control measures: mood, double blinding efficacy and fear ratings

Transient changes in mood state have been demonstrated to influence fear learning expression34. We therefore administered a visual analogue scale (VAS) questionnaire before and after drug administration (but before fear conditioning: see Fig. 3B). We also checked whether the double blinding procedure of tyrosine/placebo administration had been effective.

There was neither a significant main effect of drugs on mood scores (F (1, 44) = 0.004, p = 0.951) nor a drugs × time interaction on mood scores (F (1, 44) = 1.54, p = 0.220). The probability of participants guessing the correct drug (placebo or tyrosine) was below chance at 39% (42% for placebo and 36% for tyrosine).

In a separate control experiment, fear/valence ratings to CSs (CS+ and CS−) were evaluated using an additional computerized VAS with values ranging from 0 (no fear/stress/tension) to 100 (maximal fear/stress/tension) (see Procedures), as per administered by Haaker et al.35.

There was neither a significant main effect of block on VAS fear ratings (F (1, 17) = 3.73, p = 0.070), nor a block × stimuli significant interaction (F (1, 17) = 1.332, p = 0.264). Importantly, there was a significant main effect of stimuli (F (1, 17) = 417.4, p =  < 0.001), demonstrating robust conditioning effects to the CS+ (Fig. 2B).

In a second control experiment, participants (N = 21) were asked to rate the perceived aversiveness of the UCS based on three dimensions, unpleasantness, intensity, and how startled they were (see Procedures), as per previously published protocols36. Participants rated the UCS as unpleasant (M = 76.2, SD = 6.1), intense (M = 68.4, SD = 6.6), and were moderately to strongly startled (M = 74.2, SD = 8.2), demonstrating the overall perceived aversiveness of the UCS (Fig. S1).

SCR magnitudes in extinction

As tyrosine blocked fear expression, we cannot interpret its effects in extinction and we report these results only for completeness in the supplementary file, together with its Figure. (S2).

Discussion

Numerous studies over the last decade have demonstrated an involvement of dopamine and norepinephrine in both fear acquisition, expression and extinction. Most of these investigations have been carried out in rodents. In humans, work has been limited to manipulations of catecholaminergic activity post fear acquisition learning and hence focusing on extinction, extinction consolidation and reconsolidation processes only.

This work provides novel evidence that a catecholaminergic precursor administered before conditioning abolishes fear expression. Specifically, augmentation of putative dopamine and norepinephrine levels by tyrosine administration rendered SCR responses to CSs in expression indistinguishable from one another (i.e. SCRs magnitudes to CS+ and CS− were approximately equal and cancelled each other out: see Fig. 1A,B). Moreover, SCRs to CS+ were greater in the placebo group than in those administered tyrosine. Importantly, tyrosine administration did not alter processing of UCS information (see Fig. 2A) demonstrating that tyrosine selectively weakened CS+ UCS associations. Moreover, there were no unspecific systemic changes of tyrosine administration on measures of mood and alertness, which may have affected fear expression.

It is worthwhile discussing the learning curve observed in the fear instructed paradigm. As can be seen in Fig. 1, fear responses to CSs decreased over time. Although this pattern is common in human studies20,3739, it is the opposite in animals’ studies4,8, in which fear responses increase over time.

There are a number of potential factors that can account for this phenomenon. These factors include the type of CS used, UCS identity and intensity, the length of inter-trial and inter-stimulus interval, the reinforcement rate, the trial number and order, the CS/UCS duration, the type of instructions (e.g. explicit CS-UCS contingency or not), whether the UCS was experienced prior to testing (i.e. UCS calibration), and variations in acquisition procedures (e.g. single-cue vs differential protocols; multiple-cue protocols) (for a more thorough review, see23).

Our findings that tyrosine abolished fear expression are important and surprising given that in the animal literature, increased fear acquisition and expression has been reported when a dopaminergic D1 agonist was administered8,9 or abolished when D1/D2 receptors were blocked36. Similar results have been reported with respect to manipulations of norepinephrine1417. These contrasting findings may partially be explained by methodological differences in manipulating dopaminergic and norepinephrine activity postsynaptically in animal studies and presynaptically in the current study. It is also plausible, though speculative at present that tyrosine administration may have interfered with the process of prediction error signalling. The strength of this signal would have been greatest in early trials, when the occurrence of the UCS is most surprising40 and learning (of the CS-UCS pairing) is most likely to occur41,42. Midbrain dopaminergic neurons have long been known to convey a prediction error signal for rewards43,44, but more recent evidence also demonstrate a role for aversive events4547. Nevertheless, because we employed an instructed fear conditioning protocol, the UCS presentation in the early trials should not have been very surprising, given that participants had been made aware of what to expect. Therefore, it is perhaps more plausible to suggest that differences between the animal literature and our current finding, relate to the use of an instructed fear paradigm. Previous research, in humans, has suggested that uninstructed fear conditioning paradigms (as in most animal studies) recruit a differential neuronal circuitry to that of instructed fear studies48.

With respect to human studies, our findings cannot directly be compared to previous investigations, especially those that manipulated dopaminergic activity after fear acquisition had taken place and investigated extinction and extinction consolidation. Onur et al. manipulated norepinephrine levels before conditioning. In their study, administration of the NE reuptake inhibitor reboxetine induced an amygdala response bias towards fear signals49. Similarly, Visser et al. reported a rise in salivary-amylase levels (a marker of markers of noradrenergic activation) prior to fear conditioning which correlated with fear consolidation expression50. Taken together, in humans, increasing catecholamines (using tyrosine) before fear conditioning impairs fear expression whilst pharmacological augmentation of norepinephrine before conditioning enhance fear acquisition. In animals, targeted manipulations of both dopamine and norepinephrine that either increase or decrease their release before fear conditioning enhance and impair acquisition learning respectively.

The choice of administering tyrosine instead of L-Dopa as in past studies was motivated by a number of reasons. Firstly, conversion of tyrosine into dopamine (and other catecholamines) is restricted by the transporter shared with other amino acids and by the rate-limiting TH enzyme. L-Dopa, on the contrary, is not affected by these and its administration would result in greater concentrations being converted to catecholamines31. However, given the well-established inverted U relationship between dopamine concentration and performance51, it is foreseeable that L-Dopa would be more likely than tyrosine to shift participants at the right end of the curve. This would be particularly the case in individuals who are homozygous for the Met/Met allele on the Val(108/158)Met COMT polymorphism, and possess higher baseline dopaminergic activity52. Secondly, an additional advantage of administering tyrosine is that it produces fewer and less severe side effects than L-Dopa (e.g. nausea, insomnia, psychosis)53.

As tyrosine is the precursor for dopamine, norepinephrine (noradrenaline) and epinephrine it can be concluded that these neurotransmitters are involved in fear expression. Finally, it is worth considering the implications of our data in the context of the pathogenesis of anxiety disorders. Two large meta-analyses54,55, looked at fear conditioned responses of healthy and anxiety patients to CS+, CS− and the conditioned response (CR) difference between (CS+) - (CS−). Contrary to theories by Davies and colleagues56, who suggested that healthy individuals would produce higher CRs to (CS+) than (CS−) compared to anxious individuals in acquisition, the data from the meta-analyses demonstrated that this was not the case. Similarly, the meta-analyses found no significant differences in responding to CS+ between healthy and anxious patients. Importantly, it was found that responding to the conditioned safety cue (CS−) was higher in anxious patients compared to healthy controls. These results suggest that any pharmacological agent with anxiolytic properties should reduce conditioned responses to CS− in anxious patients to match those of controls, and that in healthy participants the pharmacological agent would reduce even further conditioned responses to the safety cue compared to a placebo group.

Based on our data, we conclude that tyrosine did not have such anxiolytic properties, but rather, that impaired fear expression responses.

Limitations and future directions

Although we established that catecholamines contributed to fear expression, our study did not set out the reveal the neuronal pathways that would have determined this effect. Therefore, future investigations using imaging techniques (e.g. PET) would be required to understand how changes in dopamine/norepinephrine in brain regions of interest regulate fear expression (e.g. amygdala-prefrontal cortex). Furthermore, to obtain greater specificity with respect to dopamine for example, one would need to co-administer a noradrenergic blocker such as Clonidine together with tyrosine/L-Dopa, or target D1/D2 receptors. More crudely, reducing catecholaminergic neurotransmission using the acute phenylalanine/tyrosine procedure57 before conditioning, would provide a potential counter test for the effects of manipulating catecholamines on fear expression.

In conclusion, our results provide evidence that manipulating catecholaminergic tone before conditioning in humans can suppress fear expression.

Method

Participants

Participants consisted of 46 university students (M = 20.4, SD = 1.7; 25 females and 21 males). The study was approved by the ethics committee of Sheffield Hallam University (SHU) and complied with the Declaration of Helsinki. All study methods were performed in accordance with SHU guidelines. Informed consent was obtained for all participants before testing could take place. Exclusion criteria included: those suffering from cardiac, hepatic, renal and neurological disorders and individuals with a history of alcohol or drug addiction, or psychiatric illness. Individuals having a history of taking tyrosine supplements were also excluded.

Drug administration

Participants received either 2.0 g of l-Tyrosine (supplied by BulkPowders Ltd.) or 2.0 g of the placebo microcrystalline cellulose (Redwells Creative Limited,UK) dissolved in 400 ml of orange juice as per previously published protocols58,59. Dosages greater than 2.0 grams have been shown not to provide additional benefits given that the rate-limiting tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) enzyme is already close to saturation under normal circumstances60. Peak plasma tyrosine levels have been reported to occur between 60 and 120 minutes post-ingestion61. In rats, increases in prefrontal dopamine concentrations can be observed 60 minutes following tyrosine administration62.

Electrodermal activity recording

Physiological recording were obtained using a BIOPAC MP150 data acquisition unit (Biopac Systems Inc., Santa Barbara, CA), and AcqKnowledge 4.4 software. Electrodermal activity (EDA) data were collected at 2000 samples/sec. Two disposable latex-free electrodes (EL507, BIOPAC) containing isotonic gel were attached to the distal phalanges of the index and middle fingers of the non-dominant hand. The EDA signal was then enhanced by a wireless BioNomadix ® amplifier. Skin conductance responses (SCR) threshold was set at 0.01 microSiemens (µS) with SCR onsets and peaks (i.e. a measure of change in tonic EDA) counted as being event related if occurring between 1–4.5 seconds following stimulus (CS+/CS−/UCS) onset. Data transformation of SCR magnitudes was applied using the square root (√SCR) as in previously published protocols63. Participants who did not produce a measurable conditioned response (i.e. classified as CS+ = SCR amplitude not reaching the 0.01 threshold across the whole conditioning session) (8/56), a similar criterion to previous reports64, were discarded from the analyses as those with poor electrode contact/motion artefacts (2/56), giving us a remaining sample of 46 volunteers. Of those 46, 24 participants were in the placebo condition and 22 in the tyrosine. To confirm that the overall results were not dependent on the exclusion of these participants, we rerun these analyses excluding only those due to poor recording (2/56) and those who did not respond to the UCS (2/56) (i.e. not the outcome measure), giving us a remaining sample of 52 volunteers, 26 placebo and 26 tyrosine. These data (see Supplementary info) confirm the conclusions reached in the results section.

Fear instructed task

The conditioning task was programmed in OpenSesame65. Visual stimuli consisted of blue and yellow circles either predicting an aversive stimulus (CS+) or a neutral fixation dot (CS−). These were presented in a counterbalanced (i.e. half of the participants had the blue circle as CS+ and the other half had the yellow circle as the CS+) and randomized order (i.e. the CS+/CS− sequence varied for each participant). Before testing began, on screen instructions told participants which of the two CSs would be paired with the aversive stimulus (UCS) (as in “In this experiment, the following stimulus [image 1 shown] will be followed by a loud beep, whilst another stimulus [image 2 shown] will be followed by a neutral fixation dot [image 3 shown]. You are not required to perform any actions but to remain as still as possible throughout the duration of the experiment. The experiment will last approximately 8 minutes”) which consisted of a loud beep (75 db measured using an audiometer) lasting 1000 ms delivered through headphones. Moreover, participants were not exposed to the UCS prior to testing. In a previous pilot study, the UCS reliably elicited SCRs in over 90% of the trials.

The task was divided into an expression and an extinction phase. Each CS presentation lasted 5000 ms with an intertrial interval (ITI) of 10000 ms consisting of a fixation dot. During expression, there were 6 pairings of CS+ and UCS (as in CS+ for 5000 ms, followed by UCS for 1000 ms and fixation dot [ITI] for 10000 ms) and 6 of CS− (as in CS− for 5000 ms, followed by the same fixation dot for 10000 ms). The duration of the ITI was chosen based on previous reports19 and permitted recovery of the SCR. In the extinction phase, both stimuli were unpaired (CS+ and CS− followed by the fixation dot) and presented 8 times each. The task lasted approximately 8 minutes. A schematic illustration of the task is shown in Fig. 3A.

Control measures: mood, double-blinding efficacy and fear ratings

We checked for the potential mood effects induced by tyrosine intake by administering a computerized adaptation of the visual analog scale (VAS) which was programmed and run in PEBL66, and has previously been used by our research group6769. The scale consists of seven dimensions (e.g. boredom, sadness, relaxation, happiness, stress, alertness, calmness) of mood/alertness. The double-blinding efficacy of placebo/tyrosine administration was checked by a questionnaire (i.e. “Please circle whether you think you received a tyrosine containing drink (experimental) or a placebo, non-tyrosine containing drink (control)”) given to participants at the end of the experiment.

Fear ratings for CS+/CS− were measured in a separate cohort of participants (n = 18) using a computerized VAS as per previous research groups35. Briefly, after the 3rd CS+/CS− pairing, and at the end of the last trial (trials 6 for both CS+ and CS−), participants rated each CS on a scale ranging from 0 (no fear/stress/tension) to 100 (maximal fear/stress/tension).

Additionally, we measured in a third cohort of participants (n = 21) their perceived aversiveness to UCS presentation using a similar approach to that of Hermans et al.36. Note that, as reviewed elsewhere2, there is no agreed consensus on the precise procedural approach to measuring aversiveness (i.e. the type of visual/verbal scale used) and which criterion constitutes the threshold for the UCS being perceived as aversive, with minimum (aversiveness) scores ranging from 5 out of 10 to 8 out of 10. Using a computerized VAS, participants rated the UCS for three characteristics; the first was the unpleasantness of the stimulus, with a score of 0 being described as pleasant, 50 neutral, and 100 unpleasant. The second was the intensity of the UCS, with 0 being classified as light, 50 intense and 100 intolerable. The third was how startled participants responded to the UCS, with 0 being labelled not at all, 50 moderately, and 100 very strongly. Participants were presented with the UCS six times, using similar timing intervals between each UCS as in the fear instructed study, but without CS+/CS−. After the last UCS, participants completed the VAS.

Procedure

This was a double-blind, placebo-controlled, mixed design experiment. Participants were required to attend a session lasting approximately 75 minutes. After screening for eligibility, participants were instructed to refrain from eating/drinking for a minimum of 3 hours. This is to reduce competition from other amino acids that share the same transporter70. They first signed a consent form followed by the mood questionnaire (VAS; time 1), and were then randomly assigned to receive either tyrosine or placebo. Participants were then asked to rest for 50 minutes following tyrosine/placebo intake, at the end of which, EDA electrodes were attached to the participant’s index and middle finger. At 59 minutes (post tyrosine/placebo consumption), the VAS was completed for a second time (time 2). At 60 minutes, fear conditioning and EDA recording began. Participants were instructed to remain as still as possible during testing to reduce motion artefacts. Following this, participants completed the tyrosine/placebo double-blind questionnaire, and were debriefed (see Fig. 3B).

Statistical analyses

Sample size was estimated using the following parameters: power 0.8, alpha 0.05, mixed design ANOVA containing 2 groups, 2 repeated measurements and a large f2 effect size of 0.4 (G*Power 3.1.9.2, Germany). The size of the effect size was based on the results of previous reports which looked at fear conditioning responses (SCR) using another catecholaminergic precursor (l-DOPA)20.

We performed a number of analyses on SCR measurements. First, we calculated expression responses on trial by trial basis (i.e. across the 6 CS+ and CS− pairings). The following variables were considered: Drugs (placebo and tyrosine), Trials (trial 1 to trial 6) and CS identity/stimuli (CS+, CS−). SCRs were measured 1–4.5 seconds following stimulus onset (CS+/CS−) which terminated after 5 second and hence did not include the UCS (which was presented between 5–6 seconds following CS+ onset), as in previous reports22,64, to isolate acquisition effects specific to the CS + and not confounded by the UCS.

Second, we run a Drug (placebo and tyrosine) × Time (early trials, and late trials) ANOVA of the responses to the UCS, here also measuring SCRs to UCS in the 1–4.5 seconds following stimulus (UCS) onset. This type of analysis has been done by other research groups71, and allows to capture UCS encoding in acquisition.

Supplementary information

Supplementary file (83.6KB, docx)

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Joseph Bretten, Ellie Holah, Lucy Longstaff, Michael Mcniffe, Adam Northall, Lorna Wooldridge for helping out to collect the data. We would like to thank Martin Thirkettle for helpful comments on the manuscript.

Author contributions

A.S. performed the statistical analysis of the data; wrote the manuscript; L.A. designed the experiments, performed the statistical analysis of the data, and wrote the manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-019-52610-x.

References

  • 1.Johansen JP, Cain CK, Ostroff LE, LeDoux JE. Molecular mechanisms of fear learning and memory. Cell. 2011;147:509–524. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.10.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lonsdorf TB, et al. Don’t fear ‘fear conditioning’: Methodological considerations for the design and analysis of studies on human fear acquisition, extinction, and return of fear. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;77:247–285. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.de Souza Caetano KA, de Oliveira AR, Brandao ML. Dopamine D2 receptors modulate the expression of contextual conditioned fear: role of the ventral tegmental area and the basolateral amygdala. Behavioural pharmacology. 2013;24:264–274. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e32836356c4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Guarraci FA, Frohardt RJ, Falls WA, Kapp BS. The effects of intra-amygdaloid infusions of a D2 dopamine receptor antagonist on Pavlovian fear conditioning. Behavioral neuroscience. 2000;114:647. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.114.3.647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Heath FC, et al. Dopamine D1-like receptor signalling in the hippocampus and amygdala modulates the acquisition of contextual fear conditioning. Psychopharmacology. 2015;232:2619–2629. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-3897-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Nader K, LeDoux J. Inhibition of the mesoamygdala dopaminergic pathway impairs the retrieval of conditioned fear associations. Behavioral neuroscience. 1999;113:891. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.113.5.891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Stubbendorff, C., Hale, E., Cassaday, H. J., Bast, T. & Stevenson, C. W. Dopamine D1-like receptors in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex regulate contextual fear conditioning. Psychopharmacology, 1–12 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 8.Guarraci FA, Frohardt RJ, Kapp BS. Amygdaloid D1 dopamine receptor involvement in Pavlovian fear conditioning. Brain research. 1999;827:28–40. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(99)01291-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Nasehi M, Hajian M, Ebrahimi-Ghiri M, Zarrindast M-R. Role of the basolateral amygdala dopamine receptors in arachidonylcyclopropylamide-induced fear learning deficits. Psychopharmacology. 2016;233:213–224. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4096-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Marzagalli R, et al. Genetic blockade of the dopamine D3 receptor enhances hippocampal expression of PACAP and receptors and alters their cortical distribution. Neuroscience. 2016;316:279–295. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.12.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Li JJ, et al. Fear memory recall potentiates opiate reward sensitivity through dissociable dopamine D1 versus D4 receptor-dependent memory mechanisms in the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:4543–4555. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3113-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Abraham AD, Cunningham CL, Lattal KM. Methylphenidate enhances extinction of contextual fear. Learning & Memory. 2012;19:67–72. doi: 10.1101/lm.024752.111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Whittle N, et al. Enhancing dopaminergic signaling and histone acetylation promotes long-term rescue of deficient fear extinction. Translational psychiatry. 2016;6:e974. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Giustino TF, Maren S. Noradrenergic modulation of fear conditioning and extinction. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience. 2018;12:43. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bush DE, Caparosa EM, Gekker A, LeDoux J. Beta-adrenergic receptors in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala contribute to the acquisition but not the consolidation of auditory fear conditioning. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience. 2010;4:154. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Díaz-Mataix L, et al. Characterization of the amplificatory effect of norepinephrine in the acquisition of Pavlovian threat associations. Learning & Memory. 2017;24:432–439. doi: 10.1101/lm.044412.116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Davies MF, et al. Activation of α 2 adrenergic receptors suppresses fear conditioning: expression of c-Fos and phosphorylated CREB in mouse amygdala. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:229. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Toth M, Ziegler M, Sun P, Gresack J, Risbrough V. Impaired conditioned fear response and startle reactivity in epinephrine deficient mice. Behavioural pharmacology. 2013;24:1. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e32835cf408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Haaker J, et al. Single dose of L-dopa makes extinction memories context-independent and prevents the return of fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110:E2428–E2436. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1303061110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Haaker J, Lonsdorf TB, Kalisch R. Effects of post-extinction l-DOPA administration on the spontaneous recovery and reinstatement of fear in a human fMRI study. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;25:1544–1555. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gerlicher A, Tüscher O, Kalisch R. Dopamine-dependent prefrontal reactivations explain long-term benefit of fear extinction. Nature communications. 2018;9:4294. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-06785-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Agren T, Furmark T, Eriksson E, Fredrikson M. Human fear reconsolidation and allelic differences in serotonergic and dopaminergic genes. Translational psychiatry. 2012;2:e76. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lonsdorf TB, et al. Genetic gating of human fear learning and extinction: possible implications for gene-environment interaction in anxiety disorder. Psychological science. 2009;20:198–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02280.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Raczka K, et al. Empirical support for an involvement of the mesostriatal dopamine system in human fear extinction. Translational psychiatry. 2011;1:e12. doi: 10.1038/tp.2011.10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kindt M, Soeter M, Vervliet B. Beyond extinction: erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Nature neuroscience. 2009;12:256. doi: 10.1038/nn.2271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Otis JM, Werner CT, Mueller D. Noradrenergic regulation of fear and drug-associated memory reconsolidation. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:793. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Sevenster D, Beckers T, Kindt M. Retrieval per se is not sufficient to trigger reconsolidation of human fear memory. Neurobiology of learning and memory. 2012;97:338–345. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Soeter M, Kindt M. Stimulation of the noradrenergic system during memory formation impairs extinction learning but not the disruption of reconsolidation. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1204. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Mithoefer MC, Wagner MT, Mithoefer AT, Jerome L, Doblin R. The safety and efficacy of±3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine-assisted psychotherapy in subjects with chronic, treatment-resistant posttraumatic stress disorder: the first randomized controlled pilot study. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2011;25:439–452. doi: 10.1177/0269881110378371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mithoefer MC, et al. Durability of improvement in post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms and absence of harmful effects or drug dependency after 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine-assisted psychotherapy: a prospective long-term follow-up study. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2013;27:28–39. doi: 10.1177/0269881112456611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Jongkees BJ, Hommel B, Kühn S, Colzato LS. Effect of tyrosine supplementation on clinical and healthy populations under stress or cognitive demands—A review. Journal of psychiatric research. 2015;70:50–57. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Carmack SA, Howell KK, Rasaei K, Reas ET, Anagnostaras SG. Animal model of methylphenidate’s long-term memory-enhancing effects. Learning & Memory. 2014;21:82–89. doi: 10.1101/lm.033613.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Holtzman-Assif O, Laurent V, Westbrook RF. Blockade of dopamine activity in the nucleus accumbens impairs learning extinction of conditioned fear. Learning & Memory. 2010;17:71–75. doi: 10.1101/lm.1668310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Zbozinek TD, Holmes EA, Craske MG. The effect of positive mood induction on reducing reinstatement fear: Relevance for long term outcomes of exposure therapy. Behaviour research and therapy. 2015;71:65–75. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2015.05.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Haaker J, Lonsdorf TB, Thanellou A, Kalisch R. Multimodal assessment of long-term memory recall and reinstatement in a combined cue and context fear conditioning and extinction paradigm in humans. PloS one. 2013;8:e76179. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hermans D, et al. Reinstatement of fear responses in human aversive conditioning. Behaviour research and therapy. 2005;43:533–551. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Ho Y, Lipp OV. Faster acquisition of conditioned fear to fear‐relevant than to nonfear‐relevant conditional stimuli. Psychophysiology. 2014;51:810–813. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Luck CC, et al. Temporal context cues in human fear conditioning: Unreinforced conditional stimuli can segment learning into distinct temporal contexts and drive fear responding. Behaviour research and therapy. 2018;108:10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2018.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Merz CJ, Hamacher-Dang TC, Wolf OT. Exposure to stress attenuates fear retrieval in healthy men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;41:89–96. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.McNally GP, Johansen JP, Blair HT. Placing prediction into the fear circuit. Trends in neurosciences. 2011;34:283–292. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Rescorla RA, Wagner AR. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. 1972;2:64–99. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Sutton RS, Barto AG. Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: expectation and prediction. Psychological review. 1981;88:135. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.88.2.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Schultz W, Dickinson A. Neuronal coding of prediction errors. Annual review of neuroscience. 2000;23:473–500. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Waelti P, Dickinson A, Schultz W. Dopamine responses comply with basic assumptions of formal learning theory. Nature. 2001;412:43. doi: 10.1038/35083500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Brischoux F, Chakraborty S, Brierley DI, Ungless MA. Phasic excitation of dopamine neurons in ventral VTA by noxious stimuli. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences. 2009;106:4894–4899. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811507106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Groessl, F. et al. Dorsal tegmental dopamine neurons gate associative learning of fear. Nature neuroscience1 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 47.Matsumoto M, Hikosaka O. Two types of dopamine neuron distinctly convey positive and negative motivational signals. Nature. 2009;459:837. doi: 10.1038/nature08028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Atlas LY, Doll BB, Li J, Daw ND, Phelps EA. Instructed knowledge shapes feedback-driven aversive learning in striatum and orbitofrontal cortex, but not the amygdala. Elife. 2016;5:e15192. doi: 10.7554/eLife.15192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Onur OA, et al. Noradrenergic enhancement of amygdala responses to fear. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience. 2009;4:119–126. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Visser RM, Kunze AE, Westhoff B, Scholte HS, Kindt M. Representational similarity analysis offers a preview of the noradrenergic modulation of long-term fear memory at the time of encoding. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;55:8–20. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Cools R, D’Esposito M. Inverted-U–shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biological psychiatry. 2011;69:e113–e125. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Lachman HM, et al. Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics. 1996;6:243–250. doi: 10.1097/00008571-199606000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Foster HD, Hoffer A. The two faces of L-DOPA: benefits and adverse side effects in the treatment of Encephalitis lethargica, Parkinson’s disease, multiple sclerosis and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Medical Hypotheses. 2004;62:177–181. doi: 10.1016/S0306-9877(03)00318-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Duits P, et al. Updated meta-analysis of classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders. Depression and anxiety. 2015;32:239–253. doi: 10.1002/da.22353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Lissek S, et al. Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Behaviour research and therapy. 2005;43:1391–1424. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Davis, M., Falls, W. A. & Gewirtz, J. In Contemporary issues in modeling psychopathology 113–141 (Springer, 2000).
  • 57.Hardman CA, Herbert VM, Brunstrom JM, Munafò MR, Rogers PJ. Dopamine and food reward: effects of acute tyrosine/phenylalanine depletion on appetite. Physiology & behavior. 2012;105:1202–1207. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.12.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Colzato L, Jongkees B, Sellaro R, Hommel B. Working memory reloaded: tyrosine repletes updating in the N-back task. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience. 2013;7:200. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Colzato LS, Jongkees BJ, Sellaro R, van den Wildenberg WPM, Hommel B. Eating to stop: Tyrosine supplementation enhances inhibitory control but not response execution. Neuropsychologia. 2014;62:398–402. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Brodnik Z, Bongiovanni R, Double M, Jaskiw GE. Increased tyrosine availability increases brain regional DOPA levels in vivo. Neurochemistry international. 2012;61:1001–1006. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2012.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Glaeser BS, Melamed E, Growdon JH, Wurtman RJ. Elevation of plasma tyrosine after a single oral dose of L-tyrosine. Life sciences. 1979;25:265–271. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(79)90294-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Tam S-Y, Elsworth J, Bradberry C, Roth R. Mesocortical dopamine neurons: high basal firing frequency predicts tyrosine dependence of dopamine synthesis. Journal of Neural Transmission/General Section JNT. 1990;81:97–110. doi: 10.1007/BF01245830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Morriss J, Christakou A, Van Reekum CM. Nothing is safe: Intolerance of uncertainty is associated with compromised fear extinction learning. Biological psychology. 2016;121:187–193. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Asthana, M. K. et al. Preventing the return of fear using reconsolidation update mechanisms depends on the met-allele of the brain derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology19 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 65.Mathôt S, Schreij D, Theeuwes J. OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior research methods. 2012;44:314–324. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Mueller ST, Piper BJ. The psychology experiment building language (PEBL) and PEBL test battery. Journal of neuroscience methods. 2014;222:250–259. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Riby LM, et al. Impulsiveness, postprandial blood glucose, and glucoregulation affect measures of behavioral flexibility. Nutrition Research. 2017;48:65–75. doi: 10.1016/j.nutres.2017.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Teik DOL, et al. Ginseng and ginkgo biloba effects on cognition as modulated by cardiovascular reactivity: a randomised trial. PloS one. 2016;11:e0150447. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0150447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Dennison O, Gao J, Lim LW, Stagg CJ, Aquili L. Catecholaminergic modulation of indices of cognitive flexibility: A pharmaco-tDCS study. Brain stimulation. 2019;12:290–295. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2018.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Fernstrom JD. Aromatic amino acids and monoamine synthesis in the central nervous system: influence of the diet. The Journal of Nutritional Biochemistry. 1990;1:508–517. doi: 10.1016/0955-2863(90)90033-H. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Cacciaglia R, et al. A risk variant for alcoholism in the NMDA receptor affects amygdala activity during fear conditioning in humans. Biological Psychology. 2013;94:74–81. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary file (83.6KB, docx)

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES