Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Nov 11.
Published in final edited form as: J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2018 Aug 29;57(11):828–836.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.07.872

Emotional Expressivity in Toddlers With Autism Spectrum Disorder

Suzanne Macari 1, Lauren DiNicola 1, Finola Kane-Grade 1, Emily Prince 1, Angelina Vernetti 1, Kelly Powell 1, Scuddy Fontenelle IV 1, Katarzyna Chawarska 1
PMCID: PMC6844292  NIHMSID: NIHMS1057771  PMID: 30392624

Abstract

Objective:

There is a prevailing notion that children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) exhibit intense negative and attenuated positive emotions, although the empirical evidence regarding their emotional expressiveness (EE) is limited. Given the importance of emotions in shaping social and cognitive development, we examined intensity and valence of EE and links between EE and autism severity and parent-reported temperament in ASD.

Method:

Toddlers (aged 21.2 months) with ASD (n = 43), developmental delay (DD, n = 16), and typical development (TD, n = 40) underwent standardized probes designed to induce anger, joy, and fear. Intensity of EE through facial and vocal channels were coded offline. Autism severity and temperament were quantified using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–2 (ADOS-2) and Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire (ECBQ).

Results:

The ASD group exhibited less intense fear compared to both the DD and TD groups, more intense anger than DD but not TD, with no differences in joy intensity. All groups showed similar levels of incongruous negative EE. Intensity of fear and anger were not associated with severity of autism symptoms, but lower intensity of joy was related to greater autism severity. Expressed fear and joy were associated with temperament.

Conclusion:

The study provides no support for a negative emotionality bias in ASD. Instead, toddlers with ASD display a muted response to threat and an accentuated response to goal blockage, whereas the ability to express positive emotions appears intact. Negative emotionality and social disability dimensions are independent. The study demonstrates the complexity of EE in ASD and motivates investigations into underlying mechanisms as well as its role in shaping complex phenotypes of affected children.

Keywords: autism, emotional expression, temperament, toddlers


Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is an early-onset complex neurodevelopmental disorder manifesting in social impairments and a range of rigid and repetitive behaviors.1 Since autism was first described in 1943, two notions concerning the emotional lives of children affected by the disorder have prevailed: one in which negative emotions dominate,2 and another in which emotional expressions are muted,3 particularly positively valenced emotions. Based on clinical observations, Tinbergen and Tinbergen argued that autism “…is characterized by an emotional imbalance, in which fear, anxiety or apprehension dominates and suppresses many forms of social and exploratory behavior.…”4 Despite the potential long-term impact that either a negative emotionality (NE) bias or an attenuation of positive emotions may have on social, emotional, and cognitive development,5 research in autism has focused primarily on how children with ASD discriminate and understand emotions of others,6-8 with relatively few studies focused on the question of how they themselves experience their world emotionally. When doing so, such studies have relied primarily on parental report measures, with few studies directly measuring intensity and valence of emotions in response to standardized response-induction procedures.

Studies using parental report of child temperament support the notion of both an NE bias and a reduced positive emotionality in children with ASD.9-11 Children with ASD reportedly exhibit higher levels of NE and lower levels of positively valenced emotions than typically developing (TD) children, from early toddler- and preschool-age9,12-14 to school-age.11 Studies that use direct measures of emotional valence and intensity in ASD typically evaluate emotional expressions across facial, vocal, or musculoskeletal channels with consideration of duration, peak intensity, or frequency of specific affective expressions. In retrospective home videos, infants later diagnosed with ASD were observed to engage in fewer social smiles than typically developing infants during the first year of life.15,16 In the context of play-based communication assessments, preschoolers with ASD did not differ in duration of positive and negative facial emotional displays from TD and DD controls.17 A more fine-grained analysis of these data revealed differences in the contextual modulation of positive affect: preschoolers with ASD spent less time compared to TD and DD controls displaying positive facial expressions toward adults during joint attention acts, but when making requests, they directed similar levels of positive affect.18 However, in the context of adult—child interactions, young children with ASD have been found to display reduced expression of positive affect compared to controls with idiopathic developmental delays (DD)19 or with Down syndrome.20

Only a few studies have used real-world probes aimed at eliciting specific emotions by means of standardized induction approaches, and these studies have focused primarily on examining negative emotions. Such studies typically involve probes based on the Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Lab-TAB).21,22 When preschoolers with ASD were presented with situations designed to elicit anger and frustration (eg, desirable toy placed in a locked box), both their intensity and duration of frustration expressed through facial and bodily channels were comparable to those observed in the TD groups.23 However, school-aged children with ASD responded to the removal of a desirable toy with increased frequency of negatively valenced facial expressions as well as more neutral and fewer positive expressions.24 When presented with fear-eliciting probes (stranger approach), preschoolers with ASD exhibited more intense facial fear compared to TD and Fragile X groups; however, there were no differences in intensity of musculoskeletal fear responses.25

Thus, the current literature reveals a rather limited and inconsistent picture of emotion expression (EE) of young children with ASD. The discrepancies between studies are likely driven by a host of methodological differences. Although highly valuable, parental report taps more broadly into emotions, mood, and attitudes across a wide array of situations and is likely to represent facets of the child’s temperament filtered through parental personality traits and dynamics of parent—child interactions. Furthermore, traits measured by parent-report scales diverge from the emotional dimensions measured using observational approaches, most notably in the domain of positive emotionality. Although pure positive EE such as smiling and laughter are represented in infant temperament scales, toddler measures capture more complex positive emotions such as eager anticipation of future events.9,12 The inconsistencies in studies using direct observation methods may stem from differences in induction techniques, intensity and valence of emotional triggers, their type (social/nonsocial), measurement methods, age of the participants, and characteristics of the control groups. The induction probes often involve social stimuli (videos of people or in vivo interaction), and thus it may be difficult to disambiguate the effects of affective impairments from social disability on EE of children with ASD. In addition, there is evidence that children with ASD may not necessarily differ in expressed intensity of emotions; rather, they may display emotions that are incongruent with the valence of real-world events.17,24,26 Improving our understanding of EE of children with ASD will advance knowledge of their motivational conditions and help clarify links between emotional and social development in autism. This knowledge will advance understanding of the heterogeneity of syndrome expression and of comorbid affective symptoms, and will inform treatment approaches.

The present study aims to address some of the gaps in knowledge of EE of toddlers with ASD. To this end, we examined their emotional responses to real-world scenarios with specific attention to their intensity, valence (joy, anger, fear), and congruence and compared their performance with that of TD and DD controls. To avoid confounding effects of social and affective vulnerabilities on EE in ASD, we strove to limit the social interaction load of the tasks. We operationalize the term “emotion” to signify observed behavioral responses to induction probes emitted through facial and vocal channels.27 The term “intensity” refers to the peak intensity of emotion expressed through these channels, and thus it reflects salience of the probes as perceived by the child rather than any absolute intensity of the probes.

Although, to date, studies have largely focused on the broader construct of negative emotionality by combining fear, anger, and other responses,8,10,12 considering that fear is related to withdrawal and anger is related to approach behaviors (eg, hitting, pushing), we considered them separately.28,29 Fear in young children arises in response to stimuli that convey threat or uncertainty, resulting in self-protection, and elevated fear responses in infancy predict later behavioral inhibition and anxiety.30 In comparison, anger reflects a child’s propensity to express negative emotions in response to restraint or goal blockage and serves the function of reinstating goals.31,32 Elevated anger in preschool predicts later externalizing behavioral problems.33 Similar to anger, joy is characterized by approach but manifests in smiling, laughter, and increased activity and is associated with later expressions of pleasure, sociability, activity level, and impulsivity.34,35

The present study examines the following predictions: (1) If EE of toddlers with ASD are indeed biased toward negative emotions, we expected that they would display higher intensity of anger and fear and lower intensity of joy in response to real-world induction challenges compared to TD and DD controls.9 (2) If toddlers with ASD are prone to expressing context-incongruent negative emotions, we expected that they would display higher intensity of fear and anger in response to positively valenced probes. (3) Consistent with our previous study,9 we hypothesized limited links between intensity of EE and severity of autism symptoms, which would suggest that the two dimensions are independent of one another. (4) Finally, as reported for TD children, we expected weak-to-moderate associations between the intensity of EE and analogous parent-reported temperamental characteristics.21,36

METHOD

Participants

Participants included 99 age-matched toddlers, including those with ASD (n = 43), those with DD (n = 16), and typically developing (TD) (n = 40) controls. The children took part in a prospective study of emotion processing development. Toddlers with DD and ASD were referred to a university-based clinic for diagnostic assessment by their parents, health care providers, or educators. TD toddlers were recruited through advertisements. The participants underwent a comprehensive diagnostic characterization examining developmental abilities using the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) and severity of autism symptoms using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–2 (ADOS-2) (Table 1). Diagnostic classification was based on clinical best estimate (CBE) diagnosis by expert clinicians. ASD diagnosis was based on the DSM-5 criteria.1 Toddlers in the DD group included those with global or specific (eg, language, communication, motor) delays. Children with history of prematurity or known genetic abnormalities were not included in the study. Research suggests that the diagnosis of ASD is highly stable between 2 and 3 to 4 years, with approximately 10% of children likely to shift from ASD to non-ASD diagnosis in preschool.37-39 This would translate to up to 4 cases in our sample, which is unlikely to compromise the obtained pattern of results. The toddlers did not differ in age (p = .204). The ASD group and DD group were comparable with regard to MSEL Visual Reception, Fine Motor, Receptive and Expressive Language mental ages (all p > .199), and their scores were significantly lower than those in the age-matched TD group (all p < .001). Social Affect and Restricted Repetitive Behavior Scores on the ADOS-2 were significantly higher in the ASD group than in the DD group (p < .001). There were significantly more males in the ASD group (88%) than in the TD (50%) and DD (48%) groups [χ2(2) = 17.42, p < .001] and thus sex was included in the model as a factor.

TABLE 1.

Characteristics of Study Sample

Characteristic ASD DD TD Contrast
N 43 16 40
% Male 88 48 50 ASD>DD=TD
Age, mo 21.9 (3.0) 20.3 (3.6) 20.8 (3.9) ASD=DD=TD
MSEL Visual Reception AE, mo 17.7 (4.9) 19.6 (5.9) 23.2 (5.5) ASD = DD<TD
MSEL Fine Motor AE, mo 18.3 (3.5) 19.3 (3.6) 22.1(3.5) ASD = DD<TD
MSEL Receptive Language AE, mo 11.8 (7.8) 15.3 (7.8) 23.6 (5.9) ASD = DD<TD
MSEL Expressive Language AE, mo 11.3 (6.2) 11.7 (4.6) 21.9 (6.2) ASD = DD<TD
ADOS-2 SA 13.8 (4.6) 7.0 (3.0) ASD>DD
ADOS-2 RRB 4.2 (1.8) 1.2 (0.9) ASD>DD

Note: Each cell displays mean (SD). ADOS = Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; AE = age equivalent; ASD = Autism Spectrum Disorder; DD = developmental delay; MSEL = Mullen Scales of Early Learning; RRB = restricted repetitive behavior; SA = social affect; TD = typical development.

Procedure

Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Lab-TAB).

To assess the intensity of emotional responses across facial and vocal modalities elicited by positively (joy) and negatively (anger and fear) valenced, we used a modified set of probes derived from the Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery—Locomotor Version.21,22 Each child was administered three emotion-eliciting conditions with three probes per condition: Anger (Car Seat, End of Line, Arm Restraint), Joy (Bubbles, Puppet Show, Penguin Race) and Fear (Spider, Masks, Dinosaur). The induction strategies involved objects (eg, mechanical toys) and activities (bubbles, puppets); none of the strategies involved social interaction or persons displaying emotions (please see Supplement 1, available online, for more detailed descriptions). Each probe consisted of one to three trials, administered in the set order listed above. In the Fear condition, we aimed to induce fear via encounters with novel, potentially threatening, and thereby negatively valenced objects. The Joy and Anger conditions sought to induce emotions from two ends of the positive–negative spectrum. In both conditions, an object or activity with a potentially high reward value either was attained (child had access to a desired activity; Joy condition) or was withdrawn (access was denied; Anger condition). To diminish carry-over effects from one probe to another, standard breaks were instituted between probes, and a new probe began only when the child returned to a neutral, unemotional state. The experiments were videotaped and subsequently coded offline for valence (anger, joy, fear) and peak intensity of emotion, thus capturing the tendency to respond to stimuli using extreme behavioral responses.40

Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire.

The Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire (ECBQ)41 is a validated parent-report measure of temperament for children aged 18 to 36 months. It consists of 18 scales targeting broadly defined Negative Affectivity, Surgency, and Effortful Control domains. Considering their relevance to the aims of the study, here we report on the results of three scales: ECBQ Fear (negative affect related to novelty, anticipated pain, distress, or threat), ECBQ Frustration (negative affect related to interruption of ongoing activity or goal blockage), and ECBQ Positive Anticipation (excitement about the anticipation of pleasurable activities).42

Data Reduction

The sessions were videotaped, and the peak intensity of emotional expression for fear, anger, and joy observed in facial and vocal channels was subsequently coded for each trial by coders blinded to the child’s diagnostic status. All coders had established reliability with a master coder (intraclass correlation coefficient [ICC] at or above 0.75) before starting to code. Facial expressions were coded based on the AFFEX system,43 which has been used successfully in studies of the stability of emotional traits44 and has shown high convergent validity with a microcoding system involving coding in discrete time intervals.45 Each trial was coded for valence and peak intensity of facial (0–3 scale) and vocal (0–5 scale) expressions (please see Supplement 1, available online, for more details). Parental compliance with task instructions was coded on a scale from 0 to 2 (0 = maintained neutrality with limited interference, 1 = some interference; soothing/eliciting behavior; and 2 = intrusive behavior that deemed the probe ineffective). All trials with a score of 2 were excluded from the analyses (3.75%). Consistent with prior research,46 intensity of valence-matched facial and vocal expressions were significantly intercorrelated in all groups, with correlations ranging from 0.44 to 0.77 (all p < .001). Thus, we computed composite raw scores for overall intensity of fear, anger, and joy responses across the facial and vocal channels for each trial. Raw peak intensity scores for all trials within each condition were then averaged into a mean intensity score for each emotion. To maintain reliability, 13% of the data files were double coded, with ICCs ranging from 0.72 to 0.91 for the 3 emotion composites.

Statistical Analysis

Within each condition, the hypotheses were tested using group (3) by sex (2) linear mixed models (LMM) analysis comparing the 3 groups on intensity of the emotions within each condition. Planned contrasts compared the ASD group to the DD and TD groups with the significance level for planned comparison set at α = .05.47 Associations between intensity of emotions and severity of social-affective symptoms based on ADOS-2 Social Affect (SA) scores as well as ECBQ Fear, Frustration, and Positive Anticipation scores in the ASD group were tested using Pearson r correlation analysis.

RESULTS

Intensity of Congruent Emotions

Three between-group LMM analyses on the intensity of the composite scores of congruent anger, fear, and joy intensity were conducted, with sex as a covariate.

Fear.

Neither an effect of sex (F1,89 = 0.01, p = .924), nor a diagnosis by sex interaction (F2,89 = 2.21, p = .116) was significant (Figure 1). However, there was a significant effect of diagnosis, (F2,89 = 3.77, p = .027). Planned contrasts revealed that toddlers with ASD expressed lower intensity of fear than TD (p = .009, d = −0.74) and DD (p = .041, d = −0.69) toddlers.

FIGURE 1. Peak Intensity (Marginal Means ± 1 SE) of Condition-Congruent Angry, Fearful, and Joyful Emotions During Anger, Fear, and Joy Conditions in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), Developmental Delay (DD), and Typical Development (TD) Groups.

FIGURE 1

Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, § = .137. Please note color figures are available online.

Anger.

There was a marginal main effect of diagnosis (F2,92 = 2.41, p = .095); planned contrasts revealed that toddlers with ASD expressed higher intensity of anger compared to DD (p = .032, d = 0.91) but not TD (p = .137) groups. A significant effect of sex (F1,192 = 4.62, p = .034) was found, driven by males who expressed higher intensity of anger than females. No diagnosis by sex interaction was found (F2,92 = 1.13, p = .327).

Joy.

There were no significant effects of diagnosis (F2,93 = 0.05, p = .951), sex (F1,93 = 0.03, p = .083), or diagnosis by sex interaction (F2,93 = 1.30, p = .278).

Intensity of Incongruent Negative Emotions

To evaluate whether toddlers with ASD are prone to expressing negative affect during positively valenced events, we conducted a diagnosis by sex LMM analysis on intensity of fear and anger during the Joy condition (Figure 2). The analyses revealed no effects of diagnosis on intensity of incongruous fear (F2,93 = 0.46, p = .636) or anger (F2,93 = 0.59, p = .555). Neither effect of sex nor sex by diagnosis interaction was significant (p values >.218).

FIGURE 2. Peak Intensity (Marginal Means (± 1 SE) of Incongruent Fearful and Angry Emotions During the Joy Condition in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), Developmental Delay (DD), and Typical Development (TD) Groups.

FIGURE 2

Note: Please note color figures are available online.

Intensity of EE and Phenotypic Features

There were no significant associations between the ADOS-2 SA score and intensity of anger [r(38) = 0.04, p = .795], or fear [r(36) = 0.01, p = .957] in the ASD group. However, there was a significant negative correlation between intensity of joy and ADOS-2 SA score [r(39) = −0.365, p = .024]. Intensity of congruent fearful expressions was positively correlated with the parent-reported ECBQ Fear scale score [r(36) = 0.449, p = .007], and intensity of congruent joyful expressions was associated with the ECBQ Positive Anticipation score [r(39) = 0.322, p = .048]. Intensity of congruent anger expressions was not associated with the ECBQ Frustration score [r(38) = 0.141, p = .405].

DISCUSSION

This is the first study to examine intensity and valence of emotional expression (EE) in toddlers with ASD. The results reveal a complex and surprising emotional landscape of very young children with ASD, a landscape that is inconsistent with the negative emotionality bias hypothesis and largely independent of autism symptom severity. The valence and intensity of emotional expression was strongly modulated by the valence of emotional triggers in all toddlers, but the toddlers with ASD diverged from non-ASD controls with respect to negative emotions, expressing less intense fear but greater anger than developmentally matched controls. The capacity for joyful expression appears to be preserved in toddlers with ASD, although the intensity of enjoyment expressed in such contexts was lower for those with higher autism symptom severity. There was no evidence to support increased intensity of incongruous negative emotions in our sample; it is possible that these become apparent later in development as the range and complexity of EE increases. Consistent with studies in non-ASD populations, effects of sex on emotional expression in ASD were rather modest and restricted primarily to greater intensity of anger in boys.48 In the ASD group, intensity of fear and joy exhibited in the laboratory were related to that exhibited in real life as reported by parents.

In response to fear-eliciting probes, toddlers with ASD showed attenuated intensity of fear compared to the two control groups. This is the first report of its kind, and the underlying mechanisms of attenuated fear response are not clear. One model of emotions posits that emotions arise from an interplay between networks involved in object recognition (sensory and somatosensory cortices), appraisal of the object value (orbitofrontal cortex [OFC], anterior cingulate cortex [ACC]), and regulation of physiological arousal (amygdala complex).49-52 Although it is plausible that the limited response to threat observed here was driven by impaired object recognition, we would expect a similar effect in other conditions as well, which was not the case. Instead, we hypothesize that the toddlers have a limited capacity to appraise the value of novel and potentially threatening stimuli, and consequently have difficulty differentiating between threatening and nonthreatening stimuli. The hypothesis is consistent with studies of classical fear conditioning in adults with ASD that suggest a limited ability to discriminate between threat and nonthreat cues as indexed by physiological53 and neuroimaging54 measures. Specifically, during fear acquisition, individuals with ASD showed attenuated autonomic arousal53 and decreased activation of the left and right amygdala.54 Preliminary analyses of the sample considered in the current study suggest, similarly, attenuated changes in autonomic arousal during the fear-inducing probes (Vernetti et al., unpublished data, August 2018) despite comparable attention to the threat stimuli in toddlers with ASD and the controls (Chawarska et al., unpublished data, August 2018). Clinically, attenuated response to potential threat may increase safety risks for toddlers with ASD. Considering the importance of threat processing to safety and the development of adaptive coping strategies and emotion regulation, more work is needed to decode factors contributing to atypical threat processing in ASD on affective, cognitive, and physiological levels. Given the ubiquity of affective problems in children and adolescents with, ASD55-57 understanding the possible role of these early unusual responses to threat in the emergence of comorbid conditions constitutes an important new area of inquiry.

When access to desirable objects or activities was denied in the Anger condition, toddlers with ASD exhibited increased intensity of anger compared to developmentally delayed peers. The probes involved restraint (either in a car seat or by a parent) and, in some cases, removal of a desirable object. Such events are common in early childhood and necessary to ensure safety. Although strong negative emotional responses in such contexts are more the norm than the exception in toddlerhood, these intense negative emotions may challenge the developing emotion regulation system in the early stages of ASD and compound already existing difficulties with communication of emotional states.

The intensity of anger and fear were not associated with severity of autism symptoms, suggesting that the negative emotionality dimensions are independent of the social disability dimension. These findings are incompatible with early accounts suggesting that emotional atypicalities are primary in ASD and contribute to the development of social deficits4,58,59 or that their emergence is secondary to impairments in, for example, face perception60 or theory of mind.61 The results, however, suggest that social and emotional abnormalities represent dissociable areas of vulnerability in ASD, and that their reciprocal influences may shape the development of complex and highly heterogeneous phenotypes in ASD from infancy onward. This view is consistent with recent reports showing a lack of association between negative emotionality traits and severity of autism symptoms in toddlers with ASD9 and in older children with ASD.62 The notion is also consistent with neurophysiological studies that suggest that emotional responses to threat and social deficits are not mediated by the same circuitry in the amygdala. Specifically, bilateral amygdala lesions result in reduced response to fear-inducing stimuli, as well as reduced reluctance to engage with a novel animal in adult monkeys63 and infant primates,64 but a preserved ability to decode and generate social gestures and initiate and receive affiliative social interactions.65

In the present study, the toddlers were passive recipients of joy-inducing activities and did not have to actively seek them. In such a context, toddlers with ASD expressed intensity of joy similar to that of the controls. Yet, clinical experience, parent report, and direct observational studies suggest lower rates of positive affect in real-world contexts.13,20,66 The discrepancy between results of the present study and observational studies and clinical reports may be related to a dissociation between the “liking” of objects or activities and the ability to pursue them. Specifically, the hedonic (liking) and motivation (wanting) circuitry of the reward system are at least partially distinct67,68 and can be differentially affected in various disorders,69 including autism.70,71 Whereas the hedonic component of the reward system appears to be spared in ASD, the motivational component appears to be altered.70-72 This may explain the observation that toddlers with ASD enjoy positively valenced objects and events, as shown in the current study, yet have difficulty pursuing them in the real world. The vast majority of joyful experiences in infancy and toddlerhood are mediated by adults, and access to them depends to a large extent on the child’s capacity to communicate his or her preferences and interests. Atypical development of intentional communication skills and social engagement, which are defining to autism and already apparent by the child’s first birthday,73-76 may put infants at risk for encountering fewer adult-mediated, positively valenced experiences, affecting neurodevelopment of reward circuitry in an experience-dependent manner. We hypothesize that although the capacity to feel joy may be intact during the early stages of ASD, the ability to seek out joyful experiences is hampered by poor social-communication skills in ASD, leading to the impression of less overall positive affect. Accordingly, joy was the only expressed emotion that was significantly associated with autism symptom severity in the current study: the greater the severity of social-affective symptoms, the lower the intensity of joyful expression. Notably, positive emotionality represents a largely neglected area of study in autism research. Harnessing the apparently intact capacity for joy in toddlers with ASD for therapeutic purposes will be essential, as activation of positive emotions motivates learning and exploration, the development of social bonds, and fosters activities that counter stress.77-79.

This study reports on emotional expressions at the earliest stages when ASD can be reliably diagnosed, leaving unanswered the question of when emotional abnormalities begin to manifest and what role they play in shaping the emerging phenotypes of children with ASD. Best-suited for addressing these questions are prospective studies of infants at risk for ASD. The probes presented in the current study were intentionally stripped of social content to limit the impact of social disability on emotional expression. Future research would benefit from direct examination of EE in response to social versus nonsocial challenges. There is also a need for better measurement of emotional expressions and vocalizations in real-world environments; however, the technology to support this type of research in young and atypically developing children is still under development. Because of the young age of the participants, the study relied primarily on behavioral displays of intensity and valence of emotions, which may not give full access to the internal experiences of our participants. Nonetheless, we report on emotional expressions before “display rules”80 come into effect, whereby children learn to voluntarily inhibit, produce, or alter their natural emotional expressions. Future studies will benefit from supplementing behavioral measures with measures of attention and physiological arousal.

In conclusion, we found no evidence for a negative emotionality bias, blunted positive affect, or incongruous negative emotional expressions in toddlers with ASD. Consistent with work in non-ASD populations,40 our results suggest that the broad dimension of proneness to distress (eg, “negative affectivity”) is less useful in capturing emotional phenotypes of toddlers with ASD compared with more fine-grained focal dimensions of fearfulness and anger-proneness.81 The discovery that toddlers with ASD exhibited a muted response to novel, intrusive stimuli is important, as it suggests atypical appraisal of threatening elements in the environment and provides motivation for examining its underlying cognitive, physiological, and neurodevelopmental mechanisms. The findings also provide motivation for an investigation into development of positive emotionality in ASD and its links with core social-communicative impairments in ASD. In particular, there is a pressing need to improve our understanding of the discrepancy between the capacity to experience and pursue activities that elicit joy and to use this information in the service of intervention. Moreover, the notion that young children with ASD characteristically present either with muted affect or with affect marked by a high degree of negativity may lead to lowering the detection rate among children who appear more affectively engaged, especially around activities that aim to elicit positive emotions. Our work on emotional expression adds a much-needed facet to our knowledge of emotion processing in young children with ASD, and may be consequential for identifying precursors to later comorbid affective problems so common in children and adolescents with ASD.82

Supplementary Material

1

Acknowledgments

The study was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health grants R01 MH111652 and R01 MH100182 (to Dr. Chawarska).

Dr. Chawarska served as the statistical expert for this research.

The authors thank Alexandra Dowd, BA, and Eugenia Gisin, BS, of the Yale School of Medicine, for their assistance in developing the probes and Lilli Flink, BA, Gabriella Greco, BS, Sharlene Lansiquot, BA, Emily Hilton, BA, and Perrine Heymann, BPhil, of the Yale School of Medicine, who coded the sessions included here. The authors are grateful to Karyn Bailey, MSW, Amy Giguere-Carney, MSW, Sophy Kim, PhD, Judah Koller, PsyD, Megan Lyons, MSW, CCC-SLP, and Elizabeth Schoen Simmons, MS, CCC-SLP, of the Yale School of Medicine, for their contributions to clinical characterization. For his guidance on the adaptation of the Lab-TAB, the authors thank Jeff Gagne, PhD, of the University of Texas at Arlington. The authors wish to express their deepest appreciation to the families and their children for their participation in this study.

Footnotes

Disclosure: Drs. Macari, Vernetti, Powell, Fontenelle, Chawarska, and Mss. DiNicola, Kane-Grade, and Prince report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM 5. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child. 1943;2(3):217–250. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders; revised (DSM-III-R). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Tinbergen E, Tinbergen N. The aetiology of childhood autism: a criticism of the Tinbergens’ theory: a rejoinder. Psychol Med. 1977;6:545–549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Eisenberg N. The core and correlates of affective social competence. Social Dev. 2001;10: 120–124. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Castelli F. Understanding emotions from standardized facial expressions in autism and normal development. Autism. 2005;9:428–449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Atkinson AP. Impaired recognition of emotions from body movements is associated with elevated motion coherence thresholds in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:3023–3029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Philip RCM, Whalley HC, Stanfield AC, et al. Deficits in facial, body movement and vocal emotional processing in autism spectrum disorders. Psychol Med. 2010;40:1919–1929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Macari SL, Koller J, Campbell DJ, Chawarska K. Temperamental markers in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58:819–828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Garon N, Bryson SE, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. Temperament and its relationship to autistic symptoms in a high-risk infant sib cohort. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37:59–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.De Pauw SSW, Mervielde I, Van Leeuwen KG, De Clercq BJ. How temperament and personality contribute to the maladjustment of children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41:196–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Clifford SM, Hudry K, Elsabbagh M, Charman T, Johnson MH. Temperament in the first 2 years of life in infants at high-risk for autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43:673–686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Capps L, Kasari C, Yirmiya N, Sigman M. Parental perception of emotional expressiveness in children with autism. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1993;61:475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Adamek L, Nichols S, Tetenbaum S, Ponzio CA, Carr EG. Individual temperament and problem behavior in children with autism spectrum disorders. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabil. 2011;26:173–183. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Adrien JL, Lenoir P, Martineau J, Perrot A, et al. Blind ratings of early symptoms of autism based upon family home movies. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1993;32: 617–626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Werner E, Dawson G, Osterling J, Dinno N. Brief report: Recognition of autism spectrum disorder before one year of age: a retrospective study based on home videotapes. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:157–162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Yirmiya N, Kasari C, Sigman M, Mundy P. Facial expressions of affect in autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1989;30:725–736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Kasari C, Sigman M, Mundy P, Yirmiya N. Affective sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic, and mentally retarded children. J Autism Dev Disord. 1990;20:87–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Snow ME, Hertzig ME, Shapiro T. Expression of emotion in young autistic children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1987;26:836–838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Joseph RM, Tager-Flusberg H. An investigation of attention and affect in children with autism and Down syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 1997;27:385–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gagne JR, Van Hulle CA, Aksan N, Essex MJ, Goldsmith H. Deriving childhood temperament measures from emotion-eliciting behavioral episodes: scale construction and initial validation. Psychol Assess. 2011;23:337–353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Goldsmith H, Rothbart MK. The Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery: Locomotor Version 1999.
  • 23.Jahromi LB, Meek SE, Ober-Reynolds S. Emotion regulation in the context of frustration in children with high functioning autism and their typical peers. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53:1250–1258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Costa AP, Steffgen G, Samson AC. Expressive incoherence and alexithymia in autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47:1659–1672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Scherr JF, Hogan AL, Hatton D, Roberts JE. Stranger fear and early risk for social anxiety in preschoolers with Fragile X syndrome contrasted to autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disorders. 2017;47:3741–3755. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Mcgee GG, Feldman RS, Chernin L. A comparison of emotional facial display by children with autism and typical preschoolers. J Early Interv. 1991;15:237–245. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Murray EA. The amygdala, reward and emotion. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:489–497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Campos JJ, Campos RG, Barrett KC. Emergent themes in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation. Dev Psychol. 1989;25:394. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Braungart-Rieker JM, Hill-Soderlund AL, Karrass J. Fear and anger reactivity trajectories from 4 to 16 months: the roles of temperament, regulation, and maternal sensitivity. Dev Psychology. 2010;46:791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Schmidt LA, Fox NA, Rubin KH, Sternberg EM. Behavioral and neuroendocrine responses in shy children. Dev Psychobiol. 1997;30:127–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Stenberg CR, Campos JJ. The development of anger expressions in infancy. Psychol Biol Approach Emot. 1990;247:82. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Goldsmith HH, Rothbart MK. Contemporary instruments for assessing early temperament by questionnaire and in the laboratory In: Strelau J, Angleitner A, eds. Explorations in Temperament: International Perspectives on Theory and Measurement. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1991:249–272. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Morris AS, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Terranova AM, Kithakye M. Concurrent and longitudinal links between children’s externalizing behavior in school and observed anger regulation in the mother–child dyad. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2010;32:48–56. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE. Temperament and personality: origins and outcomes. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2000;78:122–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Fox NA, Henderson HA, Rubin KH, Calkins SD, Schmidt LA. Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. Child Dev. 2001;72:1–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Gagne JR, Goldsmith H. A longitudinal analysis of anger and inhibitory control in twins from 12 to 36 months of age. Dev Sci. 2011;14:112–124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Guthrie W, Swineford LB, Nottke C, Wetherby AM. Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: stability and change in clinical diagnosis and symptom presentation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54:582–590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kim SH, Macari S, Koller J, Chawarska K. Examining the phenotypic heterogeneity of early autism spectrum disorder: subtypes and short-term outcomes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016;57:93–102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Chawarska K, Klin A, Paul R, Macari S, Volkmar F. A prospective study of toddlers with ASD: short-term diagnostic and cognitive outcomes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009; 50:1235–1245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Planalp EM, Van Hulle C, Gagne JR, Goldsmith HH. The Infant Version of the Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Lab-TAB): measurement properties and implications for concepts of temperament. Front Psychol. 2017;8:885–991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Putnam SP, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: the Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behav Dev. 2006;29:386–401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Putnam SP, Rothbart MK, Gartstein MA. Homotypic and heterotypic continuity of fine-grained temperament during infancy, toddlerhood, and early childhood. Infant Child Dev. 2008;17:387–405. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Izard CE, Huebner RR, Risser D, McGinnes GC, Dougherty LM. The young infant’s ability to produce discrete emotion expressions. Dev Psychol. 1980;16:140. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Durbin C, Hayden EP, Klein DN, Olino TM. Stability of laboratory-assessed temperamental emotionality traits from ages 3 to 7. Emotion. 2007;7:388–399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Durbin C, Klein DN, Hayden EP, Buckley ME, Moerk KC. Temperamental emotionality in preschoolers and parental mood disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005; 114:28–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kann SJ, O’Rawe JF, Huang AS, Klein DN, Leung H-C. Preschool negative emotionality predicts activity and connectivity of the fusiform face area and amygdala in later childhood. Social Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2017;12:1511–1519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Keppel G, Wickens T. Simultaneous Comparisons and the Control of Type I Errors. Design and Analysis: A Researcher’s Handbook. 4th ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall; 2004:111–130. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Chaplin TM, Aldao A. Gender differences in emotion expression in children: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. 2013;139:735–765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Lindquist KA, Wager TD, Kober H, Bliss-Moreau E, Barrett LF. The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Behav Brain Sci. 2012;35:121–143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET. Value, pleasure and choice in the ventral prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:56–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Barrett LF. Are emotions natural kinds? Persp Psychol Sci. 2006;1:28–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wilson-Mendenhall CD, Barrett LF, Barsalou LW. Neural evidence that human emotions share core affective properties. Psychol Sci. 2013;24:947–956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Gaigg SB, Bowler DM. Differential fear conditioning in Asperger’s syndrome: implications for an amygdala theory of autism. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2125–2134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Top DN Jr, Stephenson KG, Doxey CR, Crowley MJ, Kirwan CB, South M. Atypical amygdala response to fear conditioning in autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;1:308–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.van Steensel FJ, Bögels SM, Perrin S. Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2011;14: 302–317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Kerns CM, Kendall PC, Berry L, et al. Traditional and atypical presentations of anxiety in youth with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44: 2851–2861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Gadow KD, DeVincent CJ, Pomeroy J, Azizian A. Psychiatric symptoms in preschool children with PDD and clinic and comparison samples. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34: 379–393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Acta Paedopsychiatr. 1943/1968;35: 98–136. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Sigman M, Mundy P. Social attachments in autistic children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28:74–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Schultz RT. Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: the role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2005;23:125–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Bullmore ET, Wheelwright S, Ashwin C, Williams S. The amygdala theory of autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24:355–364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Herrington JD, Maddox BB, McVey AJ, et al. Negative valence in autism spectrum disorder: the relationship between amygdala activity, selective attention, and co-occurring anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;2:510–517. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Emery NJ, Capitanio JP, Mason WA, Machado CJ, Mendoza SP, Amaral DG. The effects of bilateral lesions of the amygdala on dyadic social interactions in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behav Neurosci. 2001;115:515–544. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Prather MD, Lavenex P, Mauldin-Jourdain ML, et al. Increased social fear and decreased fear of objects in monkeys with neonatal amygdala lesions. Neuroscience. 2001;106: 653–658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Emery NJ, Amaral DG. The role of the amygdala in primate social cognition In: Lane RD, Nadel L, eds. Cognitive Neuroscience of Emotion, Vol X New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2000:156–191. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Hobson RP, Lee A. Hello and goodbye: a study of social engagement in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 1998;28:117–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Miller EM, Shankar MU, Knutson B, McClure SM. Dissociating motivation from reward in human striatal activity. J Cogn Neurosci. 2014;26:1075–1084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Schultz W. Behavioral theories and the neurophysiology of reward. Annu Rev Psychol. 2006;57:87–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Der-Avakian A, Markou A. The neurobiology of anhedonia and other reward-related deficits. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35:68–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Kohls G, Schulte-Rüther M, Nehrkorn B, et al. Reward system dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Social Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2012;8:565–572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Dichter GS, Felder JN, Green SR, Rittenberg AM, Sasson NJ, Bodfish JW. Reward circuitry function in autism spectrum disorders. Social Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2012;7: 160–172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Wang Q, DiNicola L, Heymann P, Hampson M, Chawarska K. Impaired value learning for faces in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;57:33–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Macari SL, Wu GC, Powell KK, Fontenelle S, Macris DM, Chawarska K. Do parents and clinicians agree on ratings of autism-related behaviors at 12 months of age? A study of infants at high and low risk for ASD. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;48:1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Rowberry J, Macari S, Chen G, et al. Screening for autism spectrum disorders in 12-month-old high-risk siblings by parental report. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45: 221–229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Macari SL, Campbell D, Gengoux GW, Saulnier CA, Klin AJ, Chawarska K. Predicting developmental status from 12 to 24 months in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder: a preliminary report. J Autism Dev Disorders. 2012;42:2636–2647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Ozonoff S, Iosif A-M, Baguio F, et al. A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:256–266. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Izard CE, Woodburn EM, Finlon KJ, Krauthamer-Ewing ES, Grossman SR, Seidenfeld A. Emotion knowledge, emotion utilization, and emotion regulation. Emot Rev. 2011;3:44–52. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Mäntymaa M, Puura K, Luoma I, Latva R, Salmelin RK, Tamminen T. Shared pleasure in early mother–infant interaction: predicting lower levels of emotional and behavioral problems in the child and protecting against the influence of parental psychopathology. Infant Ment Health J. 2015;36:223–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Eisenberg N, Zhou Q, Spinrad TL, Valiente C, Fabes RA, Liew J. Relations among positive parenting, children’s effortful control, and externalizing problems: a three-wave longitudinal study. Child Dev. 2005;76:1055–1071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Ekman P, Friesen W. Unmasking the Face: A Guide to Recognizing Emotions From Facial Cues. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1975. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Buss KA, Goldsmith HH. Fear and anger regulation in infancy: Effects on the temporal dynamics of affective expression. Child development. 1998;69(2): 359–374. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Simonoff E, Pickles A, Charman T, Chandler S, Loucas T, Baird G. Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: Prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008;47(8):921–929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES