Origin and functions of intermittent transitions among sleep stages, including brief awakenings and arousals, constitute a challenge to the current homeostatic framework for sleep regulation, focusing on factors modulating sleep over large time scales. Here we propose that the complex micro-architecture characterizing sleep on scales of seconds and minutes results from intrinsic non-equilibrium critical dynamics.
Keywords: brain rhythms, cortical dynamics, criticality, sleep regulation, VLPO
Abstract
Origin and functions of intermittent transitions among sleep stages, including brief awakenings and arousals, constitute a challenge to the current homeostatic framework for sleep regulation, focusing on factors modulating sleep over large time scales. Here we propose that the complex micro-architecture characterizing sleep on scales of seconds and minutes results from intrinsic non-equilibrium critical dynamics. We investigate θ- and δ-wave dynamics in control rats and in rats where the sleep-promoting ventrolateral preoptic nucleus (VLPO) is lesioned (male Sprague-Dawley rats). We demonstrate that bursts in θ and δ cortical rhythms exhibit complex temporal organization, with long-range correlations and robust duality of power-law (θ-bursts, active phase) and exponential-like (δ-bursts, quiescent phase) duration distributions, features typical of non-equilibrium systems self-organizing at criticality. We show that such non-equilibrium behavior relates to anti-correlated coupling between θ- and δ-bursts, persists across a range of time scales, and is independent of the dominant physiologic state; indications of a basic principle in sleep regulation. Further, we find that VLPO lesions lead to a modulation of cortical dynamics resulting in altered dynamical parameters of θ- and δ-bursts and significant reduction in θ–δ coupling. Our empirical findings and model simulations demonstrate that θ–δ coupling is essential for the emerging non-equilibrium critical dynamics observed across the sleep–wake cycle, and indicate that VLPO neurons may have dual role for both sleep and arousal/brief wake activation. The uncovered critical behavior in sleep- and wake-related cortical rhythms indicates a mechanism essential for the micro-architecture of spontaneous sleep-stage and arousal transitions within a novel, non-homeostatic paradigm of sleep regulation.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT We show that the complex micro-architecture of sleep-stage/arousal transitions arises from intrinsic non-equilibrium critical dynamics, connecting the temporal organization of dominant cortical rhythms with empirical observations across scales. We link such behavior to sleep-promoting neuronal population, and demonstrate that VLPO lesion (model of insomnia) alters dynamical features of θ and δ rhythms, and leads to significant reduction in θ–δ coupling. This indicates that VLPO neurons may have dual role for both sleep and arousal/brief wake control. The reported empirical findings and modeling simulations constitute first evidences of a neurophysiological fingerprint of self-organization and criticality in sleep- and wake-related cortical rhythms; a mechanism essential for spontaneous sleep-stage and arousal transitions that lays the bases for a novel, non-homeostatic paradigm of sleep regulation.
Introduction
Sleep periods exhibit numerous transitions among sleep stages and short awakenings, with intermittent fluctuations within sleep stages that trigger micro-states and brief arousals (Halász, 1998; Hirshkowitz, 2002; Lo et al., 2002). Such behavior is typically observed in non-equilibrium systems characterized by multicomponent nonlinear feedback interactions and exhibiting critical behavior, with irregular alternation between active and quiescent phases (Bak, 1996; Chialvo, 2010; Munoz, 2018). This constitutes a challenge to the current homeostatic framework for sleep regulation, which considers sleep as an equilibrium process, and focuses on factors modulating sleep over large time scales, such as homeostatic sleep drive, sleep propensity and inertia, and ultradian and circadian rhythms (Borbély and Achermann, 1999; Saper et al., 2005; Brown et al., 2012). Existing homeostatic models, although successfully providing a good description of consolidated sleep and wakefulness over time scales of hours (Borbély and Achermann, 1999; Achermann and Borbély, 2003; Saper et al., 2001, 2010), do not capture the emergent complexity of transient and abrupt behaviors at scales of seconds and minutes (Lo et al., 2004; Olbrich et al., 2011), and do not account for the related dynamics of bursts in cortical rhythms.
The intrinsic fluctuations in cortical rhythmic activity in response to feedback interactions among sleep- and wake-promoting neuronal groups, and the corresponding complex temporal patterns of intermittent transitions in sleep micro-architecture, suggest that critical dynamics may underlie sleep regulation at small time scales, in parallel with the well established homeostatic behavior at large time scales. Such hypothesis is further motivated by recent work showing a peculiar coexistence of power-law and exponential probability distributions for the durations of brief awakenings/arousals and sleep bouts, in both humans and animal models (Lo et al., 2002, 2004; Blumberg et al., 2005; Behn et al., 2007; Dvir et al., 2018). This scenario is closely reminiscent of non-equilibrium systems self-tuning at criticality, where a quiescent phase with exponential dynamics coexists with an active phase characterized by bursts/avalanches with power-law distributed sizes and durations (Boffetta et al., 1999; Chialvo, 2010; Munoz, 2018). Because brief awakenings/arousals can be viewed as “active” states of the brain that interrupt the “inactive” phase represented by sleep periods, the coexistence of power-law distributed arousal durations with sleep bouts durations following exponential behavior has been interpreted as a fingerprint of criticality in sleep dynamics (Lo et al., 2004, 2013).
To test this hypothesis, we investigate the dynamics of dominant brain waves in the sleep–wake cycle of rats in relation to the neuronal circuitry responsible for wake and sleep control. In particular, we focus on the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus (VLPO), a key sleep promoting brain region (Sherin et al., 1996), and study whether alterations in the sleep–wake cycle are mirrored by a reorganization of dominant brain rhythms. The sleep–wake cycle of rats is largely dominated by the δ and θ rhythm. During non-rapid eye movement sleep (NREM) sleep, cortical activity is characterized by δ rhythm, low-frequency high-amplitude oscillations referred to as slow-wave activity (Steriade et al., 1993), whereas REM sleep and arousals/wake state are characterized by θ rhythm, desynchronized and localized oscillations of higher frequency and lower amplitude (Brown et al., 2012). Lesions of the central cluster of the VLPO lead to a large decrease in delta power and NREM sleep time, and a fragmentation of the sleep–wake cycle (Lu et al., 2000). REM sleep may also be affected by lesions in the VLPO area, especially in the medial and dorsal extended VLPO region (Vetrivelan et al., 2012). However, the influence of VLPO neurons on the dynamics and temporal organization of δ and θ waves during the sleep–wake cycle has not been investigated.
To this aim, we analyze long-term continuous EEG recordings in control rats and rats with lesions in the VLPO, and dissect emergent signatures of criticality in the dynamics of θ- and δ-bursts in relation to VLPO neuronal integrity. We link alterations of the sleep–wake cycle with non-equilibrium properties of the underlying dynamics, providing first evidence for a criticality-based framework of sleep regulation that unifies sleep stage and arousal transitions with basic dynamics of dominant cortical rhythms.
Materials and Methods
Experimental setup and statistical analysis
Twelve pathogen-free, 12- to 16-week-old male Sprague-Dawley rats (300–365 g; Harlan) were used for this study. Data analyzed here were selected from a previously presented study (Vetrivelan et al., 2012) with 7 control and 34 VLPO-lesioned rats. The VLPO-lesioned group used for the present study includes all animals with >85% cell loss in the VLPO; n = 7 of the entire cohort of 34 VLPO-lesioned rats. One control rat and one VLPO-lesioned rat were excluded from the final analysis after the preprocessing procedure. For the purpose of our study of sleep microdynamics, we selected and analyzed rats with <10% artifacts in the EEG (see Data preprocessing). The experimental procedure is briefly summarized below. For further details regarding data collection and protocol, please refer to Vetrivelan et al. (2012).
Rat surgery.
Animals were anesthetized with chloral hydrate (350 mg/kg body weight, i.p.) and the fur over the skull was shaved, and the skin was prepped with betadine and alcohol. The animals were then fixed in a stereotaxic frame. All surgery was conducted using sterile autoclaved instruments and under aseptic conditions. To produce VLPO lesions, orexin-B-saporin (OX-SAP; Advanced Targeting Systems) was injected into the VLPO as described below. Skin (∼1 cm) was incised and small burr holes were drilled in the skull corresponding to the VLPO coordinates. A glass micropipette (tip diameter 10–20 μm) was then lowered to coordinates (AP: −0.6 from bregma, L: 1.00, DV: 8.5) corresponding to the VLPO as per the rat atlas of Paxinos and Watson (2004) and OX-SAP (200 nl of 0.1% solution) was injected using a compressed air delivery system (Amaral and Price, 1983). The micropipette was then left in place for 5 min and slowly withdrawn. Control animals received saline (vehicle) injections into the VLPO. Following the injections, four EEG screw electrodes were implanted into the skull, in the frontal (2) and parietal bones (2) of each side, and two flexible EMG wire electrodes were placed into the neck muscles for the collection of sleep–wake data. Four burr holes were made in the skull (1 mm rostral and 3 mm lateral; 3 mm caudal and 3 mm lateral) and EEG electrodes were inserted into those burr holes so that they would be close to dura mater. EMG wire electrodes were placed onto the neck extensor muscles on either side. The free ends of the leads were soldered into a socket that was attached to the skull with dental cement, and the incision was then closed by wound clips (Lu et al., 2000, 2002).
EEG recording across dark and light and sleep–wake analysis.
EEG recordings from each rat were performed on Day 20 post-lesion using Grass polygraph. The rats were connected via flexible recording cables to a commutator, which in turn was connected to a Grass polygraph and computer. The rats were habituated to the connecting cables and the recording room conditions for 2 d and then uninterrupted recordings of the EEG/EMG (sampling rate 256 Hz) and time-lock video were conducted for 48 h (2 d, 12 h light/dark) beginning of Day 20 postlesion. EEG signals were recorded continuously from the frontal and parietal electrode on both left and right hemisphere. The parietal electrode picks up θ rhythm from the hippocampus during REM sleep; both the frontal and parietal electrodes pick up θ rhythm during wakefulness. Hippocampal θ rhythm can be also present during wakefulness, mainly during locomotion and cognitive wakefulness. The signal analyzed in this study is the difference between frontal and parietal EEG electrode potentials (frontal–parietal EEG) from one hemisphere (ipsilateral). The EEG/EMG data of each rat was divided into 12 s epochs and visually scored as wake, NREM sleep, or rapid eye movement (REM) sleep using scoring criteria previously described (Lu et al., 2000). Wakefulness was identified by the presence of desynchronized-EEG and high-EMG activity. NREM sleep was identified by the presence of a high-amplitude, slow-wave EEG and low-EMG activity relative to that of waking. REM sleep was identified by the presence of regular theta activity on EEG, coupled with low-EMG activity relative to that of NREM sleep. When two states (for example, NREM sleep and wake) occurred within a 12 s epoch, the epoch was scored for the state that predominated. Scoring was done before histological examination, so the scorer was unaware of the extent of the lesions. The daily percentage of time spent in wake, NREM sleep, and REM sleep and frequency and durations of episodes of each stage were calculated.
Histology.
On completion of the recordings, the rats were deeply anesthetized (chloral hydrate, 500 mg/kg) and transcardially perfused with 100 ml saline, followed by 500 ml of neutral phosphate buffered formalin (ThermoFisher Scientific). The brains were removed and processed for Nissl staining. For this, the harvested brains were sectioned in the coronal plane on a freezing microtome into four series of 40 μm sections and one series was mounted on gelatin-coated slides, washed in H2O, and washed in PBS. Sections were then incubated in 0.25% thionin in 0.1 m acetate buffer solution for 2 min, differentiated in graded ethanols, delipidated in xylene and coverslipped (Lu et al., 2000).
Statistical analysis.
Power-law exponent and fitting parameters for burst duration distributions were evaluated for each rat and condition (24 h: 12 h dark/light periods) in both the control group and VLPO. Pairwise comparisons between groups and conditions were conducted (see Figs. 2 and 3). The temporal correlations detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA) exponent (see Fig. 11) and θ–δ coupling Spearman's correlation coefficient (see Fig. 12) were evaluated for each rat in both groups, and then pairwise comparisons between conditions and groups were conducted. Individual group data were tested for normality using the Shapiro–Wilk test. Pairwise comparisons were conducted using Students two-tailed t test with Welch's correction, unless the Shapiro–Wilk test was significant, in which case the nonparametric Mann–Whitney U test was used. Paired tests: control 12 h dark versus 12 h light period; VLPO-lesioned 12 h dark versus 12 h light period. Comparisons between control and VLPO-lesioned group were performed for each condition (12 h dark/light period), and for the 24 h period. Surrogate tests were used to test significance of correlations between theta- and delta-bursts, and are described in Data analysis. All statistical tests were performed in MATLAB (MathWorks).
Data preprocessing
EEG recordings were first normalized to zero mean, μ = 0, and unit SD, σ = 1. For each rat, EEG signals were visually inspected and noisy segments were discarded according to the following semiautomatic procedure. Data were first examined to identify most typical noise/artifact waveforms and their specific characteristics, such as amplitude and average duration 〈T〉 expressed in number of sampling points. Based on this preliminary analysis, two amplitude thresholds S1 and s2, with S1 > s2, were introduced. The values of these thresholds are multiples of the EEG signal SD σ, and depend on the specific noise/artifact waveform for each particular rat. Typical values of threshold S1 range between 3.5σ and 6σ, and s2 is between 2.5σ and 4σ. EEG signals were then scanned using a non-overlapping window W1 = 〈T〉/2. Whenever a window W1 contained a significant number of points (e.g., ns > 15; EEG signals were sampled at 256 Hz) with amplitude exceeding the threshold S1, our algorithm identifies an artifact, and a non-overlapping sub-window of size w2 < W1 was used to scan again the identified artifact segment and clean it up. Specifically, this second step works as follows: inside the artifact window W1, segments of length w2 containing points with amplitude exceeding the threshold s2 are sequentially cleaned up by substituting all points in w2 with zeros. An artifact segment w2 that follows a preceding cleaned segment but does not contain points exceeding the threshold s2 is also cleaned; this removes subthreshold points in W1 that belong to the decaying part of an artifact. In this case, such procedure continues and the following sub-windows w2 are also cleaned until the signal crosses zero. These steps are repeated until the entire artifact segment W1 is scanned. Finally, 500 points (≈2 s) are removed from the EEG on both sides of the cleaned artifact segment W1, to eliminate possible pre-artifact and post-artifact influence on the signal. These steps carefully take into account the general slow decaying waveform of some EEG artifacts, and are needed to ensure an optimal cleaning of the data. For all rats in this study W1 = 500 and w2 = 100 data points. At the end of the preprocessing procedure EEG signals were visually inspected to ensure that all noisy segments were properly removed. The total length of removed noisy segments ranges between 5 and 10% of the 48 h recording for each rat. Of the 7 control and 7 VLPO-lesioned rats, 1 control and 1 VLPO rat had >10% of the signal removed after the preprocessing, and correspondingly were removed from the analysis. The code used for EEG preprocessing is available upon request.
Data filtering.
Data were bandpass filtered in the range 0.5–25 Hz using a FIR (finite impulse response) filter designed in MATLAB.
Data analysis
Spectral analysis.
The clean EEG signal is divided in N non-overlapping windows of size w and the spectral power in the δ band (0.5–4 Hz), Sδ, and in the θ band (4–8 Hz), Sθ, is estimated in each window using Welch's method (Welch, 1967). The analysis is performed for several values of the window size w, from 2 to 10 s. Results are generally independent of w, as shown in Figures 5, 6, and 7, and extensively discussed in the main text.
θ and δ burst detection and definition.
The ratio Rθδ = Sθ/Sδ between θ and δ power is calculated in each window k, with k = 1,2, …, N, and a time series Rθδ(k) is obtained. Given a threshold Th ≥ 1, a θ-burst is defined as a sequence of n consecutive windows where Rθδ > Th, while a δ-burst consists in a sequence of n consecutive windows where Rθδ < 1/Th (Fig. 1). The duration of a burst is given by d = n · w. Durations of θ (δ) bursts are denoted by dθ (dδ). The threshold Th is set equal to 1 throughout the analysis. Results are independent of Th, as shown in Figures 4, 5, and 6, and extensively discussed in the main text.
Surrogate test for θ- and δ-burst duration distributions.
For each rat, the time series Rθδ(k) is randomly reshuffled to obtain a surrogate Rθδ*(k′). Surrogate θ- and δ-bursts durations (see Fig. 2) are then calculated from Rθδ*(k′) following the procedure illustrated in the previous paragraph. The corresponding θ- and δ-burst duration distributions are shown in Figure 2, insets, together with distributions from original data.
Definition of quiet time Δt.
A quiet time Δt is defined as the time interval between the ending time of a burst tje and the starting time tj+1s of the following one, namely Δtj = tj+1s − tje.
Data binning.
Probability distributions of θ-burst durations are calculated using logarithmic binning, i.e., linear binning in logarithmic scale. Denoting a set of bin boundaries as B = (b1,b2, …, bk) and fixing b1 = 0.03w, the logarithmic bins fulfill the relation bi+1 = bi · 10c, which implies that the bin size is constant in logarithmic scale, i.e., logbi+1 − logbi = c. The following bin size c have been used in this study: Figures 2–9 and 15; c = 0.18. Probability distributions of δ-burst durations are calculated using the following binning procedure. Given a window size w, the bin boundaries e1,e2,…, en, …, ek are obtained using the recursive relation en = e1 + wΣi=2nbi−1, with e1 = 0.5w and n ≥ 2. The following values of the parameter b have been used in this study: Figures 10, b = 1.6; all other figures, b = 1.2.
Duration distributions: error bars.
An error δP is associated to each bin of the distribution for pooled data presented in Figures 2 and 3. The number of data n in a particular interval [D,D + dD] can be considered as given by the binomial distribution, given that the total number of data N is much larger than the range of correlations. Therefore, the associated SD is σ = . Because P(D) = n/(NdD), the error on P(D) is given by the following:
where p = P(D)dD is the probability to observe a duration D in the range [D,D + dD] and N is the total number of bursts and dD is the corresponding bin size. Power-law fits for θ-burst duration distributions shown in Figures 2 and 3 are performed on the pooled data. Estimates are reported in the corresponding figure captions together with associated errors on the fit.
Spearman's correlation.
Given to variables X and Y, the Spearman's correlation coefficient is defined as follows:
where rgX and rgY are the tied rankings of X and Y, respectively, σrgX and σrgY their SDs, and cov (rgX, rgY) indicates the covariance between rgX and rgY.
Surrogate test for correlations between consecutive θ- and δ-burst duration.
To test significance of correlations between consecutive θ- and δ-burst durations, a surrogate sequence of burst durations is generated for each rat by randomly reshuffling the original order of θ- and δ-bursts. The Spearman's correlation coefficient ρs between consecutive θ- and δ-bursts is calculated for each surrogate. The average Spearman's correlation coefficient obtained from all surrogates is then compared with the average correlation coefficient calculated from the original sequences of burst durations via t test (see Results; Fig. 12). Correlation coefficients for surrogate data for both control and VLPO-lesioned rats during dark, light, and 24 h are all with value |ρs| < 103.
DFA.
The DFA is a method based on random walk (Peng et al., 1994). It improves the classical fluctuation analysis (FA) for nonstationary signals where embedded polynomial trends mask the intrinsic correlation properties in the fluctuations (Peng et al., 1994). The performance of DFA for signals with different types of non-stationarities and artifacts has been extensively studied and compared with other methods of correlation analysis (Taqqu et al., 1995; Hu et al., 2001; Chen et al., 2005; Xu et al., 2005). The DFA method is briefly described by the following steps (Peng et al., 1994):
A given signal ui (i = 1, …, N, where N is the length of the signal) is integrated to obtain y(k) ≡ Σi=1k[u(i) − 〈u〉], where 〈u〉 is the mean of ui;
The integrated signal y(k) is divided into boxes of equal length n;
In each box of length n we fit y(k) using a first-order polynomial function, which represents the trend in that box. The y-coordinate of the fit curve in each box is denoted by yn(k);
- The integrated profile y(k) is detrended by subtracting the local trend yn(k) in each box of length n:
- For a given box length n, calculate the root mean square (rms) fluctuation function for this integrated and detrended signal:
Repeat the above computation over a broad range of box lengths n, where n represents a specific space or time scale, to obtain a functional relationship between F(n) and n.
For a power-law correlated time series, the average rms fluctuation function F(n) and the box size n are connected by a power-law relation, that is F(n) ∼ nαd. The exponent αd is a parameter that quantifies the long-range power-law correlation properties of the signal. Values of αd < 0.5 indicate the presence of anti-correlations in the time series, αd = 0.5 absence of correlations (white noise) and αd > 0.5 indicates the presence of positive correlations in the time series.
Conditional probabilities analysis.
The conditional probability of an event H for a given event X is defined as follows:
where P(H ∩ X) is the probability that H and X jointly occur, and P(X) > 0 is the probability of the event X. The condition X reduces the statistics and increases the fluctuations of the distribution P(H|X) compared with P(H). The following error is associated to each bin of the densities (Corral, 2006): ϵH = , where p = P(H)dH is the probability to observe a H in the range H,H + dH and N is the total number of events. From probability theory P(H|X) = P(X) if and only if H does not depend on X. On the contrary, P(H|X) ≠ P(X) implies that H and X are not independent of each other, and their relation can be quantified by a suitable correlation measure. In the analysis of burst coupling (see Results, Anti-correlated coupling between the durations of consecutive θ and δ-bursts), H and X are considered significantly correlated if P(H|X) − P(X) > ϵH.
Software accessibility.
Software used for data preprocessing is available upon request.
Model of anti-correlated burst coupling
The model consists of the following steps.
Random drawing and ranking.
N durations dθ and dδ are randomly drawn from the empirical distributions previously obtained using a specific window size w. dθ and dδ are separately sorted in ascending order, i.e., from shortest to longest, and get a distinct ordinal numbers from k = 1,2, …, N, which corresponds to their rank. This procedure ensures that each duration has a unique rank. The ranked dθ and dδ are then paired with a tunable degree of anti-correlation and a new time series of alternating θ- and δ-burst durations is thus generated. The coarse-grained properties of the resulting time series depends on the degree of anti-correlations used in the pairing.
Correlated pairing.
Once dθ and dδ are ranked and a distinct, unique ordinal number is associated to them, one randomly choose a dθ with rank k1 between 1 and N. To choose the following dδ, one draws a random number k2 from a Gaussian distribution with mean μ = 1 + N − k1 and SD σ, and takes dδ as the duration corresponding to rank k2. This procedure is iterated N times, and at each iteration i the mean of the Gaussian from which one draws the next random rank, ki, depends on ki−1, i.e., μ − 1 + N − ki−1. At each iteration, ki will correspond to a duration dδ from the sorted δ-burst durations if the preceding burst was a θ-burst with duration dθ, whereas ki will select a duration dθ from the sorted θ-burst durations if the preceding burst was a δ-burst with duration dδ. As a result one obtains a sequence of dθ and dδ whose degree of anti-correlations is controlled by a single parameter, σ. The smaller σ, the stronger anti-correlations are.
Binary series and coarse-graining.
To characterize the coarse-grained properties, the time series is first converted in a binary sequence, namely a sequence of “+” and “−”. Because each duration is by definition a multiple n of the unit window w, namely d = nw, the n windows belonging to a dθ are populated with +, whereas the n windows belonging to a dδ with − (see Fig. 14c). As a result one has a sequence of windows populated with + and −. This binary sequence is then coarse-grained grouping a given number Δ of consecutive windows, with Δ odd number, and assigning + or − to the new windows of size Δ according to a majority rule, i.e., one assign + (−) if the number of + is larger (smaller) than the number of − (see Fig. 14). A coarse-grained binary sequence (CGBS) is thus obtained, and dCG coarse-grained durations are calculated as shown in Figure 14.
Results
Transient dynamics in bursting activity of θ and δ rhythms
Statistics of sleep and wake bout durations show that control rats spend on average 50.47 ± 3.37% of the time in wakefulness, 42.12 ± 2.39% in NREM sleep, and 7.41 ± 1.09% in REM sleep in the 24 h period. Correspondingly, rats with VLPO lesion spend on average more time in wakefulness (61.46 ± 4.17%), and experience a corresponding decrease in the percentage of NREM (31.94 ± 3.85%) and REM sleep (6.60 ± 0.86%) across the 24 h period, in agreement with previous reports (Lu et al., 2000; Vetrivelan et al., 2012). The analysis of power spectra (Fig. 1c) for control rats indicates dominant delta rhythm in NREM and increasing spectral power in the theta band for REM and WAKE, and a decrease in relative delta power during NREM in VLPO-lesioned rats (Lu et al., 2000; Vetrivelan et al., 2012). However, total sleep–wake time, average bout duration, and spectral power reflect static characteristics of the sleep process. In contrast, to investigate emergent signatures of criticality in sleep micro-architecture, we focus on dynamical characteristics of dominant cortical rhythms at short time scales.
To dissect the temporal organization of δ and θ-bursts in the broadband brain activity across the sleep–wake cycle, we analyze the time evolution of the EEG signal by evaluating the spectral power in several frequency bands on non-overlapping windows of length w (Fig. 1a,b; see Materials and Methods, Data analysis). Figure 1d shows a typical spectrogram S(f) as a function of time for a 2 h lights-on recording of a rat in the control group. In each window, the spectral power is primarily concentrated in either the δ-wave (0–4 Hz) or the θ-wave (4–8 Hz) band, and exhibits sharp transitions from periods with dominant δ to periods with dominant θ waves. In particular, during wake periods most of the windows in the spectrogram exhibit dominant θ band (Fig. 1e), with fewer transitions to periods with dominant δ waves. Such dynamics can be understood as the temporal evolution of the ratio Rθδ = S(θ)/S(δ) between θ and δ spectral power in association with different physiological states; NREM, REM, and arousals/wake (Fig. 1 shows the transient dynamics of δ- and θ-wave power represented by the logarithm of Rθδ as a function of time t).
The ratio Rθδ(t) exhibits irregular, intermittent fluctuations between values larger and smaller than a threshold Th, a typical characteristic of non-equilibrium dynamics: Rθδ > Th = 1 indicates that the spectral power in the θ-wave band is dominant; vice versa, for Rθδ < Th = 1 the spectral power is dominated by the δ-wave (Fig. 1d,e). We define bursts in θ and δ rhythms as sequences of consecutive time windows where Rθδ > Th = 1 and Rθδ < Th = 1, respectively (Fig. 1f). We associate a duration d = n × w to each burst (see Materials and Methods, Data analysis), where n is the number of consecutive windows belonging to a given burst and w is the window length (Fig. 1f).
Distinct functional forms of θ- and δ-burst duration distributions indicative of self-organization at criticality
We next study the probability distribution of the durations of θ- and δ-bursts over a 24 h period for control and VLPO-lesioned rats (see Materials and Methods, Experimental setup). We notice that θ- and δ-bursts follow very different statistics. The distribution Pθ of θ-burst durations exhibits power-law behavior followed by a cutoff (Fig. 2a), Pθ(d) ∝ d−α, where α denotes the scaling exponent of the power-law. Power law distributions P(x) ∝ x−α are the statistical hallmark of scale invariance, i.e., they are not altered by a change of scale from x to Lx, and depending on the context, they imply that events of any size, length, or duration are likely to occur with some finite probability that is larger than expected in a random or short-range correlated process. Presence of power-law indicates absence of characteristic time scales in the underlying dynamics, which is a typical feature of physical systems at the critical point of continuous phase transition; a highly sensitive state where cooperative behavior spontaneously emerges over a range of time scales characterized by long-range correlations. Importantly, the scale-invariant power-law behavior characterizing the distribution of θ-burst durations is significantly influenced by lesions in the VLPO. Indeed, we find that α ≃ 2.4 in control rats, whereas α ≃ 2.8 in VLPO-lesioned rats (Fig. 2a). The power-law is consistent across rats in both groups, and the average exponent in the VLPO-lesioned group is significantly higher than the exponent in the control group [αCTRL = 2.29 ± 0.15 and αVLPO = 2.71 ± 0.11 (mean ± SD); t test, p = 0.0005]. An increase in the power-law exponent indicates that bilateral lesions of the VLPO alter the dynamical micro-architecture of θ-bursts across the 24 h sleep–wake cycle, leading to a decreased likelihood of long lasting θ-bursts; an effect not previously observed.
In contrast to the power-law feature of θ-burst, the statistics of δ-burst duration follows a different behavior that is described by a Weibull distribution Pδ(d; λ, β) = e−(d/λ)β, where λ indicates the characteristic time scale, and β is the shape parameter (Fig. 2b). Further, we find that the distribution of δ-burst durations follows the same Weibull functional form for both control and VLPO-lesioned groups, with similar values of the parameters λ and β.
The functional forms established for the distributions of θ- and δ-burst durations in Figure 2, indicate a very different temporal organization of θ- and δ-bursts. A surrogate test based on randomizing the sequence of windows w in the EEG spectrogram (Fig. 1d) leads to exponentially distributed θ- and δ-burst (Fig. 2, insets), and shows that the observed temporal organization in bursting activity of brain rhythms is physiologically relevant and relates to underlying regulation.
The results demonstrate a remarkable duality of scale-free, power-law dynamics for θ-bursts, and Weibull dynamics with characteristic time scale for δ-bursts. The coexistence of scale-free θ-bursts and Weibull distributed δ-bursts appears to be a general feature of cortical activity across the entire sleep–wake cycle of individual subjects in each group, and it is preserved after major lesions of the VLPO area. Remarkably, VLPO lesions, although significantly affecting sleep statistics (Lu et al., 2000; Vetrivelan et al., 2012), do not disrupt the fundamental duality of power-law and Weibull. Coexistence of scale-invariant and exponential-like behaviors is a hallmark of self-organization at criticality in non-equilibrium systems characterized by alternating active and inactive states; unlike systems at equilibrium, such systems maintain critical behavior without external tuning (Boffetta et al., 1999; Paczuski et al., 2005; Munoz, 2018). Thus, our observations point to an intrinsic common mechanism that underlies the temporal organization of bursting activity in both δ and θ cortical waves across the distinct physiological states of wake/arousals and sleep. Importantly, the deviation of the VLPO-lesioned θ-burst power-law exponent from the value measured in control rats, indicates that lesion of VLPO neurons alters the optimal underlying dynamics for sleep regulation.
To better describe bursting activity of θ- and δ-waves across the sleep–wake cycle, and the role of the VLPO neuronal population in this dynamics, we next analyze the distributions of θ and δ-burst durations separately during the 12 h dark/light periods. Although sleep and wake are characterized by different dominant brain rhythms with distinct dynamics, synchronization and coupling patterns across cortical areas (Kopell et al., 2000; Liu et al., 2015), our analysis shows that the duality of power-law and Weibull for θ- and δ-bursts duration distributions is robust and independent of the dominant physiologic state. Indeed, such coexistence appears to be a basic characteristic of cortical activity during both dark and light periods (Fig. 3). Importantly, the scaling exponent α of the power-law as well as the Weibull parameters λ and β concurrently change comparing dark to light periods, indicating a coordinated modulation of θ and δ wave dynamics across sleep and wake. In contrast to humans, rats are predominantly awake during the dark period and asleep during the light period. We observe that α is larger in the dark than in the light period (Fig. 3a,c), indicating that long θ-bursts are more likely when rats are predominantly asleep. The higher probability of longer-lasting θ-bursts during the light period could be associated with the presence of longer episodes of REM sleep, when θ-waves are dominant. This leads to an average increase of the power in the θ band and thus to a higher likelihood for longer θ-bursts during the light periods. For control rats, we find that the power-law exponent decreases from α ≈ 2.65 during dark to α ≈ 2.5 during light (Fig. 3a). This variation is more pronounced in VLPO-lesioned rats, where the exponent decreases from α ≈ 3.1 during dark to α ≈ 2.5 during light periods (Fig. 3c), and may be related to the average reduction of REM sleep. These results are consistent across rats both during dark [αCTRL = 2.62 ± 0.18; αVLPO = 3.10 ± 0.24 (mean ± SD); t test, p = 0.003] and light [αCTRL = 2.14 ± 0.13; αVLPO = 2.29 ± 0.15 (mean ± SD); t test, p = 0.131], and show significant difference particularly for the 12 h dark period. Paired tests also show significant differences between dark and light periods in each group (Control dark vs control light t test, p = 0.0005; VLPO dark vs VLPO light t test, p = 0.0010). The comparison between control and VLPO-lesioned rats shows that VLPO lesions affect the distributions of θ-burst durations, in particular during the dark period, while having no particular influence on the distributions of δ-burst durations, which overlap within error bars (Fig. 3). This indicates that, although lesions of VLPO neurons reduce δ power and total sleep time (Lu et al., 2000; Vetrivelan et al., 2012), they do not alter the dynamics of δ-bursts, but instead influence the power-law organization in θ-burst dynamics and lead to an increase of the power-law exponent, indicating a decreased likelihood for long-lasting bursts and a higher probability to observe shorter bursts; evidence of fragmentation of bursting activity in the θ-wave band that parallels the increased fragmentation of sleep in VLPO-lesioned rats.
Previous studies have shown that preceding sleep–wake history affects sleep consolidation and slow-wave activity. In particular, sleep deprivation increases sleep consolidation and continuity, reduces frequency of short wake episodes, and thus sleep fragmentation (Trachsel et al., 1991). Moreover, it has been shown that after long periods of sleep deprivation NREM episodes tend to be longer, with a larger amount of slow-wave sleep and increased EEG amplitude (Tobler and Borbély, 1986; Franken et al., 1991; Rodriguez et al., 2016). In this context, it would be important to understand how change in homeostatic drive (for example in response to sleep deprivation) would impact the reported underlying critical dynamics of cortical rhythms. While the established non-equilibrium dynamics in cortical rhythms at small time scales apparently contrast homeostatic regulation of the sleep–wake cycle (Borbély and Achermann, 1999; Achermann and Borbély, 2003), the interplay between this two mechanisms remains to be better investigated and understood in future studies. VLPO-lesioned rats, although exhibiting an increase in the percentage of wake across the 24 h period and in average duration of wake bouts (Lu et al., 2000; Vetrivelan et al., 2012), do not show extended periods of wake comparable to those used in previous experimental protocol for studying sleep deprivation (Tobler and Borbély, 1986; Franken et al., 1991; Trachsel et al., 1991), and therefore we cannot not draw any conclusion on the effect of homeostatic mechanisms on the underlying critical dynamics based on the data analyzed here.
Furthermore, loss of VLPO neurons also disinhibit the wake-promoting system including hypocretin system, and leads to their increased activity (Lu et al., 2000; Vetrivelan et al., 2012). These changes are interrelated, and may be difficult to dissociate from each other. For example, changes in EEG spectra (Fig. 1) and sleep fragmentation after VLPO lesions (Lu et al., 2000; Vetrivelan et al., 2012) could also be due to increased activity of hypocretin system (Sakurai, 2007). On the other hand, other manipulations, e.g., water/food availability, LD conditions, ambient temperature etc., could also alter EEG and sleep–wake cycle by altering VLPO neuron activity, and therefore influence the θ- and δ-burst dynamics. Our current study investigated the intrinsic properties of cortical dynamics following specific VLPO lesions, and future investigations are necessary to understand the mechanisms (changes in VLPO neural activity) by which other manipulations alter cortical dynamics, and in particular affect the ability of the system to self-tune at criticality.
Robust scale-invariant critical behavior of θ- and δ-bursts across time scales
We have shown that bursts associated with θ and δ rhythms exhibit a distinct temporal organization that is captured by specific duration distributions: a power-law for θ-bursts, indicating absence of a characteristic time scale, and a Weibull for δ-bursts, with a characteristic time scale λ (Figs. 2, 3). These findings are based on a particular observational window size w and threshold Th that were used to analyze bursting activity (Fig. 1; see Materials and Methods, Data analysis). To demonstrate that our results are independent of the particular choice of Th and w, we repeat the analyses for a range of parameter values. We find that the dynamics of burst durations across the 24 h sleep–wake cycle is indeed described by unique scaling functions.
We first examine the duration distributions of θ- and δ-bursts for different threshold values Th, keeping the window size w fixed. By increasing the threshold on the ratio Rθδ from Th = 1 to Th = 2, we find that the scaling exponent α characterizing the power-law distribution of θ-burst durations remains stable (data collapse onto a single curve; Fig. 4a,c). The scaling behavior is followed by a cutoff that, with increasing Th values, shifts to shorter burst durations dθ. For both control and VLPO-lesioned rats this behavior can be expressed in terms of the following scaling relation:
Where α is the power-law scaling exponent, f(d/Th−ϵ) is a scaling function, and ϵ expresses the dependence of the cutoff on Th. The existence of a scaling function f(d/Th−ϵ) satisfying Equation 6 is confirmed by the data collapse obtained by plotting P(d)dα versus Thϵd for several values of Th (Fig. 4a,c, insets).
Similarly, we show that the distribution of δ-burst durations is independent of the threshold Th, and is described by a single scaling function (Fig. 4b,d). Because a δ-burst is defined as a time period of consecutive windows w where Rθδ < Th = 1 (Fig. 1f), to properly explore the behavior of the duration distribution for states with increasingly dominant δ power, we repeat the analysis for different values Th < 1. We observe that, as Th decreases, the probability for long δ-bursts decreases, while short δ-bursts become more likely (Fig. 4b,d, insets). However, when distributions are rescaled by their respective mean δ-burst duration 〈dδ〉, they all collapse onto a unique function fδ, which is the same for both control and VLPO-lesioned rats (Fig. 4b,d, respectively). Such function is defined by the following scaling relation:
where η = 1.3 for both rat groups, and is well described by a Weibull functional form, as found by rescaling dδ and Pδ (Fig. 4b,d).
Repeating the analysis for 12 h dark/light periods separately, we find that Equations 6 and 7 consistently describe the dynamics of δ- and θ-bursts in both control and VLPO-lesioned groups (Figs. 5, 6).
Thus, our results indicate that the duality of power-law and Weibull behavior, as well as the scaling properties summarized in Equations 6 and 7, are robust features of the bursting activity across the sleep–wake cycle, and do not depend on the particular threshold Th used to study the dynamics of θ- and δ-bursts.
Furthermore, we find that the functional behavior of the distribution of θ- and δ-burst durations is to a large extent also independent of the window size w, used to investigate the time course of the EEG spectral power. Intuitively, a larger w would tend to fail in identifying short bursts and merge them together, thus causing an increase in the probability of observing longer durations. In that regard, considering the power-law temporal organization of θ-bursts, larger window sizes w mainly influence the tail of the distribution with longer durations, thus leading to a decrease of the scaling exponent α (Fig. 7a,c, insets). We note that the window size effect becomes visible only for extremely large w compared with the average θ-burst duration 〈dθ〉. However, when the θ-burst durations are rescaled by the window size w, all distributions collapse onto a single power-law (Fig. 7a,c), confirming the robustness of the results obtained in Figure 2a for both control and VLPO-lesioned rats. Such rescaling is defined by the following relation:
Separate analyses of 12 h dark/light periods for different window sizes w (Figs. 8a,c, Fig. 9a,c) further confirm the robustness of the established power-law behavior for the θ-burst durations (Fig. 3a,c).
A similar data collapse characterizes the dependence of the δ-burst duration distribution on window size w. Generally, we observe that for increasing w the probability for long δ-bursts increases, while short δ-bursts become less likely (Fig. 7b,d, insets). When δ-burst duration distributions corresponding to different window sizes w are rescaled by their respective mean duration 〈dδ〉, we find that all distributions collapse onto a unique function fδ following a Weibull behavior (Fig. 7b,d) and obeying the scaling relation as follows:
As in the case of θ-burst dynamics, the temporal organization of δ-bursts is robust and characterized by a scaling function (Eq. 9), which is universal for the control and VLPO groups, and remains stable during light and dark periods (Figs. 8b,d, 9b,d).
The existence of universal scaling functions (Eqs. 6–9) not only demonstrates that duration distributions are independent of the specific set of parameters used to identify θ- and δ-bursts, but also constitutes evidence of scale invariance, a property associated with systems operating at criticality.
Hierarchical micro-architecture in the temporal order of quiet (δ-bursts) and active (θ-bursts) states in the sleep–wake cycle
The results reported in the previous sections show a remarkable coexistence of scale-invariant power-law structure for the durations of θ-bursts and a Weibull functional form with a characteristic time scale for δ-burst durations (Figs. 2,3). This evidence draws a strong parallel with far-from-equilibrium phenomena that are characterized by bursting dynamics and abrupt transitions between active and quiet states, such as avalanches and earthquakes (Boffetta et al., 1999; Corral, 2004; Paczuski et al., 2005; Munoz, 2018). For instance, the intensity of avalanches and earthquakes (active states) is also described by power-law distributions, while time intervals between consecutive avalanches/earthquakes (quiet states) follow a generalized Gamma distribution with a characteristic time scale (exponential tail). In this context Gamma is a universal scaling function that is independent of spatial scales and minimum magnitude thresholds, and is consistently observed for a broad range of conditions despite the large variability associated with phenomena such as avalanches and earthquakes (Corral, 2004; Ribeiro et al., 2010; de Arcangelis et al., 2016).
In sleep dynamics, wake and brief arousals during sleep can be considered as active states that, in rodents, are characterized by bursts in θ rhythms. Hence, we hypothesize that a hierarchical structure, invariant across time scales, may underlie the occurrence of θ-bursts, in analogy with non-equilibrium critical phenomena, and investigate the relationship between the duration of θ-bursts and their temporal occurrence (Fig. 10). To this end, we consider the time sequence of θ-bursts, and we study the statistical features of the quiet times Δt separating consecutive bursts, taking into account the duration dθ of each θ-burst (Fig. 10a). Because θ-bursts vary in duration, we impose a threshold D0 representing the time scale of analysis, and we define the quiet time Δti as the period from the end of θi-burst to the beginning θi+1-burst. Thus, the statistical characteristics of Δti depend on the threshold value D0. We then obtain the probability distribution P(Δt; D0) of quiet times Δti for different values of D0 (Fig. 6c,d, insets). With increasing threshold (scale of observation) D0, the probability of longer Δti increases, whereas the probability of short Δti decreases, leading to different curves for the distributions P(Δt; D0).
Visual inspection of the complex profile formed by the time sequence of θ-bursts and their respective durations shows an apparent similarity when comparing short segments of the profile with the entire sequence above a given threshold D0 (Fig. 6b). Presence of statistical self-similarity, observed after effective coarse-graining of the profile, indicates a hierarchical structure across time scales D0 that characterizes θ-bursts occurrence times and durations, and the associated quiet times. To demonstrate statistical self-similarity in the quiet times, we systematically analyze the functional form of the probability distributions P(Δt; D0) for different thresholds D0 by rescaling each distribution by the average quiet time 〈Δt〉D0. Remarkably, we find that all distribution curves collapse onto a single function G (Fig. 6c,d), defined by the following scaling relation:
The scaling relation in Equation 10 represents a mathematical expression of the statistical self-similarity in the profile formed by the quiet times and θ-burst durations shown in Figure 6b. We find that the scaling function G(Δt/〈Δt〉) is well described by the generalized Gamma distribution G(Δt/〈Δt〉; b, v, p) = p/bv(Δt/〈t〉)v−1e−(Δt/b〈t〉)p/Γ(v/p) (Stacy, 1962), where in our analysis Δt/〈Δt〉 is a dimensionless quiet time. The Gamma functional form is homogeneous (Ivanov et al., 1996) i.e., rescaling the variable leaves the functional form unchanged. Such scaling function indicates a hierarchical structure in the quiet times between consecutive θ-bursts, independent of the scale of observation D0. In the limit of D0 = 0, the quiet time distribution P(Δt; D0) coincides with the distribution of δ-burst durations Pδ (Figs. 2b, 3b,d); a Weibull functional form that belongs to the same class of homogeneous functions as the generalized Gamma.
Our analysis shows that the scaling relation in Equation 10 and the associated Gamma functional form for the quiet times are robust: (1) we find them in the 24 h period (Fig. 10) as well as separately during light and dark periods, and (2) they do not significantly change with lesion in the VLPO (Fig. 10, compare c, d). Further, the presence of such hierarchical structure in quiet times indicates specific temporal order in the occurrence of θ-bursts. To explicitly verify this, we randomly reshuffle the sequence of θ-burst durations, while preserving the δ-burst durations corresponding to quiet times at D0 = 0, and we perform the analysis on the reshuffled sequence to obtain quiet time distributions Prand(Δt; D0) for different thresholds D0. In this case, after rescaling the distributions Prand(Δt; D0) by the average quiet time 〈Δt〉D0, their curves collapse onto an exponential distribution (Fig. 10c,d, dashed lines); a hallmark of temporal independence between consecutive events (Daley and Vere-Jones, 1988). This clearly demonstrates that temporal correlations are intimately related to the existence of non-exponential scaling functions (Eq. 10; Daley and Vere-Jones, 1988; Corral, 2004).
Notably, a similar temporal organization characterized by coexistence of power-law and generalized Gamma distribution has been reported for active states and quiet times in a range of non-equilibrium systems self-tuning at criticality (earthquakes, avalanches; Corral, 2004; Pruessner, 2012; Munoz, 2018). Thus, our findings of power-law distribution for θ-burst durations (Figs. 2,3) combined with a generalized Gamma distribution for the quiet times between consecutive θ-bursts at different scales of observation D0 (Fig. 10) are a strong evidence in support of our hypothesis that bursting activity of fundamental brain rhythms and the associated sleep micro-architecture exhibit critical non-equilibrium behavior.
Long-range power-law correlations in the durations of θ and δ bursts
Physical systems at criticality exhibit long-range correlations that span the entire system across space and time scales, and lead to the emergence of collective cooperative behavior (Stanley, 1987). Indeed, scaling features in such systems often arise in conjunction with long-range spatiotemporal correlations following power-laws. Notably, physiological systems under neuro-autonomic regulation also exhibit dynamics characterized by long-range power-law correlations; a scale-invariant structure that undergoes a phase transition with transitions from sleep to wake (Kantelhardt et al., 2003; Schmitt et al., 2009), with circadian rhythms (Hu et al., 2004; Ivanov, 2007) and under clinical conditions (Ivanov et al., 1999; Goldberger et al., 2002). Further, the randomization procedure in the previous subsection (Fig. 10) clearly demonstrates that the scale invariant structure in quiet times characterized by a Gamma scaling function (Eq. 10) can arise only in the presence of a certain temporal order in θ-bursts occurrence. Thus, we next perform correlation analysis to quantify long-range features in the temporal organization of δ- and θ-burst durations.
To this end, we use the DFA, a random walk based method specially tailored to quantify long-range power-law correlations embedded in nonstationary signals with various polynomial trends and bursting dynamics (Chen et al., 2002, 2005). The DFA method is based on evaluation of the rms fluctuation function F(n) (see Materials and Methods, Data analysis), where n is the scale of analysis expressed in number of consecutive bursts (Fig. 11). A scaling relationship of the form F(n) ∝ nαd indicates presence of long-range power-law correlations in the time series of burst durations. Correlation exponent αd ∈ (0, 0.5) indicates anti-correlations (where short burst durations tend to be followed by longer burst durations), whereas αd ∈ (0.5, 1) indicates positive persistent correlations (long bursts tend to be followed by longer bursts); αd = 0.5 corresponds to a random walk and absence of correlations.
We perform DFA on sequences of θ- and δ-burst durations separately, distinguishing between dark and light periods (Fig. 11). We find that both θ- and δ-bursts exhibit long-range power-law correlations. In control rats, the power-law exponents characterizing θ- and δ-bursts are αd = 0.58 ± 0.02 and αd = 0.64 ± 0.01 (exponent ± error on fit) for the 12 h dark period, and αd = 0.61 ± 0.01 and αd = 0.64 ± 0.01 (exponent ± error on fit) for the 12 h light period. Comparisons between group averages show that exponents for dark and light periods are not significantly different [θ-bursts: Control dark αd = 0.57 ± 0.05 vs Control light αd = 0.56 ± 0.04 (mean ± SD), t test, p = 0.636; δ-bursts: Control dark αd = 0.61 ± 0.07 vs Control light αd = 0.59 ± 0.04 (mean ± SD), Mann–Whitney U test, p = 0.751]. Moreover, our analysis indicates that VLPO lesions do not affect the nature of temporal correlations, but may influence their strength (Fig. 11). As for control rats, power-law exponents characterizing correlations during dark and light periods are not significantly different [θ-bursts: VLPO dark αd = 0.57 ± 0.07 vs VLPO light αd = 0.58 ± 0.08 (mean ± SD), t test, p = 0.839; δ-bursts: VLPO dark αd = 0.57 ± 0.04 vs VLPO light αd = 0.57 ± 0.03 (mean ± SD), t test, p = 0.986]. Comparison between group averages do not show significant differences between DFA θ-burst exponents in control and VLPO-lesioned rats (Control dark vs VLPO dark, t test, p = 0.916; Control light vs VLPO light, t test, p = 0.475). Differences appear more pronounced for the DFA δ-burst exponents, with the VLPO exponent αd = 0.59 ± 0.01 lower than the control exponent αd = 0.64 ± 0.01. However, the comparison between control and the VLPO-lesioned group do not evidence significant differences (Control dark vs VLPO dark, Mann–Whitney U test, p = 0.436; Control light vs VLPO light, t test, p = 0.190).
Anti-correlated coupling between the durations of consecutive θ- and δ-bursts
Physical and biological systems at equilibrium (homoeostasis) are typically controlled by mechanisms that either lead to dynamics with specific space or time scales characterized by exponential behaviors or to scale-invariant dynamics without characteristic scales following power-laws. Non-equilibrium systems self-organizing at criticality are unique in the sense that they combine two distinct behaviors, a scale-invariant power-law related to the dynamics of active states and an exponential related to quiet states, both of which emerge out of a single regulatory mechanism (Lo et al., 2002). In that context, our findings of (1) power-law distribution for θ-burst (active states) durations in coexistence with Weibull distribution for δ-burst (quiet states) durations (characteristic time scale in the quiet states) shown in Figures 2 and 3, (2) universal Gamma function characterizing the temporal organization of quiet times across a range of scales (Fig. 10), and (2) long-range power-law correlations in the time sequence of θ- and δ-burst durations (Fig. 11), all typical features of systems at criticality, indicate a common regulatory mechanism for the bursting activity of both θ and δ rhythms and associated sleep micro-architecture. Presence of such common mechanism may imply coupling between θ- and δ-bursts, as further suggested by concurrent change in both power-law and Weibull distribution parameters with transition from dark to light periods (Fig. 3). Thus, to further understand bursting dynamics in relation to neuronal integrity in the VLPO, we next investigate the coupling between consecutive δ- and θ-burst durations, and the role of such coupling in the emergent scaling behavior of duration distributions in control and VLPO-lesioned rats.
We first focus on the relationship between ranks of consecutive δ- and θ-burst durations, dδ and dθ. We rank burst durations in ascending order, with the shortest duration corresponding to the smallest rank, and examine the scatter plots between the ranks of consecutive dδ and dθ (Fig. 12a,b). We find that δ-bursts of high ranks (i.e., long durations) tend to be followed by θ-bursts of low ranks (i.e., short durations). This anti-correlated coupling is consistently present in both control (Fig. 12a) and VLPO-lesioned rats (Fig. 12b), and appears to be a basic characteristic of dynamics as it is observed throughout the entire sleep–wake cycle in both dark and light periods.
To quantify the coupling between consecutive δ- and θ-burst durations we use Spearman's correlation coefficient, which assesses monotonic relationships between two variables (see Materials and Methods, Data analysis). The Spearman's cross-correlation is positive when observations of two variables have similar ranks, and negative if they have opposite ranks. Our analyses show that the Spearman's coefficient calculated for consecutive δ- and θ-burst durations is always (24 h, dark/light period) significantly negative (Fig. 12c), indicating anti-correlated coupling. This is verified by a surrogate test where the sequence of consecutive δ- and θ-burst durations is randomized (Fig. 12c; see Materials and Methods, Data analysis). We find that the δ–θ anti-correlated coupling is less pronounced for VLPO-lesioned rats, and this is consistently observed in both light and dark periods. Comparing light versus dark period, our results show an increase in the anti-correlated coupling during the light period within each group (Fig. 12c).
The presence of anti-correlated coupling between consecutive δ- and θ-bursts is further supported by the analysis of conditional probabilities (Fig. 13). Specifically, we ask how the conditional probability distribution P(dδi|dθi−1 > d*) of δ-burst durations dδi changes depending on the length of preceding θ-burst duration dθi−1, i.e., we consider the probability distribution of the subset of δ-bursts which follow θ-burst durations above a given length d*. We note that if dδi is independent of the preceding θ-burst, then the conditional probability distribution is equivalent to the unconditioned Weibull distribution of δ-burst durations (Fig. 3), i.e., P(dδi|dθi−1 > d*) = P(dδi). On the other hand, finding P(dδi|dθi−1 > d*) ≠ P(dδi) implies that the duration dδi depends on the duration dθi−1 of the preceding θ-burst. By studying the behavior of the conditional probability distribution P(dδi|dθi−1 > d*) for several values of the threshold d*, we find that the probability for longer dδi systematically drops for increasing d* values (faster declining tail of the conditional distribution), while the probability for shorter dδi significantly increases (Fig. 13, insets). This dependence is observed during light and dark periods for both control and VLPO-lesioned groups, and indicates an anti-correlated coupling between the durations of consecutive θ- and δ-bursts.
Phenomenological model of coupling and criticality in θ- and δ-bursts dynamics
We next test whether the established anti-correlated coupling between consecutive δ- and θ-burst durations is essential for the emergent duality of power-law and Weibull distribution. We develop a phenomenological model (Fig. 14) based on anti-correlated pairing of θ and δ durations randomly drawn from the empirical distributions of the δ- and θ-burst durations established in our study (Fig. 2). The model allows to control the degree of anti-correlations between consecutive θ- and δ-burst durations, and thus to examine whether and how anti-correlations affect the emerging power-law and Weibull distributions of burst durations, and the related scale-invariant temporal structure of θ- and δ-bursts.
The basic steps to generate sequences of alternating θ- and δ-burst durations with the desired degree of correlations are schematically outlined in Figure 14 and explained in further detail in Materials and Methods. Specifically: (1) we randomly draw durations dθ and dδ from their respective power-law and Weibull distributions obtained from our empirical data analyses (Fig. 15a); (2) dθ and dδ are next separately sorted in ascending order, from shortest to longest, and are assigned a unique rank (Fig. 15b); (3) the durations dθ and dδ are then paired based on their ranks with a certain degree of anti-correlation (defined by and monotonically depending on a single parameter) to generate an artificial time series of alternating θ- and δ-burst durations (Fig. 15b); and (4) the obtained artificial time series is then binarized in windows w corresponding to the window size used in our EEG spectral power analysis of the original data (Fig. 1a), where + is assigned for θ-bursts and − for δ-bursts. Finally, the binary series is coarse-grained at larger time scale Δ (Fig. 14c).
We use this model to test our hypothesis that coupling between δ- and θ-bursts is essential for the emergent duality of power-law and Weibull behavior across time scales. We test to what extent δ–θ coupling strength plays role in the emergent scale-invariant organization of sleep micro-architecture. Our simulations show that the generated distributions of δ- and θ-burst durations depend on the degree of anti-correlation introduced in the model. When the Spearman's cross-correlation coefficient of burst durations generated by the model corresponds to the empirical values found in real data, the distributions obtained from the model approximate the empirical distributions (Fig. 15), and scale-invariant temporal organization in burst durations emerges over a range of coarse-graining scales Δ. In contrast, absence of anti-correlated coupling in our model (i.e., random pairing of δ and θ durations in the generated time series) leads to exponential distribution for both δ- and θ-bursts, significantly different from real data (Fig. 15).
Discussion
We studied the dynamical features of wake- and sleep-dominant brain rhythms across 48 h recordings of the sleep–wake cycle. To uncover basic principles underlying sleep dynamics and emergent sleep stage and arousals/wake transitions, we analyzed control rats and rats with lesions in the VLPO (Lu et al., 2000, 2002). We found that transient bursts in θ and δ cortical rhythms continuously occur during the sleep–wake cycle, and exhibit a complex temporal organization which is characterized by a remarkable duality of scale-invariant power-law distribution for θ-burst durations (active states) and Weibull distribution with a exponential characteristic time scale for δ-burst durations (quiet states; Fig. 2). Further, we showed the presence of anti-correlated coupling between θ- and δ-bursts dynamics (Fig. 12) and demonstrated that this coupling is essential part of the mechanism responsible for the emerging duality of power-law and Weibull behavior across time scales (Figs. 14, 15). The presence of complex temporal organization and coupling in cortical rhythms is also manifested through the hierarchical structure in the quiet time intervals separating consecutive active states (θ-bursts) above a given duration (Fig. 10), which we find to be described by a unique scaling function (generalized Gamma distribution). This self-similar structure links, across time scales, the duration of a given θ-burst with the time of its occurrence. Moreover, we found that sequences of consecutive θ- or δ-burst durations are long-range power-law correlated, indicating a scale-invariant organization in the temporal order of burst durations and a unique underlying process with persistent “memory” spanning over a wide range of scales that statistically couples the duration of a given burst with the durations of hundreds of following bursts (Fig. 11).
Importantly, our empirical analyses showed that the reported characteristics of θ- and δ-bursts dynamics do not depend on the scale of observation or on the threshold used to identify θ- from δ-bursts, and remain continuously present during dark and light periods (Figs. 4–9), under different dominant physiologic states (sleep and wake), and both in control and VLPO-lesioned rats. The presence of multiple scale-invariant characteristics related to distributions, correlations, coupling, and timing of bursting events, is a strong evidence for criticality underlying sleep micro-architecture; a signature of collective behavior over a range of time scales that emerges from neuronal interactions across brain areas. Such critical dynamics characterized by intermittent transitions between active and quiescent states at the integrated cortical level suggests the presence of a non-equilibrium mechanism that regulates sleep micro-architecture on time scales from seconds to an hour.
Our observations provide new insights about the role of the VLPO, and new methods to assess potential abnormal features in the sleep–wake cycle based on the analysis of cortical dynamics. Previous studies have demonstrated that lesions of the central cluster of the VLPO lead to a large decrease in delta power and NREM sleep time, and a fragmentation of the sleep–wake cycle (Lu et al., 2000; Vetrivelan et al., 2012). The results reported here indicate that, despite the decrease in delta power (Vetrivelan et al., 2012), VLPO lesions do not alter the dynamics of δ-bursts. On the other hand, we found that lesions of VLPO neurons significantly affect the temporal organization of θ-bursts. In particular, the power-law exponent characterizing θ-bursts increases in rats with VLPO lesions, indicating a decreased likelihood for long-lasting bursts and a higher probability to observe shorter bursts. This is an evidence of fragmentation of bursting activity in the θ-wave band that parallels the increased fragmentation of sleep in VLPO-lesioned rats. Such deviations of the power-law exponent from the values measured in control rats indicate alteration in the optimal underlying mechanism for sleep regulation, and may be used as a marker of sleep disorders. Importantly, such changes in θ-burst dynamics are indeed associated with a decreased anti-correlation between durations of consecutive θ- and δ-bursts. These findings, alongside with model simulations demonstrating that θ–δ coupling is essential for the emerging scale-invariant temporal organization in these cortical rhythms, indicate that VLPO neurons may have dual role for both sleep and arousal/brief wake activation.
The uncovered complex dynamics in θ and δ rhythms share striking similarities with natural non-equilibrium phenomena exhibiting self-organized criticality (Bak, 1996). In this context, bursts (active states) do not have a characteristic duration, and are separated by quiescent periods whose distribution depends on the details of the system and generally exhibit an exponential tail (Corral, 2004; Lombardi et al., 2014; Scarpetta and de Candia, 2014), and is an exponential for the paradigmatic sandpile model of self-organized criticality (Boffetta et al., 1999). Thus, in non-equilibrium systems exhibiting self-organized criticality, power-law and exponential behavior for active and quiet states coexist, and emerge out of the same regulatory mechanism. The duality of power-law and Weibull distribution in the bursting dynamics of θ and δ rhythms is closely reminiscent of this scenario, where scale-free θ-bursts in cortical activity can be seen as avalanches or earthquakes (active states), whereas δ-bursts can be interpreted as the quiet periods between active states. Following this analogy, we further demonstrated that the quiet times between consecutive θ-burst above a given duration are described by a unique Gamma distribution, the scaling function that also characterizes earthquake dynamics (Corral, 2004).
The interpretation of θ-bursts as active states and δ-bursts as quiet states is consistent with the basic neurophysiological understanding of δ rhythm as the cortical default mode; indeed, lesion and transection experiments have shown that interruption of sensory inputs to the cortex results in a cortical EEG similar to that in NREM sleep (von Economo, 1930; Bremer, 1935, 1937). In contrast, oscillations in the θ band are associated with REM state, arousals, and wakefulness (Boyce et al., 2016; Scammell et al., 2017). Because of the respective amount of wakefulness and REM sleep in our data (Vetrivelan et al., 2012), most of the analyzed θ-bursts are likely associated with arousals and wake. Further, during wakefulness cortical θ waves are desynchronized with a wide range of burst durations, and correspondingly our analyses show that collective cortical activity follows robust scaling laws: power-law distribution for burst durations, long-range power-law correlations in the sequence of burst durations, and a scaling function describing quiet times between bursts.
All these features outline a general picture unifying previous empirical observations of spontaneous neuronal network dynamics at different levels, from networks of dissociated cortical neurons (Pasquale et al., 2008) and local field potentials in cortex slice cultures (Beggs and Plenz, 2003) and awake monkeys (Petermann et al., 2009), to the human brain (Linkenkaer-Hansen et al., 2001; Tagliazucchi et al., 2012; Palva et al., 2013; Shriki et al., 2013; Marinazzo et al., 2014), and the dynamics of sleep-stage and arousal transitions across species (Lo et al., 2002, 2004, 2013; Blumberg et al., 2005; Sorribes et al., 2013), where either distributions or temporal correlations of active events have been studied and discussed in the context of self-organized criticality. Crucially, here we demonstrated presence of the full spectrum of scaling characteristics typical for systems self-organizing at criticality. Furthermore, we linked our observations to the collective behavior of a key sleep-promoting neuronal population leading to emerging cortical rhythms in relation to physiological alternation of sleep and wake. We found that the power-law scaling exponent of the distribution for θ-burst durations in control rats is α ≃ 2.4, close to the power-law exponent for the distribution of neuronal avalanches durations (Beggs and Plenz, 2003; Pasquale et al., 2008), and to α ≃ 2.2 reported for arousal/wake episodes in coarse-grained sleep-stage recordings in humans (Lo et al., 2002) and other species (Lo et al., 2004; Blumberg et al., 2005; Sorribes et al., 2013; Dvir et al., 2018). Importantly, we observed that lesions of VLPO lead to significantly higher values for the θ-burst power-law exponent, an effect that may be interpreted as a transition into a sub-critical behavior (a deviation from emergent criticality due to alteration of the underlying control mechanism)(Lombardi et al., 2012, 2017; Poil et al., 2012).
In summary, the reported duality of power-law and Weibull distribution and the scaling features for a full spectrum of dynamic characteristics in the bursting activity of θ and δ rhythms strongly support the hypothesis of an underlying critical dynamics for sleep regulation. These evidences lay the foundation of a new paradigm for the investigation of sleep dynamics and sleep-stage transitions mechanisms, considering sleep micro-architecture as result of a non-equilibrium process and self-organization among neuronal assemblies to maintain a critical state (a behavior that is in stark contrast to the homeostasis paradigm of sleep regulation at large time scales). Within the criticality framework arousals/brief wake and sleep-stage dynamics emerge out of a common regulatory mechanism, where arousals play an essential part in maintaining a non-equilibrium behavior at criticality, as suggested by our observations of coupling between consecutive θ- and δ-bursts durations. Systems at criticality exhibit high susceptibility and sensitivity to interactions, leading to cooperative behaviors over a range of space/time scales, and thus maintaining a critical state is important for system's flexibility and for generating spontaneous transitions (Ansmann et al., 2016; Lehnertz et al., 2018). Such transitions cannot occur intrinsically in a homeostatic (equilibrium) system. In the context of sleep micro-architecture, the proposed criticality-based paradigm may provide new insights on the origin and mechanisms underlying the dynamics of sleep-stage and arousal transitions, and offers a unifying picture of sleep and wake.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (Grant 1R01-HL098437), the US-Israel Binational Science Foundation (Grant 2012219), the Office of Naval Research (Grant 000141010078), the Keck Foundation, the Spanish Junta de Andalucia (FQM-7964), the Marie Sklodowska-Curie Grant (754411), NIH-NIA (AG09975) and the NIH-NHLBI (HL095461).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Achermann P, Borbély AA (2003) Mathematical models of sleep regulation. Front Biosci 8:S683–S693. 10.2741/1064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amaral DG, Price JL (1983) An air pressure system for the injection of tracer substances into the brain. J Neurosci Methods 9:35–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ansmann G, Lehnertz K, Feudel U (2016) Self-induced switchings between multiple space-time patterns on complex networks of excitable units. Phys Rev X 6:011030 10.1103/PhysRevX.6.011030 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bak P. (1996) How nature works. New York: Copernicus. [Google Scholar]
- Beggs JM, Plenz D (2003) Neuronal avalanches in neocortical circuits. J Neurosci 23:11167–11177. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-35-11167.2003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behn CG, Brown EN, Scammell TE, Kopell NJ (2007) Mathematical model of network dynamics governing mouse sleep–wake behavior. J Neurophysiol 97:3828–3840. 10.1152/jn.01184.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumberg MS, Seelke AM, Lowen SB, Karlsson KA (2005) Dynamics of sleep–wake cyclicity in developing rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:14860–14864. 10.1073/pnas.0506340102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boffetta G, Carbone V, Giuliani P, Veltri P, Vulpiani A (1999) Power laws in solar flares: self-organized criticality or turbulence? Phys Rev Lett 83:4662 10.1103/PhysRevLett.83.4662 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Borbély AA, Achermann P (1999) Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep regulation. J Biol Rhythms 14:559–570. 10.1177/074873099129000894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyce R, Glasgow SD, Williams S, Adamantidis A (2016) Causal evidence for the role of rem sleep theta rhythm in contextual memory consolidation. Science 352:812–816. 10.1126/science.aad5252 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bremer F. (1935) Cerveau “Isole” et physiologie du sommeil. Comptes rendus de la Societe de Biologie 118:1235–1241. [Google Scholar]
- Bremer F. (1937) L' activite cerebrale au cours du sommeil et de la narcose. Contribution a l'etude mecanistique du sommeil. Academie royale de medecine de Belgique 2:68–86. [Google Scholar]
- Brown RE, Basheer R, McKenna JT, Strecker RE, McCarley RW (2012) Control of sleep and wakefulness. Physiol Rev 92:1087–1187. 10.1152/physrev.00032.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen Z, Ivanov PC, Hu K, Stanley HE (2002) Effect of nonstationarities on detrended fluctuation analysis. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 65:041107. 10.1103/PhysRevE.65.041107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen Z, Hu K, Carpena P, Bernaola-Galvan P, Stanley HE, Ivanov PC (2005) Effect of nonlinear filters on detrended fluctuation analysis. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 71:011104. 10.1103/PhysRevE.71.011104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chialvo D. (2010) Emergent complex neural dynamics. Nat Physics 6:744–750. 10.1038/nphys1803 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Corral Á. (2004) Long-term clustering, scaling, and universality in the temporal occurrence of earthquakes. Phys Rev Lett 92:108501. 10.1103/PhysRevLett.92.108501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corral Á. (2006) Dependence of earthquake recurrence times and independence of magnitudes on seismicity history. Tectonophysics 424:177–193. 10.1016/j.tecto.2006.03.035 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Daley DJ, Vere-Jones D (1988) An introduction to the theory of point processes. New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- de Arcangelis L, Godano C, Grasso JR, Lippiello E (2016) Statistical physics approach to earthquake occurrence and forecasting. Phys Rep 628:1–91. 10.1016/j.physrep.2016.03.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dvir H, Elbaz I, Havlin S, Appelbaum L, Ivanov PC, Bartsch RP (2018) Neuronal noise as an origin of sleep arousals and its role in sudden infant death syndrome. Sci Adv 4:eaar6277. 10.1126/sciadv.aar6277 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franken P, Dijk DJ, Tobler I, Borbély AA (1991) Sleep deprivation in rats: effects on EEG power spectra, vigilance states, and cortical temperature. Am J Physiol 261:R198–R208. 10.1152/ajpregu.1991.261.1.R198 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberger AL, Amaral LA, Hausdorff JM, Ivanov PCh, Peng CK, Stanley HE (2002) Fractal dynamics in physiology: alterations with disease and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:2466–2472. 10.1073/pnas.012579499 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halász P. (1998) Hierarchy of micro-arousals and the microstructure of sleep. Neurophysiol Clin 28:461–475. 10.1016/S0987-7053(99)80016-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirshkowitz M. (2002) Arousals and anti-arousals. Sleep Med 3:203–204. 10.1016/S1389-9457(02)00018-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu K, Ivanov PC, Chen Z, Carpena P, Stanley HE (2001) Effect of trends on detrended fluctuation analysis. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 64:011114. 10.1103/PhysRevE.64.011114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu K, Ivanov PC, Hilton MF, Chen Z, Ayers RT, Stanley HE, Shea SA (2004) Endogenous circadian rhythm in an index of cardiac vulnerability independent of changes in behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:18223–18227. 10.1073/pnas.0408243101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivanov PC. (2007) Scale-invariant aspects of cardiac dynamics across sleep stages and circadian phases. IEEE Eng Med Biol Mag 26:33–37. 10.1109/EMB.2007.907093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivanov PC, Rosenblum MG, Peng CK, Mietus J, Havlin S, Stanley HE, Goldberger AL (1996) Scaling behaviour of heartbeat intervals obtained by wavelet-based time-series analysis. Nature 383:323–327. 10.1038/383323a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivanov PC, Amaral LA, Goldberger AL, Havlin S, Rosenblum MG, Struzik ZR, Stanley HE (1999) Multifractality in human heartbeat dynamics. Nature 399:461–465. 10.1038/20924 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kantelhardt J, Havlin S, Ivanov PC (2003) Modeling transient correlations in heartbeat dynamics during sleep. Europhys Lett 62:147 10.1209/epl/i2003-00332-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kopell N, Ermentrout GB, Whittington MA, Traub RD (2000) Gamma rhythms and beta rhythms have different synchronization properties. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:1867–1872. 10.1073/pnas.97.4.1867 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehnertz K, Zabawa L, Tabar MRR (2018) Characterizing abrupt transitions in stochastic dynamics. New J Physics 20:113043 10.1088/1367-2630/aaf0d7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Linkenkaer-Hansen K, Nikouline VV, Palva JM, IImoniemi RJ (2001) Long-range temporal correlations and scaling behavior in human brain oscillations. J Neurosci 21:1370–1377. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-04-01370.2001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu KK, Bartsch RP, Lin A, Mantegna RN, Ivanov PC (2015) Plasticity of brain wave network interactions and evolution across physiologic states. Front Neural Circuits 9:62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lombardi F, Herrmann HJ, Perrone-Capano C, Plenz D, de Arcangelis L (2012) Balance between excitation and inhibition controls the temporal organization of neuronal avalanches. Phys Rev Lett 108:228703. 10.1103/PhysRevLett.108.228703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lombardi F, Herrmann HJ, Plenz D, de Arcangelis L (2014) On the temporal organization of neuronal avalanches. Front Syst Neurosci 8:204. 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00204 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lombardi F, Herrmann HJ, de Arcangelis L (2017) Balance between excitation and inhibition determines 1/f power spectrum in neuronal networks. Chaos 27:047402. 10.1063/1.4979043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lo CC, Amaral LN, Havlin S, Ivanov PC, Penzel T, Peter JH, Stanley HE (2002) Dynamics of sleep–wake transitions during sleep. Europhys Lett 57:625 10.1209/epl/i2002-00508-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lo CC, Chou T, Penzel T, Scammell TE, Strecker RE, Stanley HE, Ivanov PC (2004) Common scale-invariant patterns of sleep–wake transitions across mammalian species. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:17545–17548. 10.1073/pnas.0408242101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lo CC, Bartsch RP, Ivanov PC (2013) Asymmetry and basic pathways in sleep-stage transitions. Europhys Lett 102:10008. 10.1209/0295-5075/102/10008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu J, Greco MA, Shiromani P, Saper CB (2000) Effect of lesions of the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus on NREM and REM sleep. J Neurosci 20:3830–3842. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-10-03830.2000 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu J, Bjorkum AA, Xu M, Gaus SE, Shiromani PJ, Saper CB (2002) Selective activation of the extended ventrolateral preoptic nucleus during rapid eye movement sleep. J Neurosci 22:4568–4576. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-11-04568.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marinazzo D, Pellicoro M, Wu G, Angelini L, Cortés JM, Stramaglia S (2014) Information transfer and criticality in the Ising model on the human connectome. PLoS One 9:e93616. 10.1371/journal.pone.0093616 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Munoz MA. (2018) Colloqium: criticality and dynamical scaling in living systems. Rev Mod Phys 90:031001 10.1103/RevModPhys.90.031001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Olbrich E, Achermann P, Wennekers T (2011) The sleeping brain as a complex system. Philos Trans A Math Phys Eng Sci 369:3697–3707. 10.1098/rsta.2011.0199 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paczuski M, Boettcher S, Baiesi M (2005) Interoccurrence times in the Bak–Tang–Wiesenfeld Sandpile model: a comparison with the observed statistics of solar flares. Phys Rev Lett 95:181102. 10.1103/PhysRevLett.95.181102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palva JM, Zhigalov A, Hirvonen J, Korhonen O, Linkenkaer-Hansen K, Palva S (2013) Neuronal long-range temporal correlations and avalanche dynamics are correlated with behavioral scaling laws. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110:3585–3590. 10.1073/pnas.1216855110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pasquale V, Massobrio P, Bologna LL, Chiappalone M, Martinoia S (2008) Self-organization and neuronal avalanches in networks of dissociated cortical neurons. Neuroscience 153:1354–1369. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.03.050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G, Watson C (2004) The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. 5 ed. Amsterdam; Boston: Elsevier Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Peng CK, Buldyrev SV, Havlin S, Simons M, Stanley HE, Goldberger AL (1994) Mosaic organization of DNA nucleotides. Phys Rev E Stat Phys Plasmas Fluids Relat Interdiscip Topics 49:1685–1689. 10.1103/PhysRevE.49.1685 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petermann T, Thiagarajan TC, Lebedev MA, Nicolelis MA, Chialvo DR, Plenz D (2009) Spontaneous cortical activity in awake monkeys composed of neuronal avalanches. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:15921–15926. 10.1073/pnas.0904089106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poil SS, Hardstone R, Mansvelder HD, Linkenkaer-Hansen K (2012) Critical-state dynamics of avalanches and oscillations jointly emerge from balanced excitation/inhibition in neuronal networks. J Neurosci 32:9817–9823. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5990-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pruessner G. (2012) Self-organised criticality: theory, models and characterisation. Cambridge UK: Cambridge UP. [Google Scholar]
- Ribeiro TL, Copelli M, Caixeta F, Belchior H, Chialvo DR, Nicolelis MA, Ribeiro S (2010) Spike avalanches exhibit universal dynamics across the sleep–wake cycle. PLoS One 5:e14129. 10.1371/journal.pone.0014129 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodriguez AV, Funk CM, Vyazovskiy VV, Nir Y, Tononi G, Cirelli C (2016) Slow-wave activity increase after extended wake? Assessing the effects of increased cortical firing during wake and sleep. J Neurosci 36:12436–12447. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1614-16.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakurai T. (2007) The neural circuit of orexin (hypocretin): maintaining sleep and wakefulness. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:171–181. 10.1038/nrn2092 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saper CB, Chou TC, Scammell TE (2001) The sleep switch: hypothalamic control of sleep and wakefulness. Trends Neurosci 24:726–731. 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)02002-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saper CB, Cano G, Scammell TE (2005) Homeostatic, circadian, and emotional regulation of sleep. J Comp Neurol 493:92–98. 10.1002/cne.20770 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saper CB, Fuller PM, Pedersen NP, Lu J, Scammell TE (2010) Sleep state switching. Neuron 68:1023–1042. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scammell TE, Arrigoni E, Lipton JO (2017) Neural circuitry of wakefulness and sleep. Neuron 93:747–765. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.01.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scarpetta S, de Candia A (2014) Alternation of up and down states at a dynamical phase-transition of a neural network with spatiotemporal attractors. Front Syst Neurosci 8:88. 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitt DT, Stein PK, Ivanov PC (2009) Stratification pattern of static and scale-invariant dynamic measures of heartbeat fluctuations across sleep stages in young and elderly. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 56:1564–1573. 10.1109/TBME.2009.2014819 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherin JE, Shiromani PJ, McCarley RW, Saper CB (1996) Activation of ventrolateral preoptic neurons during sleep. Science 271:216–219. 10.1126/science.271.5246.216 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shriki O, Alstott J, Carver F, Holroyd T, Hanson RN, Smith ML, Coppola R, Bullmore E, Plenz D (2013) Neuronal avalanches in the resting meg of the human brain. J Neurosci 33:7079–7090. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4286-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sorribes A, Thornorsteinsson H, Porsteinsson H, Arnardóttir H, Jóhannesdóttir IÞ, Sigurgeirsson B, De Polavieja GG, Karlsson KÆ (2013) The ontogeny of sleep–wake cycles in zebrafish: a comparison to humans. Front Neural Circuits 7:178. 10.3389/fncir.2013.00178 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stacy EW. (1962) A generalization of the gamma distribution. Ann Math Stat 33:1187–1192. 10.1214/aoms/1177704481 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stanley H. (1987) Introduction to phase transitions and critical phenomena. New York: Oxford UP. [Google Scholar]
- Steriade M, Nuñez A, Amzica F (1993) A novel slow (<1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neurons in vivo: depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J Neurosci 13:3252–3265. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-08-03252.1993 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tagliazucchi E, Balenzuela P, Fraiman D, Chialvo DR (2012) Criticality in large-scale brain fMRI dynamics unveiled by a novel point process analysis. Front Physiol 3:15. 10.3389/fphys.2012.00015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taqqu MS, Teverovsky V, Willinger W (1995) Estimators for long-range dependence: an empirical study. Fractals 3:785–798. 10.1142/S0218348X95000692 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tobler I, Borbély AA (1986) Sleep eeg in the rat as a function of prior waking. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 64:74–76. 10.1016/0013-4694(86)90044-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trachsel L, Tobler I, Achermann P, Borbély AA (1991) Sleep continuity and the rem-nonREM cycle in the rat under baseline conditions and after sleep deprivation. Physiol Behav 49:575–580. 10.1016/0031-9384(91)90283-T [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vetrivelan R, Fuller PM, Yokota S, Lu J, Saper CB (2012) Metabolic effects of chronic sleep restriction in rats. Sleep 35:1511–1520. 10.5665/sleep.2200 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Economo C. (1930) Sleep as a problem of localization. J Nerv Ment Dis 71:1–5. 10.1097/00005053-193001000-00001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Welch P. (1967) The use of fast Fourier transform for the estimation of power spectra: a method based on time averaging over short, modified periodograms. IEEE Trans Audio Electroacoust 15:70–73. 10.1109/TAU.1967.1161901 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Xu L, Ivanov PC, Hu K, Chen Z, Carbone A, Stanley HE (2005) Quantifying signals with power-law correlations: a comparative study of detrended fluctuation analysis and detrended moving average techniques. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 71:051101. 10.1103/PhysRevE.71.051101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]