Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Med. 2020 Jan 7;50(3):367–383. doi: 10.1017/S003329171900391X

Characterizing the phenomenology of passive suicidal ideation: A meta-analysis of its prevalence, psychiatric comorbidity, correlates, and comparisons with active suicidal ideation

Richard T Liu 1, Alexandra H Bettis 1, Taylor A Burke 1
PMCID: PMC7024002  NIHMSID: NIHMS1067912  PMID: 31907085

Abstract

Background:

Compared to active ideation, passive ideation remains relatively understudied and its clinical importance poorly defined. The weight that should be accorded passive ideation in clinical risk assessment is therefore unclear.

Methods:

We conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of the prevalence of passive ideation, its psychiatric comorbidity, associated sociodemographic characteristics, as well as psychological and environmental correlates. For reference, pooled effects were also calculated for direct comparisons of passive and active ideation with respect to potential correlates. Relevant articles published since inception to September 9, 2019, were identified through a systematic search of MEDLINE and PsycINFO.

Results:

A total of 86 studies were included in this review. The prevalence of passive ideation was high across sample types, ranging from 5.8% for one-year prevalence to 10.6% for lifetime prevalence in the general population. Passive ideation was strongly associated with sexual minority status, psychiatric comorbidity, psychological characteristics implicated in risk, and suicide attempts. Preliminary evidence exists for a large association with suicide deaths. The effects sizes for individual correlates of passive and active ideation were largely equivalent and mostly non-significant in head-to-head comparisons.

Conclusions:

Passive ideation is a prevalent clinical phenomenon associated with significant psychiatric comorbidity. Current evidence also suggests notable similarities exist between passive and active ideation in terms of psychiatric comorbidity and psychological and other characteristics traditionally associated with risk.

Keywords: epidemiology, meta-analysis, suicidal ideation, suicide

Introduction

Suicide is one of the leading causes of death worldwide and reducing the rate of its occurrence is a global imperative (Lozano et al., 2012; World Health Organization, 2014). One of the top three risk factors for suicide deaths (Franklin et al., 2017), and an important clinical concern in its own right, is suicidal ideation. Understanding the phenomenology and etiology of suicidal thoughts is therefore essential for advancing strategies to assess risk for and to prevent suicidal behavior. Suicidal ideation ranges in severity from passive ideation (i.e., a desire to be dead) to active ideation (i.e., a desire to kill oneself). The idea that passive ideation is of comparably moderate clinical severity may often lead to the conclusion in suicide risk assessments that individuals presenting with passive ideation are at relatively lower risk than those with active ideation for engaging in suicidal behavior, and to decisions regarding clinical care being made accordingly (Jacobs, 2009; National Action Alliance for Suicide Prevention, 2014; Simon, 2014). It is uncertain, however, to what degree this position is empirically supported. Moreover, in a recent report commissioned by the National Institute of Mental Health, the National Action Alliance for Suicide Prevention has put forth the view that passive ideation may in fact be comparable to active ideation in its association with negative mental health outcomes, including suicidal behavior (National Action Alliance for Suicide Prevention, 2014).

Hindering critical evaluation of this issue is the relative want of research on passive ideation, especially compared to the considerable body of empirical literature on active ideation. Indeed, prevalence estimates of suicidal ideation in several of the most important epidemiological surveys to date (e.g., the World Health Organization World Mental Health [WMH] Survey Initiative; (WHO World Mental Health Survey Consortium, 2004)] as well as the National Comorbidity Survey Replication and Adolescent Supplement [NCS-R and NCS-A; Husky et al., 2012; Kessler, Berglund, Borges, Nock, & Wang, 2005; Nock et al., 2013]) are in actuality of active ideation only. That passive ideation has received less attention in clinical research is also reflected by the fact that several of the most essential measures of depression and suicide include assessments of active but not passive ideation (e.g., Beck et al. 1996; Osman et al. 2001; Kessler & Üstün 2004; Kovacs 2010). Consequently, several fundamental aspects of passive ideation remain poorly understood. This relative neglect of passive ideation in the empirical literature is doubtless in some measure a reflection of the greater clinical importance accorded active ideation in comparison to passive ideation. The clinical weight of passive ideation is therefore unclear, and insofar as passive ideation is clinically important, the scale of suicidal ideation as a public health problem is likely to be significantly underestimated in epidemiological data.

As an important step toward characterizing the phenomenology of passive ideation and its clinical significance, the current review provides a quantitative synthesis systematically characterizing the empirical literature to date on passive ideation. First, pooled estimates of the prevalence of passive ideation were calculated for different time-frames (e.g., current/past week and lifetime) and populations (e.g., psychiatric and community). Pooled effects were derived for sociodemographic, clinical, psychological, and other correlates of passive ideation. For each of these correlates, corresponding pooled effects from the same studies were also calculated for active ideation, thereby to facilitate comparisons between passive and active ideation. For this reason too, pooled effects were also calculated whenever possible for direct comparisons between active and passive ideation with respect to correlates of interest. Thus, we aimed to address the following, with active ideation serving as a reference point: (i) what are the sociodemographic characteristics associated with passive ideation; (ii) the psychiatric comorbidity of passive ideation, thereby providing a preliminary evaluation of the clinical importance of passive ideation; and (iii) the strength of psychological and environmental correlates of passive ideation, thereby identifying candidate risk factors to be evaluated in future research ultimately to inform future risk identification and clinical intervention strategies.

Method

Search strategy and eligibility criteria

A systematic search of the literature was conducted in PsycINFO and MEDLINE to identify studies of potential relevance to the current review published from inception to September 9, 2019. The following search string was applied: (passive and suicide*) OR “passive SI” OR “desire for death” OR “suicidal desire” OR “death ideation” OR “thought of death” OR “thoughts of death.” The search results were limited to: (i) English-language publications and (ii) peer-reviewed journals. This search strategy yielded a total of 2,192 articles, of which 1,668 were unique reports. In cases where the eligibility of an article could not be ruled out based on the title and abstract, the full text was also examined.

The study inclusion criteria were: (i) passive ideation was analyzed separately from other constructs (e.g., active ideation); (ii) passive ideation was assessed systematically over a standardized time-frame; and (iii) quantitative data were presented on either the prevalence of passive ideation or its correlates. In the case of studies where more information on the measurement of the constructs of interest was needed to determine study eligibility, every effort was made to obtain additional details in other publications describing the measure (e.g., other publications based on the same dataset) or by contacting the corresponding author.

Data extraction

We extracted data for six study characteristics. These included three sample characteristics: (i) mean age of sample, (ii) sample type (i.e., epidemiological, community, at-risk/mixed/medical, and psychiatric), and (iii) percentage of female participants in the study sample. Data for three study design characteristics were also extracted: (i) suicidal ideation instrument (i.e., self-report versus interview), (ii) time-frame of suicidal ideation assessment, and (iii) whether an assessment of “pure” passive suicidal ideation was used (i.e., passive ideation without co-occurring active ideation).

Data analysis

All analyses were conducted with Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Version 3.3.070 (Biostat, 2014). The standardized mean difference (SMD; Cohen’s d) was used as the primary index of effect size for analyses of potential correlates of passive and active ideation, respectively. An SMD of .20 is interpreted as a small effect size, .50 as medium, and .80 as large (Kraemer et al., 2003). All pooled effects were calculated such that values greater than zero reflected a positive association between the correlate of interest and suicidal ideation (i.e., the correlate is associated with greater experiences of suicidal ideation). For analyses directly comparing passive to active ideation with respect to a given correlate of interest, effect sizes were calculated such that values larger than zero indicated that the correlate was more strongly associated with active than passive ideation. Weighted effect sizes were calculated by pooling effects across all relevant studies. For all analyses, random-effects models were generated in preference to fixed-effects models, so as to account for the high expected heterogeneity across studies resulting from differences in samples, measures, and design. Random-effects models are more appropriate than fixed-effects models in cases where high heterogeneity is observed, in that they account for this heterogeneity by incorporating both sampling and study-level errors, with the pooled effect size representing the mean of a distribution of true effect sizes instead of a single true effect size. In contrast, fixed-effects models estimate only within-study variance, as it assumes that a single true effect size exists across all studies and any variance detected is due strictly to sampling error. Heterogeneity across the studies was evaluated using the I2 statistic. I2 indicates the percentage of the variance in an effect estimate that is due to heterogeneity across studies rather than sampling error (i.e., chance). Low heterogeneity is indicated by I2 values of around 25%, and moderate heterogeneity by I2 values of 50%. Substantial heterogeneity that is due to real differences in study samples and methodology is indicated by an I2 value of 75%, which suggests that the observed heterogeneity is more than would be expected with random error (Higgins, Thompson, Deeks, & Altman, 2003).

High heterogeneity indicates the need to conduct moderator analyses to account for potential sources of this heterogeneity. Each potential moderator was assessed individually, with the effect size at each level of the moderator estimated. Additionally, given that passive and active ideation often co-occur, moderator analysis was conducted to determine whether the prevalence estimates for passive ideation differed as a function of whether individual studies assessed “pure” passive ideation.

A common concern in conducting meta-analyses is the possibility of publication bias. Studies with small effect sizes or non-significant findings are less likely to be published, and thus may be more likely to be excluded from meta-analyses, resulting in a potentially inflated estimate of the overall effect size. The following publication bias indices were calculated to assess for the presence of potential publication bias: Duval and Tweedie’s trim-and-fill analysis (Duval & Tweedie, 2000) and Egger’s regression intercept (Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider, & Minder, 1997). Duval and Tweedie’s trim-and-fill analysis produces an estimate of the number of missing studies based on asymmetry in a funnel plot of the standard error of each study in a meta-analysis (based on the study’s sample size) against the study’s effect size. This analysis also calculates an effect size estimate and confidence interval, adjusting for these missing studies. It is important to note that this procedure assumes homogeneity of effect sizes, and thus, its results must be interpreted with a degree of caution in cases where significant heterogeneity is present. Egger’s regression intercept also provides an estimate of potential publication bias using a linear regression approach assessing study effect sizes relative to their standard error.

Results

Based on the inclusion criteria, we excluded 1,231 reports based on their titles and abstracts. After this initial screen, 344 additional reports were excluded based on a detailed full-text review, resulting in 93 studies satisfying the eligibility criteria. Additional data required for meta-analysis were obtained from the authors of seven of these studies (Barrigón et al., 2017; Glassmire, Tarescavage, Burchett, Martinez, & Gomez, 2016; Guidry & Cukrowicz, 2016; Handley, Adams, Manly, Cicchetti, & Toth, 2019; Jahn, Poindexter, Graham, & Cukrowicz, 2012; Rufino, Viswanath, Wagner, & Patriquin, 2018; Tal et al., 2017). Five studies included prevalence data for which not enough unique effects existed for meta-analysis (e.g., six days) and were thus excluded from this review. For studies with overlapping samples, determination of which study to include in the meta-analysis was based, in descending order, on: (i) inclusion of sufficient reported data for meta-analysis and (ii) largest sample size for the relevant analysis. In cases where two studies used overlapping samples but examined different associations (e.g., passive ideation with different correlates), both studies were retained for the relevant analyses. Whenever it remained unclear after full-text inspection whether two studies reported on overlapping samples, the study authors were contacted to seek clarity on this issue. Seventeen studies featured overlapping samples and two were excluded at this stage, resulting in a final set of 86 studies included in the current review (see Figure 1 and Table 1). Only three of these studies featured longitudinal analyses with passive ideation as an outcome (Nrugham, Larsson, & Sund, 2008; Raue, Meyers, Rowe, Heo, & Bruce, 2007; Schimanski, Mouat, Billinghurst, & Linscott, 2017), precluding consideration of this design feature in analyses of correlates.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

PRISMA flow chart of literature search

Table 1.

Study characteristics

Study Author(s) (year) Na % Femalea Mean Agea Sample Type Passive Suicidal Ideation
Measure(s) Format Time Frame(s)
Alaimo et al. (2002) 754 49.30 15.53 Community DIS I Lifetime
Allen et al. (2013)b 1,068 57.70 --- Medical C-SSRS I 2 Weeks
Ashrafioun et al. (2018) 17,176 11.27 49.90 Psychiatric PSS Q 1 Year
Ayalon & Litwin (2009)1 1,712 48.40 --- Community Euro-D Q 1 Month
Ayalon et al. (2007)2,c 15,590 34.84 74.00 Community SSM Q 2 Weeks
Baca-Garcia et al. (2011) Sample 1 43,093 --- --- Community AUDADIS-IV I Lifetime
Baca-Garcia et al. (2011) Sample 23 42,862 --- --- Community AUDADIS-IV I Lifetime
Barrigón et al. (2017) 13,811 62.04 47.20 Psychiatric SSM Q 1 Month
Barry et al. (2016)b 124 37.10 56.40 At-risk GSIS Q Current
Bartels et al. (2002)2,c 2,240 23.95 --- Psychiatric PSS Q 1 Year
Bingham et al. (2017) 258 64.34 58.00 Psychiatric BSIS I 1 Week
Briggs et al. (2018)b 7,055 54.66 64.97 Community SSM Q 1 Month
Caetano et al. (2006) 43 41.86 11.20 Psychiatric K-SADS I Lifetime
Castle et al. (2004)b 5,005 36.98 --- Mixed SSM I 1 Month
Charles et al. (2003) 1,271 100.00 --- At-risk DIS I Lifetime
Coêlho et al. (2010) 1,464 --- --- Community CIDI I Lifetime
Cohen et al. (2008) 1,074 62.10 67.71 Community SSM Q Lifetime
Cook et al. (2002) 835 76.89 73.09 Community GHQ Q Current
Cottler et al. (2005) 990 31.92 32.20 Psychiatric PSS Q Lifetime
Cukrowicz et al. (2013)4 239 60.25 72.40 Community GSIS Q Current
de Graaf et al. (2006)5 5,998 52.02 40.59 Community CIDI I Lifetime
Dell’Osso et al. (2009)b 243 62.55 36.13 Mixed MOODS-SR Q Lifetime
Dennis et al. (2007) 8,580 55.10 --- Community SSM I 1 Week, 1 Year
Deykin & Buka (1994) 300 25.33 16.65 Psychiatric DIS I Lifetime
Draper et al. (1998) 221 56.56 71.60 Psychiatric HDRS I 1 Week
Dutta et al. (2017)b 5,922 54.07 37.23 Community SSM Q Lifetime
Estrada et al. (2019) 171 33.90 15.93 At-Risk C-SSRS I 1 Month, Lifetime
Farrell & Ganzini (1995) 63 2.99 70.57 Psychiatric SSM Q Current
Flint et al. (1998) 79 0 55.80 Psychiatric DIS I 1 Year
Fu-I & Wang (2008) 58 53.45 12.80 Psychiatric DICA I 1 Month
Ghanizadeh et al. (2006) 110 46.40 14.60 Medical K-SADS I 1 Year
Glassmire et al. (2016) 229 36.20 41.99 Psychiatric MMPI-2 Q Current
Guidry & Cukrowicz (2016)4 148 50.33 73.58 Community GSIS Q Current
Handley et al. (2019) 164 100.00 14.00 Psychiatric K-SADS I 2 Months
Heisel & Flett (2006) 107 75.70 81.50 Mixed GSIS Q Current
Heisel & Flett (2016) 173 70.52 73.90 Community GSIS Q Current
Herrell et al. (1999) 6,656 0 --- At-risk DIS I Lifetime
Jahn et al. (2012)4 272 60.66 72.89 Community GSIS Q Current
Kienhorst et al. (1990) 9,393 42.08 17.30 Community SSM Q Current
Kim et al. (2006) 355 75.77 75.29 Community CIDI I 1 Year
Kliem et al. (2017) 2,450 53.90 50.51 Community BSIS Q 1 Week
Lapierre et al. (2012)b 2,777 59.09 73.80 Community SSM I 1 Year
Linden & Barnow (1997)b 516 --- --- Community HDRS I 1 Week
Liu et al. (2006) 553 44.80 11.70 Psychiatric ISCA-D I 1 Month, Lifetime
Lutz et al. (2016)1 35,664 55.78 64.88 Community Euro-D Q 1 Month
Lytle et al. (2015) 24,121 62.28 24.98 Community SSM Q 1 Year
Magni et al. (1998) 4,964 57.09 --- Community DIS I Lifetime
Malfent et al. (2010) 129 82.90 80.30 Community SSM Q Lifetime
Manu et al. (1991)b 200 64.00 39.25 Medical DIS I Lifetime
May et al. (2015) 140 12.14 53.20 Psychiatric PSIS Q Current
McBride et al. (2016)3 1,523 --- --- At-risk AUDADIS-IV I Lifetime
Meerwijk & Weiss (2018) 218 56.42 32.30 At-risk SSM Q 1 Week
Mendonca & Holden (1996) 97 47.42 29.90 Psychiatric BSIS Q Current
Mitchell et al. (2017) 104 58.65 36.92 Psychiatric BSIS Q 1 Month
Mitchell et al. (in press) 318 67.30 27.16 Psychiatric BSIS Q 1 Week
Naidoo & Collings (2017) 239 67.36 36.49 Psychiatric BSIS Q Current
Nazem et al. (2008) 116 25 66.70 Medical PSIS Q 1 Month
Neeleman et al. (2004)5 5,618 --- --- Community CIDI I 1 Year
Nrugham et al. (2008) 345 72.50 14.90 Mixed K-SADS I Lifetime
O’Riley et al. (2014)6 377 31.83 77.00 Community PSS Q 1 Year
Pawluk et al. (1995) 11 45.45 54.50 Medical SSM I Current
Rabkin et al. (1993) Study 1 52 0 39.00 Medical DIS I Lifetime
Rabkin et al. (1993) Study 2 464 0 39.00 Community DIS I Lifetime
Raue et al. (2007)b 539 65.10 78.40 Community SCID, HRSD I 1 Month, 1 Year
Robertson et al. (2006) 246 28.05 35.22 Mixed DIS I Lifetime
Romans et al. (2007) 1,758 62.17 --- Psychiatric CIDI I 1 Year
Roth et al. (2011) 22,962 55.74 --- Community SSM I 1 Year
Rufino et al. (2018) 432 78.20 28.59 Psychiatric C-SSRS I 1 Month
Saïas et al. (2012)1 11,440 58.90 --- Community Euro-D Q 1 Month
Schimanski et al. (2017) 47 70.21 14.93 Mixed APS-SF Q 2 Years
Scocco et al. (2001) 611 63.01 75.70 Community PSS Q 1 Month, 1 Year, Lifetime
Segal et al. (2015) 109 60.55 71.40 Community GSIS Q Current
Smith et al. (2004) 153 56.60 44.70 Medical SCISH-CP I Lifetime
Smith et al. (2012) 106 100 35.00 Psychiatric HRSD I 1 Week
Snyder et al. (1973) 119 94.96 --- Community SCS Q 1 Week, 1 Month, 1 Year
Storch et al. (2017) 101 55.45 31.07 Psychiatric C-SSRS I 1 Month, Lifetime
Tal et al. (2017) 395 77.97 52.95 Psychiatric C-SSRS I Lifetime
Tateno et al. (2018) 1,980 --- --- Community SIBQ Q Lifetime
Undheim (2013) 2,362 50.81 13.70 Community MFQ Q 2 Weeks
Urrila et al. (2012) 166 82.53 16.50 Psychiatric K-SADS I Current
van Duijn et al. (2018) 1,451 54.83 48.40 Medical C-SSRS I Lifetime
Van Orden et al. (2013) 345 70.30 --- Community PSS Q Lifetime
Van Orden et al. (2015)6 377 31.83 77.00 Community PSS Q 1 Year
Vera et al. (2011) 2,068 75.90 --- Medical MINI I 1 Month
Villa et al. (2018) 162 47.60 50.60 Psychiatric C-SSRS I Lifetime
Walker et al. (2011) 4,506 --- --- Medical SSM I 1 Month
Yoder et al. (2008) 444 56.31 17.33 At-risk SSM Q 1 Year
Zhou et al. (2015) 656 0 67.00 Medical SSM Q 1 Year

Note. APS-SF = Adolescent Psychopathology Scale – Short Form; AUDADIS-IV = The Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Disabilities Interview Schedule-IV; BSIS = Beck Scale for Suicide Ideation; CIDI = Composite International Diagnostic Interview; C-SSRS = Columbia-Suicide Severity Rating Scale; DICA = Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents; DIS = Diagnostic Interview Schedule; GHQ = General Health Questionnaire; GSSI = Geriatric Scale for Suicide Ideation; HRSD = Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; ISCA-D = Interview Schedule for Children & Adolescents – Diagnostic Version; K-SADS = Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia; MINI = Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview; MMPI-2 = Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory; MOODS-SR = Mood Spectrum Self-Report; PSS = Paykel Suicide Scale; PSIS = Passive Suicide Ideation Scale; SCISH-CP = Structured Clinical Interview for Suicide History in Chronic Pain; SCS = Social Concerns Scale; SIBQ = Suicide Ideation and Behavior Questionnaire; SSM = study-specific measure

I = Interview; Q = Questionnaire

1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6

Studies with identical superscripts were drawn from same or overlapping samples but presented unique data included in this review.

a

The sample size, mean age, and percentage female for participants included in relevant analyses, rather than of the entire study sample, are presented and were incorporated in moderator analyses whenever available. For ease of presentation, whenever the sample size, mean age, or percentage female varied across multiple relevant analyses within a study, data for the cumulative number of unique participants across these analyses are presented here, and the sample size used in each analysis was retained in the relevant meta-analysis for purposes of obtaining weighted effect sizes.

b

These studies allowed for estimates of prevalence of passive ideation in a clinical subgroup of the full study sample.

c

These studies drew from a sample that overlaps with those of others included in this review but conducted analyses with a different measure of suicidal ideation.

Prevalence of passive ideation

Pooled prevalence rates for passive ideation were calculated for four different sample types (epidemiological, community, at-risk/mixed/medical, and psychiatric) across four different time-frames (current/one-week, one month, one year, and lifetime). Epidemiological samples were a subset of those included in analyses of community samples. The prevalence of passive ideation was highest in psychiatric samples, ranging from 33.57% for current/one-week ideation to 47.03% for lifetime ideation, and lowest in epidemiological samples, ranging from 2.35% for current/one-week ideation to 10.57% for lifetime ideation (Table 2).

Table 2.

Prevalence rates for passive suicidal ideation by sample type

Sample Type k N Prevalence Estimates
Heterogeneity Analyses
% 95% CI I2 P
Epidemiological
 Lifetime 8 99,968 10.57% 9.52% – 11.72% 95.36% <.001
 1 Year 9 40,292 5.81% 4.29% – 7.82% 98.18% <.001
 1 Month 4 42,719 3.77% 2.23% – 6.31% 97.52% <.001
 1 Week/Current 5 11,546 2.35% 0.78% – 6.89% 98.66% <.001
Community
 Lifetime 19 109,468 12.53% 10.91% – 14.35% 97.74% <.001
 1 Year 20 64,541 9.84% 8.03% – 12.01% 98.07% <.001
 1 Month 8 43,855 5.74% 3.77% – 8.66% 96.36% <.001
 1 Week/Current 9 20,267 3.48% 1.90% – 6.27% 98.37% <.001
At-Risk/Mixed/Medical
 Lifetime 23 10,754 25.36% 18.65% – 33.49% 97.65% <.001
 1 Year 9 2,509 24.99% 13.93% – 40.67% 97.93% <.001
 1 Month 7 10,582 8.67% 5.28% – 13.92% 97.44% <.001
 1 Week/Current 5 348 17.93% 11.74% – 26.39% 61.82% .03
Psychiatric
 Lifetime 16 3,373 47.03% 36.31% – 58.03% 96.70% <.001
 1 Year 9 21,385 35.63% 26.14% – 46.41% 99.09% <.001
 1 Month 11 16,722 37.50% 26.94% – 49.41% 99.05% <.001
 1 Week/Current 14 1,856 33.57% 24.57% – 43.95% 93.42% <.001

Note. CI = confidence interval; k = number of unique effects; N = total number of participants included in pooled analyses

Heterogeneity was uniformly high across all prevalence analyses, indicating that moderator analyses were warranted. Moderator analyses were conducted for each time-frame, with age, percentage of female participants in each sample, and measure type (i.e., self-report versus interview) evaluated as candidate moderators. Results were generally consistent across the four time-points (Table 3). Specifically, age significantly moderated prevalence estimates for all time-frames, with the exception of current/one-week passive ideation. For the three remaining time-frames, age was negatively associated with prevalence of passive ideation; samples with a lower mean age generally had higher prevalence rates than did samples with a higher mean age. Neither sample sex composition nor measure type emerged as a significant moderator of passive ideation prevalence rate for any time-frame. Prevalence estimates for passive ideation did not differ as a function of whether “pure” passive ideation was assessed or if passive ideation was assessed irrespective of the co-occurrence of active ideation.

Table 3.

Moderator analyses for prevalence of passive suicidal ideation.

Time-frame k N Prevalence Analyses
b SE % 95% CI P
Lifetime
 Age 35 20,282 −.02 .01 -- -- .02
 % Female 49 33,518 .01 <.01 -- -- .10
 Measure Type .45
  Interview 44 11,1542 -- -- 24.72% 21.20% – 28.61%
  Questionnaire 13 11,294 -- -- 21.74% 15.85% – 29.06%
 Passive Suicidal Ideation Constructa -- -- -- -- -- -- --
1 Year
 Age 9 22,585 −.04 .01 -- -- <.001
 % Female 19 49,633 <.01 .01 -- -- .84
 Measure Type .13
  Interview 15 42,639 -- -- 13.38% 8.17% – 21.14%
  Questionnaire 22 45,633 -- -- 20.02% 15.69% – 25.19%
 Passive Suicidal Ideation Constructa .08
  “Pure” Passive Suicidal Ideation 5 19,785 -- -- 20.53% 16.25% – 25.58%
  Not “Pure” Passive Suicidal Ideation 30 68,368 -- -- 14.86% 11.07% – 19.67%
1 Month
 Age 15 58,783 −.04 .01 -- -- <.001
 % Female 24 41,638 <.01 .01 -- -- .73
 Measure Type .20
  Interview 15 12,887 -- -- 19.16% 10.17% – 33.16%
  Questionnaire 11 57,480 -- -- 11.63% 7.37% – 17.88%
 Passive Suicidal Ideation Constructa .12
  “Pure” Passive Suicidal Ideation 6 7,112 -- -- 9.07% 4.14% – 18.73%
  Not “Pure” Passive Suicidal Ideation 18 63,136 -- -- 17.68% 11.80% – 25.62%
1 Week/Current
 Age 12 4,279 −.02 .02 -- -- .22
 % Female 19 13,119 <.01 .01 -- -- .79
 Measure Type .31
  Interview 11 9,856 -- -- 9.75% 3.37% – 25.09%
  Questionnaire 15 12,357 -- -- 17.30% 10.43% – 27.31%
 Passive Suicidal Ideation Constructa .80
  “Pure” Passive Suicidal Ideation 7 1,225 -- -- 15.69% 8.65% – 26.78%
  Not “Pure” Passive Suicidal Ideation 17 20,869 -- -- 14.16% 7.88% – 24.14%

Note. CI = confidence interval; k = number of unique effects; N = total number of subjects included in pooled analyses

a

In moderator analyses of the construct of passive suicidal ideation, prevalence of “pure” passive suicidal ideation was based only on individuals who endorsed passive but not active suicidal ideation; studies were considered conservatively not to have assessed “pure” passive suicidal ideation if they either included individuals with active ideation in their assessment of passive ideation or were unclear as to whether that decision was made. For lifetime passive suicidal ideation, only one study with two unique effects for “pure” passive suicidal ideation was available for lifetime prevalence of passive suicidal ideation, and thus moderator analysis was not conducted.

Analyses of publication bias were conducted for lifetime prevalence of passive ideation by sample type. Across all analyses, there was relatively little evidence of publication bias. Egger’s regression test indicated significant publication bias only in the case of at-risk/mixed/medical samples (intercept = 6.77, p <.01) and not for the remaining three sample types (interceptEpidemiological Samples = −.45, p = .88; interceptCommunity Samples = 2.09, p = .33; interceptPsychiatric Samples = 3.40, p = .26). The trim-and-fill method yielded slightly different lifetime prevalence rates for only epidemiological (adjusted prevalence = 9.74%, 95% CI = 8.94% – 10.96%) and community samples (adjusted prevalence = 14.51%, 95% CI = 12.52% – 16.76%). Evidence of asymmetry in the funnel plots of the effect sizes for all sample types was correspondingly modest, suggesting limited presence of publication bias (Figures 2a through 2d).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Funnel plots for effect sizes in the meta-analyses. The vertical line indicates the weighted mean effect. Open circles indicate observed effects for actual studies, and closed circles indicate imputed effects for studies believed to be missing due to publication bias. The clear diamond reflects the unadjusted weighted mean effect size, whereas the black diamond reflects the weighted mean effect size after adjusting for publication bias.

2a. Lifetime prevalence of passive ideation in epidemiological samples

2b. Lifetime prevalence of passive ideation in community samples

2c. Lifetime prevalence of passive ideation in at-risk/mixed/medical samples

2d. Lifetime prevalence of passive ideation in psychiatric samples

Correlates of passive and active ideation

Table 4 presents results for analyses of correlates of passive and active ideation with all time frames for suicidal ideation assessment combined, and where possible, of direct comparisons between passive and active ideation with respect to correlates. Most of the effects included in these analyses were for sociodemographic and clinical factors. In general, passive and active ideation were comparable to each other in their relation to the correlates under study.

Table 4.

Correlates of passive and active ideation.

Correlate Passive Ideation
Active Ideation
Active Ideation vs. Passive Ideation
k d 95% CI p k d 95% CI p k d 95% CI p
Sociodemographic Factors
 Sex (Female) 28 .23 .16 – .31 <.001 20 .19 .06 – .32 <.01 8 −.01 −.12 – .11 .91
 Race (White) 16 .10 .01 – .19 .04 14 .12 .01 – .23 .03 6 −.01 −.06 – .04 .72
 Ethnicity (Hispanic) 5 .05 −.06 – .15 .38 3 .07 −.05 – .19 .23
 Education (Fewer Years) 13 .17 .06 – .29 <.01 10 .15 −.02 – .31 .08 4 −.21 −.49 – .07 .13
 Marital Status
  Not Married vs. Married 17 .23 .14 – .33 <.001 12 .22 .10 – .33 <.001 4 .02 −.02 – .07 .33
  Divorced vs. Married 8 .32 .11 – .53 <.01 6 .54 .32 – .75 <.001
  Never Married vs. Married 11 .15 −.02 – .32 .08 6 .25 .00a – .49 <.05
 Sexual Minority 10 .48 .33 – .63 <.001 10 .60 .41 – .79 <.001
Clinical Factors
 Psychiatric Problems 45 .81 .67 – .95 <.001 35 .76 .62 – .90 <.001 10 .10 −.06 – .25 .24
 Depression 41 .96 .81 – 1.10 <.001 32 .93 .74 – 1.11 <.001 10 .16 −.03 – .34 .10
 Anxiety 12 .73 .43 – 1.02 <.001 10 .83 .43 – 1.23 <.001 5 −.02 −.44 – .40 .91
 Alcohol Use Problems 13 .14 .00a – .28 .04 11 .30 .15 – .45 <.001 5 .17 .11 – .24 <.001
 Substance Use Problems 7 .16 −.01 – .32 .07 5 .38 .23 – .53 <.001 -- -- -- --
 Psychosis 3 1.00 .03 – 1.96 .04 3 .71 .37 – 1.04 <.001 -- -- -- --
 Psychiatric Treatment 5 .19 −.05 – .43 .12 5 .31 .11 – .52 <.01 5 .10 .04 – .16 <.001
Psychological Factors
 Aggression/Anger/ Irritability 3 .51 .22 – .80 <.001 3 .41 .16 – .67 <.01 -- -- -- --
 Low Cognitive Functioning 6 .11 −.11 – .32 .32 5 −.19 −.45 – .07 .16 4 −.18 −.56 – .21 .37
 Hopelessness 8 .80 .52 – 1.08 <.001 6 .69 .31 – 1.06 <.001 -- -- -- --
 Perceived Burdensomeness 4 1.17 .78 – 1.56 <.001 3 .87 .58 – 1.15 <.001 -- -- -- --
 Thwarted Belongingness 4 .77 .61 – .92 <.001 3 .60 .37 – .82 <.001 -- -- -- --
Other Factors
 Financial Difficulties 14 .31 .28 – .34 <.001 10 .31 .21 – .42 <.001 5 .07 .02 – .12 .01
 Physical Health Problems 14 .50 .38 – .62 <.001 10 .48 .30 – .66 <.001 6 <.01 −.04 – .04 .96
 Physical Pain 8 .31 .15 – .46 .001 8 .26 .07 – .45 .01 -- -- -- --
 Low Social Support 8 .34 .13 – .55 <.01 6 .40 .05 – .74 .02 -- -- -- --
 Stress 9 .41 .17 – .65 <.001 7 .44 .12 – .76 .01 3 .21 .03 – .38 .02

Note. CI = confidence interval; k = number of unique effects; effect size estimates where k = 3 should be interpreted with a degree of caution.

a

The lower end of the confidence interval was rounded down but exceeded 0.

Although passive ideation was significantly correlated with sociodemographic factors, save for ethnic minority status and being never married, the pooled effects sizes were small in almost every case. The exceptions were being divorced, which had a small-to-medium pooled effect, and sexual minority status, with a medium pooled effect. A similar pattern of results emerged for active ideation, the primary difference being that the correlation with education status was no longer significant. In head-to-head comparisons, none of the sociodemographic factors significantly differentiated between passive and active ideation, and pooled effect sizes ranged from trivial to small.

The largest effects for passive ideation were observed for clinical and psychological factors. In the case of the former, generally large and significant pooled effects were found for psychiatric problems, depression, anxiety, and psychosis. Only substance use and psychiatric treatment were not significantly correlated with passive ideation. A similar set of results was found for active ideation, again with the largest pooled effects for psychiatric problems, depression, anxiety, and psychosis. Additionally, small-to-medium significant pooled effects were evident for active ideation in relation to substance use and psychiatric care. In direct comparisons of passive and active ideation, small significant pooled effects indicating a stronger association with active ideation were observed for alcohol use problems and psychiatric treatment utilization. As for psychological factors, all except for cognitive functioning were associated with passive ideation, with large pooled effects in every case. These findings were largely mirrored in analyses of active ideation, again with only cognitive functioning demonstrating a non-significant association and large pooled effects detected for all other factors. Direct comparison between passive and active ideation was only possible for cognitive functioning and a non-significant pooled effect was found.

All remaining factors, relating to difficulties in financial, physical, and interpersonal domains, as well as general stress, were significantly correlated with passive ideation, with the size of pooled effects ranging from small-to-medium to medium. Results for active ideation were essentially the same, with small-to-medium and medium pooled effects observed across the factors under consideration.

Finally, analyses were conducted evaluating the association between passive ideation and suicide-related outcomes, specifically suicide attempts and deaths (Table 5). A large pooled effect was observed for passive ideation in relation to suicide attempts. This was also the case for the association between active ideation and attempts. Given the clinical interest in whether passive and active ideation differ in their association with suicide attempts, a preliminary analysis was conducted of this head-to-head comparison with the two available relevant studies, and a small and non-significant pooled effect was found. For the same reasons, preliminary analyses were conducted for passive and active ideation, respectively, predicting suicide deaths. Large pooled effects were detected in both cases. As only two unique effects were included in each of these preliminary analyses, caution should be taken in interpreting their results.

Table 5.

Prediction of suicide attempts and deaths by passive and active ideation.

Suicide Attempts
Suicide Deaths
k d 95% CI p k d 95% CI p
Passive Ideation 9 1.19 .77 – 1.62 <.001 2 1.01 .87 – 1.14 <.001
Active Ideation 8 1.62 1.08 – 2.17 <.001 2 1.77 1.49 – 2.05 <.001
Active Ideation vs. Passive Ideation 2 .16 −.43 – .75 .59

Note. CI = confidence interval; k = number of unique effects; effect size estimates where k = 2 should be interpreted with a degree of caution.

Discussion

The aim of the current review was to characterize the existing literature on passive ideation, particularly the prevalence of this clinical phenomenon, its associated sociodemographic characteristics, the nature of its psychiatric comorbidity, and its psychological and environmental correlates. Furthermore, it aimed systematically to evaluate the degree to which passive ideation differs from active ideation in the strength of its association with these correlates.

Passive ideation was highly prevalent in psychiatric populations, with approximately a third of all individuals experiencing current passive ideation and almost half of all individuals having a lifetime history of passive ideation. Although considerably lower, the prevalence of passive ideation is still concerning in epidemiological studies. Pooled estimates from these studies revealed that approximately one in 20 individuals in the general population experience passive ideation in any given year, and this figure increases to one in 10 individuals for lifetime passive ideation. The one-year prevalence of passive ideation in epidemiological samples (5.8%) was somewhat higher than for active ideation reported in the NCS-R (3.3%; Kessler et al., 2005) and NCS-A (3.6%; Husky et al., 2012). Lifetime prevalence of passive ideation in epidemiological samples in the current meta-analysis (10.6%) fell within the range of lifetime rates reported for active ideation in the WMH surveys (9.2%; Nock, Borges, Bromet, Alonso, et al., 2008), NCS-A (12.1%; Nock et al., 2013) and NCS (13.5%; Kessler, Borges, & Walters, 1999) There was little evidence that the prevalence estimates in the current review were influenced by publication bias.

That age was inversely related to prevalence of passive ideation, particularly in the case of lifetime prevalence, is a curious finding warranting discussion. This finding suggests that younger individuals have a higher lifetime prevalence of passive ideation than do older counterparts, a pattern that seems counterintuitive but has also been found for lifetime prevalence of active ideation (Nock, Borges, Bromet, Alonso, et al., 2008). One possible explanation for the current finding is the presence of a cohort effect, with passive ideation becoming more common among younger individuals. Another explanation may be that older individuals are more likely to forget experiences of passive ideation over time, especially if they occurred many years in the past. Indeed, it is possible that passive ideation might be less memorable compared to active ideation, particularly when recall of its occurrence is over longer periods of time. In support of this latter possibility, one study has found some adolescents to forget previously endorsed suicidal ideation, albeit active ideation, over a four-year period (Goldney, Smith, Winefield, Tiggeman, & Winefield, 1991). It is also consistent with the broader literature, in which higher rates of psychiatric disorders have been found when assessed prospectively than retrospectively (Liu, 2016; Moffitt et al., 2010). These two possibilities, a cohort effect and decreased rates due to forgetting over time, are not mutually exclusive, however, and require additional research to resolve.

Regarding the inverse association for age relative to one-month and one-year prevalence of passive ideation, a different explanation is likely relevant. Specifically, active ideation has been found to peak during adolescence and early adulthood (Kessler et al., 1999), an implication of which is that prevalence of current or recent active ideation should be higher among adolescents and young adults than older age groups. It is a reasonable possibility that a similar pattern may hold for passive ideation, and it may therefore be expected to have higher incidence among younger age groups. The absence of a similar inverse relation between age and current/one-week passive ideation, on the other hand, is likely due to the availability of only one study with a youth sample (Urrila et al., 2012) in the relevant analysis. That is, this modest representation of younger samples likely limited the ability to detect potential moderating effects of age on prevalence rates.

In analyses of correlates, sociodemographic characteristics were generally modestly associated with passive ideation, and are consequently of limited utility in risk screening strategies. As these sociodemographic characteristics are generally easier and quicker to ascertain, and thus more amenable to inclusion in brief clinical risk assessment protocols, than are the other correlates included in this review, this pattern of findings underscores the significant challenges of screening for risk for passive ideation. One notable exception is sexual minority individuals, who appear to be an especially at-risk population, with a medium pooled effect observed in the current review, a finding that is consistent with the broader suicide literature (Haas et al., 2011; Hottes, Bogaert, Rhodes, Brennan, & Gesink, 2016; O’Brien, Liu, Putney, Burke, & Aguinaldo, 2017). This group may therefore particularly benefit from increased preventive intervention efforts and be given greater weight when risk stratification is warranted.

Underscoring the clinical significance of passive ideation, it was associated with considerable psychiatric comorbidity as well as psychological characteristics traditionally implicated in risk for suicide and other negative mental health outcomes, with some of the largest pooled effects being observed for these correlates. These findings appeared most robust for overall psychopathology, depression, and anxiety, based on the number of unique effects available for each analysis and the size of their corresponding pooled effects. Moreover, passive ideation was strongly associated with suicide attempts, and although preliminary, suicide deaths. Indeed, among the largest pooled effects for passive ideation were found in its association with these outcomes. Collectively, these findings point to the need for future research investigating the clinical importance of passive ideation. It is therefore concerning that passive ideation was not associated with receipt of psychiatric care in our analyses.

Although sizeable pooled effects were observed for the remaining correlates involving general and domain-specific stress (i.e., financial, physical health, social), it is important to note that with most of these correlates (i.e., financial and physical health problems) and associated studies, the focus of research in this area has been predominantly more relevant to adult populations than to youth. Insofar as passive ideation mirrors active ideation with regards to its dramatic increase in first onset during adolescence (Kessler et al., 1999; Nock, Borges, Bromet, Cha, et al., 2008), a discordance exists between where the emphasis has been in the empirical literature and where it is most needed. Although identifying potential markers of risk relevant to passive ideation in adults is undoubtedly important, more research is needed to elucidate potential factors underlying this clinical phenomenon during adolescence, a developmental period of particular risk. For example, timing of pubertal maturation may be a promising developmentally relevant candidate for future investigation, given that it has been associated with general psychopathology, depression, and anxiety (Hamilton, Hamlat, Stange, Abramson, & Alloy, 2014; Hamlat, Snyder, Young, & Hankin, 2019; Reardon, Leen-Feldner, & Hayward, 2009; Ullsperger & Nikolas, 2017), which as mentioned above, are all strongly correlated with passive ideation.

Of note, these results are cross-sectional in nature, and thus temporal relationships between passive or active ideation and clinical, sociodemographic, suicide-related, and stress-related variables cannot be determined. It is plausible that some correlates may serve as risk factors for the onset and/or maintenance of passive or active ideation, and that these relationships may differ across the lifespan. Alternatively, at least in the case of clinical correlates, it may be possible that passive ideation may prospectively predict negative mental health outcomes, essentially functioning as a prodromal marker of risk, particularly in the case of younger individuals who may be earlier in their clinical course. Prospective research is needed to determine the nature and directionality of these relationships over time to inform screening and intervention efforts.

The similarities between passive and active ideation were notable. Not only were their pooled effects for individual correlates largely equivalent in size, but head-to-head comparisons also consistently yielded trivial to small pooled effects, most of which were not significant. When interpreted together with the aforementioned findings of psychiatric and psychological correlates, the analyses involving active ideation lend weight to the possibility that passive ideation may be comparable to active ideation in terms of clinical significance and associated risk. Although results from this meta-analysis are preliminary, our findings highlight the need for rigorous longitudinal studies to explore whether these associations are maintained over time. Specifically, the most logical next steps are to conduct studies (i) evaluating prospective predictors of passive and active ideation and (ii) directly comparing passive and active ideation in prospectively predicting suicidal behavior. Such a design would permit analyses necessary to be able confidently to evaluate the degree to which passive and active ideation may have a shared or different etiology and the common clinical view that passive ideation is not of high clinical concern.

Several limitations should be mentioned. In spite of the significance of the findings from the current review, there is a near absence of longitudinal studies. As noted above, cross-sectional research provides an important first step for addressing these research questions. Future research, however, should employ longitudinal designs to delineate the temporal nature of observed effects. Such studies are necessary to form the important distinction between concomitants and risk factors, and eventually to elucidate the etiological pathways underlying risk for passive ideation (Kraemer et al., 1997). Although concomitants are of value for aiding in the identification of who may be currently experiencing passive ideation, risk factors have added importance for potentially enhancing efforts to intervene before the onset of ideation. Work on identifying risk factors is also valuable for its potential to identify targets for the development of future intervention strategies. The possibility that the temporal direction of some of the associations under study may be such that passive ideation is the risk factor, rather than the outcome, should also be considered. For instance, it has been hypothesized that suicidal ideation may lead to prospectively greater rates of interpersonal stress during times of clinical acuity (Liu & Spirito, 2019). Consistent with this possibility, suicidal ideation has been associated with compromised interpersonal functioning during times of stress (Williams, Barnhofer, Crane, & Beck, 2005). This greater interpersonal stress, in turn, may produce feelings of thwarted belongingness in vulnerable individuals.

An additional limitation worth noting is related to the measurement of lifetime suicidal ideation across the studies included in this review. As mentioned above, and like all measurements requiring recall over the lifetime, the assessment of lifetime suicidal ideation may lead to underreporting as a result of memory bias. This type of measurement error may disproportionally affect older individuals, given the greater length of time required for recall. It may also disproportionally affect the recall of less salient content (e.g., passive ideation as compared to active ideation). It is important to consider these potential sources of measurement error when interpreting the current findings. Indeed, these considerations collectively suggest that the prevalence rates of passive ideation reported here may be underestimates and this may especially be the case for older adults.

Additionally, several fundamental characteristics relating to the clinical course of passive ideation (e.g., incidence, chronicity, and recurrence) remain undetermined and await future investigation. Furthermore, individual differences in trajectories of suicidal ideation as a broader construct has been recently observed (Czyz & King, 2015; Wolff et al., 2018). For what proportion of individuals does passive ideation specifically abate, or alternatively, transition to active ideation or suicidal behavior? Moreover, understanding what characteristics differentiate between those who do and do not transition to active ideation or behavior, as well as the determinants of the timing of these transitions, has potential value for risk stratification strategies. Suicidal behavior has been previously found most often to occur within a year of onset of suicidal ideation as a general construct (Nock, Borges, Bromet, Alonso, et al., 2008). Clarifying to what degree this holds true or differs for passive ideation specifically may be clinically informative regarding the potential length of the temporal window for intervening in the course from suicidal ideation to action.

Finally, several challenges in conducting prospective research in this area are notable. The low base rate of suicidal behavior introduces substantial challenges for achieving sufficient statistical power to conduct meaningful analysis of predictors of suicide. Integrative data analysis may offer one solution to this challenge, by pooling data across multiple studies in order to answer questions about phenomena that require larger sample sizes (Hussong, Curran, & Bauer, 2013). Further contributing to the challenges of research in this area is the length of time over which samples are followed. Given that suicidal behavior is a low-base-rate event, past studies have often utilized long follow up periods in order to increase the likelihood of capturing these events. Studies of long-term risk are important for identifying who may be at risk for suicidal behaviors, but do not inform our understanding of when individuals are at greatest risk for suicide. Recent research has shifted towards greater emphasis on understanding short-term predictors of suicide risk, utilizing, for example, ecological momentary assessment and passive data collection methods, with the goal of identifying factors predicting the transition from suicidal ideation to behavior (Glenn, Cha, Kleiman, & Nock, 2017; Glenn & Nock, 2014).

In conclusion, the current review found the lifetime prevalence of passive ideation in the general population to be substantial. That this is clinically concerning is indicated by the high psychiatric comorbidity found in our analyses, the strong association with suicide attempts, and preliminary evidence of a comparably strong relation with suicide deaths. One group that emerged as being particularly at risk, and thus a priority for the development of screening and intervention protocols, is sexual minority individuals. The current findings are also suggestive of notable similarities between passive and active ideation, particularly in terms of psychiatric comorbidity and psychological and other characteristics traditionally associated with risk. Collectively, these findings are indicative of the need for greater focus on passive ideation in research and clinical contexts.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (RF1MH120830, R01MH101138, R01MH115905, and R21MH112055). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agency.

Footnotes

Declaration of Interest

The authors report no conflict of interest.

References

* Articles marked with an asterisk were included in the meta-analysis.

  1. *Alaimo K, Olson CM, & Frongillo EA. (2002). Family food insufficiency, but not low family income, is positively associated with dysthymia and suicide symptoms in adolescents. Journal of Nutrition, 132, 719–725. doi: 10.1093/jn/132.4.719 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. *Allen MH, Abar BW, McCormick M, Barnes DH, Haukoos J, Garmel GM, & Boudreaux ED. (2013). Screening for suicidal ideation and attempts among emergency department medical patients: Instrument and results from the psychiatric emergency research collaboration. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 43, 313–323. doi: 10.1111/sltb.12018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. *Ashrafioun L, Leong SH, Pigeon WR, & Oslin DW. (2018). The associations between suicidality and mental health factors and pain interference in veterans being referred to primary care mental health integration. Psychiatry Research, 269, 264–270. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. *Ayalon L, & Litwin H. (2009). What cognitive functions are associated with passive suicidal ideation? Findings from a national sample of community dwelling Israelis. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 24, 472–478. doi: 10.1002/gps.2140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. *Ayalon L, Mackin S, Arean PA, Chen H, & McDonel Herr EC. (2007). The role of cognitive functioning and distress in suicidal ideation in older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 55, 1090–1094. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01237.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. *Baca-Garcia E, Perez-Rodriguez MM, Oquendo MA, Keyes KM, Hasin DS, Grant BF, & Blanco C. (2011). Estimating risk for suicide attempt: are we asking the right questions? Passive suicidal ideation as a marker for suicidal behavior. Journal of Affective Disorders, 134, 327–332. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.06.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. *Barrigón ML, Berrouiguet S, Carballo JJ, Bonal-Giménez C, Fernández-Navarro P, Pfang B, Delgado-Gómez D, et al. (2017). User profiles of an electronic mental health tool for ecological momentary assessment: MEmind. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 26, 1–9. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1554 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. *Barry LC, Wakefield DB, Trestman RL, & Conwell Y. (2016). Active and passive suicidal ideation in older prisoners. Crisis, 37, 88–94. doi: 10.1027/0227-5910/a000350 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. *Bartels SJ, Coakley E, Oxman TE, Constantino G, Oslin D, Chen H, Zubritsky C, et al. (2002). Suicidal and death ideation in older primary care patients with depression, anxiety, and at-risk alcohol use. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 10, 417–427. doi: 10.2459/JCM.0b013e32830f42c8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Beck AT, Brown G, & Steer RA. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory II manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  11. *Bingham KS, Rothschild AJ, Mulsant BH, Whyte EM, Meyers BS, Banerjee S, Szanto K, et al. (2017). The association of baseline suicidality with treatment outcome in psychotic depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 78, 1149–1154. doi: 10.4088/JCP.14m09658 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Biostat. (2014). Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Version 3. Englewood, NJ: Biostat. [Google Scholar]
  13. *Briggs R, Tobin K, Kenny RA, & Kennelly SP. (2018). What is the prevalence of untreated depression and death ideation in older people? Data from the Irish Longitudinal Study on Aging. International Psychogeriatrics, 30, 1393–1401. doi: 10.1017/S104161021700299X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. *Caetano SC, Olvera RL, Hunter K, Hatch JP, Najt P, Bowden C, Pliszka S, et al. (2006). Association of psychosis with suicidality in pediatric bipolar I, II and bipolar NOS patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 91, 33–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.12.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. *Castle K, Duberstein PR, Meldrum S, Conner KR, & Conwell Y. (2004). Risk factors for suicide in blacks and whites: An analysis of data from the 1993 National Mortality Followback Survey. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 452–458. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.3.452 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. *Charles DR, Abram KM, Mcclelland GM, & Teplin LA. (2003). Suicidal ideation and behavior among women in jail. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 19, 65–81. doi: 10.1177/1043986202239742 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. *Coêlho BM, Andrade LH, Guarniero FB, & Wang YP. (2010). The influence of the comorbidity between depression and alcohol use disorder on suicidal behaviors in the São Paulo Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study, Brazil. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 32, 396–408. doi: 10.1590/S1516-44462010005000027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. *Cohen CI, Colemon Y, Yaffee R, & Casimir GJ. (2008). Racial differences in suicidality in an older urban population. Gerontologist, 48, 71–78. doi: 10.1093/geront/48.1.71 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. *Cook JM, Pearson JL, Thompson R, Black BS, & Rabins PV. (2002). Suicidality in older African Americans: Findings from the EPOCH study. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 10, 437–446. doi: 10.1097/00019442-200207000-00010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. *Cottler LB, Campbell W, Krishna VAS, Cunningham-Williams RM, & Abdallah A. Ben. (2005). Predictors of high rates of suicidal ideation among drug users. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 193, 431–437. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000168245.56563.90 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. *Cukrowicz KC, Jahn DR, Graham RD, Poindexter EK, & Williams RB. (2013). Suicide risk in older adults: Evaluating models of risk and predicting excess zeros in a primary care sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 1021–1030. doi: 10.1037/a0034953 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Czyz EK, & King CA. (2015). Longitudinal trajectories of suicidal ideation and subsequent suicide attempts among adolescent inpatients. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44, 181–193. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.836454 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. *de Graaf R, Sandfort TGM, & Ten Have M. (2006). Suicidality and sexual orientation: Differences between men and women in a general population-based sample from The Netherlands. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35, 253–262. doi: 10.1007/s10508-006-9020-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. *Dell’Osso L, Carmassi C, Carlini M, Rucci P, Torri P, Cesari D, Landi P, et al. (2009). Sexual dysfunctions and suicidality in patients with bipolar disorder and unipolar depression. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 6, 3063–3070. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2009.01455.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. *Dennis M, Baillon S, Brugha T, Lindesay J, Stewart R, & Meltzer H. (2007). The spectrum of suicidal ideation in Great Britain: Comparisons across a 16–74 years age range. Psychological Medicine, 37, 795–805. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. *Deykin EY, & Buka SL. (1994). Suicidal ideation and attempts among chemically dependent adolescents. American Journal of Public Health, 84, 634–639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. *Draper B, Maccuspie-Moore C, & Brodaty H. (1998). Suicidal ideation and the “wish to die” in dementia patients: The role of depression. Age and Ageing, 27, 503–507. doi: 10.1093/ageing/27.4.503 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. *Dutta R, Ball HA, Siribaddana SH, Sumathipala A, Samaraweera S, McGuffin P, & Hotopf M. (2017). Genetic and other risk factors for suicidal ideation and the relationship with depression. Psychological Medicine, 47, 2438–2449. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717000940 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Duval S, & Tweedie R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56, 455–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, & Minder C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ, 315, 629–634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. *Estrada CAM, Nonaka D, Gregorio ER, Leynes CR, Del Castillo RT, Hernandez PMR, Hayakawa T, et al. (2019). Suicidal ideation, suicidal behaviors, and attitudes towards suicide of adolescents enrolled in the Alternative Learning System in Manila, Philippines - A mixed methods study. Tropical Medicine and Health, 47, 1–18. doi: 10.1186/s41182-019-0149-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. *Farrell KR, & Ganzini L. (1995). Misdiagnosing delirium as depression in medically ill elderly patients. Archives of Internal Medicine, 155, 2459–2464. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. *Flint EP, Hays JC, Krishnan KRR, Meador KG, & Blazer DG. (1998). Suicidal behaviors in depressed men with a family history of suicide: Effects of psychosocial factors and age. Aging and Mental Health, 2, 286–299. doi: 10.1080/13607869856533 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Franklin JC, Ribeiro JD, Fox KR, Bentley KH, Kleiman EM, Huang X, Musacchio KM, et al. (2017). Risk factors for suicidal thoughts and behaviors: A meta-analysis of 50 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 187–232. doi: 10.1037/bul0000084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. *Fu-I L, & Wang YP. (2008). Comparison of demographic and clinical characteristics between children and adolescents with major depressive disorder. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 30, 124–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. *Ghanizadeh A, Khajavian S, & Ashkani H. (2006). Prevalence of psychiatric disorders, depression, and suicidal behavior in child and adolescent with thalassemia major. Journal Of Pediatric Hematology/Oncology, 28, 781–784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. *Glassmire DM, Tarescavage AM, Burchett D, Martinez J, & Gomez A. (2016). Clinical utility of the MMPI-2-RF SUI items and scale in a forensic inpatient setting: Association with interview self-report and future suicidal behaviors. Psychological Assessment, 28, 1502–1509. doi: 10.1037/pas0000220 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Glenn CR, Cha CB, Kleiman EM, & Nock MK. (2017). Understanding suicide risk within the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework: Insights, challenges, and future research considerations. Clinical Psychological Science, 5, 568–592. doi: 10.1177/2167702616686854 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Glenn CR, & Nock MK. (2014). Improving the short-term prediction of suicidal behavior. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 47, S176–180. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2014.06.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Goldney RD, Smith S, Winefield AH, Tiggeman M, & Winefield HR. (1991). Suicidal ideation: its enduring nature and associated morbidity. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 83, 115–120. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1991.tb07375.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. *Guidry ET, & Cukrowicz KC. (2016). Death ideation in older adults: psychological symptoms of depression, thwarted belongingness, and perceived burdensomeness. Aging and Mental Health, 20, 823–830. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1040721 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Haas AP, Eliason M, Mays VM, Mathy RM, Cochran SD, D’Augelli AR, Silverman MM, et al. (2011). Suicide and suicide risk in lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender populations: Review and recommendations. Journal of Homosexuality, 58, 10–51. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2011.534038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hamilton JL, Hamlat EJ, Stange JP, Abramson LY, & Alloy LB. (2014). Pubertal timing and vulnerabilities to depression in early adolescence: Differential pathways to depressive symptoms by sex. Journal of Adolescence, 37, 165–174. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.11.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hamlat EJ, Snyder HR, Young JF, & Hankin BL. (2019). Pubertal timing as a transdiagnostic risk for psychopathology in youth. Clinical Psychological Science, 7, 411–429. doi: 10.1177/2167702618810518 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. *Handley ED, Adams TR, Manly JT, Cicchetti D, & Toth SL. (2019). Mother–daughter interpersonal processes underlying the association between child maltreatment and adolescent suicide ideation. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 49, 1232–1240. doi: 10.1111/sltb.12522 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. *Heisel MJ, & Flett GL. (2006). The development and initial validation of the Geriatric Suicide Ideation Scale. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 742–751. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000218699.27899.f9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. *Heisel MJ, & Flett GL. (2016). Investigating the psychometric properties of the Geriatric Suicide Ideation Scale (GSIS) among community-residing older adults. Aging and Mental Health, 20, 208–221. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1072798 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. *Herrell R, Goldberg J, True WR, Ramakrishnan V, Lyons M, Eisen S, & Tsuang MT. (1999). Sexual orientation and suicidality: A co-twin control study in adult men. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 867. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.10.867 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, & Altman DG. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ, 327, 557–560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hottes TS, Bogaert L, Rhodes AE, Brennan DJ, & Gesink D. (2016). Lifetime prevalence of suicide attempts among sexual minority adults by study sampling strategies: A systematic review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Public Health, 106, 921. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2016.303088a [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Husky MM, Olfson M, He J, Nock MK, Swanson SA, & Merikangas KR. (2012). Twelve-month suicidal symptoms and use of services among adolescents: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Psychiatric Services, 63, 989–996. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201200058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hussong AM, Curran PJ, & Bauer DJ. (2013). Integrative data analysis in clinical psychology research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 61–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185522 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Jacobs D. (2009). Suicide Assessment Five-Step Evaluation and Triage (SAFE-T). Retrieved from https://www.integration.samhsa.gov/images/res/SAFE_T.pdf
  54. *Jahn DR, Poindexter EK, Graham RD, & Cukrowicz KC. (2012). The moderating effect of the negative impact of recent life events on the relation between intrinsic religiosity and death ideation in older adults. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 42, 589–601. doi: 10.1111/j.1943-278X.2012.00114.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Borges G, Nock M, & Wang PS. (2005). Trends in suicide ideation, plans, gestures, and attempts in the United States, 1990–1992 to 2001–2003. JAMA, 293, 2487–2495. doi: 10.1001/jama.293.20.2487 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kessler RC, Borges G, & Walters EE. (1999). Prevalence of and risk factors for lifetime suicide attempts in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 617–626. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.7.617 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kessler RC, & Üstün TB. (2004). The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative Version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 13, 93–121. doi: 10.1002/mpr.168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. *Kienhorst CWM, de Wilde EJ, van den Bout J, Broese van Groenou MI, Diekstra RFW, & Wolters WHG. (1990). Self-reported suicidal behavior in Dutch secondary education students. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 20, 101–112. doi: 10.1111/j.1943-278X.1990.tb00093.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. *Kim YA, Bogner HR, Brown GK, & Gallo JJ. (2006). Chronic medical conditions and wishes to die among older primary care patients. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 36, 183–198. doi: 10.2190/3QXD-UR0H-K8FH-2CU8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. *Kliem S, Lohmann A, Mosle T, & Brahler E. (2017). German Beck Scale for Suicide Ideation (BSS): Psychometric properties from a representative population survey. BMC psychiatry, 17, 389. doi: 10.1186/s12888-017-1559-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kovacs M. (2010). Children’s Depression Inventory - Second Edition (CDI-2) Manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  62. Kraemer HC, Kazdin AE, Offord DR, Kessler RC, Jensen PS, & Kupfer DJ. (1997). Coming to terms with the terms of risk. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 337–343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kraemer HC, Morgan GA, Leech NL, Gliner JA, Vaske JJ, & Harmon RJ. (2003). Measures of clinical significance. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 1524–1529. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200312000-00022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. *Lapierre S, Boyer R, Desjardins S, Dubé M, Lorrain D, Préville M, & Brassard J. (2012). Daily hassles, physical illness, and sleep problems in older adults with wishes to die. International Psychogeriatrics, 24, 243–252. doi: 10.1017/S1041610211001591 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. *Linden M, & Barnow S. (1997). The wish to die in very old persons near the end of life: A psychiatric problem? Results from the Berlin aging study. International Psychogeriatrics, 9, 291–307. doi: 10.1017/S1041610297004456 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Liu RT. (2016). Taxometric evidence of a dimensional latent structure for depression in an epidemiological sample of children and adolescents. Psychological Medicine, 46, 1265–1275. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715002792 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Liu RT, & Spirito A. (2019). Suicidal behavior and stress generation in adolescents. Clinical Psychological Science, 7, 488–501. doi: 10.1177/2167702618810227 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. *Liu X, Gentzler AL, Tepper P, Kiss E, Kothencné VO, Tamás Z, Vetró Á, et al. (2006). Clinical features of depressed children and adolescents with various forms of suicidality. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 67, 1442–1450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Lozano R, Naghavi M, Foreman K, Lim S, Shibuya K, Aboyans V, Abraham J, et al. (2012). Global and regional mortality from 235 causes of death for 20 age groups in 1990 and 2010: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet, 380, 2095–2128. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61728-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. *Lutz J, Morton K, Turiano NA, & Fiske A. (2016). Health conditions and passive suicidal ideation in the survey of health, ageing, and retirement in Europe. Journals of Gerontology - Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 71, 936–946. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. *Lytle MC, De Luca SM, Blosnich JR, & Brownson C. (2015). Associations of racial/ethnic identities and religious affiliation with suicidal ideation among lesbian, gay, bisexual, and questioning individuals. Journal of Affective Disorders, 178, 39–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.07.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. *Magni G, Rigatti-Luchini S, Fracca F, & Merskey H. (1998). Suicidality in chronic abdominal pain: An analysis of the Hispanic Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (HHANES). Pain, 76, 137–144. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00035-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. *Malfent D, Wondrak T, Kapusta ND, & Sonneck G. (2010). Suicidal ideation and its correlates among elderly in residential care homes. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25, 843–849. doi: 10.1002/gps.2426 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. *Manu P, Matthews DA, & Lane TJ. (1991). Panic disorder among patients with chronic fatigue. Southern Medical Journal, 84, 451–456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. *May CN, Overholser JC, Ridley J, & Raymond D. (2015). Passive suicidal ideation: A clinically relevant risk factor for suicide in treatment-seeking veterans. Illness Crisis and Loss, 23, 261–277. doi: 10.1177/1054137315585422 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. *McBride O, Cheng HG, Slade T, & Lynskey MT. (2016). The role of specific alcohol-related problems in predicting depressive experiences in a cross-sectional National Household Survey. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 51, 655–663. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agw010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. *Meerwijk EL, & Weiss SJ. (2018). Tolerance for psychological pain and capability for suicide: Contributions to suicidal ideation and behavior. Psychiatry Research, 262, 203–208. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. *Mendonca JD, & Holden RR. (1996). Are all suicidal ideas closely linked to hopelessness? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 93, 246–251. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1996.tb10642.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. *Mitchell SM, Brown SL, Roush JF, Bolaños AD, Littlefield AK, Marshall AJ, Jahn DR, et al. (2017). The clinical application of suicide risk assessment: A theory-driven approach. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 24, 1406–1420. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2086 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. *Mitchell SM, Brown SL, Roush JF, Tucker RP, Cukrowicz KC, & Joiner TE. (in press). The Interpersonal Needs Questionnaire: Statistical considerations for improved clinical application. Assessment. doi: 10.1177/1073191118824660 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  81. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, Kokaua J, Milne BJ, Polanczyk G, & Poulton R. (2010). How common are common mental disorders? Evidence that lifetime prevalence rates are doubled by prospective versus retrospective ascertainment. Psychological Medicine, 40, 899–909. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709991036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. *Naidoo S, & Collings SJ. (2017). Suicidal and death ideation in a cohort of psychiatric outpatients: Prevalence and risk factors. Psychology and Developing Societies, 29, 288–300. doi: 10.1177/0971333617716849 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  83. National Action Alliance for Suicide Prevention. (2014). A prioritized research agenda for suicide prevention: An action plan to save lives. Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health and the Research Prioritization Task Force. [Google Scholar]
  84. *Nazem S, Siderowf AD, Duda JE, Brown GK, Have T. Ten, Stern MB, & Weintraub D. (2008). Suicidal and death ideation in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders, 23, 1573–1579. doi: 10.1002/mds.22130 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. *Neeleman J, de Graaf R, & Vollebergh W. (2004). The suicidal process; prospective comparison between early and later stages. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82, 43–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2003.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Nock MK, Borges G, Bromet EJ, Alonso J, Angermeyer M, Beautrais A, Bruffaerts R, et al. (2008). Cross-national prevalence and risk factors for suicidal ideation, plans and attempts. British Journal of Psychiatry, 192, 98–105. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.040113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Nock MK, Borges G, Bromet EJ, Cha CB, Kessler RC, & Lee S. (2008). Suicide and suicidal behavior. Epidemiologic Reviews, 30, 133–154. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxn002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Nock MK, Green JG, Hwang I, McLaughlin KA, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, & Kessler RC. (2013). Prevalence, correlates, and treatment of lifetime suicidal behavior among adolescents: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 300–310. doi: 10.1001/2013.jamapsychiatry.55 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. *Nrugham L, Larsson B, & Sund AM. (2008). Specific depressive symptoms and disorders as associates and predictors of suicidal acts across adolescence. Journal of Affective Disorders, 111, 83–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.02.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. O’Brien KHM, Liu RT, Putney J, Burke TA, & Aguinaldo L. (2017). Suicide and non-suicidal self-injury In Smalley KB, Warren JC, & Barefoot KN. (Eds.), LGBT Health: Meeting the Health Needs of Gender and Sexual Minorities (pp. 181–198). New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  91. *O’Riley AA, Van Orden KA, He H, Richardson TM, Podgorski C, & Conwell Y. (2014). Suicide and death ideation in older adults obtaining aging services. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22, 614–622. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2012.12.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Osman A, Bagge CL, Gutierrez PM, Konick LC, Kopper BA, & Barrios FX. (2001). The Suicidal Behaviors Questionnaire-Revised (SBQ-R): validation with clinical and nonclinical samples. Assessment, 8, 443–454. doi: 10.1177/107319110100800409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. *Pawluk LK, Hurwitz TD, Schluter JL, Ullevig C, & Mahowald MW. (1995). Psychiatric morbidity in narcoleptics on chronic high dose methylphenidate therapy. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 183, 45–48. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199501000-00009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. *Rabkin JG, Remien R, Katoff L, & Williams JBW. (1993). Suicidality in AIDS long-term survivors: What is the evidence? AIDS Care, 5, 401–411. doi: 10.1080/09540129308258010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. *Raue PJ, Meyers BS, Rowe JL, Heo M, & Bruce ML. (2007). Suicidal ideation among elderly homecare patients. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22, 32–37. doi: 10.1002/gps.1649 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Reardon LE, Leen-Feldner EW, & Hayward C. (2009). A critical review of the empirical literature on the relation between anxiety and puberty. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 1–23. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.09.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. *Robertson K, Parsons TD, Van Der Horst C, & Hall C. (2006). Thoughts of death and suicidal ideation in nonpsychiatric human immunodeficiency virus seropositive individuals. Death Studies, 30, 455–469. doi: 10.1080/07481180600614435 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. *Romans SE, Tyas J, Cohen MM, & Silverstone T. (2007). Gender differences in the symptoms of major depressive disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195, 905–911. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181594cb7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. *Roth KB, Borges G, Medina-Mora ME, Orozco R, Ouéda C, & Wilcox HC. (2011). Depressed mood and antisocial behavior problems as correlates for suicide-related behaviors in Mexico. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45, 596–602. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. *Rufino KA, Viswanath H, Wagner R, & Patriquin MA. (2018). Body dissatisfaction and suicidal ideation among psychiatric inpatients with eating disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 84, 22–25. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2018.03.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. *Saïas T, Beck F, Bodard J, Guignard R, & du Roscoät E. (2012). Social Participation, Social Environment and Death Ideations in Later Life. PLoS ONE, 7, 1–8. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046723 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. *Schimanski ID, Mouat KL, Billinghurst BL, & Linscott RJ. (2017). Preliminary evidence that schizophrenia liability at age 15 predicts suicidal ideation two years later. Schizophrenia Research, 181, 60–62. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.08.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. *Scocco P, Meneghel G, Dello Buono M, & De Leo D. (2001). Hostility as a feature of elderly suicidal ideators. Psychological Reports, 88, 863–868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. *Segal DL, Gottschling J, Marty M, Meyer WJ, & Coolidge FL. (2015). Relationships among depressive, passive-aggressive, sadistic and self-defeating personality disorder features with suicidal ideation and reasons for living among older adults. Aging and Mental Health, 19, 1071–1077. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2014.1003280 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Simon RI. (2014). Passive suicidal ideation: Still a high-risk clinical scenario. Current Psychiatry, 13, 13–15. [Google Scholar]
  106. *Smith MT, Edwards RR, Robinson RC, & Dworkin RH. (2004). Suicidal ideation, plans, and attempts in chronic pain patients: Factors associated with increased risk. Pain, 111, 201–208. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.06.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. *Smith PN, Gamble SA, Cort NA, Ward EA, Conwell Y, & Talbot NL. (2012). The relationships of attachment style and social maladjustment to death ideation in depressed women with a history of childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68, 78–87. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20852 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. *Snyder M, Gertler R, & Ferneau E. (1973). Changes in nursing students’ attitudes toward death and dying: A measurement of curriculum integration effectiveness. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 19, 294–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. *Storch EA, Kay B, Wu MS, Nadeau JM, & Riemann B. (2017). Suicidal and death ideation among adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder presenting for intensive intervention. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 29, 46–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. *Tal I, Mauro C, Reynolds CF, Shear MK, Simon N, Lebowitz B, Skritskaya N, et al. (2017). Complicated grief after suicide bereavement and other causes of death. Death Studies, 41, 267–275. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2016.1265028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. *Tateno M, Jovanović N, Beezhold J, Uehara-Aoyama K, Umene-Nakano W, Nakamae T, Uchida N, et al. (2018). Suicidal ideation and burnout among psychiatric trainees in Japan. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 12, 935–937. doi: 10.1111/eip.12466 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Ullsperger JM, & Nikolas MA. (2017). A meta-analytic review of the association between pubertal timing and psychopathology in adolescence: Are there sex differences in risk? Psychological Bulletin, 143, 903–938. doi: 10.1037/bul0000106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. *Undheim AM. (2013). Involvement in bullying as predictor of suicidal ideation among 12- to 15-year-old Norwegian adolescents. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 22, 357–365. doi: 10.1007/s00787-012-0373-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. *Urrila AS, Karlsson L, Kiviruusu O, Pelkonen M, Strandholm T, & Marttunen M. (2012). Sleep complaints among adolescent outpatients with major depressive disorder. Sleep Medicine, 13, 816–823. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2012.04.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. *van Duijn E, Vrijmoeth EM, Giltay EJ, & Bernhard Landwehrmeyer G. (2018). Suicidal ideation and suicidal behavior according to the C-SSRS in a European cohort of Huntington’s disease gene expansion carriers. Journal of Affective Disorders, 228, 194–204. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. *Van Orden KA, O’Riley AA, Simning A, Podgorski C, Richardson TM, & Conwell Y. (2015). Passive suicide ideation: An indicator of risk among older adults seeking aging services? Gerontologist, 55, 972–980. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnu026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. *Van Orden KA, Simning A, Conwell Y, Skoog I, & Waern M. (2013). Characteristics and comorbid symptoms of older adults reporting death ideation. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21, 803–810. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2013.01.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. *Vera M, Reyes-Rabanillo ML, Huertas S, Juarbe D, Perez-Pedrogo C, Huertas A, & Pena M. (2011). Suicide ideation, plans, and attempts among general practice patients with chronic health conditions in Puerto Rico. International Journal of General Medicine, 4, 197–205. doi: 10.2147/IJGM.S17156 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. *Villa J, Choi J, Kangas JL, Kaufmann CN, Harvey PD, & Depp CA. (2018). Associations of suicidality with cognitive ability and cognitive insight in outpatients with Schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 192, 340–344. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.06.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. *Walker J, Hansen CH, Butcher I, Sharma N, Wall L, Murray G, & Sharpe M. (2011). Thoughts of death and suicide reported by cancer patients who endorsed the “suicidal thoughts” item of the PHQ-9 during routine screening for depression. Psychosomatics, 52, 424–427. doi: 10.1016/j.psym.2011.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. WHO World Mental Health Survey Consortium. (2004). Prevalence, severity, and unmet need for treatment of mental disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Journal of American Medical Association, 291, 2581–2590. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.21.2581 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Williams JMG, Barnhofer T, Crane C, & Beck AT. (2005). Problem solving deteriorates following mood challenge in formerly depressed patients with a history of suicidal ideation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 421–431. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.3.421 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Wolff JC, Davis S, Liu RT, Cha CB, Cheek SM, Nestor BA, Frazier EA, et al. (2018). Trajectories of suicidal ideation among adolescents following psychiatric hospitalization. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46, 355–363. doi: 10.1007/s10802-017-0293-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. World Health Organization. (2014). Preventing suicide: a global imperative. Geneva: World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
  125. *Yoder KA, Whitbeck LB, & Hoyt DR. (2008). Dimensionality of thoughts of death and suicide: Evidence from a study of homeless adolescents. Social Indicators Research, 86, 83–100. doi: 10.1007/s11205-007-9095-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  126. Zhou ES, Hu JC, Kantoff PW, & Recklitis CJ. (2015). Identifying suicidal symptoms in prostate cancer survivors using brief self-report. Journal of Cancer Survivorship, 9, 59–67. doi: 10.1007/s11764-014-0385-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES