Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Mar 12;15(3):e0230256. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0230256

Maternal depression in Latinas and child socioemotional development: A systematic review

Rebeca Alvarado Harris 1, Hudson P Santos Jr 1,2,*
Editor: James Swain3
PMCID: PMC7067456  PMID: 32163494

Abstract

Background

Although substantial research exists on the debilitating effects of maternal depression on child development, little is known about Latina mothers with depression and their young children within the broader context of sociocultural and economic stressors.

Objectives

What is the relationship between maternal depression in Latina mothers and their children’s socioemotional outcomes through early developmental windows (0–5 years)?

Methods

We searched electronic databases PubMed, CINAHL, and PsycINFO in this systematic review, pre-registered via PROSPERO (CRD42019128686). Based on pre-determined criteria, we identified 56 studies and included 15 in the final sample. After extracting data, we assessed study quality with the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies.

Results

We found inverse correlations between maternal depression and child socioemotional outcomes; furthermore, we found evidence of a moderating and mediating role of maternal depression between contextual stressors and child outcomes. Children of U.S.-born Latina mothers had poorer developmental outcomes than children of foreign-born Latina mothers across socioemotional domains and throughout early developmental windows.

Conclusions

Future research must examine underlying mechanisms for the potential Latino paradox in young Latino children’s socioemotional outcomes. Policies should support mental health of Latina mothers as early as the prenatal period.

Introduction

Substantial research exists on maternal depression and childhood poverty and their debilitating effects on both maternal health and early child development. Although most of the research implicating the pathways by which maternal depression disrupts child development has been conducted on European-White descendent families [1], evidence suggests that the mother-child relationship is situated within the economic and sociocultural contexts in which families live [2]. As such, the intersection between maternal depression and poverty may affect children from ethnic minorities differently. Although Latino families are affected disproportionately by poverty [3], we found few research studies and no systematic reviews on the effect of maternal depression on child development within this vulnerable population. Latina mothers living in poverty experience numerous stressors related to both social adversity and ethnic minority status and are subsequently susceptible to depressive symptomology [4]. Prevalence rates of depression (mild to severe) in Latina mothers range from 12 to 59% in the perinatal period, compared to 10–15% in the general population [5]. Moreover, lack of accessible, affordable, and culturally competent mental health care for Latina mothers has been well-documented [6]. Little is known, however, about the extent to which mental health in Latinas is related to their children’s socioemotional development. This is the first systematic review to consolidate studies examining the relationship between Latina mothers’ depression and child socioemotional outcomes. Based on a social-ecological perspective, our review aims to study interpersonal and contextual pathways linking maternal depression in Latinas to a young child’s (birth to five years old) socioemotional outcomes in the broader context of sociocultural and economic stressors (Fig 1).

Fig 1. Potential pathways linking maternal depression in Latinas to child socioemotional outcomes.

Fig 1

Socioemotional outcomes

Socioemotional competencies underlie early childhood mental health and encompass “a child’s developing capacity from birth through five years of age to form close and secure adult and peer relationships; experience, regulate, and express emotions in socially and culturally appropriate ways; and explore the environment and learn—all in the context of family, community, and culture.” [7] (p. 2). The relationship between maternal depression and disruptions in a young child’s socioemotional development and related behavioral manifestations has been well replicated throughout infancy and early childhood [1,8].

Our review focuses on adverse early social and emotional outcomes associated with maternal depression and greater risk for psychopathology later in life [9]: 1) temperamental traits (e.g., higher negative emotionality and lower self-regulatory capacity) in infants that increase vulnerability to stressful caregiving environments; 2) infant social withdrawal behavior; 3) insecure mother-child attachment [10]; and 4) internalizing (e.g., fearfulness, anxiety) and externalizing (e.g., aggression) behaviors in toddlers and preschoolers [9]. We focus on maternal rather than paternal depression due to the high prevalence of maternal depression during sensitive developmental periods [11] and fetal exposure to prenatal depression during pregnancy.

Maternal sensitivity

Prenatally, maternal depression may alter biochemical pathways associated with physiological and emotional regulation, thus, undermining the foundation on which children develop socioemotional competencies [12,13]. Adverse outcomes, however, may still be mitigated by nurturing mother-child interactions [14]. The early social and cultural caregiving environment continues to shape a young child’s socioemotional developmental trajectory and, thereby, promotes future resiliency or exacerbates risk for mental health disorders over the life course [15]. The influence of maternal depression on parenting behaviors (e.g. maternal sensitivity) may underlie the relationship between maternal depression and child socioemotional maladjustment [16,17]. Mothers who are depressed may be less able to provide sensitive interactions needed to develop and sustain early socioemotional competencies, especially in the context of economic stressors [18,19].

Given that cultural beliefs and values influence parenting and subsequent child development [20,21], the interaction between maternal sensitivity and maternal mental health on young children’s socioemotional outcomes may differ based on sociocultural context [2,22,23]. In fact, similar parenting behaviors often result in contradictory socioemotional child outcomes depending on the cultural meaning underlying the behavior [20]. The effects of maternal depression on maternal sensitivity and subsequent mother-child interactions are both a modifiable and necessary target for intervention [24]. Empirical evidence suggests that integrated interventions during the first five years of life to reduce maternal depression and enhance parenting skills may ultimately improve children’s socioemotional developmental trajectories [16], particularly for the most vulnerable mother-child dyads [25]. However, culturally relevant interventions necessitate further research into the intersection of these variables and related child outcomes within the broader sociocultural and economic context.

Economic and sociocultural stressors

The relationship between mother and child occurs within the context of poverty in >26% of Latino families with young children [26]. Poverty-related stressors, which have also been used as a proxy for poverty, encompass numerous risk factors for children: food insecurity, overcrowded housing, and community violence. In fact, meta-analyses within the general population have shown that poverty heightens the significant positive association between maternal depression and children’s socioemotional difficulties [1,27]. Ethnic minority status also magnifies these effects, but understudied social and cultural pathways underlying such disparities have not been disentangled [1].

Theoretical frameworks such as the Family Stress Model (FSM) hypothesize that economic stressors engender parental stress and depression, compromising parental quality which could otherwise serve as a child’s protective buffer from poverty; as such, maternal psychological functioning may serve as a mediating mechanism by which poverty-related stressors predict child maladjustment [28]. The few FSM studies examining Latino children have focused primarily on middle childhood and adolescence and provide evidence for the mediating role of maternal depression between poverty-related stressors and socioemotional child outcomes [29,30] as well as revealing culture-specific ways in which these variables may interact [31,32]. However, not only do we know little about the validity of these models for Latino children during early childhood [33] but culture-specific stressors have rarely been used as anchors for these studies–despite the integral role of cultural influences on developmental processes [34,35].

Culture-specific stressors (e.g., discrimination, stressful life events, and acculturative stress) have predicted parental depression [36,37] and provide key sociocultural contexts in which to examine maternal and child health [38]. Acculturative stress, stemming from psychosocial adjustment to a host culture, may affect both immigrants and later generations who have simultaneously assimilated to a new culture and maintained cultural heritage [39]. In immigrant women, acculturative stress is often accompanied by family separation, social and linguistic isolation, and discrimination [40]—salient contributors of perinatal depression among Mexican-American mothers [41]. The cumulative effects of these stressors on the relationship between maternal mental health and the child’s gestational and early childhood experiences may alter not only the child’s life course but the physiology of subsequent generations via biological (e.g., epigenetic) mechanisms [38,42,43] and interpersonal (e.g., parenting) processes [42]. The Latino paradox, wherein less acculturated Latinos experience better health outcomes than more acculturated Latinos, has been documented for maternal and child health: birth weights [44] and breastfeeding rates [45]. However, investigators have not, to our knowledge, explored the relationship between maternal biopsychosocial functioning (e.g., depression) and early child socioemotional outcomes in the context of the Latino paradox.

Current study

Although the Latino population comprises the largest ethnic minority group in the US [3], young Latino children remain an understudied population. For public policies to be effective, we need to know what harms and what protects children most profoundly amid the economic and sociocultural contexts in which their lives unfold. Research relevant to understanding Latina mothers and their young children may inform interventions that meaningfully support mothers struggling with depression, thereby potentially minimizing or even averting a developmental cascade that undermines critical socioemotional competencies formed early in life. Therefore, this systematic review addresses the following question: during the foundational years from the prenatal period to early childhood (0–5 years), how is maternal depression in Latina mothers related to their children’s socioemotional outcomes?

Methods

This systematic review is pre-registered via PROSPERO (CRD42019128686) and follows the guidelines recommended in the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) [46]. Ethical approval was not required because data was synthesized from previously published studies.

Study inclusion criteria

We screened studies for eligibility based on predetermined inclusion criteria per the study protocol. To be considered for inclusion, the study needed to assess the relationship between maternal depressive symptoms and children’s socioemotional outcomes within Latina mother-child dyads living in the US. We used the term “Latina” to refer to a female of Latin American origin or descent (e.g., Mexico, Cuba, Puerto Rico, South and Central America), regardless of race. Maternal depression could encompass depressive symptoms on a continuum, elevated scores on self-report measures, or clinical diagnosis of a depressive disorder. We did not exclude for co-morbid symptoms or diagnoses such as anxiety, stress, or another form of psychological distress. Self-report depression measures (e.g. CES-D) often used in population-based studies may more broadly capture psychological distress and diverse affective symptoms (e.g. anxiety) rather than symptoms specific to a depressive disorder [18,47,48]. We defined socioemotional development as a child’s emerging capacity for the emotional, behavioral, and social competencies underlying early childhood mental health [9]. Further inclusion criteria were a) young children exposed to maternal depression at any point prenatally up until age five, allowing us to focus on foundational sensitive early developmental periods; b) adult mothers ≥ 18 years old; and c) specified maternal economic status. Studies of subpopulations with confounding secondary diseases or conditions altering maternal depressive symptoms or socioemotional outcomes (e.g., cancer, natural disasters, or developmental delays) were excluded. There were no time restrictions applied to ensure comprehensive results; we completed the search in October 2018. Studies could be in English or Spanish.

Search strategy

We identified studies through manual searches and through PubMed, CINAHL, and PsycINFO. We applied a comprehensive search string to all three databases to capture studies examining the impact of maternal depression on young children’s socioemotional and cognitive outcomes in the Latino population: (Postpartum OR Depression OR Depressive Disorder OR Depress*) AND (Postpartum Period OR Postnatal Care OR Prepartum OR pre-partum OR prenatal OR pre-natal OR antenatal OR perinatal OR peripartum OR postpartum OR post-partum OR postnatal OR post-natal OR puerperium OR parturition OR pregnanc* OR pregnant or mother* OR newborn*) AND (Hispanic Americans OR Latina* OR Latino* OR Hispanic*) AND (child* OR infant OR toddler OR preschooler OR pediatric) AND (develop* OR behavior* OR cognitive OR emotion* OR conduct OR language OR temperament OR socio* OR regulation OR internalizing OR externalizing OR psych*). We manually searched reference lists from relevant literature for any additional eligible studies.

Study selection and data extraction

We selected studies using the four-phase process for systematic reviews recommended by the PRISMA Group [46] (Fig 2). From the electronic database search, we identified 914 studies after removing 466 duplicates. We screened titles and abstracts from identified studies for eligibility and excluded 858 studies not meeting inclusion criteria. We retrieved the full text of the 56 remaining to determine eligibility, and recorded reasons for exclusion for the 38 non-eligible studies. In the final phase, 17 studies from the database search and one study from the manual search met all eligibility criteria and were selected for data extraction. Data from included literature was extracted onto a template which included study purpose, first author, publication year, study design, sample characteristics, maternal depression measures and data collection points, socioemotional or cognitive outcome measures and data collection points, main analytical approach and covariates, and main results. When available, country of origin and acculturation data was extracted. A second reviewer evaluated the extracted data for thoroughness. During the extraction phase, two studies did not include maternal age ranges, and three studies included broad maternal age ranges that extended into adolescence. Reviewers agreed to include these studies; the broad age range would provide a representative sample of Latina mothers in the United States. We contacted authors for maternal age distribution if needed. Moreover, we included three dissertations meeting inclusion criteria.

Fig 2. PRISMA flow chart diagram.

Fig 2

We extracted data for cognitive outcomes because they are interwoven with developing socioemotional competencies early in life [9]. However, these were excluded from our final qualitative synthesis and placed in a supplemental table in order to focus our attention on early social and emotional outcomes (S1 Table).

Quality assessment

Study quality (Table 1) was assessed by two reviewers following the guidelines established in the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI) Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies [49].

Table 1. Quality assessment tool for observational cohort and cross-sectional studies.

Aisenberg, (2001) Burtchen et al., (2013) De Leon Siantz et al., (2010) Doudna, (2016) Fuller et al., (2018) Huang et al., (2012) La Roche et al., (1995) Luecken et al., (2015) Lequerica et. al, (1995) Martinez (2014) Mennen et al., (2015) Palermo et al., (2018) Somers et al., (2018) Waters et al., (2015) Westbrook & Harden, (2010)
Was the research question or objective clearly stated? Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
Was the study population clearly specified and defined? Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
Was the participation rate of eligible persons at least 50%? N NR NR N NR Y Y Y Y Y Y NR NR NR Y
Were all the subjects selected or recruited from the same or similar populations (including the same time period)? Were inclusion and exclusion criteria for being in the study pre-specified and applied uniformly to all participants? Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
Was a sample size justification, power description, or variance and effect estimates provided? N N N N N N N N N Y N N N N N
For the analyses in this paper, were the exposure(s) of interest measured prior to the outcome(s) being measured? N N N N Y Y Y Y N Y Y Y Y Y Y
Was the timeframe sufficient so that one could reasonably expect to see an association between exposure and outcome if it existed? N N N N Y Y Y Y N Y Y Y Y Y Y
For exposures that can vary in amount or level, did the study examine different levels of the exposure as related to the outcome (e.g., categories of exposure, or exposure measured as continuous variable)? Y Y N N N Y N N N Y N N Y Y Y
Were the exposure measures (independent variables) clearly defined, valid, reliable, and implemented consistently across all study participants? Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
Was the exposure(s) assessed more than once over time? N N N N N Y Y Y N N Y N Y Y N
Were the outcome measures (dependent variables) clearly defined, valid, reliable, and implemented consistently across all study participants? Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
Were the outcome assessors blinded to the exposure status of participants? NR NR NR NR NR NR Y NR N NR NR NR NR NR NR
Was loss to follow-up after baseline 20% or less? Y Y Y Y N Y Y Y Y N N N N N Y
Were key potential confounding variables measured and adjusted statistically for their impact on the relationship between exposure(s) and outcome(s)? N N Y N Y Y N Y N Y Y Y Y Y Y

Y = Yes, N = No, NR = Not reported

Results

Study characteristics

In this systematic review, we had a final sample of 15 studies examining Latina maternal depression in relation to child socioemotional development (Table 2). Study samples ranged from 26 to 1,600 mother-child participants per study (totaling 5,656 dyads). Participants in most studies (n = 14) came from low-income backgrounds, except one study using data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study–Birth Cohort (ECLS-B)—a nationally representative cohort of children born in 2001. Income levels for participants in the ECLS-B ranged from ≤ 20,0000 to ≥75,0000. Country of origin was reported in seven studies; six studies had mostly Mexican descent participants while one study was predominantly represented by participants of Puerto Rican descent [50]. Five studies employed samples from Head Start programs, which serves low-income families. Twelve of 15 studies were completed within the past ten years, and nine of those studies were published within the past five years.

Table 2. Summary of studies included in the review (n = 15, sample size = 5,656).

Study Purpose Study Design Sample/Setting Measure of Maternal Depression and data collection points Measure of Socio-emotional outcome and data collection points Country of origin and any acculturation data Analysis and covariates Results
Prenatal and Infancy
Burtchen et al., (2013) To examine the relationship between maternal major depression and infant social withdrawal behavior Cross-sectional: infant’s 6- month primary care visit n = 155 DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for Major Depression Disorder at 6 months after delivery: standardized psychiatric interview by a board-certified bilingual psychiatrist Infant social withdrawal behavior at 6-months: ADBB (scale modified with author’s permission) Country of origin not reported maternal time in U.S. (M = 8.2 years) Analysis: chi-square test or independent samples t test Infants with mothers diagnosed with major depression scored significantly higher on infant withdrawal behaviors than infants of mothers without depression, (6.1 vs 3.34, p ≤ .001)
(93%) Low-income predominantly (87%) Latina mother-child dyads from a subsample of a research project examining perinatal mood disorders and infant development
Covariates: potential confounding variables (i.e. maternal age, mode of delivery, number of children, child gender, tobacco, alcohol, and drug use) measured and deemed non-confounding
Exposure to clinical depression: (mean = 4 months prenatal, 6 months postnatal)
Infants with depressed mothers scored significantly higher on intrapersonal and interpersonal social withdrawal behaviors: facial expression, vocalizations, relationship, attraction, eye contact, and self-stimulation
Maternal age: >18
M = 28
SD = 6.2
6-month old full-term infants with no medical disorder or physical complaints at primary care routine visits
Significantly negatively correlated with maternal major depression: parents living together, father involved in childcare, emotional support, food security
Fuller et al., (2018) To examine the relationship between prenatal material hardship with infant temperament at 10 months; prenatal depression used as a moderator Longitudinal:
28-32-weeks gestation and 10 months of age
n = 412 PHQ-9 prenatally:
28-32-week gestation
Infant temperament (orienting/regulatory capacity, negative effect, surgency / extraversion) at 10 months: IBQ, Very Short Form U.S. or foreign-born mothers Analysis: linear regression Prenatal depressive symptoms significantly moderated the relationship between neighborhood stress and orienting/regulatory capacity scores, (standardized ß = -0.28, ß = -0.86; SE, 0.26; 95% CI, -1.37 to -0.34)
Low-income Hispanic pregnant mothers enrolled in an obesity prevention program (Starting Early) uncomplicated singleton pregnancies at primary care clinics
Country of origin not reported Covariates: marital status, immigration status, education, parity, infant sex, infant birth weight, intervention group, prenatal depression (used as either covariate or moderator), PPD at 3 months
U.S. born mothers had infants with higher negativity ratings (p = .01) unrelated to depressive symptoms
Maternal age:
>18
Luecken et al., (2015) (1) Examine the relationship between prenatal maternal depressive symptoms and infant temperamental negativity as predictors of infant cortisol response (2) Explore the interaction between maternal depressive symptoms and infant negativity Longitudinal:
(26 to 38-week gestation), 6 and 12 weeks postpartum
n = 322 10-item EPDS≥13 prenatally (26 to 38- week gestation) Infant temperament negativity at 6 weeks: infant negativity subscale of IBQ-R 86% born in Mexico; 14% U.S. born; 82% mostly Spanish speaking Analysis: regression analyses (not specified) Higher prenatal maternal depressive symptoms correlated with elevated cortisol measures for 12-week old infants with high temperamental negativity but lower cortisol for infants with low negativity, (unstandardized estimate for the interaction = .019, SE = .007, 95% CI [.005, .033], p = .008).
Mexican-American low-income (below $25,000) mother-infant dyads with healthy singleton pregnancies at hospital-based prenatal clinic
Covariates: time of day, postpartum: maternal AUCg, depressive symptoms, mood
Infant dysregulation: saliva cortisol at 12 weeks: AUCg—prior to task(T0), at 0 (T1), 20 (T2), and 40 (T3) minutes after task complete Acculturation measured (as potential covariate) by ARSMA II.
9 and 12 weeks post-partum
Models analyzed with and without covariates
Mexican-born mothers reported decreased infant negativity compared to U.S.-born mothers(p < .01), significantly unrelated to other measured variables in study
Maternal age: M = 27.8 SD = 6.5
18–42
The effect of low social support followed a similar trend.
Neither low prenatal depressive symptoms nor high prenatal social support predicted infant cortisol outcomes.
Bi-directionally, higher infant negativity at 6 weeks predicted increased maternal depressive symptoms at 12 weeks (unstandardized ß = 1.12, t (297) = 3.29, p = .001; model R2 = .18).
Toddlerhood
Huang et al., (2012) To examine the relationship between maternal depression, maternal sensitivity and child attachment Longitudinal:
9 and 24 months of age
n = 1600 Modified CES-D at 9 months Child attachment measured at 24 months: TAS-45 Country of origin not reported Analysis: logistic regression and ANOVA Chronic maternal depressive symptoms in Hispanic mothers posed the highest odds ratio for child insecure attachment (OR = 8.12, 95% CI = 1.07–61.68, p = .04).
Mother-child dyads from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study–Birth Cohort
CIDI Short Form at 24 months U.S. Born Hispanic: n = 750
Covariates: household income, maternal education, childcare arrangement
Foreign Born Hispanic:
n = 850
Maternal Age, U.S.-born:
M = 26
15–40+
Maternal sensitivity at 24 months:
Two Bag Task
“Maternal sensitivity” did not significantly mediate the relationship between maternal depression and insecure attachment (OR = .85, 95% CI = .71–1.03, p = .10).
Compared with U.S. born Hispanic women, foreign-born mothers are significantly less likely to have an insecurely attached child (OR = .69, 95% CI = .51-.95, p = .02).
Maternal age, foreign-born: M = 27.8
15–40+
Child age at Time 1:
9 months
The most common depressive pattern in Hispanic women, later onset at 24 months, predicted the least likelihood for insecure attachment at 24 months (OR = .32, 95% CI = .12-.88, p = .03).
Child age at Time 2:
24 months
Martinez, (2014) To examine the relationship between maternal depression and child behaviors (aggression, compliance, and negative emotionality) Longitudinal:
Baseline and 6-month follow up
n = 47 CES-D≥16 at baseline Child behavior (aggression, compliance, and negative emotionality) at baseline and at 6- month follow-up: CBCL and ITSEA-R Country of origin not reported Analysis: descriptive statistics, zero-order correlations, and hierarchal multiple regression analyses Maternal depressive symptoms at baseline significantly predicted child negative emotionality 6 months later: [F (5, 23) = 3.56, p < .05, R2 = .44, R2Δ = .09, p < .10]
Latinas with limited English proficiency, low-income and a positive depression screen; mother-child dyads from a larger Interpersonal Psychotherapy study enrolled in Early Head Start programs
Years in U.S: M = 5.3 years, SD = 5.9
Acculturation: SASH Covariates: child gender, child age, treatment condition, and child behavior at baseline
Severity of maternal depressive symptoms x child negative emotionality at baseline improved models predicting child negative emotionality 6 months later:
F (5, 22) = 4.10, p < .01, R2 = .53, R2Δ = .09, p = .051
Maternal age: M = 27.13
SD = 5.6
Child age:
M = 23.1 months
SD = 8.7
Maternal depressive symptoms significantly moderated the relationship between negative emotionality at baseline and at 6-month follow-up:
(B = -.02, p < .05)
Depression severity significantly moderated these associations:
Low: t (22) = 3.74, p = .001
Average: t (22) = 3.47, p = .002
High: t (22) = 1.58, n.s.
Child aggression and compliance were not significantly associated with maternal depression
Preschool
Aisenberg, (2001) To examine the psychological and behavioral effects of exposure to community violence Cross-Sectional:
maternal and child measures at study entry
n = 31 “Maternal Distress Symptomology” at study entry: Child behavioral functioning measured at study entry: CBCL 80.6%: Born in Mexico Analysis: Univariate t tests without correction for multiple comparisons and bivariate correlations, multiple regressions Maternal distress symptomatology did not moderate relationship between exposure to community violence and CBCL scores
Low-income Latina mother-child dyads enrolled in a Head Start program
6.1%: Born in El Salvador
Adult PTSD: IES-R 12.9%: U.S.-born mothers
Maternal age:
M = 28.74
SD = 4.77
21–43
Maternal distress symptomatology significantly mediated the relationship between exposure to community violence and CBCL scores, ß = .45, p < .05.
Depression and anxiety:
BSI≥63
Covariates: none reported
Child age:
48–58 months
M = 53.22
SD = 2.16
Proportion of children with behavior problems based on CBCL≥60: 30%.
De Leon Siantz et al., (2010) To examine the relationship between maternal functioning and child behavior problems Cross-Sectional Prospective Design: maternal and child measures at study entry n = 205 “Maternal Functioning” at study entry: Child behavioral functioning measured at study entry: CBCL Acculturation measured by: HHANES and language preference 35.9% born in U.S., 61.4% born in Mexico Analysis: step-wise regressions Internalizing problems significantly predicted by both maternal stress and depressive symptoms in both genders: (ß = .295, R2 = .164, df = 2,145, F = 15.99, p = .000)
Latino children from “Migrant Head Start Programs”,
Covariate: Maternal years in U.S.
Depression: CES-D ≥16,
Maternal age: M = 32
SD = 7.43
19–67
How long child has been in Head Start could be confounding variable but not addressed
Maternal stress: FILE Maternal stress and depressive symptoms more likely to predict behavior problems in girls while maternal stress and harsher parenting style more likely to predict behavior problems in boys
Child age:
36–72 months M = 51
SD = 6.24
Parenting Style: PARQ
Proportion of children with behavior problems based on CBCL 12%
Doudna, (2016) To examine the relationship between household food insecurity, parenting alliance, and maternal depressive symptoms on child socioemotional outcomes within Latino families in rural America using the Family Stress Model Cross-sectional: maternal and child measures at study entry n = 99 CES-D, short form ≥10 at study entry Child behavioral functioning measured at study entry: CBCL Not reported Analysis: path analysis Depressive symptoms were negatively associated with parenting alliance (ß = -2.7, p = .12)
Low-income Latina mothers living in rural communities enrolled in “Rural Families Speak about Health” project
Covariate: financial distress measured by PFW
Maternal Depressive symptoms did not significantly mediate the relationship between household food insecurity and child behavioral functioning
No other potential confounding variables such as wide age range measured
Maternal age:
>18
M = 32.36 SD = 7.87
Child age:
18–71 months
La Roche et al., (1995) To examine the relationship between toddlers' behavioral difficulties, mothers' depression, self-efficacy, and social support Longitudinal:
Baseline and 3-month follow-up
n = 26 At baseline and 3-month follow-up: Preschool Country of origin and acculturation data not reported Analysis: Pearson correlations, multiple regression analyses Significant relationship between perceived social support at baseline and maternal depressive symptoms at 3-month follow-up, r = -.46 (p<0.05), -.51 (p < .05)
Low SES, Spanish-speaking Latina mother-child dyads attending a behavioral group in a community mental health center Behavioral Checklist completed by both bilingual psychologists and mothers at baseline (independent variable) and 3-month follow-up (dependent variable)
Maternal age:
M = 26.7 years
21–36
Social Support:
Norbeck Social Suport Network
Scale
Covariates: none reported Maternal depressive symptoms at time one were not significantly associated with their toddlers' behavioral difficulties at 3-month follow-up.
Child age:
M = 34 months
24–60
Depression:
CES-D≥16
Self-Efficacy:
The Maternal Efficacy
Questionnaire adapted for toddlers
Lequerica Et. al, (1995) To examine mothers' concerns about their children's behaviors at home in relation to stressful family life events, maternal depression, methods of discipline, and demographic factors Cross-sectional: maternal and child measures during out-patient pediatric clinic visit n = 52 At study entry: Child behavioral functioning measured at study entry: CBCL (shortened version with 65 questions) Country of origin:
Puerto Rico: 55.8% Santo Domingo/Central America: 34.6%
Analysis: Pearson correlations, chi squares, and analysis of variance CBCL scores were not significantly related to family life stressful events or maternal depressive symptoms.
Low-income mother-child dyads seeking services at a pediatric outpatient clinic Depression: subscale of Ilfeld's Psychiatric Symptom Index
South America: 9.6% Covariate: none reported
Maternal age:
Not reported
Stressful life events:
Social Readjustment Rating Scale by Holmes and Rahe,—shortened version
Acculturation data not reported CBCL questions with higher frequencies than normal or psychiatrically referred non-Latino children:
Child age:
24–36 months: 32.7%
36–48: 38.5% 48–60: 28.8%
Clings to adults: 80% of 4 to 5 year olds compared to 32% in Achenbach's normal
Unable to sit still/hyperactive: 90% of 4 to 5 year olds compared to 40% in Achenbach's normal
Mennen et al., (2015) To examine the relationship between clinical maternal depression and children’s progress during mental health treatment Longitudinal: at study entry and at 6-month intervals up to 3 years n = 147 CES-D ≥16 at study entry and at 6-month intervals up to 3 years Child behavioral functioning measured at study entry and at 6-month intervals up to 3 years: CBCL for children ages 1.5–5 Country of origin and acculturation data not reported Analysis: univariate statistics, growth curve modeling Significant associations between maternal depressive symptoms and child behavior problems at entry:
higher internalizing scores (17.70 vs 14.23, p = .018), higher externalizing scores (24.65 vs 21.45, p = .044), higher total CBCL scores (65.09 vs 52.99, p = .006), and lower socialization scores, (95.58 vs 103.36, p = .022)
Low-income predominantly Latina (94%) mother-child dyads
Covariates: child sex and child welfare involvement
Child in or at high risk for child welfare services, diagnosed with emotional, behavioral or mental disorder, and enrolled in an inner-city mental health treatment program (Project ABC) Study did not adjust for maternal depression, a potential confounding variable given self-reported child functioning measures
Adaptive functioning measured at study entry and at 6-month intervals for up to three years: Vineland Screener
Children’s CBCL scores improved more slowly when their mother was depressed: Internalizing = 1.59, p < .05; Externalizing = 17.75, p< .05; Total = 4.66, p < .05); daily living skills: time-by-depression interaction coefficient = 2.99, p < .05).
Maternal age: M = 30
SD = 6.6
Child age:
M = 34 months SD = 14.8, 58% boys
Children of mothers wither higher depressive symptoms: finished treatment with lower levels of behavioral functioning, received more services (96.3 vs. 61.4, F = 4.18, p = .043), and remained in treatment for longer (435 vs. 257 days, F = 4.41, p = .039)
No significant associations with maternal depressive symptoms on child’s communication, daily living skills, and motor skills
Palermo et al., (2018) To examine the relationship between economic hardship during infancy, “maternal mental health problems”, “maternal positive parenting behaviors”, and Latino children’s socio-behavioral difficulties and academic skills prior to kindergarten entry- using a culturally integrated Family Stress Model; do acculturation levels moderate the pattern of associations? Longitudinal: children at 4, 14, 24 and 36 months, and half a year before kindergarten n = 714 At 14 months, “maternal mental health problems” quantified by Depression: CES-D, Parenting Stress: PSI-SF, Perceived Control: Pearlin Mastery Scale Both measures within half a year before kindergarten:
-Socio-behavioral health problems (SBHP):
Problem Behavior scale
82% Mexican-American; 59% foreign-born; 9% Central American; 6% Puerto Rican Analysis:
Structural equation modeling
Positive association between economic hardship and maternal mental health problems (ß = .50, SE = .08, p < .001)
Early Head Start Research and Evaluation Project (EHSREP) Low-income Latina mothers
At 24 months old, mother’s acculturation levels measured by generational status, English use preference, and proficiency: Multi-cultural Acculturation Scale, Picture Vocabulary subscale of the Woodstock-Johnson Achievement III Covariates:
- child’s gender, maternal education, family structure
-At 14 months: hyperactivity measured by 1 item from the Bayley Scales of Infant Development II
Negative association between maternal mental health problems and maternal positive parenting behaviors (ß = -.16, SE = .06, p = .008)
Maternal age:
84.6%≥18
M = 24
SD = 6
13–43
Subscales: aggressive behavioral scale, hyperactive behavior scale
Maternal mental health problems and maternal positive behaviors did not significantly mediate the association between economic hardship and children’s socio-behavioral problems (ß = -.02, SE = .01, p = .061)
Child age at study entry: M = 4 months Maternal sensitivity at 14 months:
Three Bag Task
Maternal positive parenting behaviors significantly mediated the association between maternal mental health problems and social behavioral problems (ß = .03, SE = .02, p = .047)
Positive association between maternal acculturation and positive parenting behaviors (ß = .22, SE = .06, p < .001)
Relationship patterns did not vary by acculturation levels
Westbrook & Harden, (2010) To examine the relationship of proximal risk factors (family structure and maternal depression) and distal risk factors (community violence) on parenting style and subsequent preschool children’s socioemotional outcomes in an ethnically and racially stratified sample using the Family Stress Model Longitudinal: Data Analysis from the FACES 2000 cohort from Fall 2000 (Time 1) to Spring 2001 (Time 2) for families with children aged 3–4 years old n = 425 Modified CES-D used to measure depressive symptomology at baseline (Time 1) Measured at Fall 2000 and Spring 2001:
Social skills measured by selected items from the Personal Maturity Scale and the Social Skills Rating System
Country of origin and acculturation data not reported Analysis: structural equation model No significant direct associations between individual study variables (violence exposure, family structure, maternal depression, parenting style) on child socio-emotional outcomes
FACES 2000 (The Family and Child Experiences) study cohort derived from a Nationally representative sample of Head Start programs intended to measure program quality through socioemotional and academic outcomes
Covariates: maternal age, maternal education, maternal employment, family poverty status, child age, child gender, baseline social-emotional functioning
Behavior Problems measured by selected items on Personal Maturity Scale, CBCL, Teacher Report and the Behavior Problems Index Cumulative effect of variables explained 36% of the European-American model and 22% of the Latino model
Maternal age:
>18
M not provided
Child age:
M = 48.3 months
SD = 6.4 at baseline (Time 1)
Infancy through Early Childhood
Somers et al., (2018) (1) To examine the relationship between postpartum maternal depression and children’s biological sensitivity to behavior problems via children’s dysregulation and (2) To explore sex differences in behavior problems due to PPD Longitudinal:
Every 3 weeks postpartum from 6 to 24 weeks, at 24 months, and at 36 months
n = 322 10-item EPDS every 3 weeks from 6 weeks to 6 months ≥13 Child dysregulation at 24 months: validated dysregulation coding system 86% born in Mexico; 13.7% born in U.S Analysis: structural equation model Total child behavior problems were significantly predicted by PPD symptoms x infant RSA: (B = -0.221, SE(B) = 0.069, p = 0.001)
Low-income Mexican-American mother-child dyads from a larger study (Las Madres Nuevas)
Covariates: maternal country of origin, maternal age, number of biological children, and concurrent depressive symptoms
A healthy singleton infant Children of U.S.-born mothers:
-higher RSA, p = .003
-more behavior problems, p = .001 country of birth predicted behavior problems (B = -14.350, SE(B) = 4.712, p = 0.002 CI (-23.59, -5.12)
CES-D at 36 months (as covariate) ≥16 Child Behavior Problems at 36 months: 113-item CBCL Low infant RSA x PPD symptoms conferred the most susceptibility (B = 0.190, SE(B) = 0.097, p = 0.05)
Maternal age:
>18
M = 27.8 SD = 6.5
PPD symptoms x infant RSA predicted:
-internalizing behavior problems: (B = -0.083, SE (B) = 0.022, p ≤ .001)
-externalizing behavior problems, (B = -0.068, SE (B) = 0.025, p = 0.006)
Higher maternal 36 month depressive symptoms associations:
-lower infant RSA, r = -0.177, p = 0.22
-greater PPD symptoms, r = .349, p ≤ .001
-more behavior problems, r = 0.482, p ≤ .001
Higher depressive symptomology = more significant effect on child behavior problems
Child dysregulation was not significantly predicted by any of the study variables
No significant sex differences
Waters et al., (2015) Does chronic maternal depression and chronic overcrowding moderate the relationship between ANS (automatic nervous system) reactivity at 6 months and externalizing behaviors at 7 years old? Longitudinal:
6 months, 1 year, 3.5 years, and 7 years of age
n = 99 CES-D ≥16 at 1- year old and 3.5-year old visits Child externalizing problems at 7-years old: BASC-2 92% women born in Mexico, 94% only/mostly Spanish speaking Analysis: multiple linear regression In children with low resting sinus arrhythmia (RSA) reactivity, chronic maternal depressive symptoms predicted highest levels of externalizing difficulties at 7 years, t (74) 5 3.98, p < .001, 95% CI [10.04, 30.17].
The Center for the Health Assessment of Mothers and Children of Salinas birth cohort (CHAMACOS)
Covariates: child sex, maternal years in the U.S.
Mexican-American low-income mother-child dyads In children with high RSA reactivity, chronic maternal depressive symptoms predicted the lowest levels of externalizing difficulties at 7 years
Maternal age:
M = 26.49
SD = 4.75
18–42
No association between children’s automatic nervous system reactivity scores and maternal depressive symptoms
No associations between overcrowded housing and externalizing behaviors

ADBB: Alarm Distress Baby Scale; ARSM II: Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican Americans; BASC-2: Behavior Assessment System for Children 2; BSI: Brief Symptom Inventory; BSID-II: Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Second Edition; CES-D: Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression Scale; CBCL: Child Behavior Checklist; CIDI: Composite International Diagnostic Interview; IBQ-R: Infant Behavior Questionnaire Revised; IES-R: Impact of Events Scale–Revised; FILE: Family Inventory of Life Events and Changes Scale; ITSEA-R: Infant Toddler Social Emotional Assessment; HHANES: Hispanic Health and Nutrition Examination Survey; MDI Bayley: Bayley II Mental Developmental Index; PARQ: Parental Acceptance/Rejection Questionnaire; PFW: Personal Financial Wellness Scale; PHQ-9: Patient Health Questionnaire-9; PSI-SF: The Parenting Stress Index–Short Form; SASH: Short Acculturation Scale for Hispanics; TAS-45: Toddler Attachment Sort-45 Instrument;.

Study designs included five cross-sectional studies and 10 longitudinal studies. Most studies measured child socioemotional outcomes through mother-reported scales, such as a) Child Behavior Checklist for ages 1 ½ to five (CBCL, n = 8), which measures internalizing and externalizing behavioral functioning [51]; and b) Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ-R, n = 2) which assesses infant’s regulatory capacity, negative effect, and extraversion [52]. Maternal depressive symptomology was measured by self-reported symptoms in all but one study, which used the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for Major depressive disorder [53]. The most prevalent measure was the Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D, n = 9), which measures depression symptoms within the past week. Maternal acculturation was measured in nine studies, mostly through nativity (n = 6). Two studies measured maternal sensitivity: a mother’s ability to perceive, interpret, and appropriately respond to child signals [54]. Measures of maternal sensitivity included The Three Bag Task or Two Bag Task [55], in which the dyad is asked to play with three toys during a 15-minute video-recorded session; trained coders later assess parent behavior.

Data synthesis

We grouped key findings from selected studies into three categories based on individual, interpersonal, and contextual pathways linking maternal depression to child development (Fig 3): 1) child’s socioemotional outcomes in relation to chronological developmental windows and the course and timing of maternal depression (prenatal, infancy, toddlerhood, preschool, and infancy through early childhood); 2) associations between maternal depression and maternal sensitivity in relation to child socioemotional outcomes; and 3) contextual stressors culturally relevant to Latina mothers and their young children living in poverty (economic and sociocultural stressors).

Fig 3. Timeline of studies reviewed (n = 15) based on maternal depression and child outcome assessments.

Fig 3

Socioemotional outcomes

Prenatal

Studies (n = 2) conducted in the prenatal period found a significant relationship between prenatal depressive symptoms and infant self-regulation [56,57]. In Fuller et al.’s study, higher prenatal maternal depressive symptoms on a continuum (Patient Health Questionnaire-9, score range 5–27) positively moderated the relationship between neighborhood stress and infant self-regulatory difficulties (IBQ-R). Luecken et al.’s study measured the relationship between infant self-regulation (cortisol responses) and prenatal depressive symptoms (Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale ≥13). Both prenatal depressive symptoms and perceived lack of social support predicted disrupted infant self-regulatory processes as indicated through dysfunctional cortisol responses: heightened cortisol output for 12-week old infants with high temperamental negativity but blunted cortisol for infants with low negativity [57]. Both studies co-varied for postpartum depressive symptoms, suggesting that dysfunctional self-regulatory processes were associated with the prenatal rather than postnatal psychosocial environment.

Infancy

Studies conducted during infancy (n = 2) revealed significant correlations between maternal depression and adverse infant socioemotional outcomes. Six-month old infants whose mothers were diagnosed with major depression (based on the DSM-IV criteria) scored significantly higher than infants with non-depressed mothers on both intrapersonal and interpersonal social withdrawal behaviors (Alarm Distress Baby Scale): facial expression, vocalizations, relationship, attraction, eye contact, and self-stimulation [53]. Bidirectional interpersonal associations, implying disrupted social synchrony, were noted in a separate study: higher infant negativity (IBQ-R) at six weeks was associated with increased maternal depressive symptoms (EPDS > 13) at 12 weeks; moreover, increased maternal depressive symptoms were associated with decreased social support [57]. Each study measuring infant outcomes (n = 4) yielded a significant association between maternal depressive symptoms and adverse infant outcomes regardless of study design, analytical analysis, child outcome measures, or maternal depression measures.

Toddlerhood

Two studies focused on the relationship between maternal depressive symptoms and child outcomes in toddlers aged 12 to 36 months. Martinez examined the relationship between maternal depressive symptoms (CES-D ≥ 16) and child aggression, compliance, and negative emotionality (CBCL and Infant Toddler Social Emotional Assessment) [58]. Mild and moderate depressive symptoms at 24 months significantly predicted increased negative infant emotionality six months later [58]. Huang et al. examined the relationship between maternal depressive symptoms (CES-D ≥ 16 at nine months, Composite International Diagnostic Interview Short Form Instrument at 24 months), maternal sensitivity, and toddler child attachment (Toddler Attachment Sort-45 Instrument). Analyses stratified by ethnicity revealed that chronic maternal depression in Latina mothers posed a higher odds ratio for child insecure attachment at 24 months than any other ethnicity (Odds Ratio = 8.12, 95% Confidence Interval = 1.07–61.68, p = .04) [2]; however, the significance of this association disappeared when maternal sensitivity was added into the model [2]. As such, both studies found significant relationships between toddler socioemotional outcomes and the chronicity [2] or severity [58] of maternal depressive symptoms.

Preschool

Four studies examined the correlation between maternal depressive symptoms and behavioral functioning (CBCL) in preschoolers. Neither La Roche et al. or Lequericia and Hermosa found significant associations between maternal depressive symptoms (CES-D ≥ 16 or subscale of Ilfield’s Psychiatric Symptom Index) and preschool behavioral functioning [50,59]. However, neither study adjusted for potential confounding variables (e.g., maternal demographic characteristics, child’s age), which could have obscured possible associations. Only when maternal depressive symptoms (CES-D ≥ 16) were combined with measures of stress did significant associations emerge between maternal depression and child behaviors: maternal depressive symptoms in conjunction with stress was significantly positively associated with child internalizing problems [60]. Two of these studies were cross-sectional [50,60], and one study spanned a three-month time-frame [59], providing a limited perspective into the trajectory of these associations.

During children’s mental health treatment spanning three years, Mennen et al. found significant associations between maternal depressive symptoms (CES-D ≥ 16) and preschoolers’ behavioral functioning: externalizing and internalizing behaviors (CBLC) and socialization (Vineland Screener) [61]. Compared to children whose mothers were not depressed, children of mothers with depression began treatment with higher levels of internalizing and externalizing behaviors and lower socialization scores, progressed more slowly during treatment despite receiving more mental health services, and finished treatment with lower levels of behavioral functioning [61]. Unlike the consistently significant associations found between maternal depression and adverse infant outcomes, significant correlations during the preschool years emerged only when 1) the study design and analysis captured developmental changes over time or when 2) maternal depressive symptoms were combined with a measurement of stress.

Infancy through early childhood

In longitudinal studies spanning infancy through early childhood (n = 2), the interaction between maternal depressive symptoms and an infant’s regulatory capacity was found to predict child behavioral functioning through the preschool years [62] and middle childhood [63]. Infants’ resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) [62] and RSA reactivity [63] were measured as a physiological index for vagal tone and subsequent underlying regulatory capacity. The interaction between low infant RSA reactivity and exposure to maternal depressive symptoms (CES-D ≥ 16) at one and three-and-a-half years old predicted significantly greater externalizing difficulties (Behavior Assessment System for Children) at 7-years-old [63]. Likewise, the interaction between low infant resting RSA and exposure to depressive symptoms (EPDS ≥ 13) from three weeks to six months old predicted greater internalizing and externalizing child behavior problems (CBCL) at 36 months [62]. Additionally, higher maternal depressive symptoms (CES-D ≥ 16) at 36 months yielded other significant correlations: maternal depressive symptoms (EPDS ≥ 13) at six months and more child behavior problems [62]. In summary, the interaction between infant self-regulatory processes and the chronicity of maternal depressive symptoms predicted greater child behavioral difficulties [62,63]. Measurements assessing exposure to maternal depressive symptoms across multiple points in the child’s life strengthened both studies.

Maternal sensitivity

Two studies examined the relationship between maternal depressive symptoms, maternal sensitivity, and acculturation during toddlerhood and preschool [2,64]. In the ECLS-B’s full sample representing the general population, maternal depressive symptoms measured at nine months (CES-D ≥ 16) and 24 months (Composite International Diagnostic Interview Short Form Instrument) were inversely associated with maternal sensitivity scores [2]. However, in analyses stratified by ethnicity, patterns of depression at nine months and 24 months (e.g., remittent, late onset, chronic) were not significantly associated with maternal sensitivity in Latina mothers [2]. In contrast, Palermo et al. found a significant inverse association between maternal sensitivity and maternal mental health problems–a variable combining maternal depressive symptoms (CES-D), parenting stress, and self-efficacy measures. In both studies, more acculturated mothers scored significantly higher on maternal sensitivity measures than less acculturated mothers. In fact, maternal acculturation (as measured by either primary language use, nativity, or a combination of generational status and language proficiency), not maternal depressive symptoms, was most consistently and significantly associated with maternal sensitivity scores [2,64].

Maternal sensitivity was also tested as a mediator between maternal depressive symptoms and child outcomes. Combining stress and depression measures into one variable again yielded the stronger association: maternal sensitivity significantly mediated the association between maternal mental health problems and child behavioral functioning (Problem Behavior Scale) in preschoolers [64]. Although Huang et al. found that maternal sensitivity mediated the associations between maternal depressive symptoms and child attachment in toddlers, the mediation was not significant in Latina mother-child dyads–a finding that differed from the ECLS-B’s full sample [2]. In summary, unless maternal depressive symptoms were combined with maternal stress, maternal sensitivity scores were not correlated with maternal depression and did not function as a significant mediator between maternal depressive symptoms and child outcomes. Both studies assessed and adjusted for demographic moderators (e.g., socioeconomic status, maternal education).

Economic and sociocultural stressors

Economic stressors

Studies using the FSM (n = 3) during early childhood found consistent positive correlations between maternal depressive symptoms and economic stressors: food insecurity, community violence, and economic hardship [6466]. Maternal depressive symptoms were then tested as a mediator between these poverty-related stressors and child outcomes. In a cross-sectional study examining Latino families in rural America across 13 states, maternal depressive symptoms (CES-D short form ≥ 10) were not found to significantly mediate the relationship between household food insecurity and child behavioral functioning (CBCL) [65]. The study was limited by its cross-sectional nature and the potentially confounding child age range (one-and-a-half to five years old, mean = 42 months). However, a longitudinal study examining children from a nationally representative sample of Head Start programs across one school year also found statistically non-significant effects: child behavioral functioning (Personal Maturity Scale, CBCL, Teacher Report, and the Behavior Problems Index) was not significantly associated with individual study variables: maternal depressive symptoms (CES-D short form ≥ 10), violence exposure, family structure, or parenting style [66]. Furthermore, maternal mental health problems (CES-D ≥ 16, parenting stress, self-efficacy) and maternal sensitivity did not mediate the association between economic hardship and child behavioral functioning (Problem Behavior Scale) in a longer longitudinal study spanning 14 months until kindergarten entry [64].

Although the FSM studies provided no support for the mediating role of maternal depressive symptoms between poverty-related stressors and child outcomes, Aisenberg’s cross-sectional study found that maternal distress symptomatology, as measured by depressive symptoms (BSI ≥ 63), anxiety, and adult post-traumatic stress disorder, significantly mediated the relationship between exposure to community violence and externalizing and internalizing behaviors (CBCL) in preschool children [67].

Two studies examined maternal depressive symptoms as a moderator between economic stressors and child outcomes. Waters et al. tested both chronic overcrowded housing and chronic maternal depressive symptoms (CES-D ≥ 16) as potential moderators between child RSA reactivity and child externalizing behavioral difficulties (BASC-2) from infancy through middle childhood [63]. Only maternal depressive symptoms functioned as a significant moderator, indicating that maternal depression posed a stronger risk for children than overcrowded housing in a sample of Mexican-American dyads [63]. Fuller et al. tested maternal depressive symptoms on a continuum (PHQ-9, score range 5–27) as a moderator between infant temperament (IBQ-R) and prenatal material hardship (food insecurity, housing disrepair, financial difficulty, neighborhood stress) [56]. Prenatal depressive symptoms significantly moderated the relationship between neighborhood stress and low infant regulatory capacity [56].

Acculturation

Two studies examined the relationship between maternal depressive symptoms and child socioemotional outcomes in context of acculturation (i.e., cultural adaptation) [2,64]. A culturally integrated FSM tested whether acculturation levels moderated the pattern of associations between maternal mental health problems (CES-D, parenting stress, self-efficacy) and preschool behavioral functioning (Problem Behavior Scale) [64]. Palermo et al. found significantly higher measures of maternal sensitivity in more acculturated mothers compared to less acculturated mothers, but adding maternal acculturation measures to models did not moderate the associations between maternal mental health problems and child outcomes. Nor did maternal nativity status moderate the associations among maternal depressive symptoms (CES-D ≥ 16), maternal sensitivity, and child attachment [2].

Despite the lack of studies explicitly examining the effects of maternal acculturation on the relationship between maternal depression and child development, a significant pattern repeatedly emerged across studies. Children of U.S.-born Latina mothers had consistently poorer socioemotional outcomes than children of foreign-born Latina mothers across domains and developmental windows: higher infant negativity in two studies [56,57]; higher rates of insecure attachment in toddlers, including almost double the rate (20.7% vs 11.5%) of disorganized attachment, the most maladaptive style [2]; and higher externalizing and internalizing behavioral problems in preschoolers [62]. Higher rates of insecure attachment in toddlers (49.4% vs 39.6%) occurred despite higher maternal sensitivity scores, family incomes, and maternal education levels in U.S-born Latina mothers from the ECLS-B [2]. Furthermore, higher child internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in preschoolers occurred despite higher resting RSA levels, a measure found to protect children significantly from socioemotional difficulties in the full sample of Mexican-American children [62]. Potential links between maternal nativity status and maternal depression in relation to child outcomes remained predominantly unexplored, leaving a gap in our understanding.

Discussion

This systematic review examined the relationship between maternal depression in Latina mothers and their children’s socioemotional outcomes from birth to five years of age. Fifteen studies with a total of n = 5,656 mother-child participants met our inclusion criteria. Of the eleven studies published within the past ten years, seven were published in the past five years, indicating that the relationship between maternal depression in Latinas and child development has only recently begun to receive scholarly attention.

Consistent with previous meta-analyses conducted on European-White mother-child dyads, the preschool years were the least likely to yield statistically significant inverse associations between maternal depression and child well-being; infancy had the strongest inverse correlations, suggesting that younger children may be more vulnerable to the influences of maternal depression [1,8]. Chronicity and severity of maternal depression also predicted child outcomes; more frequent exposure to maternal depression across infancy and early childhood was linked to increased child maladjustment by early and middle childhood. We also found some evidence for the moderating [56,63] and mediating role [67] of maternal depression between contextual stressors and child outcomes. Maternal stress in conjunction with depression heightened associations between maternal depression and child maladjustment [60,64,67]. In contrast, maternal sensitivity less consistently affected the relationship between maternal depression and child outcomes [2,64]. One of the most consistent patterns emerged between maternal nativity and child socioemotional development: children of U.S.-born Latina mothers had significantly poorer developmental outcomes than children of foreign-born Latina mothers across socioemotional domains and throughout early developmental windows—from infant negativity to toddler attachment and preschool behavioral functioning [2,56,57,62].

Maternal sensitivity

Maternal sensitivity scores were more consistently correlated with maternal acculturation than maternal depression, whereby high levels of maternal acculturation were linked to high levels of maternal sensitivity scores—thus raising the question “Are maternal sensitivity measures more closely linked to cultural assimilation than maternal processes that protect children?” Although previous research has supported maternal sensitivity as a protective cross-cultural construct critical for healthy child developmental outcomes [68], some evidence asserts that measures used to operationalize this complex construct may be culture-specific [69]. In fact, the Nursing Child Assessment Teaching Scale, used to measure maternal sensitivity during infancy in the ECLS-B, may detect maternal knowledge of early child development more sensitively than either maternal depression or self-efficacy [70]. Although positive correlations between maternal sensitivity and preschooler’s behavioral functioning suggest that maternal sensitivity measures may at least partially apply to Latina mothers in relation to child well-being [64], measures are likely not capturing key maternal processes specific to less acculturated Latina mothers; these processes warrant further exploration. Moreover, considering that increased maternal sensitivity scores in more acculturated Latina mothers did not moderate the relationship between maternal depression and child outcomes in toddlers or preschoolers [2,64], unexplored interpersonal or contextual factors in relation to maternal depression may play a superseding role in determining child well-being.

Economic and sociocultural stressors

Depression has long been recognized as an interdependent phenomenon inextricably linked to interpersonal and contextual factors [71]. As such, economic stressors and lack of social support were significantly associated with maternal depression in our studies. We also found some evidence for the mediating [67] and moderating [56,63] role of maternal depression between poverty-related stressors and child socioemotional outcomes. Our review found no support for using the FSM during the preschool years, in contrast to FSM studies conducted during middle childhood and adolescence within the broader literature [31,32,72]. Perhaps the cumulative effects of chronic stressors are not measurable until later in childhood. The preschool years, however, were the least likely to yield significant associations between maternal depression and child development in our studies unless maternal depression was combined with a measure of stress. Elevated scores on both maternal depression and stress measures may reflect more chronic depressive symptoms [73], a pattern of depression associated with greater risk for child maladjustment in our longitudinal studies [2,62,63]. Moreover, some empirical evidence suggests that maternal stress (e.g., parenting stress, limited social support, marital conflict) may mediate the relationship between maternal depression and behavior problems in toddlers [74] and preschoolers [73]. In immigrant Latino families, economic stressors combined with parental immigration-related stress have predicted increased behavioral difficulties in preschoolers [75].

Although studies in our review did not address specific sociocultural stressors, a small but growing body of literature shows that culturally relevant precursors of maternal depression in Latina mothers (e.g., stressful life events, discrimination, acculturative stress) have been associated with socioemotional maladjustment during early developmental windows, suggesting that maternal stress in general may affect a child’s capacity for healthy socioemotional outcomes beginning as early as the prenatal period [38,7678]. Moreover, bio-behavioral profiles indicative of chronic stress across succeeding generations have been found in Mexican-origin women, whereby third generation women exhibited blunted maternal cortisol during pregnancy [79].

These increasing socioemotional vulnerabilities echo our findings linking consistently poorer socioemotional outcomes to children of U.S.-born mothers compared to children of foreign-born mothers. Such outcomes occurred even within the context of higher family incomes, maternal education, and higher levels of maternal sensitivity scores frequently found in succeeding generations [2]. A previous study examining school readiness also noted healthier socioemotional adjustment in kindergarten children of foreign-born Latino parents compared to children of U.S.-born parents of other races and ethnicities, as rated by teachers [80]. Similarly, a study examining the relationship between cultural adaptation in parents and behavioral functioning in preschoolers found that high US identity in Latino parents was associated with more externalizing behaviors, whereas higher ethnic parental identity predicted lower levels of child externalizing and internalizing behaviors [81]. Increased parental acculturation, as measured by language usage, has also been linked to lower (i.e., less adaptive) cortisol responses in preschoolers living in poverty [75]. Although cross-generational research supports an immigrant paradox in behavioral outcomes during adolescence (e.g., substance abuse, teen pregnancy, delinquency), research examining socioemotional outcomes in children under five years of age has been scarce [82]. This is the first review, to our knowledge, to have found support for the Latino paradox in early childhood mental health. If an immigrant advantage exists throughout early developmental windows in the socioemotional domain, why and when does it erode?

The interaction between maternal nativity and maternal mental health in relation to child outcomes was rarely examined in our studies, preventing us from fully grasping the underlying mechanisms for this apparent decline. However, our findings clearly indicate that a) this pattern can be observed within sensitive early developmental windows; b) maternal mental health and early child development are intimately connected; and c) contextual stressors are salient precursors of maternal mental health. Maternal biopsychosocial stress across generations stemming from the chronic stressors inherent in unequitable societal structures may be a salient contributing factor; protective cultural factors such as collectivism that confer psychological resilience [83] may also be lost in subsequent generations. Combining models based on risk (e.g., developmental-risk framework) and resilience (e.g., ecocultural theory) may help elucidate underlying mechanisms for the Latino Paradox [84].

Clinical implications

Maternal depression is treatable, and its effects on child socioemotional outcomes can be minimized. A meta-analysis revealed that non-pharmaceutical treatment of depression during pregnancy not only improved maternal mood but improved child functioning–particularly infant regulatory capacity [85]. These outcomes provide causational support for the link between prenatal depression and infant socioemotional outcomes as well as subsequent evidence for the intergenerational transmission of mental health disparities. Furthermore, the World Health Organization recommends that interventions should treat maternal depression and support the mother-child relationship concurrently [24]. Well-designed, culturally responsive interventions that mitigate maternal stress and strengthen the mother-child relationship may also foster healthy child development. Although depression is treatable and support is critical during early developmental windows when interventions would be most preventative and cost-effective, treatment often remains inaccessible to Latina mothers even after controlling for income levels [6,86]. Given that maternal depression is highly correlated with adversity, lack of appropriate treatment for mothers suffering from depression and their young children may further compound disparities in children’s socioemotional development. Policies should address the need to support maternal mental health as early as the prenatal period.

Effective interventions for maternal depression in Latinas have focused on culturally sensitive psychoeducation, emotional support, and access to community resources [87]. Exploratory family-level interventions have also embraced potential sociocultural strengths such as kinship and familism (i.e., family interdependence), resulting in decreased maternal depressive symptoms and decreased internalizing and externalizing behaviors in children [88]. Moreover, a culturally-tailored intervention targeting postpartum depression and mother-child interactions in infants and toddlers lowered maternal depression scores and improved maternal perceptions of the child’s socioemotional competence [89]. Upstream approaches in immigrant women should address culturally relevant stressors and barriers that contribute to depression and stress, such as potential and poignant losses experienced during immigration: social support networks lost through family separation, a sense of belonging dislodged by discrimination, and access to community resources impeded by language barriers [41].

Limitations

Due to scarce research examining maternal depression in Latinas and child socioemotional outcomes, we had a limited number of studies–five of which were cross-sectional. Studies were too heterogeneous to conduct a meta-analysis. Furthermore, most studies during the preschool years used maternal-reported measures of child behavior such as the CBCL. Behavior ratings may be influenced by maternal cultural expectations [20] and maternal depression. Mothers with depression may be more likely to view their children negatively, resulting in potentially biased ratings [1]. However, three out of five studies using maternal reported child measures in longitudinal studies adjusted for concurrent depression.

We also only included studies conducted in the US. Although this focus prevented confounding factors that may have arisen from research in several host countries, our findings cannot be generalized outside of the US. Moreover, the Latino population is not homogeneous; subpopulations from Latin American countries have various socioeconomic and cultural backgrounds as well as reasons for coming to the United States [90]. Furthermore, acculturation may differ among Latino sub-groups [38]. Although we extracted data such as country of origin and acculturation, eight studies did not include country of origin, and seven did not measure acculturation.

Conclusions

This is the first systematic review to examine Latina mother-child dyads in relation to maternal depression and child socioemotional development. We found significant positive correlations between maternal depression and contextual stressors, inverse correlations between maternal depression and child well-being, and evidence of the moderating and mediating role of maternal depression between contextual stressors and child socioemotional outcomes. We also connected fragmented evidence across studies delineating a concerning and significant cross-generational decline in young Latino children’s socioemotional outcomes; future research should examine the underlying mechanisms. Indeed, socioemotional developmental processes are critical throughout infancy and early childhood—processes allowing children to form friendships, learn in school, develop resiliency, and eventually strengthen our intrinsically interconnected society [91].

Supporting information

S1 Table. Prisma 2009 checklist.

(PDF)

S2 Table. Cognitive outcomes.

(DOCX)

S1 File. Protocol for systematic review.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Dr. Beth Black for editorial suggestions.

Data Availability

All data is presented within the manuscript

Funding Statement

The author received no specific funding for this work. HS academic effort was funded by National Institute of Nursing Research (1K23NR017898-01). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, Broth MR, Hall CM, et al. (2011) Maternal depression and child psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 14: 1–27. 10.1007/s10567-010-0080-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Huang ZJ, Lewin A, Mitchell SJ, Zhang J (2012) Variations in the relationship between maternal depression, maternal sensitivity, and child attachment by race/ethnicity and nativity: findings from a nationally representative cohort study. Matern Child Health J 16: 40–50. 10.1007/s10995-010-0716-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.U.S. Census Bureau (2017) 2017 American community survey 1-year estimates. . Available: https://factfinder.census.gov/faces/tableservices/jsf/pages/productview.xhtml?src=bkmk. Accessed 24 August 2019.
  • 4.Lara-Cinisomo S, Wisner KL. Perinatal depression among Spanish-speaking and Latin American women. New York, NY: Springer New York; 2014. 10.1007/978-1-4614-8045-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Blackmore ER, Chaudron L (2014) Psychosocial and cultural considerations in detecting and treating depression in Latina perinatal women in the United States. In: Lara-Cinisomo S, Wisner KL, editors. Perinatal Depression among Spanish-Speaking and Latin American Women New York, NY: Springer New York; pp. 83–96. 10.1007/978-1-4614-8045-7_6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lara-Cinisomo S, Clark CT, Wood J (2018) Increasing diagnosis and treatment of perinatal depression in Latinas and African American women: addressing stigma is not enough. Womens Health Issues 28: 201–204. 10.1016/j.whi.2018.01.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Yates T, Ostrosky M, Cheatham G, Fettig A, Shaffer L, et al. (2008) Research synthesis on screening and assessing social-emotional competence. Available: http://csefel.vanderbilt.edu/documents/rs_screening_assessment.pdf Accessed 06 December 2019.
  • 8.Beck CT (1998) The effects of postpartum depression on child development: a meta-analysis. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 12: 12–20. 10.1016/s0883-9417(98)80004-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Campbell SB, Denham SA, Howarth GZ, Jones SM, Whittaker JV, et al. (2016) Commentary on the review of measures of early childhood social and emotional development: Conceptualization, critique, and recommendations. J Appl Dev Psychol 45: 19–41. 10.1016/j.appdev.2016.01.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Sroufe LA (2005) Attachment and development: a prospective, longitudinal study from birth to adulthood. Attach Hum Dev 7: 349–367. 10.1080/14616730500365928 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ertel KA, Rich-Edwards JW, Koenen KC (2011) Maternal depression in the United States: nationally representative rates and risks. J Womens Health (Larchmt) 20: 1609–1617. 10.1089/jwh.2010.2657 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Entringer S, Buss C, Wadhwa PD (2015) Prenatal stress, development, health and disease risk: A psychobiological perspective-2015 Curt Richter Award Paper. Psychoneuroendocrinology 62: 366–375. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Van den Bergh BRH, van den Heuvel MI, Lahti M, Braeken M, de Rooij SR, et al. (2017) Prenatal developmental origins of behavior and mental health: The influence of maternal stress in pregnancy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Johnson M, Deardorff J, Davis EL, Martinez W, Eskenazi B, et al. (2017) The relationship between maternal responsivity, socioeconomic status, and resting autonomic nervous system functioning in Mexican American children. Int J Psychophysiol 116: 45–52. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.02.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Chiao JY, Li S-C, Seligman R, Turner R. The oxford handbook of cultural neuroscience. Oxford University Press; 2015. 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199357376.001.0001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Goodman SH, Garber J (2017) Evidence-based interventions for depressed mothers and their young children. Child Dev 88: 368–377. 10.1111/cdev.12732 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bernard K, Nissim G, Vaccaro S, Harris JL, Lindhiem O (2018) Association between maternal depression and maternal sensitivity from birth to 12 months: a meta-analysis. Attach Hum Dev 20: 578–599. 10.1080/14616734.2018.1430839 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lovejoy MC, Graczyk PA, O’Hare E, Neuman G (2000) Maternal depression and parenting behavior: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 20: 561–592. 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00100-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mesman J, van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ (2012) Unequal in opportunity, equal in process: parental sensitivity promotes positive child development in ethnic minority families. Child Dev Perspect 6: 239–250. 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00223.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Chen X, Rubin KH (2011) Socioemotional development in cultural context. New York: Guilford Press, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gosh Ippen C (2009) The sociocultural context of infant mental health: Towards contextually congruent interventions. In: Zeanah CH, editor. Handbook of Infant Mental Health New York: Guilford Press; 2009. p. 104–119. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Iruka IU, Laforett DR, Odom EC (2012) Examining the validity of the family investment and stress models and relationship to children’s school readiness across five cultural groups. J Fam Psychol 26: 359–370. 10.1037/a0028290 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ispa JM, Fine MA, Halgunseth LC, Harper S, Robinson J, et al. (2004) Maternal intrusiveness, maternal warmth, and mother-toddler relationship outcomes: variations across low-income ethnic and acculturation groups. Child Dev 75: 1613–1631. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00806.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Parsons CE, Young KS, Rochat TJ, Kringelbach ML, Stein A (2012) Postnatal depression and its effects on child development: a review of evidence from low- and middle-income countries. Br Med Bull 101: 57–79. 10.1093/bmb/ldr047 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Zeanah CH (2019) Handbook of infant mental health. Available: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&scope=site&db=nlebk&db=nlabk&AN=1843598.
  • 26.U.S.Census Bureau (2017) People in families by family structure, age, and sex, iterated by income-to-poverty ratio and race. Available: https://www.census.gov/data/tables/time-series/demo/income-poverty/cps-pov/pov-02.html. Accessed 24 August 2019.
  • 27.Madigan S, Oatley H, Racine N, Fearon RMP, Schumacher L, et al. (2018) A meta-analysis of maternal prenatal depression and anxiety on child socioemotional development. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 57: 645–657.e8. 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.06.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Masarik AS, Conger RD (2017) Stress and child development: a review of the family stress model. Curr Opin Psychol 13: 85–90. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Mistry RS, Vandewater EA, Huston AC, McLoyd VC (2002) Economic well-being and children’s social adjustment: the role of family process in an ethnically diverse low-income sample. Child Dev 73: 935–951. 10.1111/1467-8624.00448 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Borthwick-Duffy J (2003) Economic pressure, maternal depression, and child adjustment in Latino families: An exploratory study. Journal of Family and Economic Issues 24: 183–202. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Parke RD, Coltrane S, Duffy S, Buriel R, Dennis J, et al. (2004) Economic stress, parenting, and child adjustment in Mexican American and European American families. Child Dev 75: 1632–1656. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00807.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Pachter LM, Auinger P, Palmer R, Weitzman M (2006) Do parenting and the home environment, maternal depression, neighborhood, and chronic poverty affect child behavioral problems differently in different racial-ethnic groups? Pediatrics 117: 1329–1338. 10.1542/peds.2005-1784 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hurwich-Reiss E, Watamura SE (2019) Family processes among Latino Early Head Start families: Understanding the role of caregiver acculturation. J Community Psychol 47: 1433–1448. 10.1002/jcop.22198 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Coll CG, Akerman A, Cicchetti D (2000) Cultural influences on developmental processes and outcomes: Implications for the study of development and psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol 12: 333–356 10.1017/s0954579400003059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Causadias JM, Cicchetti D (2018) Cultural development and psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol 30: 1549–1555. 10.1017/S0954579418001220 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Berry JW (2006) Acculturative Stress. In: Wong PTP, Wong LCJ, editors. Handbook of multicultural perspectives on stress and coping Boston, MA: Springer US; pp. 287–298. 10.1007/0-387-26238-5_12 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Zeiders KH, Umaña-Taylor AJ, Jahromi LB, Updegraff KA, White RMB (2016) Discrimination and acculturations stress: A longitudinal study of children’s well-being from prenatal development to 5 years of age. J Dev Behav Pediatr 37: 557–564. 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000321 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Rubin LP (2016) Maternal and pediatric health and disease: integrating biopsychosocial models and epigenetics. Pediatr Res 79: 127–135. 10.1038/pr.2015.203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Romero A, Piña-Watson B (2017) Acculturative stress and bicultural stress. Schwartz SJ, Unger J, editors Oxford University Press; 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190215217.013.8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ornelas IJ, Perreira KM (2011) The role of migration in the development of depressive symptoms among Latino immigrant parents in the USA. Soc Sci Med 73: 1169–1177. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.07.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.D’Anna-Hernandez KL, Aleman B, Flores A-M (2015) Acculturative stress negatively impacts maternal depressive symptoms in Mexican-American women during pregnancy. J Affect Disord 176: 35–42. 10.1016/j.jad.2015.01.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Fox M, Thayer ZM, Ramos IF, Meskal SJ, Wadhwa PD (2018) Prenatal and postnatal mother-to-child transmission of acculturation’s health effects in Hispanic Americans. J Womens Health (Larchmt) 27: 1054–1063. 10.1089/jwh.2017.6526 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Santos HP, Nephew BC, Bhattacharya A, Tan X, Smith L, et al. (2018) Discrimination exposure and DNA methylation of stress-related genes in Latina mothers. Psychoneuroendocrinology 98: 131–138. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.08.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Flores MES, Simonsen SE, Manuck TA, Dyer JM, Turok DK (2012) The “Latina epidemiologic paradox”: Contrasting patterns of adverse birth outcomes in U.S.-born and foreign-born Latinas. Womens Health Issues 22: e501–7. 10.1016/j.whi.2012.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Gill SL (2009) Breastfeeding by Hispanic women. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs 38: 244–252. 10.1111/j.1552-6909.2009.01013.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009. July 21;6(7):e1000097 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Santos H, Fried EI, Asafu-Adjei J, Ruiz RJ (2017) Network structure of perinatal depressive symptoms in Latinas: relationship to stress and reproductive biomarkers. Res Nurs Health 40: 218–228. 10.1002/nur.21784 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Holditch-Davis D, Santos H, Levy J, White-Traut R, O’Shea TM, et al. (2015) Patterns of psychological distress in mothers of preterm infants. Infant Behav Dev 41: 154–163. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.10.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Study Quality Assessment Tools | National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI) [Internet]. [cited 2019 Jan 3]. Available from: https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools
  • 50.Lequerica M, Hermosa B (1995) Maternal reports of behavior problems in preschool Hispanic children: An exploratory study in preventive pediatrics. J Natl Med Assoc 87: 861–868. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Achenbach TM (1992) Manual for the child behavior checklist/2-3 and 1992 profile. Burlington, VT: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont, c1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Putnam SP, Helbig AL, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK, Leerkes E (2014) Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the infant behavior questionnaire-revised. J Pers Assess 96: 445–458. 10.1080/00223891.2013.841171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Burtchen N, Alvarez-Segura M, Mendelsohn AL, Dreyer BP, Castellanos FX, et al. (2013) Screening for sustained social withdrawal behaviors in six-month-old infants during pediatric primary care visits: results from an at-risk latino immigrant sample with high rates of maternal major depressive disorder. Infant Ment Health J 34: 542–552. 10.1002/imhj.21418 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Richards Martin P. M. (1974) The integration of a child into a social world. London; New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Fuligni AS, Brooks-Gunn J (2013) Mother–child Interactions in Early Head Start: Age and ethnic differences in low-income dyads. Parenting 13: 1–26. 10.1080/15295192.2013.732422 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Fuller A, Messito MJ, Mendelsohn AL, Oyeku SO, Gross RS (2018) Prenatal material hardships and infant regulatory capacity at 10 months old in low-income Hispanic Mother-Infant Pairs. Acad Pediatr 18: 897–904. 10.1016/j.acap.2018.04.134 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Luecken LJ, MacKinnon DP, Jewell SL, Crnic KA, Gonzales NA (2015) Effects of prenatal factors and temperament on infant cortisol regulation in low-income Mexican American families. Dev Psychobiol 57: 961–973. 10.1002/dev.21328 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Martinez MI. Young Latino children at risk for psychopathology: Effects of maternal depressive symptoms on child behavior. Dissertation, The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. 2015. Available from: https://cdr.lib.unc.edu/indexablecontent/uuid:7c507236-28a1-4adf-8498-762982fd4d51.
  • 59.Roche La, Castellano Kalick LR MJ .Turner C.Kalick S(1995) Latina mothers and their toddlers’ behavioral difficulties. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences 17: 375–384. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Siantz ML de L, Coronado N, Dovydaitis T (2010) Maternal predictors of behavioral problems among Mexican migrant farmworker children. J Fam Nurs 16: 322–343. 10.1177/1074840710376946 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Mennen FE, Pohle C, Monro WL, Duan L, Finello KM, et al. (2015) The effect of maternal depression on young children’s progress in treatment. J Child Fam Stud 24: 2088–2098. 10.1007/s10826-014-0010-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Somers JA, Luecken LJ, Spinrad TL, Crnic KA (2018) Biological sensitivity to the effects of maternal postpartum depressive symptoms on children’s behavior problems. Child Dev. 10.1111/cdev.13114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Waters SF, Boyce WT, Eskenazi B, Alkon A (2016) The impact of maternal depression and overcrowded housing on associations between autonomic nervous system reactivity and externalizing behavior problems in vulnerable Latino children. Psychophysiology 53: 97–104. 10.1111/psyp.12539 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Palermo F, Ispa JM, Carlo G, Streit C (2018) Economic hardship during infancy and U.S. Latino preschoolers’ sociobehavioral health and academic readiness. Dev Psychol 54: 890–902. 10.1037/dev0000476 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Doudna K. Application of the family stress model in populations of rural Latina mothers. Dissertation, Iowa State University. 2016. Available from: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/etd/15118
  • 66.Westbrook TR, Harden BJ (2010) Pathways among exposure to violence, maternal depression, family structure, and child outcomes through parenting: a multigroup analysis. Am J Orthopsychiatry 80: 386–400. 10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01042.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Aisenberg E (2001) The Effects of Exposure to Community Violence upon Latina Mothers and Preschool Children. Hisp J Behav Sci 23: 378–398. 10.1177/0739986301234003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Posada G, Trumbell J, Noblega M, Plata S, Peña P, et al. (2016) Maternal sensitivity and child secure base use in early childhood: studies in different cultural contexts. Child Dev 87: 297–311. 10.1111/cdev.12454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Dawson N, Bain K, Mesman J (2018) Comparing two measures of maternal sensitivity: goodness of fit with a South African cultural context. Attach Hum Dev: 1–9. 10.1080/14616734.2018.1454056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Gross D, Conrad B, Fogg L, Willis L, Garvey C (1993) What does the NCATS measure? Nur.s Res 42: 260–265. 10.1097/00006199-199309000-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Coyne JC, Downey G (1991) Social factors and psychopathology: stress, social support, and coping processes. Annu Rev Psychol 42: 401–425. 10.1146/annurev.ps.42.020191.002153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Lorenzo-Blanco EI, Meca A, Unger JB, Romero A, Szapocznik J, et al. (2017) Longitudinal effects of Latino parent cultural stress, depressive symptoms, and family functioning on youth emotional well-being and health risk behaviors. Fam Process 56: 981–996. 10.1111/famp.12258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Dawson G, Ashman SB, Panagiotides H, Hessl D, Self J, et al. (2003) Preschool outcomes of children of depressed mothers: role of maternal behavior, contextual risk, and children’s brain activity. Child Dev 74: 1158–1175. 10.1111/1467-8624.00599 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Toth SL (1998) Maternal depressive disorder and contextual risk: Contributions to the development of attachment insecurity and behavior problems in toddlerhood. Dev Psychopathol 10: 283–300. 10.1017/s0954579498001618 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Mendoza MM, Dmitrieva J, Perreira KM, Hurwich-Reiss E, Watamura SE (2017) The effects of economic and sociocultural stressors on the well-being of children of Latino immigrants living in poverty. Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol 23: 15–26. 10.1037/cdp0000111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Lin B, Crnic KA, Luecken LJ, Gonzales NA (2014) Maternal prenatal stress and infant regulatory capacity in Mexican Americans. Infant Behav Dev 37: 571–582. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Lin B, Crnic KA, Luecken LJ, Gonzales NA (2017) Ontogeny of emotional and behavioral problems in a low-income, Mexican American sample. Dev Psychol 53: 2245–2260. 10.1037/dev0000391 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Rosenthal L, Earnshaw VA, Moore JM, Ferguson DN, Lewis TT, et al. (2018) Intergenerational consequences: Women’s experiences of discrimination in pregnancy predict infant social-emotional development at 6 months and 1 Year. J Dev Behav Pediatr 39: 228–237. 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000529 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Ruiz RJ, Stowe RP, Brown A, Wommack J (2012) Acculturation and biobehavioral profiles in pregnant women of Hispanic origin: generational differences. ANS Adv Nurs Sci 35: E1–E10. 10.1097/ANS.0b013e3182626199 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Crosnoe R, Turley RNL (2011) K-12 educational outcomes of immigrant youth. Future Child 21: 129–152. 10.1353/foc.2011.0008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Calzada EJ, Brotman LM, Huang K-Y, Bat-Chava Y, Kingston S (2009) Parent cultural adaptation and child functioning in culturally diverse, urban families of preschoolers. J Appl Dev Psychol 30: 515–524. 10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.García Coll C, Marks AK, editors (2012) The immigrant paradox in children and adolescents: Is becoming American a developmental risk? Washington: American Psychological Association; 10.1037/13094-000 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Chiao JY, Blizinsky KD (2010) Culture-gene coevolution of individualism-collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proc Biol Sci 277: 529–537. 10.1098/rspb.2009.1650 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Marks AK, Ejesi K, García Coll C (2014) Understanding the U.S. immigrant paradox in childhood and adolescence. Child Dev Perspect 8: 59–64. 10.1111/cdep.12071 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Goodman SH, Cullum KA, Dimidjian S, River LM, Kim CY (2018) Opening windows of opportunities: Evidence for interventions to prevent or treat depression in pregnant women being associated with changes in offspring’s developmental trajectories of psychopathology risk. Dev Psychopathol 30: 1179–1196. 10.1017/S0954579418000536 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Kozhimannil KB, Trinacty CM, Busch AB, Huskamp HA, Adams AS (2011) Racial and ethnic disparities in postpartum depression care among low-income women. Psychiatr Serv 62: 619–625. 10.1176/ps.62.6.pss6206_0619 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Baker-Ericzén MJ, Connelly CD, Hazen AL, Dueñas C, Landsverk JA, et al. (2012) A collaborative care telemedicine intervention to overcome treatment barriers for Latina women with depression during the perinatal period. Fam Syst Health 30: 224–240. 10.1037/a0028750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Valdez CR, Abegglen J, Hauser CT (2013) Fortalezas familiares program: building sociocultural and family strengths in latina women with depression and their families. Fam Process 52: 378–393. 10.1111/famp.12008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Beeber LS, Holditch-Davis D, Perreira K, Schwartz TA, Lewis V, et al. (2010) Short-term in-home intervention reduces depressive symptoms in Early Head Start Latina mothers of infants and toddlers. Res Nurs Health 33: 60–76. 10.1002/nur.20363 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Wildsmith E, Ansari A, Guzman L. Improving data infrastructure to recognize Hispanic diversity in the United States. National Research Center on Hispanic Children and Families; 2015. Available from: https://www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2015/06/Measure-Brief_Final.pdf [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Cohen J, Onunaku N, Clothier S, Poppe J. Helping young children succeed: Strategies to promote early childhood social and emotional development; 2005. Available from: https://www.zerotothree.org/resources/136-helping-young-children-succeed-strategies-to-promote-early-childhood-social-and-emotional-develop

Decision Letter 0

James Swain

13 Nov 2019

PONE-D-19-24059

Maternal depression in Latinas and child socioemotional and cognitive development: A systematic review

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Santos Jr,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Dec 28 2019 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

James Swain, M.D., Ph.D., F.R.C.P.C.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

Additional Editor Comments (if provided):

Dear colleagues,

Your manuscript entitled "Maternal depression in Latinas and child socioemotional and cognitive development: A

systematic review" has now been reviewed and the reviewer comments are appended below. I agree with them that your work is of importance and interest - however, they have raised points that need to be addressed by a major revision.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This is a meta-analysis and systematic review of 18 studies that examined relationship between maternal depression in Latina mothers and their children’s social, emotional and cognitive outcomes over the first five years of life. The study advances the area of research on the effects of maternal depression on child development by addressing both economic and sociocultural contexts of family functioning. The main results of the study included inverse associations between maternal depression and child socioemotional and cognitive outcomes and that children of U.S.-born Latina mothers had poorer developmental outcomes than children of foreign-born Latina mothers. the study is also innovative in that this is the first systematic review of studies examining Latina mothers’ depression and child psychological outcomes. The authors may consider the following comments:

1) Given that socioemotional and cognitive outcomes reflect a very wide range of constructs, were there any inclusion criteria for the reliability of measures that were used to study these constructs in young children?

2) Maybe I did not see in this in the study inclusion criteria section of the paper, but was there a requirement for how depression was evaluated and diagnosed in mothers who participated in studies included in this review?

3) The quality assessment criteria in Table 1 follow the guidelines from the National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute. Are these criteria commonly used to determine the quality of studies assessing mental health outcomes?

4) Related to question 1 above, it is indicated on page 18 of the paper that most studies measured child’s socioemotional development by mother-rated scales? Is it possible that these ratings were affected by the mother’s depression?

5) The qualitative review of the literature if clearly described and Table 2 is very informative but more detail would help to understand the quantitative data analytic strategy. Is it possible to obtain the magnitude of effect sizes for the strength of association among the variables of interest?

Thank you for the opportunity to review this interesting and important paper.

Reviewer #2: Thank you for opportunity to review the manuscript "Maternal Depression in Latinas and child socioemotional and cognitive development: A systematic review.” The authors are addressing a very important topic with implications for psychological care for Latino populations for whom psychological interventions have failed to serve well historically. This manuscript has a number of primarily methodological strengths such as pre-registering the study, using the PRISMA guidelines, and coding to data quality which are all very important. However there are several concerns primarily in the introduction in terms of setting up the aims and scope of the review that detract from the manuscript’s potential.

General comments

1. Use consistent terms for your constructs (many different terms for the outcome throughout) and define your constructs upfront and all that they encompass for clarity. There are many examples of this but for instance you say you at just looking at 0-5 years of age but cite a handful with kids older than 5 and say you are only looking at maternal depression but cite a handful that combine stress and depression or depression and other psychopathology.

2. Mediation and moderation are distinct conceptually and have different implications so the many instances of grouping them together is misleading. Set them up separately and interpret them separately since they tell us different things.

Introduction

1. My main comment is that your outcome (essentially all biological social and psychological aspects of development) is far too broad to make a cohesive argument and defined differently in different places. You look at child neurodevelopment, language development, social development, emotion regulation and self regulation, psychophysiology (RSA, EEG, ANS reactivity), attachment and psychological problems (e.g. externalizing). It would be very hard to review the background literature on all of these things adequately in the introduction which makes for a premise that is not well set up. I would strongly recommend narrowing your focus and in particular getting rid of the one study here and there that looks at a different construct entirely than the others (e.g. attachment and psychophysiology). I would recommend focusing on psychological outcomes (social behavior and externalizing) and closely related constructs (e.g. emotion regulation) since the studies seem to overwhelmingly focus on this, its much easier to build a cohesive theory around this, and you can clearly define the literature and your construct for readers to begin with.

2. Related to defining your constructs clearly what is meant by economic hardship you go on to describe community violence, overcrowded housing, poverty (defined as below the poverty line I assume) as variables in the studies so make it clear these are all proxy for poverty that you will be considering under the umbrella of “economic hardship.”

3. Restructuring thoughts: Your paper would benefit from an integrated conceptual model presented early on (a figure would be helpful), then the evidence of the model in white families, then the evidence in LatinX families and the need for research in LatinX families. Instead you highlighted different pieces from different angles (eg. Maternal sensitivity paragraph, FSM paragraph, and acculturative stress paragraph). It seems that your full model is that acculturative stress and economic stress (poverty) leads to maternal depression which impacts poor child outcomes through compromised parenting. Then that we should focus on Latina moms because they disproportionately experience acculturative stress and poverty. You should also mention that not all of the studies cover all pieces of the model but they all have the maternal depression and child outcomes piece in Latina samples.

4. Make the argument or mothers vs. fathers. The argument is maternal depression has a robust impact on child outcomes prenatally an there are less studies on fathers I assume but this needs to be explicit.

5. You should comment on the immigrant paradox literature explicitly in the argument for Latinas specifically section.

6. Line 53: comment on depression rates in Latinx populations specifically

7. Developmental argument: I would recommend adding to your developmental windows section talking about why you chose 0-5 year olds specifically (e.g. include the age ranges in the studies you are citing) given that this is never said until the last sentence of the introduction. What is the evidence (not just the theory) that this range is integral? For example, you could cite here evidence that intervening on child outcomes for 0-5 year olds with depressed mothers improves child psychological outcomes long term. Also explain why you use 5 as a cutoff and not earlier? Attachment theory may also be helpful to augment this developmental argument. I love the developmental figure though, that was very helpful.

8. Relatedly don’t review literature on adolescent outcomes (line 112) if your focus is 0-5 years of age, this is inconsistent with your argument for focusing on 0-5.

9. Biology is not “permanent” as you say in line 70, there is overwhelming evidence of plasticity in biological responses which is hopeful for interventions.

Methods

1. Your methods were very strong on the whole and my main comment is taking out the less relevant articles as stated above since it detracts from being able to make tangible and focused conclusions but also have some minor comments below.

2. Line 157: did you exclude for psychological co-morbid diagnoses (e.g. anxiety etc)?

3. It may be helpful to describe the last date of searching databases so future researchers will know where to pick up for later reviews. For example “ we reviewed all studies before June of 2019.”

4. Line 233: brief description of the three bag and two bag paradigms would be helpful here.

5. You need to acknowledge the diversity of what could be meant by depression and continue to clarify what you mean by depression when you introduce a study. For instance, are we talking about an MDD diagnosis (or a PDD, or a past MDE) or depression symptoms on a continuum that may or may not reach clinical cutoffs (e.g. CESD).

6. Line 367: 71 months is more than 5 years so you should justify why it was included there are also there are other studies that include older children (such as in line 320).

7. Line 376: It doesn’t seem that the literature is mixed it seems that it is leaning towards there is a not a significant mediation by maternal depression.

8. Line 381: I don’t understand what the first part of the sentence is saying, what is the finding? I think there may be some words missing. Please clarify.

9. Give the acculturation findings their own cohesive paragraph instead of sprinkling them throughout multiple sections to improve clarity.

Discussion

1. Line 430: “contextual stressors in conjunction with depression heightened associations between maternal depression and child maladjustment” is not supported in what you found, you found 2 of these studies were not significant and one was but only when combined with stress and explaining the lack of findings would be helpful.

2. I would tie in the immigrant paradox literature (e.g. Garcia Coll’s work) again to interpret your finding that things get worse with greater acculturation (Lines 485-487). There is a lot of literature on this.

3. Interpret why depression alone did not predict child outcomes as strongly as depression and stress together. For instance this could go after line 468.

Minor points

1. Imprecise language should be specified for instance:

a. Line 53: should be prevalence rates of major depressive disorder (or whatever you define in your construct) for clarity.

b. Line 124 these findings are specific to first compared to second generation immigrants not “intergenerational” broadly.

c. Line 129 should be largest ethnic minority (it isn’t sexual or gender minorities or necessarily racial minority)

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Review PLOS.docx

Decision Letter 1

James Swain

26 Feb 2020

Maternal depression in Latinas and child socioemotional development: A systematic review

PONE-D-19-24059R1

Dear Dr. Santos Jr,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it complies with all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you will receive an e-mail containing information on the amendments required prior to publication. When all required modifications have been addressed, you will receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will proceed to our production department and be scheduled for publication.

Shortly after the formal acceptance letter is sent, an invoice for payment will follow. To ensure an efficient production and billing process, please log into Editorial Manager at https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, you must inform our press team as soon as possible and no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

With kind regards,

James Swain, M.D., Ph.D., F.R.C.P.C.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: N/A

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed and I think the paper will make a strong contribution to the area of effects of maternal depression on child development

Reviewer #2: The authors were very thorough in addressing my initial concerns and the manuscript as it stands is much clearer and more cohesive.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

James Swain

2 Mar 2020

PONE-D-19-24059R1

Maternal depression in Latinas and child socioemotional development: A systematic review

Dear Dr. Santos Jr:

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

For any other questions or concerns, please email plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE.

With kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr James Swain

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Prisma 2009 checklist.

    (PDF)

    S2 Table. Cognitive outcomes.

    (DOCX)

    S1 File. Protocol for systematic review.

    (PDF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Review PLOS.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response_to_reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All data is presented within the manuscript


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES