Abstract
Purpose
The purpose of this study was to investigate associations between performance on a clinical speech-in-noise measure with a comprehensive neurocognitive battery of tests.
Method
A group of older adults (N = 250, M age = 77 years, age range: 67.3–89.1 years) enrolled in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Neurocognitive Study took part in the hearing pilot study (2013) that included testing for audiometric thresholds and speech-in-noise performance (Quick Speech-in-Noise Test; Killion, Niquette, Gudmundsen, Revit, & Banerjee, 2004). This research study analyzed the associations between domain-specific cognitive function and speech-in-noise performance after adjusting for hearing thresholds and other demographic and cardiovascular factors.
Results
Multivariable-adjusted associations were found between all cognitive domains and speech-in-noise performance in the full sample, but the observed associations varied when participants with varying levels of moderate to moderately severe hearing loss were excluded from the analysis.
Conclusions
The findings are discussed in terms considering the cognitive status of older adults in relation to their speech-in-noise performance during audiological evaluation and implications for aural rehabilitation.
Understanding speech-in-noise is fundamental to human communication. However, the ability to understand speech in the presence of background noise deteriorates with age due to both peripheral and central changes in auditory processing and cognitive decline (e.g., Baltes & Lindenberger, 1997; Committee on Hearing, Bioacoustics, and Biomechanics, 1988; Goossens, Vercammen, Wouters, & van Wieringen, 2017; Humes, 2013). Peripherally, age-related hearing loss, which affects two thirds of adults over the age of 70 years (Lin, Niparko, & Ferrucci, 2011), degrades precise encoding of the speech signal (e.g., Hao et al., 2018). Centrally, auditory processing requires the decoding and integration of peripheral input while complex cognitive processes disentangle and discern the target speech from the presence of other auditory distractors (e.g., Craik, 2007). The full contribution of cognitive processes in speech-in-noise performance is likely complex, with converging evidence from imaging and behavioral studies to suggest that various brain regions and cognitive processes are activated depending on the auditory task (for a review, see Peelle, 2018).
There is an extensive literature investigating the effects of aging on speech-in-noise performance (for a comprehensive review of the topic, see Humes & Dubno, 2010). Some of this literature aims to isolate auditory processing aspects of speech understanding, while others focus on the cognitive demands of the task. First among the multitude of auditory factors that contribute to speech-in-noise perception, reduced audibility among older adults is associated with declines in speech-in-noise performance (e.g., Humes & Roberts, 1990; Humes et al., 1994; van Rooij & Plomp, 1992). Moreover, with sensorineural hearing loss, there are changes in spectral resolution that contribute to difficulties with speech-in-noise perception (e.g., Smith, Pichora-Fuller, Wilson, & Macdonald, 2012). Furthermore, even in older adults with clinically normal or near-normal auditory thresholds, reduced temporal processing abilities are associated with declines in speech-in-noise performance (e.g., Dubno, Horwitz, & Ahlstrom, 2002; Füllgrabe, Sęk, & Moore, 2018; Gifford, Bacon, & Williams, 2007; Hopkins & Moore, 2011). Finally, the interactions between audibility and auditory processing changes for older adults cannot be easily disentangled from central auditory processing and cognitive changes associated with advancing age (e.g., Divenyi, Stark, & Haupt, 2005; Füllgrabe, Moore, & Stone, 2015; Humes et al., 2012; Schneider, Daneman, & Pichora-Fuller, 2002).
Above and beyond the associated auditory factors, cognitive changes likely contribute to age-related difficulties with speech-in-noise performance (e.g., Wingfield, 1996). Older adults show performance deficits on speech-in-noise tasks when the cognitive load for the task is manipulated. For example, in a dual-task interference paradigm, older adults (with and without age-related hearing loss) exhibit secondary task deficits, even when the presentation level for the auditory task allows for correct word identification in the single-task condition (Tun, McCoy, & Wingfield, 2009). Furthermore, evidence from the literature on speech-on-speech masking demonstrates that older adults are more distracted by speech maskers with meaningful semantic content compared to younger adults, suggesting reduced cognitive processing abilities necessary for selectively attending to a target message (Helfer, Chevalier, & Freyman, 2010; Rossi-Katz & Arehart, 2009; Tun, O'Kane, & Wingfield, 2002).
Previous literature directly investigating the relationship between speech-in-noise understanding and cognitive performance has often used single measures of cognitive function, which may contribute to overly simplistic conclusions and measurement error (Akeroyd, 2008; Dryden, Allen, Henshaw, & Heinrich, 2017). For example, a relatively consistent finding in the speech-in-noise and aging literature points to associations with working memory abilities (for reviews, see Akeroyd, 2008; Dryden et al., 2017). However, within the working memory literature, it is unclear whether older adults have poor working memory skills or whether poor perceptual sensitivity induces excessive demands on the working memory resources of older adults (e.g., Füllgrabe & Rosen, 2016; Gordon-Salant & Cole, 2016; Pichora-Fuller, Schneider, & Daneman, 1995). Additionally, as highlighted by a recent systematic review and meta-analysis, many studies do not control for important potential confounders (e.g., age), are limited by small sample size, and investigate a limited number of cognitive domains (Dryden et al., 2017).
The current population-based investigation examined a relatively large sample (N = 250) of older adults in a cross-sectional study of associations between four specific cognitive domains, as measured with a comprehensive neurocognitive battery, and speech-in-noise performance. Based on evidence that older adults show broad activation networks during speech-in-noise tasks (Peelle, Troiani, Wingfield, & Grossman, 2010), we hypothesized that all cognitive domains are associated with speech-in-noise performance after adjusting for audibility, age, and other demographic and cardiovascular risk factors. The rationale for this hypothesis is based on the broad and complex cognitive processes that contribute to the understanding of speech in noisy backgrounds.
Method
Study Population
The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study is a population-based prospective cohort study of 15,792 men and women aged 45–64 years recruited in 1987–1989 from four U.S. communities (Washington County, Maryland; Forsyth County, North Carolina; Jackson, Mississippi; and Minneapolis, Minnesota). In 2011–2013, ARIC participants returned for a fifth visit as part of the ARIC Neurocognitive Study. At that time, the Washington County site invited 307 participants to complete hearing testing as a pilot investigation. Six declined, and 46 did not complete the examination (primarily due to impacted cerumen). Two non-White participants were excluded, as were three who were missing complete cognitive data, resulting in an analytic sample of 250. Compared to all 2011–2013 ARIC Neurocognitive Study participants, participants in our hearing pilot study tended to be older and to have fewer years of education (Deal et al., 2015).
Hearing Thresholds and Speech Perception Assessment
Pure-tone hearing thresholds and speech-in-noise performance were measured in a sound-attenuated booth by a trained technician. All testing was completed using insert earphones (EARTone 3A, 3M) and an Interacoustics AD629 audiometer (Interacoustics A/S). Hearing thresholds were measured at octave frequencies between 0.5 and 8 kHz; a speech frequency pure-tone average (PTA) was calculated by taking the average across four octave frequencies: 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz. The PTA from the better hearing ear was modeled both as a continuous variable and categorized into clinically defined cut-points (normal hearing: ≤ 25 dB HL, mild loss: 26–40 dB HL, moderate/severe loss: > 40 dB HL).
Speech-in-noise performance was assessed using the Quick Speech-in-Noise Test (QuickSIN), which presents sentences in the presence of multitalker babble (Killion, Niquette, Gudmundsen, Revit, & Banerjee, 2004). While no speech-in-noise performance test has been evaluated in population-based/nationally representative studies, in clinical trials, the QuickSIN has been shown to be sensitive to speech-in-noise difficulty and has been recommended for speech-in-noise assessment (Wilson, McArdle, & Smith, 2007). The listener was asked to repeat aloud what they heard and was instructed to guess if uncertain. A score of 0–5 was assigned per sentence based on correct identification of five target words. Each list contains six sentences that were presented at successively more difficult signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs) ranging from +25 to 0 dB SNR in 5-dB decrements. The sentences were presented binaurally via insert earphones with a fixed presentation level for the speech (70 dB HL) and incremental increases in noise level for each SNR condition. Each sentence was syntactically correct with limited semantic meaning or contextual clues. The score at the end of each list was a calculation of the necessary SNR for the person to get 50% of the target words correct and was reported as “SNR loss” against the expected SNR necessary for someone with audiometrically normal hearing.
In this study protocol, listeners heard one practice list and then two test lists. SNR loss was recorded as the average score from the two test lists. In the primary analysis, SNR loss was modeled continuously. In secondary analyses, SNR was modeled categorically according to manufacturer-defined cut-points (no loss: 0–3 dB, mild SNR loss: 4–7 dB, moderate SNR loss: 8–15 dB, severe SNR loss: ≥ 16 dB). Due to of the small number of participants with severe SNR loss (n = 28), the moderate and severe categories were combined.
Cognitive Outcomes
A comprehensive neurocognitive battery was administered during the fifth ARIC clinic visit (2011–2013). Multiple standardized tests from several domains were administered (see Table 1 for individual tests). In order to facilitate effect estimate comparisons across cognitive domains, domain-specific z scores were calculated for the domains of memory, language, and speed of processing/executive function based on a priori cognitive test categorization and previous work in this cohort (Deal et al., 2015; see Table 1). A global composite score was created by averaging the three domain-specific scores. The global composite score and all domain-specific scores were scaled so that a one-unit change is equivalent to 1 SD of that score.
Table 1.
Characteristic | Total cohort |
Speech-in-noise performance (QuickSIN) category |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
(N = 250) |
No SNR loss (n = 77) |
Mild SNR loss (n = 86) |
Moderate or worse SNR loss (n = 87) |
|
M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | |
Age, years*** | 77.3 (5.4) | 74.7 (4.3) | 76.9 (5.0) | 80.1 (5.4) |
Hearing level (PTA dB HL)*** | 35.0 (15.0) | 22.6 (9.2) | 33.6 (8.9) | 47.4 (14.1) |
Wide Range Achievement Test*** | 45.5 (6.1) | 47.5 (6.1) | 45.2 (5.8) | 43.9 (5.8) |
Depression |
3.4 (3.0) |
3.2 (2.9) |
3.5 (3.1) |
3.5 (2.9) |
n (%)
|
n (%)
|
n (%)
|
n (%)
|
|
High school education or less | 147 (59) | 38 (49) | 58 (67) | 51 (59) |
Male sex*** | 97 (39) | 19 (25) | 29 (34) | 49 (56) |
Ever smoker | 119 (48) | 39 (51) | 39 (45) | 41 (47) |
Diabetes | 86 (34) | 25 (32) | 24 (28) | 37 (43) |
Hypertension | 181 (72) | 54 (70) | 62 (72) | 65 (75) |
Depression (above median score) | 102 (41) | 27 (35) | 34 (40) | 41 (47) |
Hearing category*** | ||||
Normal hearing | 72 (29) | 52 (68) | 15 (17) | 5 (6) |
Mild loss | 95 (38) | 24 (31) | 52 (60) | 19 (22) |
Moderate loss or worse | 83 (33) | 1 (1) | 19 (22) | 63 (72) |
Hearing aid use | 52 (21) | 3 (4) | 12 (14) | 21 (36) |
Duration of hearing aid use | 4 (2, 10) | 3 (1, 7) | 3 (1.5, 6) | 4 (2, 7) |
Note. Hearing loss categories defined as normal (PTA ≤ 25 dB HL), mild loss (PTA > 25–40 dB HL), and moderate loss or worse (PTA ≥ 40 dB HL). Hearing aid use was based on a yes/no question. Duration of hearing aid use reported in median years (interquartile range). QuickSIN = Quick Speech-in-Noise Test; SNR = signal-to-noise ratio; PTA = pure-tone average.
p < .001.
Other Independent Variables
Demographic information was collected in 1987–1989, including age (years), sex, and education (highest grade or year of school completed). For analysis, education was defined as less than high school versus greater than or equal to high school. Smoking status was self-reported and coded as “ever” or “never” for analysis. Hypertension was considered present if diastolic blood pressure was ≥ 90 mm Hg, systolic blood pressure was ≥ 140 mm Hg, or the participant took antihypertensive medication. Diabetes was defined as fasting blood glucose level of ≥ 126 mg/dl, nonfasting blood glucose level of ≥ 200 mg/dl, self-reported physician's diagnosis of diabetes, or use of medication for diabetes. Depressive symptoms were measured using the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (Kohout, Berkman, Evans, & Cornoni-Huntley, 1993), and premorbid intelligence was measured using the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT; Wilkinson, 1993). Both the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression and the WRAT were modeled as continuous variables. Sex and education were measured at the ARIC study baseline (1987–1989), and all other covariates were measured at the time of audiometric testing.
Statistical Analysis
Distributions of demographic and disease covariates and cognitive test performance were compared across SNR loss categories using Kruskal–Wallis (continuous variables) and chi-square (categorical variables) tests. In order to assess which cognitive domains may be associated with central auditory processing, we used multivariable linear regression to model the relationship between domain-specific cognitive performance and central auditory processing as measured by speech-in-noise performance. Each of three specific cognitive domains (memory, language, and processing speech/executive function) and a global composite score were regressed on speech-in-noise performance in independent models. In secondary analyses, multivariable-adjusted ordinal logistic regression was used to model the association between domain-specific cognitive performance and QuickSIN SNR loss categories. The use of standardized domain-specific z scores and independent models for each domain addresses the primary research question by allowing for the direct comparison of effect estimates across models without concern that correlation between cognitive domain scores may statistically influence results if all domains were included in the same model. Pearson correlation coefficients among the cognitive scores ranged from .55 to .58 between the specific cognitive domains and from .77 to .90 between specific domains and the global composite score (note that the global score was constructed from the scores of the independent domains, so the correlation is understandably high).
Models were adjusted for hearing thresholds (PTA), demographic, and disease covariates. Model 1 adjusted for hearing thresholds (PTA). Model 2 added the demographic factors of age, sex, and education. Finally, Model 3 also adjusted for cardiovascular risk factors, as well as depressive symptoms and premorbid intelligence (WRAT). Hearing thresholds and age were both modeled using linear and quadratic splines to account for nonlinear associations with SNR performance.
Out of concern that QuickSIN performance in participants with a high degree of hearing loss could be driven by peripheral auditory impairment, two sensitivity analyses were conducted. First, models were rerun excluding all participants with a difference in PTA between ears of > 15 dB (n = 20) out of concern that these participants might perform worse than expected on the QuickSIN due to reliance on only the better hearing ear rather than binaural hearing. Second, models were rerun excluding participants with a better ear PTA of > 40 dB HL (n = 83), > 50 dB HL (n = 41), > 55 dB HL (n = 21), and > 60 dB HL (n = 16) in order to ensure audibility of the sentences and to investigate robustness of findings to varying assumptions of the impact of peripheral hearing on the modeled association.
Results
Of 250 participants, 77 (30%) had no SNR loss, 87 (34%) had mild SNR loss, and 89 (35%) had moderate or greater SNR loss (see Table 1). Mean age at the time of hearing assessment was 77.4 years (SD = 5.4), and mean PTA in the better hearing ear was 35.2 dB HL (SD = 15.2), consistent with a mild hearing loss. On average, participants with more SNR loss (i.e., poorer performance) tended to be older (80.1 years), male (56%), have poorer scores on the WRAT, and have poorer hearing thresholds (better hearing ear PTA = 47.8 dB HL, consistent with a moderate hearing loss). In addition, unadjusted analyses suggested distributions that all cognitive tests differed by SNR loss category, except for Digit Span Backward Test (Wechsler, 1981) and Trail Making Test Part A (Spreen & Strauss, 1991; see Table 2).
Table 2.
Cognitive domain and individual tests | Total cohort |
Speech-in-noise performance (QuickSIN) category |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
(N = 250) |
No SNR loss (n = 77) |
Mild SNR loss (n = 86) |
Moderate or worse SNR loss (n = 87) |
|
M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | |
Memory | ||||
Delayed Word Recall Test a *** | 5.6 (1.7) | 6.2 (1.5) | 5.7 (1.6) | 4.9 (1.6) |
Incidental learning*** | 3.7 (2.4) | 4.8 (2.6) | 3.3 (2.3) | 3.3 (2.1) |
Logical memory b *** | 20.3 (7.0) | 23.3 (6.4) | 18.8 (7.0) | 19.2 (6.6.) |
Language | ||||
Word Fluency a ** | 33.5 (11.2) | 37.2 (10.8) | 32.3 (11.1) | 31.4 (11.0) |
Animal Naming Test** | 16.7 (4.4) | 18.2 (4.3) | 16.5 (4.1) | 15.5 (4.3) |
Boston Naming Test c ** | 26.6 (3.3) | 27.7 (2.0) | 26.2 (3.2) | 25.9 (4.1) |
Processing speed/executive function | ||||
Digit Symbol Substitution Test*** | 40.3 (9.8) | 45.1 (9.0) | 39.8 (9.3) | 36.7 (9.3) |
Digit Span Backward Test b | 5.8 (1.7) | 6.2 (1.9) | 5.7 (1.5) | 5.5 (1.7) |
Trail Making Test Part A a | 35.2 (12.7) | 33.8 (10.0) | 36.1 (13.1) | 35.4 (14.3) |
Trail Making Test Part B d ** | 117.4 (52.7) | 102.1 (47.1) | 118.3 (49.9) | 131.1 (57.1) |
Note. There are missing data on some of the individual cognitive tests; all missing data were from the moderate or worse signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) loss group unless otherwise stated. QuickSIN = Quick Speech-in-Noise Test.
Delayed Word Recall, Word Fluency, and Trail Making Test Part A measured in N = 249 participants.
Logical Memory and Digit Span Backward Test measured in N = 247 participants.
Boston Naming Test measured in N = 248 participants.
Trail Making Test Part B measures in N = 235 participants (one missing from no SNR loss and seven missing from mild SNR loss group).
p < .01.
p < .001.
In multivariable-adjusted analyses, SNR loss score was significantly associated with each of the cognitive domains in all three models (see Table 3). After adjusting for PTA and other demographic and disease factors, each 1-dB increase in SNR loss (i.e., worse performance) was associated with a significantly lower z score for all cognitive domains and the global composite factor: memory, β = −0.80, 95% CI [−1.35, −0.24]; language, β = −0.92, 95% CI [−1.49, −0.34]; processing speed/executive function, β = −0.65, 95% CI [−1.20, −0.09]; and global composite score, β = −1.11, 95% CI [−1.72, −0.50] (see Table 3). Inferences were unchanged in the secondary analyses of QuickSIN categories and in the first sensitivity analysis that excluded asymmetrical hearing loss (data not shown).
Table 3.
Cognitive domain | Model 1 |
Model 2 |
Model 3 |
|||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | 95% CI | p | R 2 | β | 95% CI | p | R 2 | β | 95% CI | p | R 2 | |
Memory | −1.32 | [−1.82, −0.81] | < .001 | .63 | −1.10 | [−1.62, −0.59] | < .0001 | .65 | −0.80 | [−1.35, −0.24] | .014 | .67 |
Language | −1.30 | [−1.81, −0.80] | < .001 | .62 | −1.26 | [−1.77, −0.75] | < .0001 | .65 | −0.92 | [−1.49, −0.34] | .006 | .67 |
Processing speed/ executive function | −1.18 | [−1.65, −0.70] | < .001 | .62 | −0.95 | [−1.46, −0.45] | .014 | .65 | −0.65 | [−1.20, −0.09] | .021 | .66 |
Global composite score | −1.51 | [−2.00, −1.01] | < .001 | .64 | −1.39 | [−1.90, −0.88] | < .001 | .67 | −1.11 | [−1.72, −0.50] | .003 | .68 |
Note. Results for each domain and for the global composite score come from separate models. Model 1 adjusts for hearing thresholds as defined by a pure-tone average calculated as the average thresholds across four octave frequencies important for hearing speech (0.5–4 kHz) and entered into the model as a continuous variable. Model 2 adjusts for hearing thresholds and demographic factors, which includes the continuous variable of age and the categorical variables of education (high school degree or less) and sex. Model 3 adjusts for hearing thresholds, demographics, and potential risk factors, such as cardiovascular risk factors including ”ever smoker,” diabetes, and hypertension as well as depression (measured with the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale; all categorical) and premorbid intelligence (continuous; measured with the Wide Range Achievement Test).
However, in a second sensitivity analysis that excluded participants with moderate to moderately severe PTA, the number of significant associations was reduced, and these associations varied by the PTA cut-point used for exclusion. No significant associations were observed when the analysis excluded all participants with a moderate or greater hearing loss (> 40 dB HL). When excluding participants with a PTA of > 50 dB HL, only memory and the global composite score were significantly associated with speech-in-noise performance after full adjustment (β = −0.67 and β = −0.79, respectively). A significant association for language was recovered when the cut-point for exclusion was moved to > 55 dB HL (β = −0.74), but a relationship for processing speed/executive function was not recovered, even at a threshold for exclusion of > 60 dB HL (see Table 4).
Table 4.
Cognitive domain | PTA > 40 db HL (excluded) (N = 167) |
PTA > 50 db HL (excluded) (N = 209) |
PTA > 55 db HL (N = 229) |
PTA > 60 db HL (N = 234) |
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | 95% CI | R 2 | β | 95% CI | R 2 | β | 95% CI | R 2 | β | 95% CI | R 2 | |
Memory | −0.51 | [−1.09, 0.07] | .26 | −0.67 | [−1.25, −0.09] | .39 | −0.63 | [−1.20, −0.06] | .50 | −0.65 | [−1.21, −0.08] | .57 |
Language | −0.36 | [−0.96, 0.24] | .26 | −0.57 | [−1.17, 0.04] | .38 | −0.74 | [−1.34, −0.14] | .51 | −0.84 | [−1.42, −0.26] | .58 |
Processing speed/executive function | −0.25 | [−0.82, 0.32] | .25 | −0.35 | [−0.92, 0.22] | .38 | −0.50 | [−1.06, 0.06] | .50 | −0.52 | [−1.07, 0.04] | .57 |
Global composite score | −0.51 | [−1.15, 0.12] | .26 | −0.79 | [−1.42, −0.16] | .39 | −0.92 | [−1.55, −0.28] | .50 | −0.99 | [−1.60, −0.37] | .58 |
Note. Adjusted for hearing thresholds as defined by a pure-tone average (PTA) calculated as the average thresholds across four octave frequencies important for hearing speech (0.5–4 kHz) and entered into the model as a continuous variable; demographic factors, including age (continuous), education (high school degree or less), and sex; and potential risk factors, including ever smoking, diabetes, hypertension, depression (measured with the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale; all categorical), and premorbid intelligence (continuous; measured with the Wide Range Achievement Test).
Discussion
In this pilot study of 250 older adults from Washington County, Maryland (M age = 78 years), we found that poorer cognitive performance in the domains of memory, language, executive functioning, and global function was associated with poorer speech-in-noise performance, independent of peripheral audiometric hearing thresholds and demographic and disease factors. However, the domain-specific relationships with poorer speech-in-noise performance were attenuated, and many not statistically significant, when participants with moderate or greater hearing loss were excluded from the analysis.
Previous studies that have looked at the relationship between speech-in-noise and domain-specific cognitive function have generally found mixed and inconsistent associations with measures of IQ, processing speed, inhibitory control, memory, and working memory (Akeroyd, 2008). For example, Humes (2002) found, using principal component analysis, that after hearing loss (standardized coefficient = 0.73) the next two cognitive factors taken from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (Wechsler, 1981) that were most predictive were verbal IQ and nonverbal IQ (standardized coefficients = 0.27 and 0.23, respectively). On the other hand, a meta-analysis by Dryden et al. (2017) found significant associations in pooled analyses for processing speed, inhibitory control, working memory, and episodic memory, but no relationship for crystallized IQ. Our findings contribute to the literature because, (a) in comparison, this is a large community-based sample of older adults with varying degrees of hearing thresholds, (b) we utilized a comprehensive neurocognitive battery to define domain-specific cognitive performance, and (c) our regression models (as compared to simple correlations, which are more commonly found in this literature) adjust for age, sex, education, depression, premorbid intelligence, and cardiovascular risk factors. Much like the meta-analysis by Dryden and colleagues, the current study found associations between speech-in-noise performance and multiple cognitive domains.
In a sensitivity analysis that excluded participants with a better ear PTA of > 40 dB HL, no cognitive domain was significantly associated with speech-in-noise performance. As participants with more severe hearing loss were increasingly added back into the model, memory was the only cognitive domain consistently associated with speech-in-noise performance. The language domain association was significant when participants with a PTA of > 50 dB HL were included. One potential explanation for this finding is that hearing loss continues to confound the relationship between cognitive function and speech-in-noise performance, even after adjusting for PTA in the model. However, it is also noteworthy that the confidence intervals are wide (related to decreased sample size), and so inference is limited as to whether the effect estimates from the different models truly differ from each other. When Dryden et al. (2017) separated studies that included normal to mild hearing loss and studies that included normal to moderate hearing loss participants, they found consistent associations between speech-in-noise performance and cognition collapsed across all cognitive tests. As such, their evidence does not seem to suggest that particular cognitive domains are differentially impacted by the degree of hearing loss.
On the other hand, based on the results presented here, one might surmise that if mild peripheral hearing loss is not causing audibility problems in the speech task, then the cognitive domains of language and processing speed are less important for speech understanding. The Ease of Language Understanding model suggests that degrading the acoustic signal (e.g., by presenting speech in the presence of noise) causes phonological mismatches as it relates to our long-term memory for the automatic processing of speech (Rönnberg et al., 2011). Perhaps the reliance on episodic long-term memory as proposed by the Ease of Language Understanding model explains why memory emerged as the cognitive domain that was most associated with speech-in-noise performance when issues of audibility were minimized.
Generalizability of these results may be limited due to the single-center, all-White study cohort. Nevertheless, the current sample is larger than other studies that have considered the associations of cognition with speech-in-noise performance, and unlike prior studies, which are primarily clinical or convenience samples (Akeroyd, 2008), ARIC is a community-based random sample. In addition, the adjustment for hearing thresholds was based on a four-frequency PTA in the better hearing ear, but the speech material was presented binaurally. To address this, a sensitivity analysis was performed that excluded all participants with a difference in PTA of > 15 dB, and there was no difference in the pattern of the results.
These findings may have important implications for auditory rehabilitation and clinical practice. Previous rehabilitation research has focused on auditory perceptual training of individual speech sounds, word-based training, and specific cognitive-based tasks related to working memory skills (Ferguson & Henshaw, 2015; Humes, Kinney, Brown, Kiener, & Quigley, 2014). However, the significant association across all cognitive domains to speech-in-noise performance in this cohort could possibly suggest that auditory rehabilitation may benefit from enhancing not only perceptual acuity and executive function but also language and memory domains. Given mixed results regarding generalization of auditory training to everyday listening situations (Ferguson & Henshaw, 2015), perhaps a more global cognitive training approach would be beneficial based on the broad contribution of all cognitive domains to speech-in-noise performance in this cohort.
Clinically, there have been recent calls for audiologists to screen for cognitive decline during the hearing appointment (Shen, Anderson, Arehart, & Souza, 2016). Much of this discussion is motivated by research demonstrating hearing loss as a risk factor for cognitive decline and the nature of serving older adult populations. In the context of speech understanding, screening for cognitive decline may also be beneficial. Importantly, the sensitivity results of this study suggest that cognitive domains are not significantly associated with speech-in-noise understanding when more severe hearing loss is excluded. In other words, it is possible that persons with greater degrees of hearing loss will be more impacted by their cognitive processing abilities when it comes to understanding speech in noisy environments. More research is needed, but one might consider a clinical model whereby higher degrees of hearing loss trigger an in-office cognitive screening by appropriately trained audiologists in order to (a) assess target areas for auditory rehabilitation to improve speech-in-noise understanding or (b) to help expectation management with speech understanding when using hearing aids.
In conclusion, this study documented an independent association between poorer cognitive performance in the domains of memory, language, executive function, and global cognitive function and poorer speech-in-noise performance in 250 community-dwelling older adults with an average age of 77 years. These findings could have implications for auditory rehabilitation. Although not assessed as part of this study, given the broad network of cognitive processing required for speech understanding in noisy backgrounds, auditory rehabilitation plans that emphasize training across a range of cognitive domains could potentially be more successful in supporting communication among adults with age-related hearing loss. Future studies of auditory rehabilitation should address these possible clinical implications.
Acknowledgments
The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) was supported by National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute contracts (HHSN268201100005C, HHSN268201100006C, HHSN268201100007C, HHSN268201100008C, HHSN268201100009C, HHSN268201100010C, HHSN268201100011C, and HHSN268201100012C). Neurocognitive data in ARIC were collected by U01 2U01HL096812, 2U01HL096814, 2U01HL096899, 2U01HL096902, and 2U01HL096917 from the National Institutes of Health (National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institute on Aging, and National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders) and with previous brain magnetic resonance imaging examinations funded by R01-HL70825 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. The collection of pilot audiometric data was supported by National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders Grant K23DC011279 and by the Eleanor Schwartz Charitable Foundation. J. A. Deal was supported by National Institute on Aging Grant K01AG23291. J. A. Deal and N. S. Reed were supported in part by the Johns Hopkins Cochlear Center for Hearing and Public Health. S. K. Mamo was supported by National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders Grant K23DC016855. The authors thank the staff and participants of the ARIC Study for their important contributions.
Funding Statement
The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) was supported by National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute contracts (HHSN268201100005C, HHSN268201100006C, HHSN268201100007C, HHSN268201100008C, HHSN268201100009C, HHSN268201100010C, HHSN268201100011C, and HHSN268201100012C). Neurocognitive data in ARIC were collected by U01 2U01HL096812, 2U01HL096814, 2U01HL096899, 2U01HL096902, and 2U01HL096917 from the National Institutes of Health (National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institute on Aging, and National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders) and with previous brain magnetic resonance imaging examinations funded by R01-HL70825 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. The collection of pilot audiometric data was supported by National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders Grant K23DC011279 and by the Eleanor Schwartz Charitable Foundation. J. A. Deal was supported by National Institute on Aging Grant K01AG23291. J. A. Deal and N. S. Reed were supported in part by the Johns Hopkins Cochlear Center for Hearing and Public Health. S. K. Mamo was supported by National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders Grant K23DC016855.
References
- Akeroyd M. A. (2008). Are individual differences in speech reception related to individual differences in cognitive ability? A survey of twenty experimental studies with normal and hearing-impaired adults. International Journal of Audiology, 47(Suppl. 2), S53–S71. https://doi.org/10.1080/14992020802301142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baltes P. B., & Lindenberger U. (1997). Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: A new window to the study of cognitive aging? Psychology and Aging, 12(1), 12–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Committee on Hearing, Bioacoustics, and Biomechanics. (1988). Speech understanding and aging. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 83, 859–895. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craik F. I. (2007). The role of cognition in age-related hearing loss. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 18(7), 539–547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deal J. A., Sharrett A. R., Albert M. S., Coresh J., Mosley T. H., Knopman D., … Lin F. R. (2015). Hearing impairment and cognitive decline: A pilot study conducted within the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Neurocognitive Study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 181(9), 680–690. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwu333 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Divenyi P. L., Stark P. B., & Haupt K. M. (2005). Decline of speech understanding and auditory thresholds in the older adults. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118(2), 1089–1100. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1953207 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dryden A., Allen H. A., Henshaw H., & Heinrich A. (2017). The association between cognitive performance and speech-in-noise perception for adult listeners: A systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Trends in Hearing, 21 https://doi.org/10.1177/2331216517744675 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubno J. R., Horwitz A. R., & Ahlstrom J. B. (2002). Benefit of modulated maskers for speech recognition by younger and older adults with normal hearing. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 111(6), 2897–2907. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1480421 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson M. A., & Henshaw H. (2015). Auditory training can improve working memory, attention, and communication in adverse conditions for adults with hearing loss. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(556). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00556 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Füllgrabe C., Moore B. C., & Stone M. A. (2015). Age-group differences in speech identification despite matched audiometrically normal hearing: Contributions from auditory temporal processing and cognition. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 6(347). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00347 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Füllgrabe C., & Rosen S. (2016). On the (un)importance of working memory in speech-in-noise processing for listeners with normal hearing thresholds. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(1268). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01268 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Füllgrabe C., Sęk A. P., & Moore B. C. J. (2018). Senescent changes in sensitivity to binaural temporal fine structure. Trends in Hearing, 22 https://doi.org/10.1177/2331216518788224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gifford R. H., Bacon S. P., & Williams E. J. (2007). An examination of speech recognition in a modulated background and of forward masking in younger and older listeners. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 857–864. http://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/060) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goossens T., Vercammen C., Wouters J., & van Wieringen A. (2017). Masked speech perception across the adult lifespan: Impact of age and hearing impairment. Hearing Research, 344, 109–124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2016.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon-Salant S., & Cole S. S. (2016). Effects of age and working memory capacity on speech recognition performance in noise among listeners with normal hearing. Ear and Hearing, 37(5), 593–602. https://doi.org/10.1097/aud.0000000000000316 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hao W., Wang Q., Li L., Qiao Y., Gao Z., Ni D., & Shang Y. (2018). Effects of phase-locking deficits on speech recognition in older adults with presbycusis. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 10(397), https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helfer K. S., Chevalier J., & Freyman R. L. (2010). Aging, spatial cues, and single- versus dual-task performance in competing speech perception. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 128(6), 3625–3633. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3502462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopkins K., & Moore B. C. (2011). The effects of age and cochlear hearing loss on temporal fine structure sensitivity, frequency selectivity, and speech reception in noise. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130(1), 334–349. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3585848 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humes L. E. (2002). Factors underlying the speech-recognition performance of elderly hearing-aid wearers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 112(3), 1112–1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humes L. E. (2013). Understanding the speech-understanding problems of older adults. American Journal of Audiology, 22(2), 303–305. https://doi.org/10.1044/1059-0889(2013/12-0066) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humes L. E., & Dubno J. R. (2010). Factors affecting speech understanding in older adults. In Gordon-Salant S., Frisina R. D., Popper A. N., & Fay R. R. (Eds.), The aging auditory system (pp. 211–258). New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Humes L. E., Dubno J. R., Gordon-Salant S., Lister J. J., Cacace A. T., Cruickshanks K. J., … Wingfield A. (2012). Central presbycusis: A review and evaluation of the evidence. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 23(8), 635–666. https://doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.23.8.5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humes L. E., Kinney D. L., Brown S. E., Kiener A. L., & Quigley T. M. (2014). The effects of dosage and duration of auditory training for older adults with hearing impairment. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 136(3), EL224 https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4890663 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humes L. E., & Roberts L. (1990). Speech-recognition difficulties of the hearing-impaired elderly: The contributions of audibility. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33(4), 726–735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humes L. E., Watson B. U., Christensen L. A., Cokely C. G., Halling D. C., & Lee L. (1994). Factors associated with individual differences in clinical measures of speech recognition among the elderly. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37(2), 465–474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killion M. C., Niquette P. A., Gudmundsen G. I., Revit L. J., & Banerjee S. (2004). Development of a Quick Speech-in-Noise Test for measuring signal-to-noise ratio loss in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 116(4, Pt. 1), 2395–2405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohout F. J., Berkman L. F., Evans D. A., & Cornoni-Huntley J. (1993). Two shorter forms of the CES-D depression symptoms index. Journal of Aging and Health, 5(2), 179–193. https://doi.org/10.1177/089826439300500202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin F. R., Niparko J. K., & Ferrucci L. (2011). Hearing loss prevalence in the United States. Archives of Internal Medicine, 171(20), 1851–1853. https://doi.org/10.1001/archinternmed.2011.506 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peelle J. E. (2018). Listening effort: How the cognitive consequences of acoustic challenge are reflected in brain and behavior. Ear and Hearing, 39(2), 204–214. https://doi.org/10.1097/aud.0000000000000494 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peelle J. E., Troiani V., Wingfield A., & Grossman M. (2010). Neural processing during older adults' comprehension of spoken sentences: Age differences in resource allocation and connectivity. Cerebral Cortex, 20(4), 773–782. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp142 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pichora-Fuller M. K., Schneider B. A., & Daneman M. (1995). How young and old adults listen to and remember speech in noise. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97(1), 593–608. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.412282 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossi-Katz J., & Arehart K. H. (2009). Message and talker identification in older adults: Effects of task, distinctiveness of the talkers' voices, and meaningfulness of the competing message. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(2), 435–453. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0243) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rönnberg J., Danielsson H., Rudner M., Arlinger S., Sternäng O., Wahlin Å., & Nilsson L.-G. (2011). Hearing loss is negatively related to episodic and semantic long-term memory but not to short-term memory. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54(2), 705–726. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0088) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider B. A., Daneman M., & Pichora-Fuller M. K. (2002). Listening in aging adults: From discourse comprehension to psychoacoustics. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56(3), 139–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shen J., Anderson M. C., Arehart K. H., & Souza P. E. (2016). Using cognitive screening tests in audiology. American Journal of Audiology, 25(4), 319–331. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_AJA-16-0032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith S. L., Pichora-Fuller M. K., Wilson R. H., & Macdonald E. N. (2012). Word recognition for temporally and spectrally distorted materials: The effects of age and hearing loss. Ear and Hearing, 33(3), 349–366. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0b013e318242571c [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreen O., & Strauss E. A. (1991). Compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Tun P. A., McCoy S., & Wingfield A. (2009). Aging, hearing acuity, and the attentional costs of effortful listening. Psychology and Aging, 24(3), 761–766. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014802 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tun P. A., O'Kane G., & Wingfield A. (2002). Distraction by competing speech in young and older adult listeners. Psychology and Aging, 17(3), 453–467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rooij J. C., & Plomp R. (1992). Auditive and cognitive factors in speech perception by elderly listeners. III. Additional data and final discussion. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 91(2), 1028–1033. https:/i.org/10.1121/1.402628 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (2nd ed.). New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Wilkinson G. S. (1993). WRAT-3: The Wide Range Achievement Test–Third Edition administration manual. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson R. H., McArdle R. A., & Smith S. L. (2007). An evaluation of the BKB-SIN, HINT, QuickSIN, and WIN materials on listeners with normal hearing and listeners with hearing loss. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 844–856. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/059) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wingfield A. (1996). Cognitive factors in auditory performance: Context, speed of processing, and constraints of memory. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 7(3), 175–182. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]