Abstract
Mindfulness and anxiety are often linked as inversely related traits and there have been several theoretical and mediational models proposed suggesting such a relationship between these two traits. The current review report offers an account of self-report measures, behavioral, electrophysiological, hemodynamic, and biological studies, which provide converging evidence for an inverse relationship between mindfulness and anxiety. To our knowledge, there are no comprehensive accounts of empirical evidence that investigate this relationship. After reviewing several empirical studies, we propose a schematic model, where a stressor can trigger the activation of amygdala which activates the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) pathway. This hyperactive HPA axis leads to a cascade of psychological, behavioral, electrophysiological, immunological, endocrine, and genetic reactions in the body, primarily mediated by a sympathetic pathway. Conversely, mindfulness protects from deleterious effects of these triggered reactions by downregulating the HPA axis activity via a parasympathetic pathway. Finally, we propose a model suggesting a comprehensive scheme through which mindfulness and anxiety may interact through emotion regulation. It is recommended that future mindfulness intervention studies should examine a broad spectrum of measurement indices where possible, keeping logistic feasibility in mind and look at mindfulness in conjunction with anxiety rather than independently.
Keywords: mindfulness, anxiety, stress, emotion regulation, measurement indices
Ancient practices of Buddhism and Asian traditions frequently emphasize attending to experiences with equanimity to liberate one from suffering. Attending to experiences with equanimity can be referred as mindful attention (Grossman, 2010), often undertaken to experience reduced “sufferings” such as the pain of anxiety (Kabat-Zinn et al., 1992). Mindfulness has been defined as directing one’s attention in the present moment while adopting a nonjudgmental perspective toward experiences (Kabat-Zinn, 2005). Trait or dispositional mindfulness represents the tendency to behave like this as an innate individual characteristic (Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006), while state mindfulness refers to changes in the state resulting from, for example, meditation interventions (Lau et al., 2006). It has been frequently suggested that mindfulness either as trait or practice might show beneficial effects through emotion regulation (see review Chambers, Gullone, & Allen, 2009). Anxiety, on the other hand, can be regarded as an affective state, disposition, or trait (Scherer, 2009) and impairs top-down processing/executive control, with an example being enhanced susceptibility toward irrelevant salient stimuli (Eysenck & Calvo, 1992; Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo, 2007; Moser, Becker, & Moran, 2012). Anxiety may be elicited transiently in response to a threatening situation (state anxiety) or it may also be sustained as a stable property of an individual reflecting their anxiety trait (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970). Furthermore, it has been suggested that stressful events occurring in early or recent life may contribute to psychopathology underlying some anxiety disorder (Faravelli et al., 2012). Stress may be regarded as an emergent process that interacts with the environment, past or recent events, homeostatic states and brings about psychophysiological reactions (Epel et al., 2018). The physical or psychological stimulus or event that may trigger physiological responses in the form of a stressful or anxious state typically described as stressors. These stressors are often attributed as a potential and sometimes an unpredictable threat to the individual (Hannibal & Bishop, 2014). The most widely studied mindfulness intervention program, mindfulness-based stress reduction, was developed initially to relieve the stress occurring in patients suffering from chronic pain (Kabat-Zinn, Lipworth, & Burney, 1985).
Several empirical studies consistently suggest that mindfulness and anxiety are inversely related to each other either at state or trait levels. Here, we present a summary of the lines of evidence which may support this inverse relationship between mindfulness and anxiety, possibly through an emotion regulation mechanism. In this review article, state and trait form of mindfulness and anxiety will be used interchangeably as one, as both forms are intimately linked to each other. The current review provides the accounts of a range of measurement indices that have been used in investigations in the mindfulness and anxiety literature. These indices comprise subjective well-being (Chang, Huang, & Lin, 2015; Coffey & Hartman, 2008; Walsh, Balint, Smolira, Fredericksen, & Madsen, 2009), cognitive processes (Di Francesco et al., 2017; Moore & Malinowski, 2009; Pacheco-Unguetti, Acosta, Callejas, & Lupiáñez, 2010; Tang et al., 2007), electrophysiology neuroanatomy (Bishop, 2009; Etkin et al., 2004; Mocaiber et al., 2009; Tang et al., 2010; Way, Creswell, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2010), neuroendocrine markers (Brown, Weinstein, & Creswell, 2012; Rosenkranz et al., 2013), immunological (Black & Slavich, 2016; Davidson et al., 2003; Rosenkranz et al., 2016), and genetic research (Epel, Daubenmier, Moskowitz, Folkman, & Blackburn, 2009; Jacobs et al., 2011; Schutte & Malouff, 2014). The current review article is an attempt to aggregate this set of evidence to offer a broad spectrum of indices for future mindfulness-based intervention (MBI) research. This may facilitate the investigation of the efficacy of such programs by reducing the cost of the intervention and proving sensitive indices that may reveal the individual differences in responsiveness to the program under consideration.
1. Evidence suggesting an inverse relationship between mindfulness and anxiety
1.1. Self-report measures
Self-report measures are subjective measures estimating subjective feelings such as traits or states measured by questionnaires or interviews on a measurable scale and estimate these features as mathematical scores (Howard, 1994; Razavi, 2001). Due to the lack of consensus on the operational definition of mindfulness, there are an array of mindfulness questionnaires which measure different aspects of mindfulness (see Table 1). Although the current article lists a range of questionnaires mentioned in mindfulness literature, it does not provide any specific recommendation on just one questionnaire (for details, see review by Chiesa, 2013) with the appropriateness of the use of each depending on how mindfulness is described in a study. In contrast, anxiety, for which there is a better consensus on its definition, is most widely measured by the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, developed by Spielberger et al. (1970), while stress is often quantified by the Perceived Stress Scale developed by Cohen, Kamarck, and Mermelstein (1994).
Table 1.
Summary of questionnaires available to measure various aspects of mindfulness
Questionnaires | Source | Components measured |
---|---|---|
Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) | Brown and Ryan (2003) | “present-centred attention” |
Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS) | Baer, Smith, and Allen (2004) | “observing, describing, acting with awareness, and accepting without judgment” |
Five Factors Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ) | Baer et al. (2006) | “nonreactivity, observing, acting with awareness, describing, and nonjudging” |
Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI) | Walach, Buchheld, Buttenmüller, Kleinknecht, and Schmidt (2006) | “specific to experienced meditators” |
Toronto Mindfulness Scale (TMS) | Lau et al. (2006) | “state-like mindfulness” |
Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale (PMS) | Cardaciotto, Herbert, Forman, Moitra, and Farrow (2008) | “present-moment awareness and acceptance” |
Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale (CAMS)-Revised | Kumar, Feldman, and Hayes (2008) | “attention, awareness, present-focus, and acceptance/non-judgment” |
Southampton Mindfulness Questionnaire (SMQ) | Chadwick et al. (2008) | “mindful observation, letting go, nonaversion, and nonjudgment” |
State Mindfulness Scale (SMS) | Tanay and Bernstein (2013) | “state-like mental behavior” |
Source: Information from (Chiesa, 2013).
Two studies on self-report measures, one in a nonclinical group (Coffey & Hartman, 2008) and another in a clinical population (with mood and anxiety disorder) (Desrosiers, Vine, Klemanski, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2013), showed that both mindfulness and anxiety could be mediated through emotion regulation strategies. These strategies could be worry, reappraisal, nonacceptance, or rumination, which might be attenuated by “buffering actions” of mindfulness, which might consequently mitigate the symptoms associated with anxiety. In addition, a study showed that trait mindfulness could predict lower anxiety and negative affect in response to anxiogenic stressors (Arch & Craske, 2010). A mindfulness-based longitudinal survey also showed a change in the trajectory of state mindfulness could predict post-intervention enhancement in trait mindfulness and decreased psychological distress levels after 7 weeks of training (Kiken, Garland, Bluth, Palsson, & Gaylord, 2015). Furthermore, it was suggested that mindfulness-based therapy could alleviate the symptoms of anxiety via emotion regulation (Hofmann, Sawyer, Witt, & Oh, 2010; Tang, Holzel, & Posner, 2015). Therefore, future mindfulness studies investigating anxiety should consider the role of emotion regulation in mediation between mindfulness and anxiety.
1.2. Behavioral measures
There are some independent studies which have demonstrated that higher trait mindfulness, irrespective of whether one practices meditation or not, is associated with better executive functions. Executive functions can be defined as a set of regulatory mechanisms or higher order cognitive functions necessary for general-purpose control of behavior (Miyake et al., 2000; Miyake and Friedman, 2012). For instance, conflict control is an executive function that was shown to be predicted by trait mindfulness scores in a study employing the color Stroop task (Moore & Malinowski, 2009) and another studying using the Attentional Network Test (Di Francesco et al., 2017). Furthermore, a recent meta-analytic review on anxiety suggested that it, in addition to being related to impairment of working memory, is correlated to measures on a wide range of cognitive process (see a review by Moran, 2016). For example, it was shown that under a low cognitive load state, individuals with higher trait anxiety levels showed impaired conflict control (Bishop, 2009). Additionally, a recent study (Jaiswal, Tsai, Juan, Liang, & Muggleton, 2018) showed that when participants were grouped based on their mindfulness and anxiety scores, the group with high mindfulness and low anxiety had higher efficiency of cognitive control, reflected in higher accuracy on the Stroop task as well as in its associated electrophysiology (Jaiswal, Tsai, Juan, Muggleton, & Liang, 2019). They also showed higher visual working memory capacity on a change-detection task. It seems reasonable to observe impaired conflict control and working memory in high-anxiety or low mindfulness individuals, as both executive functions have been reported to be correlated with each other (Kane & Engle, 2003). These reports indicate that conflict control and working memory capacity are two cognitive functions which are sensitive to both mindfulness as well as anxiety. However, the above-mentioned studies did not account for the role of emotion regulation in these cognitive functions. Therefore, future studies should integrate emotional manipulation to observe the effect of mindfulness and anxiety over these behavioral measures.
1.3. Electrophysiological measures
1.3.1. Event-related potential (ERP)
Electroencephalography (EEG) is an objective approach that may allow assessment of fundamental brain properties (temporally as well as oscillatory) of both mindfulness and anxiety. Event-related potential (ERP) is a temporally sensitive measure of EEG that provides measures of electrophysiological dynamics associated with behavioral and emotional phenomena (Luck & Kappenman, 2012; Schupp et al., 2000). A study that employed passive viewing of emotional and neutral images showed that the late positive potential (LPP) component of ERPs was higher in amplitude in a low mindfulness group than in a high mindfulness group in response to high arousal unpleasant images (Brown, Goodman, & Inzlicht, 2013). This component is a positive deflection in the ERP that appears after the presentation of emotional stimuli and is regarded as an index of emotional responses in the visual modality (Schupp et al., 2000). A higher LPP amplitude observed in a low mindfulness group was suggested to reflect a higher emotional reactivity in this group in response to unpleasant stimuli (Brown et al., 2013). In another study, it was shown that high-trait anxiety people showed a higher LPP in response to unpleasant stimuli than for neutral stimuli, but this was not the case in low-trait anxiety people (Mocaiber et al., 2009). However, this observation was contextual and was modulated by the instructions the participants were given. When they were told the stimuli presented were fictitious, the difference between unpleasant and neutral condition LPP amplitudes was not significant in either group. When the instructions stated that images were obtained from real-life conditions, only the high-trait anxiety group showed a higher LPP amplitude in the unpleasant condition than in the neutral condition. The lack of any difference in LPP amplitude between the unpleasant and neutral stimuli in the fictitious condition suggests that even high-anxiety individuals can attenuate emotional reactivity, presumably by employing a reappraisal strategy for emotion regulation. Future psychotherapeutic programs should incorporate reappraisal to understand the role of emotion regulation in attenuation of emotional reactivity and the LPP component can be a potential index to measure the degree of emotional reactivity among different sets of individuals and in different conditions. Although both studies (Brown et al., 2013; Mocaiber et al., 2009) were carried out independently, the pattern of LPP modulation as a function of trait mindfulness and trait anxiety is consistent with an inverse relationship between these two traits. Therefore, it will be beneficial to measure the LPP component by employing an emotional manipulation while assessing behavioral or electrophysiological characteristics of populations varying in levels of mindfulness and anxiety.
1.3.2. Resting-state EEG
Resting-state EEG, a measure of spontaneous brain activity, can be quantified by a power spectrum over the range of EEG oscillations (Putman, 2011). The EEG oscillations reflect the complexity of activity in neural populations in the brain and can be used to estimate the degree of integration among different brain areas. For instance, global mode oscillations in delta, theta, and alpha frequencies reflect global activity between different cortical regions, while local mode oscillations in beta and gamma frequencies denote events in local cortical areas (Knyazev, 2007). Global mode oscillations have been thought of as reflecting the properties of three critical mental faculties: motivation, emotion, and attentional inhibition (Knyazev, 2007). Mindfulness is suggested to affect the regulation of motivation and emotion, as well as attentional control (Greeson & Brantley, 2009; Tang et al., 2015). These processes are reflected by physiological brain oscillations, where these oscillations, in general, have been linked to motivation (delta frequencies), to emotion (theta frequencies) and attentional inhibition processes (alpha frequencies) (Knyazev, 2007). A recent meta-analysis showed that mindfulness, in general, is associated with elevated theta and alpha activity, argued to reflect a relaxed yet alert state. No systematic patterns in the delta, beta, or gamma oscillations were reported (Lomas, Ivtzan, & Fu, 2015). There appears to be a need for more comprehensive research on oscillatory neural characteristics relating to mindfulness and how it may be reconciled with EEG oscillation research on anxiety (see review Schutter & Knyazev, 2012).
Resting-state EEG is often used as a biomarker for the psychophysiological pathology associated with anxiety and other mental disorders (Fitzgerald & Watson, 2018; Oathes, Bruce, & Nitschke, 2008; Schutter & Knyazev, 2012). Several reports have suggested that gamma activity may be a suitable marker of fear and anxiety, with an elevated level of gamma power observed in anxious people (Gemignani et al., 2000; Oathes et al., 2008; Oya, Kawasaki, Howard, & Adolphs, 2002; Sebastiani, Simoni, Gemignani, Ghelarducci, & Santarcangelo, 2003). In addition to investigation of EEG oscillations in anxiety, cross-frequency coupling is another critical index that might predict levels of anxiety and cognitive abilities. Cumulative evidence demonstrates that coupling between delta and beta frequencies becomes stronger in anxious situations or among anxious people (see Schutter & Knyazev, 2012). The stronger delta–beta coherence in anxious people may be indicative of a functional relationship between the cortex and limbic system (Poppelaars, Harrewijn, Westenberg, & van der Molen, 2018; Putman, 2011) that may be a factor in impairment of emotion regulation. Currently, there is a lack of resting-state EEG studies looking at how different global mode oscillations might modulate the amplitude of local mode oscillations. Consequently, there is potential insight to be gained from investigating the modulations of fundamental brain oscillations during a resting state. While it appears that there is plenty of literature available on resting state in the context of anxiety, very few studies offer a comprehensive insight into the dynamics of brain oscillations associated with mindfulness in a resting state. Since the resting state which, unlike a meditation state involved in mindfulness practice, does not require any active mental effort, it might be associated with brain oscillations features which can better represent the characteristics of individuals and potentially relate to their levels of trait mindfulness and/or trait anxiety. Future studies could beneficially attempt to investigate the dynamics of brain oscillations and could aid the reconciliation of the relationship between anxiety and mindfulness.
1.3.3. Electrocardiograms/heart rate variability (HRV)
Electrocardiogram (ECG) is a measure of the electrical activity of the heart and is another electrophysiological measure which is affected by anxiogenic or stressful stimulus. The beat-to-beat variation in ECG is measured by an index called heart rate variability (HRV) (Akselrod et al., 1981). HRV can be a biomarker of flexibility and it is affected by continuous interaction between the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems to make adjustments in response to the environmental changes (Thayer & Lane, 2007; Thayer & Sternberg, 2006). Increased HRV is an indicator of better well-being and cardiovascular health and is often found to be reduced in patients suffering from anxiety (Kemp & Quintana, 2013). There are submeasures of HRV, such as high-frequency HRV (HF-HRV) (0.15–0.40 Hz) which corresponds to respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) and low-frequency HRV (LF-HRV) (0.04–0.15 Hz) corresponding to the Traube–Hering–Mayer wave(Krygier et al., 2013; Shaffer & Ginsberg, 2017). Examples of changes in these measures can be seen in vagus nerve regulation of parasympathetic activity which modulates changes in RSA and HF-HRV. The LF-HRV can be produced by both parasympathetic and sympathetic activity or baroreflex activity, but it is principally produced by the parasympathetic system (Shaffer & Ginsberg, 2017). The neuroendocrine system mainly regulates sympathetic activity through secretion of norepinephrine and catecholamine hormones, and this brings about transient fluctuations in HRV. HF-HRV and LF-HRV are the most widely studied HRV measures in the context of mindfulness effects (Krygier et al., 2013; Libby, Worhunsky, Pilver, & Brewer, 2012; Sun et al., 2019; Takahashi et al., 2005).
A line of studies has shown that mindfulness meditation is associated with higher HRV (Krygier et al., 2013; Libby et al., 2012; Takahashi et al., 2005). This increase in HRV is often illustrated by an increase in normalized units of HF (HFn.u.), which is estimated by the formula (HF/(HF-LF)) (Takahashi et al., 2005). Therefore, it should be kept in mind while interpreting a higher HRV that it not necessarily always contributed by increases in the HF band, it may also be due to a decrease in the LF band. For example, Krygier et al. (2013) observed that after 10 days of Vipassana training, a form of mindfulness training, an increase in HFn.u. was seen, which was further reportedly contributed to by a decrease in LF power rather than an increase in HF power. An increase in HFn.u. is interpreted as enhanced parasympathetic activity, which is a characteristic feature associated with mindfulness training that involves slowing down of breath and bringing “calmness” to the body and relief from stressful reactions (Krygier et al., 2013). Therefore, HRV may serve as a potential index for measuring several features associated with mindfulness and anxiety that involve dynamic fluctuations in the body in response to a changing internal and external environment.
1.4. Hemodynamic neuroimaging
Hemodynamic neuroimaging involves blood oxygen level-dependent measurements made using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). fMRI that offers a very high spatial resolution, but a relatively low temporal resolution and provides an indirect measure of the activity of a brain region either related to a task or due to spontaneous modulation. Due to its high spatial resolution, fMRI has become a broadly used technique, including in numerous clinical studies of cognition (e.g., see the review by Glover, 2011). Some fMRI studies of emotional responses have shown that individual differences in mindfulness (Creswell, Way, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2007; Way et al., 2010) or in anxiety (Bishop, Duncan, Brett, & Lawrence, 2004; Etkin et al., 2004; Gold et al., 2016; Killgore & Yurgelun-Todd, 2005) can predict the level of activation in amygdala and prefrontal cortex (PFC). For example, Creswell et al. (2007) observed that people with higher trait mindfulness showed enhanced PFC activity and attenuated amygdala activity while performing an affect-labeling task compared to effects in a gender-labeling (control) task. High-trait anxiety individuals have shown an opposite pattern, with Etkin et al. (2004) observing that people with high-trait anxiety showed higher activity in basolateral activity in the amygdala when they processed masked fearful faces unconsciously in comparison to conscious processing of non-masked faces. In addition to the amygdala, PFC is actively involved in high-anxiety individuals, with these individuals showing reduced recruitment of PFC in response to fearful distractors compared to neutral face distractor conditions while performing an identification task (Bishop et al., 2004).
The above pattern of findings are further strengthened by converging evidence suggesting functional connectivity between PFC and amygdala that aids emotion regulation processes modulated by either mindfulness or anxiety (Creswell et al., 2007; Davidson, 2002; Gold et al., 2016; Holzel et al., 2013; Shin & Liberzon, 2010). It was demonstrated that in generalized anxiety disorder patients who showed negative coupling between amygdala and PFC functional connectivity altered into positive coupling after 8 weeks of mindfulness training, but this was not seen in a stress management education (control) group (Holzel et al., 2013). However, functional connectivity in most studies has been examined for specific tasks with very few studies investigating this network in a spontaneous resting state context which can better reflect individual differences due to traits. (Lim, Teng, Patanaik, Tandi, & Massar, 2018). For instance, Bishop (2009) observed reduced PFC activity due to high-trait anxiety while performing a conflict task in a low attentionally demanding state, while Creswell et al. (2007) found high PFC during an affect-labeling task in high mindful people, in addition to reduced bilateral amygdala activity.
Although these neuroimaging reports show evidence in line with an inverse relationship between mindfulness and anxiety, the nature of amygdala–PFC activity appears to be generally task-dependent. A recent study on task-independent resting state (Lim et al., 2018) showed that high-trait mindfulness individuals showed greater connectivity and anticorrelations between default mode network and salience network than did low mindfulness people. Lim et al. (2018) further explained about the possible roles of these two networks, with the default mode associated with mind-wandering which is decreased by an increase in the level of trait mindfulness (Mrazek, Smallwood, & Schooler, 2012). The salience network was suggested to play a role in coordinating between externally and internally directed attention supported by executive control and default mode networks, respectively (Menon & Uddin, 2010), thus facilitate switching between mind-wandering and task-readiness states (Sridharan, Levitin, & Menon, 2008). Therefore, stronger connectivity between the default mode and salience networks might indicate greater ability to regulate mind-wandering and task-readiness states in high mindfulness people. These observations (Lim et al., 2018) highlight the importance of exploring brain regions beyond the amygdala and PFC as are frequently reported in mindfulness and anxiety literature. For example, Frewen et al. (2010) observed a positive correlation between left amygdala activity and a trait mindfulness “observing” component, unlike many previous studies reporting negative correlation between trait mindfulness and amygdala (Creswell et al., 2007; Way et al., 2010). This difference in Frewen et al. (2010) study from previous studies could be due to the fact that they used a paradigm of auditory modality in which the participants had to listen to a writtenscript and further asked to imagine as if those incidents were happening at that moment. Unlike Frewen et al. (2010), the previous studies reported here had shown a negative correlation with amygdala and had used paradigms in visual modality wherein the participants would either label the affect (Creswell et al., 2007) or match the affect (Way et al., 2010) from the faces shown on a screen. Therefore, the way instructions given to participants and nature of modality of paradigms may bring about different pattern in amygdala activities. Therefore, future fMRI studies should look into using a more task-independent approach, such as investigation of resting-state conditions, that can potentially reconcile the nature of amygdala and PFC activity, as well as shed light on other networks such as the default mode network and salience networks relating to mindfulness and anxiety. This may be helpful when comparing manipulations of mindfulness and/or anxiety to investigate the brain regions associated with better emotion regulation ability.
1.5. Molecular biomarkers
Reports have suggested consequential impact of stress and anxiety on the body (Fava, Cosci, & Sonino, 2017), which are typically known as psychosomatic disorders (Walker, 1956). For example, Fink (2011) suggested that post-traumatic stress disorder is a form of anxiety disorder that leads to high arousal with concurrent activation of the autonomic nervous system, principally sympathetic adrenomedullary (SAM) pathway. It was described as stress, prompting an activation response from a neuroendocrine system that is regulated by the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and the SAM. The hyperactivity of the two systems (HPA and SAM) may trigger a cascade of abnormal responses in behavior, hormonal secretions, immune system, and genetics (biological aging) (Black & Slavich, 2016; Fink, 2011). Previous review articles have already addressed the behavioral implications of anxiety and mindfulness in the above sections. Therefore, the following section summarizes the literature principally relating to three biomarkers: neuroendocrine, immunological and genetic that further strengthen the proposed linkage between mindfulness and anxiety.
1.5.1. Neuroendocrine biomarkers
One primary neuroendocrine product of the HPA axis is cortisol, a steroid hormone which under normal condition maintains the homeostasis of the body (Viau, 2002). However, in response to stress or anxious condition, cortisol leads to increase in blood pressure, suppression of immune function in addition to several other bodily changes mediated by SAM (Hannibal & Bishop, 2014; Tsigos & Chrousos, 2002). Conversely, mindfulness has been suggested to act as a buffer that protects from pathological effects of stress (Brown et al., 2012; Rosenkranz et al., 2013, 2016). In their study, Brown et al. (2012) showed that individual differences in mindfulness level could predict cortisol responses. Participants performed Trier Social Stress Test (Kirschbaum, Pirke, & Hellhammer, 1993), a standard task in which participants need to deliver a speech for minutes in front of two critical evaluators and, consequently, induced stress response. Participants with high mindfulness showed lower cortisol reflecting lower stress responses than was seen in low mindfulness individuals (Brown et al., 2012). The role of mindfulness in stress buffering was further strengthened by the results from a short-term meditation study (Tang et al., 2007) that employed integrative body–mind training (IBMT) with mindfulness training as a critical component. The control and meditating groups initially had no baseline differences in their cortisol levels. After 5 days of IBMT training, meditators showed lower cortisol responses than a control group after stress induction as well as in the condition after additional training (Tang et al., 2007). Brown et al. (2012) explained the neural basis of the mindfulness and cortisol responses as a high level of mindfulness leading to a reduction in amygdala activity in response to stressors (Creswell et al., 2007). Since the amygdala is connected to the HPA pathway through hypothalamic projections (Sullivan et al., 2004), attenuation of amygdala activity will reduce the HPA axis secretion of cortisol. Therefore, cortisol may serve as a promising biomarker for future mindfulness studies investing stress and anxiety, being directly sensitive to variability in mindfulness and anxiety either at the trait or at the state level.
1.5.2. Immunological biomarkers
The interaction between two stress axes, HPA and SAM, interferes with normal activities of the immune system (Black & Slavich, 2016), which can be downregulated by buffering action of mindfulness. Creswell and Lindsay (2014) proposed that mindfulness may alleviate the activity of SAM either directly or indirectly via upregulation of the parasympathetic pathway. This alleviated activity of the SAM axis may lead to reduction in peripheral stress responses, including immune functions. Immunoglobin A (IgA) is a secretory antibody that is generated in response to a stressor in saliva (Deinzer, Kleineidam, Stiller-Winkler, Idel, & Bachg, 2000). Tang et al. (2007) in their study showed that a meditating group showed a higher concentration of salivary IgA than the control group, indicating a better response to stress in the meditating group. The effect of mindfulness on secretory immune responses was further complimented by mucosal immunity (Davidson et al., 2003). A meditating group and waitlist control group were vaccinated with influenza vaccine after the end of 8 weeks of mindfulness training. The mindfulness group showed higher antibody levels than did the control group, suggesting they had better immunoreactivity (Davidson et al., 2003).
There are immunoreactive responses triggered by stress that may have adverse health effects. For instance, tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-α) and interleukin-8 (IL-8), which are regarded as pro-inflammatory cytokines, are modulated by stress (Glaser et al., 1999). For example, Rosenkranz et al. (2013) showed that individuals with mindfulness training showed a larger reduction in levels of TNF-α and IL-8 than that a matched control group, indicating less of an inflammatory response after being exposed to stress in the mindfulness group. Additionally, C-reactive protein (CRP), another pro-inflammatory protein, which was reduced after mindfulness training in normal individuals (Malarkey, Jarjoura, & Klatt, 2013) and in ulcer colitis patients (Jedel et al., 2014). CD4+ T lymphocyte cell count is another important index of better immune function, and chronic stress may lead to a decline in its count. This reduction in CD4+ T lymphocyte cell count may lead to the absence of defense line against disease onset and progression often observed in cancer and HIV patients (Mellors et al., 1997; Reiche, Nunes, & Morimoto, 2004). Mindfulness intervention has been shown to either increase the count of or attenuate the decline in count of CD4+ T lymphocyte cell in breast cancer patients (Lengacher et al., 2013) as well as in HIV patients (Creswell, Myers, Cole, & Irwin, 2009). In sum, mindfulness seems to offer a comprehensive way to protect the immune system either by promoting the humoral (IgA) and mucosal (CD4+ T lymphocyte) immune responses or by decreasing the levels of pro-inflammatory molecules (IL-8, TNF-α, CRP).
1.5.3. Genetic biomarkers
The stress response of the HPA axis not only affect the immune response at the humoral and mucosal levels, but also at a genetic level which is instrumental for the generation of an array of extracellular proteins (Slavich & Irwin, 2014). For example, the production of pro-inflammatory cytokines is regulated by nuclear factor-kB (NF-kB), a DNA-binding protein, and upregulates the gene expression of these inflammatory proteins (Bharti & Aggarwal, 2002). Therefore, an increase in NF-kB level has been proposed as a direct indicator of augmented responses to stress and upregulates the expression of proteins promoting inflammation (Bierhaus et al., 2003). Conversely, mindfulness intervention has been shown to reduce the level of NFkB protein. For example, the protective effect of mindfulness was evident in two independent studies where they observed a reduced NF-kB transcription activity in older adults suffering from loneliness (Creswell et al., 2012) and in breast cancer patients (Bower et al., 2015).
Biological aging is another deleterious effect caused by chronic stress exposure leading to reduced telomerase activity and, consequently, facilitation of the cellular aging process (Epel et al., 2009). Telomerase is an enzyme that maintains the lengths of the telomeres (protective caps found at the terminals of the chromosomes) after every mitotic cell division (Blackburn, 2005). Thus, telomerase activity prevents early cell death and thus promotes longevity or delays the aging process. In their study, Needham et al. (2015) showed that women with high anxiety or panic disorder had shorter telomere length than non-anxious women, but no such effect was observed in men. Additionally, major depression patients treated with antidepressants showed shorter telomere length, which was suggested to be a possible side effect of the treatment (Needham et al., 2015). Furthermore, it was proposed by Epel et al. (2009) that mindfulness facilitated protection against the cellular aging by positive cognitive states and positive arousal by downregulating the negative affect that brings about a stress response (Epel et al., 2009). A series of randomized controlled trial studies further strengthened this idea by showing mindfulness training led to an increase in telomere lengths in immune cells (peripheral blood mononuclear cells) (see review Schutte & Malouff, 2014).
1.6. Summary of the dynamic relationships among indices of mindfulness and anxiety
Emotion regulation strategies tend to change the emotional state and, consequently, generate a cascade of psychological, behavioral, cognitive, physiological, immunological, and genetic responses. In a simplified model, we propose that a stressor or anxiogenic event (recent or past) increases amygdala activation. A hyperactive amygdala, in turn, upregulates the HPA axis pathway mediated by the SAM pathway (Fink, 2011). Increased activity of the HPA axis negatively affects a broad spectrum of human characteristics (see Figure 1). Conversely, mindfulness downregulates the activity of the amygdala and attenuates the HPA axis response to stressful situations (Creswell et al., 2007). Consequently, mindfulness provides protection from harmful effects caused stress and anxiety. Another novel and potential biomarker to estimate efficacy of mindfulness programs objectively can be human microbiota assessed with stool samples (Househam, Peterson, Mills, & Chopra, 2017). The gut microbiome is susceptible to being disturbed by stress (Santos et al., 1999). The microbiota serve as protective barriers for the gut epithelium by producing anti-inflammatory short-chain fatty acids, while stress leads to an increase in gut barrier permeability and increased inflammatory immune responses (Groschwitz et al., 2009). Therefore, an investigation of how stress management through mindfulness may be reflected in human gut microbiota will insightful in understanding the mechanism of stress physiology.
Figure 1.
(Color online) A schematic overview of the indices of mindfulness and anxiety demonstrating their inverse relationship. A stressor in an anxious situation enhances the activity of the amygdala, the center of emotional reactions that trigger the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) pathway. This hyperactive HPA axis leads to a cascade of reactions on different human characteristics mediated by the sympathetic nervous system. The immunological reactions show a decrease in salivary IgA, decrease in CD4+ T lymphocyte count/activity, increase in levels of tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-α), Interleukin-8 (IL-8), and C-reactive protein (CRP)), while endocrine reactions exhibit an increase in cortisol hormone levels. Electrophysiological effects are evident in the rise in late positive potential (LPP) amplitude in response to threatening visual stimuli, stronger delta–beta coherence, higher gamma power and a decrease in heart rate variability (HRV). In genetic effects, an increase in nuclear factor-kB (NF-kB) level and a decrease in telomerase activity are often observed. Behaviorally, anxiety impairs executive function. Conversely, mindfulness is proposed to act in the opposite direction to this with a cascade of reactions in response to a stressor mediated by the parasympathetic nervous system. Psychologically, chronic exposure to stress may lead to the more stable trait of anxiety, while the practice of mindfulness meditation may alter the mindfulness trait, suggesting a plastic nature of both traits which may be enhanced by continuous accumulation of these states. Neuroanatomically, emotion regulation is principally governed by functional interaction between amygdala and prefrontal cortex (PFC) regions, which are respectively involved in monitoring emotional reactions and higher order cognitive functions. It is proposed that mindfulness-related skills facilitate better emotion regulation strategies, such as by alleviating worry and rumination and enhancing acceptance and reappraisal abilities. This proposed schematic overview was adapted from concepts mentioned relating to the measurement indices (blue arrow: activity contributed by mindfulness, dark red arrow: activity contributed by anxiety, upward arrow: increase in activity, and downward arrow: decrease in activity).
1.7. Challenges, limitations, and future directions
There are some studies which have reported an absence of effect of mindfulness, while others showed positive effects. For instance, the results from two recent studies on trait mindfulness showed one reporting that dispositional mindfulness could successfully predict emotional reactivity (Brown et al., 2013), while other reported that it could not (Cosme & Wiens, 2015). These differences between the two studies might be due to dilution of effect sizes in psychologically healthy participants. This dilution effect may also influence the outcomes of mindfulness intervention programs, as such intervention programs may only produce large enough effects in a subgroup of a population under investigation (Flaxman and Bond., 2010). The median split analysis is one common approach that has been frequently used to determine two subgroups based on mindfulness or anxiety levels of the participants (e.g., Bishop., 2009; Brown et al., 2013). However, researchers have argued that it is inappropriate to consider that values just above or below the median, as would be the case for many individuals when a median split categorization is used, are meaningfully different from each other (MacCallum, Zhang, Preacher, & Rucker, 2002). Additionally, it has been noted as being quite challenging to delineate an effect that already exists using such a split (Aiken, West, & Reno, 1991; McClelland et al., 2015). Therefore, it has been suggested that employing a mean ± standard deviation criterion rather than using median split analysis (MacCallum et al., 2002), will enable avoidance of the dilution effect that may result from participants with intermediate levels of mindfulness or anxiety.
Another alternative to avoid the dilution effect in mindfulness intervention programs may be employing “double selection criteria”, an approach to categorize groups based on high mindfulness-low anxiety and low mindfulness-high anxiety (Jaiswal et al., 2018). This is an objective criterion based on the assumption of the inverse relationship between mindfulness anxiety and can provide two matched comparison groups. It is proposed that the low mindfulness-high anxiety group that shows poorer executive function than a high mindfulness-low anxiety group may have a stronger response to a mindfulness intervention program and subsequently have improved cognition and other health outcomes.
Despite accumulating evidence of a close relationship between mindfulness and anxiety, it is still not clear whether both or one of these is an emergent property of the brain. From an evolutionary perspective, anxiety is attributed as a conserved property in humans as part of a self-defense mechanism (Rozin & Royzman, 2001) and its effects on cognition are entirely consistent with this (Eysenck et al., 2007). While emotions are indeed regarded as emergent processes of the brain (Knyazev, 2007; Scherer, 2009), it appears anxiety may be better reflected as an emergent process than is the case for mindfulness. Therefore, it is necessary to understand how mindfulness is governed through brain processes to further proceed in developing an operational definition of mindfulness.
2. Conclusions
Considering the above self-report, behavioral, electrophysiological, fMRI, and biological markers evidence leads us to propose that mindfulness and anxiety are indeed mediated by emotion regulation strategies such as worry, reappraisal, nonacceptance, and rumination, through which they may interact in an antagonistic manner (Coffey & Hartman, 2008; Desrosiers et al., 2013; Greeson & Brantley, 2009). One recommendation arising is that future studies on the effects of mindfulness (or anxiety) on cognitive processes should look at these in conjunction with anxiety (or mindfulness), rather than independently. Since longitudinal intervention programs can be resource-intensive and come with logistic constraints, one should consider to what extent these measures are feasible to estimate. It is often recommended that the greater the amount of information that can be collected, the richer the quality of the research will be. Therefore, combining mindfulness and anxiety into investigations may facilitate the efficacy of assessment of MBI programs by providing a broad spectrum of indices which may reveal the individual differences in responsiveness to the program. Additionally, it is suggested that future investigations should also investigate the neural mechanisms associated with the resting state in a task-independent manner related to mindfulness and anxiety.
Financial Support
This work was sponsored by the Ministry of Science and Technology, Taiwan (grant numbers: 107-2420-H-008-009-;108-2639-H-008-001-ASP; 108-2321-B-075 -004 -MY2;107-2628-H-008 -002 -MY3; 106-2410H-008-038-MY3; 106-2628-H-008-002-MY4; 107-2410-H-008 -040 -MY3) and sponsored by Taiwan Ministry of Education’s "Academic Strategic Alliance: Taiwan and Oxford University" project grant (MOE Oxford – NCU-BRC collaborative project).
Conflicts of Interest
The authors have nothing to disclose.
References
- Aiken, L. S. , West, S. G. , & Reno, R. R. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Akselrod, S. , Gordon, D. , Ubel, F. A. , Shannon, D. C. , Berger, A. C. , & Cohen, R. J. (1981). Power spectrum analysis of heart rate fluctuation: A quantitative probe of beat-to-beat cardiovascular control. Science, 213, 220–222. 10.1126/science.6166045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arch, J. J. , & Craske, M. G. (2010). Laboratory stressors in clinically anxious and non-anxious individuals: The moderating role of mindfulness. Behaviour Research Therapy, 48, 495–505. 10.1016/j.brat.2010.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baer, R. A. , Smith, G. T. , & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: The Kentucky inventory of mindfulness skills. Assessment, 11, 191–206. 10.1177/1073191104268029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baer, R. A. , Smith, G. T. , Hopkins, J. , Krietemeyer, J. , & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27–45. 10.1177/1073191105283504 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bharti, A. C. , & Aggarwal, B. B. (2002). Nuclear factor-kappa B and cancer: Its role in prevention and therapy. Biochemical Pharmacology, 64, 883–888. 10.1016/S0006-2952(02)01154-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bierhaus, A. , Wolf, J. , Andrassy, M. , Rohleder, N. , Humpert, P. M. , Petrov, D. , … Nawroth, P. P. (2003). A mechanism converting psychosocial stress into mononuclear cell activation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 1920–1925. 10.1073/pnas.0438019100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop, S. , Duncan, J. , Brett, M. , & Lawrence, A. D. (2004). Prefrontal cortical function and anxiety: Controlling attention to threat-related stimuli. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 184 https://www.nature.com/articles/nn1173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop, S. J. (2009). Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 92–98. 10.1038/nn.2242 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Black, D. S. , & Slavich, G. M. (2016). Mindfulness meditation and the immune system: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1373, 13 10.1111/nyas.12998 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blackburn, E. H. (2005). Telomeres and telomerase: Their mechanisms of action and the effects of altering their functions. FEBS Letters, 579, 859–862. 10.1016/j.febslet.2004.11.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bower, J. E. , Crosswell, A. D. , Stanton, A. L. , Crespi, C. M. , Winston, D. , Arevalo, J. , … Ganz, P. A. (2015). Mindfulness meditation for younger breast cancer survivors: A randomized controlled trial. Cancer, 121, 1231–1240. 10.1002/cncr.29194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown, K. W. , Goodman, R. J. , & Inzlicht, M. (2013). Dispositional mindfulness and the attenuation of neural responses to emotional stimuli. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 93–99. 10.1093/scan/nss004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown, K. W. , & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848. 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown, K. W. , Weinstein, N. , & Creswell, J. D. (2012). Trait mindfulness modulates neuroendocrine and affective responses to social evaluative threat. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 2037–2041. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardaciotto, L. , Herbert, J. D. , Forman, E. M. , Moitra, E. , & Farrow, V. (2008). The assessment of present-moment awareness and acceptance: The Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale. Assessment, 15, 204–223. 10.1177/1073191107311467 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chadwick, P. , Hember, M. , Symes, J. , Peters, E. , Kuipers, E. , & Dagnan, D. (2008). Responding mindfully to unpleasant thoughts and images: Reliability and validity of the Southampton mindfulness questionnaire (SMQ). British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 451–455. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18573227 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers, R. , Gullone, E. , & Allen, N. B. (2009). Mindful emotion regulation: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 560–572. 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang, J. H. , Huang, C. L. , & Lin, Y. C. (2015). Mindfulness, basic psychological needs fulfillment, and well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 16, 1149–1162. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1007/s10902-014-9551-2 [Google Scholar]
- Chiesa, A. (2013). The difficulty of defining mindfulness: Current thought and critical issues. Mindfulness, 4, 255–268. 10.1007/s12671-012-0123-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Coffey, K. A. , & Hartman, M. (2008). Mechanisms of action in the inverse relationship between mindfulness and psychological distress. Complementary Health Practice Review, 13, 79–91. 10.1177/1533210108316307 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, S. , Kamarck, T. , & Mermelstein, R. (1994). Perceived stress scale. In Measuring stress: A guide for health and social scientists (pp. 10). http://www.mindgarden.com/documents/PerceivedStressScale.pdf
- Cosme, D. , & Wiens, S. (2015). Self-reported trait mindfulness and affective reactivity: A motivational approach using multiple psychophysiological measures. PLoS One, 10, e0119466 10.1371/journal.pone.0119466 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creswell, J. D. , Irwin, M. R. , Burklund, L. J. , Lieberman, M. D. , Arevalo, J. M. , Ma, J. , … Cole, S. W. (2012). Mindfulness-based stress reduction training reduces loneliness and pro-inflammatory gene expression in older adults: A small randomized controlled trial. Brain, Behavior and Immunity, 26, 1095–1101. 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.07.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creswell, J. D. , & Lindsay, E. K. (2014). How does mindfulness training affect health? A mindfulness stress buffering account. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 401–407. 10.1177/0963721414547415 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Creswell, J. D. , Myers, H. F. , Cole, S. W. , & Irwin, M. R. (2009). Mindfulness meditation training effects on CD4+ T lymphocytes in HIV-1 infected adults: A small randomized controlled trial. Brain, Behavior and Immunity, 23, 184–188. 10.1016/j.bbi.2008.07.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creswell, J. D. , Way, B. M. , Eisenberger, N. I. , & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural correlates of dispositional mindfulness during affect labeling. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 560–565. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3180f6171f [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson, R. J. (2002). Anxiety and affective style: Role of prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 68–80. 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01328-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson, R. J. , Kabat-Zinn, J. , Schumacher, J. , Rosenkranz, M. , Muller, D. , Santorelli, S. F. , … Sheridan, J. F. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 564–570. 10.1097/01.psy.0000077505.67574.e3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deinzer, R. , Kleineidam, C. , Stiller-Winkler, R. , Idel, H. , & Bachg, D. (2000). Prolonged reduction of salivary immunoglobulin A (sIgA) after a major academic exam. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 37, 219–232. 10.1016/S0167-8760(99)00112-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desrosiers, A. , Vine, V. , Klemanski, D. H. , & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2013). Mindfulness and emotion regulation in depression and anxiety: Common and distinct mechanisms of action. Depression and Anxiety, 30, 654–661. 10.1002/da.22124 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Francesco, S. A. , Simione, L. , López-Ramón, M. F. , Belardinelli, M. O. , Lupiáñez, J. , & Raffone, A. (2017). Dispositional mindfulness facets predict the efficiency of attentional networks. Mindfulness, 8, 101–109. https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12671-016-0634-5 [Google Scholar]
- Epel, E. , Daubenmier, J. , Moskowitz, J. T. , Folkman, S. , & Blackburn, E. (2009). Can meditation slow rate of cellular aging? Cognitive stress, mindfulness, and telomeres. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1172, 34 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04414.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epel, E. S. , Crosswell, A. D. , Mayer, S. E. , Prather, A. A. , Slavich, G. M. , Puterman, E. , & Mendes, W. B. (2018). More than a feeling: A unified view of stress measurement for population science. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 49, 146–169. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2018.03.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Etkin, A. , Klemenhagen, K. C. , Dudman, J. T. , Rogan, M. T. , Hen, R. , Kandel, E. R. , & Hirsch, J. (2004). Individual differences in trait anxiety predict the response of the basolateral amygdala to unconsciously processed fearful faces. Neuron, 44, 1043–1055. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck, M. W. , & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409–434. 10.1080/02699939208409696 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck, M. W. , Derakshan, N. , Santos, R. , & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faravelli, C. , Lo Sauro, C. , Lelli, L. , Pietrini, F. , Lazzeretti, L. , Godini, L. , … Ricca, V. (2012). The role of life events and HPA axis in anxiety disorders: A review. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 18, 5663 10.2174/138161212803530907 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fava, G. A. , Cosci, F. , & Sonino, N. (2017). Current psychosomatic practice. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 86, 13–30. 10.1159/000448856 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fink, G. (2011). Stress controversies: Post-traumatic stress disorder, hippocampal volume, gastroduodenal ulceration. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 23, 107–117. 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2010.02089.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgerald, P. J. , & Watson, B. O. (2018). Gamma oscillations as a biomarker for major depression: An emerging topic. Translational Psychiatry, 8, 1–7. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41398-018-0239-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flaxman, P. E. , & Bond, F. W. (2010). Worksite stress management training: Moderated effects and clinical significance. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 15, 347 10.1037/a0020522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frewen, P. A. , Dozois, D. J. , Neufeld, R. W. , Lane, R. D. , Densmore, M. , Stevens, T. K. , & Lanius, R. A. (2010). Individual differences in trait mindfulness predict dorsomedial prefrontal and amygdala response during emotional imagery: An fMRI study. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 479–484. 10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gemignani, A. , Santarcangelo, E. , Sebastiani, L. , Marchese, C. , Mammoliti, R. , Simoni, A. , & Ghelarducci, B. (2000). Changes in autonomic and EEG patterns induced by hypnotic imagination of aversive stimuli in man. Brain Research Bulletin, 53, 105–111. 10.1016/S0361-9230(00)00314-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glaser, R. , Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. , Marucha, P. T. , MacCallum, R. C. , Laskowski, B. F. , & Malarkey, W. B. (1999). Stress-related changes in proinflammatory cytokine production in wounds. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 450–456. 10.1001/archpsyc.56.5.450 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glover, G. H. (2011). Overview of functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosurgery Clinics of North America, 22, 133–139. 10.1016/j.nec.2010.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold, A. L. , Shechner, T. , Farber, M. J. , Spiro, C. N. , Leibenluft, E. , Pine, D. S. , & Britton, J. C. (2016). Amygdala-cortical connectivity: Associations with anxiety, development, and threat. Depression and Anxiety, 33, 917–926. 10.1002/da.22470 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greeson, J. , & Brantley, J. (2009). Mindfulness and anxiety disorders: Developing a wise relationship with the inner experience of fear In Didonna F. (Ed.), Clinical handbook of mindfulness (pp. 171–188). New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Groschwitz, K. R. , Ahrens, R. , Osterfeld, H. , Gurish, M. F. , Han, X. , Åbrink, M. , … & Hogan, S. P. (2009). Mast cells regulate homeostatic intestinal epithelial migration and barrier function by a chymase/Mcpt4-dependent mechanism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 22381–22386. 10.1073/pnas.0906372106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossman, P. (2010). Mindfulness for psychologists: Paying kind attention to the perceptible. Mindfulness, 1, 87–97. 10.1007/s12671-010-0012-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hannibal, K. E. , & Bishop, M. D. (2014). Chronic stress, cortisol dysfunction, and pain: A psychoneuroendocrine rationale for stress management in pain rehabilitation. Physical Therapy, 94, 1816–1825. 10.2522/ptj.20130597 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hofmann, S. G. , Sawyer, A. T. , Witt, A. A. , & Oh, D. (2010). The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 169 10.1037/a0018555 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holzel, B. K. , Hoge, E. A. , Greve, D. N. , Gard, T. , Creswell, J. D. , Brown, K. W. , … Lazar, S. W. (2013). Neural mechanisms of symptom improvements in generalized anxiety disorder following mindfulness training. Neuroimage Clinical, 2, 448–458. 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.03.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Househam, A. M. , Peterson, C. T. , Mills, P. J. , & Chopra, D. (2017). The effects of stress and meditation on the immune system, human microbiota, and epigenetics. Advances in Mind Body Medicine, 31, 10–25. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29306937 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howard, G. S. (1994). Why do people say nasty things about self-reports? Journal of Organizational Behavior, 15, 399–404. 10.1002/job.4030150505 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs, T. L. , Epel, E. S. , Lin, J. , Blackburn, E. H. , Wolkowitz, O. M. , Bridwell, D. A. , … Saron, C. D. (2011). Intensive meditation training, immune cell telomerase activity, and psychological mediators. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36, 664–681. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.09.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaiswal, S. , Tsai, S. Y. , Juan, C. H. , Liang, W. K. , & Muggleton, N. G. (2018). Better cognitive performance is associated with the combination of high trait mindfulness and low trait anxiety. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 627. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00627 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaiswal, S. , Tsai, S. Y. , Juan, C. H. , Muggleton, N. G. , & Liang, W. K. (2019). Low delta and high alpha power are associated with better conflict control and working memory in high mindfulness, low anxiety individuals. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14, 645–655. 10.1093/scan/nsz038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jedel, S. , Hoffman, A. , Merriman, P. , Swanson, B. , Voigt, R. , Rajan, K. B. , … Keshavarzian, A. (2014). A randomized controlled trial of mindfulness-based stress reduction to prevent flare-up in patients with inactive ulcerative colitis. Digestion, 89, 142–155. 10.1159/000356316 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kabat-Zinn, J. (2005). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness (15th anniversary ed.). New York: Delta Trade Paperback/Bantam Dell. [Google Scholar]
- Kabat-Zinn, J. , Lipworth, L. , & Burney, R. (1985). The clinical use of mindfulness meditation for the self-regulation of chronic pain. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 8, 163–190. 10.1007/BF00845519 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kabat-Zinn, J. , Massion, A. O. , Kristeller, J. , Peterson, L. G. , Fletcher, K. E. , Pbert, L. , … Santorelli, S. F. (1992). Effectiveness of a meditation-based stress reduction program in the treatment of anxiety disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 936–943. 10.1176/ajp.149.7.936 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane, M. J. , & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47 10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemp, A. H. , & Quintana, D. S. (2013). The relationship between mental and physical health: Insights from the study of heart rate variability. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 89, 288–296. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiken, L. G. , Garland, E. L. , Bluth, K. , Palsson, O. S. , & Gaylord, S. A. (2015). From a state to a trait: Trajectories of state mindfulness in meditation during intervention predict changes in trait mindfulness. Personality and Individual Differences, 81, 41–46. 10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killgore, W. D. , & Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2005). Social anxiety predicts amygdala activation in adolescents viewing fearful faces. Neuroreport, 16, 1671–1675. 10.1097/01.wnr.0000180143.99267.bd [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirschbaum, C. , Pirke, K. M. , & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The “Trier Social Stress Test” – a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28, 76–81. 10.1159/000119004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knyazev, G. G. (2007). Motivation, emotion, and their inhibitory control mirrored in brain oscillations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 31, 377–395. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krygier, J. R. , Heathers, J. A. , Shahrestani, S. , Abbott, M. , Gross, J. J. , & Kemp, A. H. (2013). Mindfulness meditation, well-being, and heart rate variability: A preliminary investigation into the impact of intensive Vipassana meditation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 89, 305–313. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar, S. , Feldman, G. , & Hayes, A. (2008). Changes in mindfulness and emotion regulation in an exposure-based cognitive therapy for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 734 10.1002/da.22124 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lau, M. A. , Bishop, S. R. , Segal, Z. V. , Buis, T. , Anderson, N. D. , Carlson, L. , … Devins, G. (2006). The Toronto mindfulness scale: Development and validation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 1445–1467. 10.1002/jclp.20326 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lengacher, C. A. , Kip, K. E. , Post-White, J. , Fitzgerald, S. , Newton, C. , Barta, M. , … Klein, T. W. (2013). Lymphocyte recovery after breast cancer treatment and mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) therapy. Biological Research for Nursing, 15, 37–47. 10.1177/1099800411419245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Libby, D. J. , Worhunsky, P. D. , Pilver, C. E. , & Brewer, J. A. (2012). Meditation-induced changes in high-frequency heart rate variability predict smoking outcomes. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim, J. , Teng, J. , Patanaik, A. , Tandi, J. , & Massar, S. A. A. (2018). Dynamic functional connectivity markers of objective trait mindfulness. NeuroImage, 176, 193–202. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lomas, T. , Ivtzan, I. , & Fu, C. H. Y. (2015). A systematic review of the neurophysiology of mindfulness on EEG oscillations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 401–410. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luck, S. J. , & Kappenman, E. S. (2012). The Oxford handbook of event-related potential components. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- MacCallum, R. C. , Zhang, S. , Preacher, K. J. , & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 7, 19–40. 10.1037//1082-989X.7.1.19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malarkey, W. B. , Jarjoura, D. , & Klatt, M. (2013). Workplace based mindfulness practice and inflammation: A randomized trial. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 27, 145–154. 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClelland, G. H. , Lynch, J. J. G. , Irwin, J. R. , Spiller, S. A. , & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2015). Median splits, Type II errors, and false–positive consumer psychology: Don’t fight the power. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25, 679–689. 10.1016/j.jcps.2015.05.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mellors, J. W. , Munoz, A. , Giorgi, J. V. , Margolick, J. B. , Tassoni, C. J. , Gupta, P. , … Rinaldo, C. R., Jr. (1997). Plasma viral load and CD4+ lymphocytes as prognostic markers of HIV-1 infection. Annals of Internal Medicine, 126, 946–954. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9182471 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon, V. , & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure & Function, 214, 655–667. 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyake, A. , & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14. 10.1177/0963721411429458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyake, A. , Friedman, N. P. , Emerson, M. J. , Witzki, A. H. , Howerter, A. , & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mocaiber, I. , Pereira, M. G. , Erthal, F. S. , Figueira, I. , Machado-Pinheiro, W. , Cagy, M. , … de Oliveira, L. (2009). Regulation of negative emotions in high trait anxious individuals: An ERP study. Psychology & Neuroscience, 2, 211–217. 10.3922/j.psns.2009.2.014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moore, A. , & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Conscious and Cognition, 18, 176–186. 10.1016/j.concog.2008.12.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moran, T. P. (2016). Anxiety and working memory capacity: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 831 10.1037/bul0000051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moser, J. S. , Becker, M. W. , & Moran, T. P. (2012). Enhanced attentional capture in trait anxiety. Emotion, 12, 213 10.1037/a0026156 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mrazek, M. D. , Smallwood, J. , & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Mindfulness and mind-wandering: Finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion, 12, 442 10.1037/a0026678 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Needham, B. L. , Mezuk, B. , Bareis, N. , Lin, J. , Blackburn, E. H. , & Epel, E. S. (2015). Depression, anxiety and telomere length in young adults: Evidence from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Molecular Psychiatry, 20, 520–528. 10.1038/mp.2014.89 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oathes, D. J. , Ray, W. J. , Yamasaki, A. S. , Borkovec, T. D. , Castonguay, L. G. , Newman, M. G. , & Nitschke, J. (2008). Worry, generalized anxiety disorder, and emotion: Evidence from the EEG gamma band. Biological Psychology, 79, 165–170. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.04.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oya, H. , Kawasaki, H. , Howard, M. A., 3rd , & Adolphs, R. (2002). Electrophysiological responses in the human amygdala discriminate emotion categories of complex visual stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 9502–9512. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-21-09502.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pacheco-Unguetti, A. P. , Acosta, A. , Callejas, A. , & Lupiáñez, J. (2010). Attention and anxiety. Psychological Science, 21, 298–304. 10.1177/0956797609359624 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poppelaars, E. S. , Harrewijn, A. , Westenberg, P. M. , & van der Molen, M. J. W. (2018). Frontal delta-beta cross-frequency coupling in high and low social anxiety: An index of stress regulation? Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18, 764–777. 10.3758/s13415-018-0603-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Putman, P. (2011). Resting state EEG delta-beta coherence in relation to anxiety, behavioral inhibition, and selective attentional processing of threatening stimuli. International Jouranl of Psychophysiology, 80, 63–68. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.01.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Razavi, T. (2001). Self-report measures: An overview of concerns and limitations of questionnaire use in occupational stress research Discussion Papers in Accounting and Management Science, 01-175. Southampton: University of Southampton. https://eprints.soton.ac.uk/id/eprint/35712
- Reiche, E. M. , Nunes, S. O. , & Morimoto, H. K. (2004). Stress, depression, the immune system, and cancer. The Lancet Oncology, 5, 617–625. 10.1016/S1470-2045(04)01597-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenkranz, M. A. , Davidson, R. J. , Maccoon, D. G. , Sheridan, J. F. , Kalin, N. H. , & Lutz, A. (2013). A comparison of mindfulness-based stress reduction and an active control in modulation of neurogenic inflammation. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 27, 174–184. 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.10.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenkranz, M. A. , Lutz, A. , Perlman, D. M. , Bachhuber, D. R. , Schuyler, B. S. , MacCoon, D. G. , & Davidson, R. J. (2016). Reduced stress and inflammatory responsiveness in experienced meditators compared to a matched healthy control group. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 68, 117–125. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.02.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rozin, P. , & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 296–320. 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Santos, J. , Saunders, P. R. , Hanssen, N. P. , Yang, P. C. , Yates, D. , Groot, J. A. , & Perdue, M. H. (1999). Corticotropin-releasing hormone mimics stress-induced colonic epithelial pathophysiology in the rat. American Journal of Physiology-Gastrointestinal and Liver Physiology, 277, G391–G399. 10.1152/ajpgi.1999.277.2.G391 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scherer, K. R. (2009). Emotions are emergent processes: They require a dynamic computational architecture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 3459–3474. 10.1098/rstb.2009.0141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schupp, H. T. , Cuthbert, B. N. , Bradley, M. M. , Cacioppo, J. T. , Ito, T. , & Lang, P. J. (2000). Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology, 37, 257–261. 10.1111/1469-8986.3720257 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schutte, N. S. , & Malouff, J. M. (2014). A meta-analytic review of the effects of mindfulness meditation on telomerase activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 42, 45–48. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.12.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schutter, D. J. L. G. , & Knyazev, G. G. (2012). Cross-frequency coupling of brain oscillations in studying motivation and emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 36, 46–54. 10.1007/s11031-011-9237-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sebastiani, L. , Simoni, A. , Gemignani, A. , Ghelarducci, B. , & Santarcangelo, E. L. (2003). Human hypnosis: Autonomic and electroencephalographic correlates of a guided multimodal cognitive-emotional imagery. Neuroscience Letters, 338, 41–44. 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)01358-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaffer, F. , & Ginsberg, J. P. (2017). An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Frontiers in Public Health, 5, 258 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shin, L. M. , & Liberzon, I. (2010). The neurocircuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 169–191. 10.1038/npp.2009.83 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slavich, G. M. , & Irwin, M. R. (2014). From stress to inflammation and major depressive disorder: A social signal transduction theory of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 774–815. 10.1037/a0035302 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger, C. D. , Gorsuch, R. L. , & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sridharan, D. , Levitin, D. J. , & Menon, V. (2008). A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 12569–12574. 10.1073/pnas.0800005105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan, G. M. , Apergis, J. , Bush, D. E. , Johnson, L. R. , Hou, M. , & Ledoux, J. E. (2004). Lesions in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis disrupt corticosterone and freezing responses elicited by a contextual but not by a specific cue-conditioned fear stimulus. Neuroscience, 128, 7–14. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.06.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun, S. , Hu, C. , Pan, J. , Liu, C. , & Huang, M. (2019). Trait mindfulness is associated with the self-similarity of heart rate variability. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 314. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00314 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi, T. , Murata, T. , Hamada, T. , Omori, M. , Kosaka, H. , Kikuchi, M. , … Wada, Y. (2005). Changes in EEG and autonomic nervous activity during meditation and their association with personality traits. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 55, 199–207. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanay, G. , & Bernstein, A. (2013). State Mindfulness Scale (SMS): Development and initial validation. Psychological Assessment, 25, 1286 10.1037/a0034044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang, Y. Y. , Holzel, B. K. , & Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nature Review Neuroscience, 16, 213 https://www.nature.com/articles/nrn3916 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang, Y. Y. , Lu, Q. , Geng, X. , Stein, E. A. , Yang, Y. , & Posner, M. I. (2010). Short-term meditation induces white matter changes in the anterior cingulate. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 15649–15652. 10.1073/pnas.1011043107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang, Y. Y. , Ma, Y. , Wang, J. , Fan, Y. , Feng, S. , Lu, Q. , … Posner, M. I. (2007). Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 17152–17156. 10.1073/pnas.0707678104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thayer, J. F. , & Lane, R. D. (2007). The role of vagal function in the risk for cardiovascular disease and mortality. Biological Psychology, 74, 224–242. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thayer, J. F. , & Sternberg, E. (2006). Beyond heart rate variability: Vagal regulation of allostatic systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1088, 361–372. 10.1196/annals.1366.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsigos, C. , & Chrousos, G. P. (2002). Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, neuroendocrine factors and stress. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, 865–871. 10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00429-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viau, V. (2002). Functional cross-talk between the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal and -adrenal axes. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 14, 506–513. 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2002.00798.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walach, H. , Buchheld, N. , Buttenmüller, V. , Kleinknecht, N. , & Schmidt, S. (2006). Measuring mindfulness – The Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI). Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1543–1555. 10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Walker, N. (1956). The definition of psychosomatic disorder. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 6, 265–299. 10.1093/bjps/VI.24.265 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh, J. J. , Balint, M. G. , Smolira, D. R. , Fredericksen, L. K. , & Madsen, S. (2009). Predicting individual differences in mindfulness: The role of trait anxiety, attachment anxiety and attentional control. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 94–99. 10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Way, B. M. , Creswell, J. D. , Eisenberger, N. I. , & Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Dispositional mindfulness and depressive symptomatology: Correlations with limbic and self-referential neural activity during rest. Emotion, 10, 12 https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0018312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]