Skip to main content
GeoHealth logoLink to GeoHealth
. 2020 May 16;4(5):e2019GH000237. doi: 10.1029/2019GH000237

Fossil Fuel Combustion Is Driving Indoor CO2 Toward Levels Harmful to Human Cognition

Kristopher B Karnauskas 1,2,3,, Shelly L Miller 4, Anna C Schapiro 5
PMCID: PMC7229519  PMID: 32426622

Abstract

Human activities are elevating atmospheric carbon dioxide concentrations to levels unprecedented in human history. The majority of anticipated impacts of anthropogenic CO2 emissions are mediated by climate warming. Recent experimental studies in the fields of indoor air quality and cognitive psychology and neuroscience, however, have revealed significant direct effects of indoor CO2 levels on cognitive function. Here, we shed light on this connection and estimate the impact of continued fossil fuel emissions on human cognition. We conclude that indoor CO2 levels may indeed reach levels harmful to cognition by the end of this century, and the best way to prevent this hidden consequence of climate change is to reduce fossil fuel emissions. Finally, we offer recommendations for a broad, interdisciplinary approach to improving such understanding and prediction.

Keywords: carbon dioxide, human cognition, climate impacts, carbon emissions, health, climate change

Key Points

  • Atmospheric carbon dioxide concentrations are reaching levels never experienced by Homo sapiens

  • Recent experiments have linked high indoor carbon dioxide concentrations to reduced cognitive function

  • Our models predict that future carbon emissions will increase indoor concentrations to levels harmful to human cognition


The vast majority of the impacts of anthropogenic climate change on human health and society are indirect to the carbon emissions, for example, increased mortality due to more frequent heat waves is an expected outcome, but humans are not adding significant heat directly to the atmosphere. Modern human activity emits greenhouse gases, which raise the near‐surface air temperature via the greenhouse effect and make heat waves more probable (Gasparrini et al., 2017; Stott et al., 2004). Consider the loss of habitat due to sea level rise. The absorption and omnidirectional reradiation of longwave radiation associated with increasing concentrations of greenhouse gases such as carbon dioxide (CO2) in the atmosphere cause seawater to warm and thus expand, and glaciers and ice sheets to melt into the sea, both of which increase the volume of the ocean such that coastlines retreat and low‐lying islands are at least partially submerged (Church et al., 2013). Indeed, most of the perceptible impacts of climate change are linked indirectly to the underlying emissions, with atmospheric warming playing a prominent role along the chain of causality.

But not all impacts are brokered by warming. In fact, oceanographers have long stressed that even if technologies are developed and deployed that prevent Earth's surface temperature from increasing despite rapidly growing CO2 emissions (e.g., geoengineering proposals such as solar radiation management), CO2 itself has a direct and extremely dangerous impact on marine ecosystems. Carbon dioxide entering the surface ocean undergoes a chemical reaction to raise the acidity (lower the pH) of seawater and ultimately prevent corals and other photosynthetic organisms—the base of the marine food web—from efficiently building their skeleta (National Research Council, 2010). Here, we argue that the human species has an analogous danger lurking in the shadows of global warming—a significant risk to our well‐being and survival caused directly by the CO2 itself.

Occasional revision to the date of speciation notwithstanding, the full existence of the biological species Homo sapiens (or so‐called anatomically modern humans) is well covered by the record of atmospheric CO2 concentration derived from air bubbles in Antarctic ice cores (Figure 1). Prior to the Industrial Revolution and going back about 800,000 years, atmospheric CO2 concentration bounced between about 200–300 parts per million (ppm), reaching a lowest value of 172 ppm seven ice ages ago and a peak of 300 ppm three interglacials ago (Lüthi et al., 2008). The nearly 50% increase in CO2 concentration between 1813 (280 ppm) and 2019 (411 ppm) is easily attributed to human emissions, in particular fossil fuel burning (Rubino et al., 2013) with smaller yet significant contributions from land use change and cement manufacture. Superimposed on the relatively smooth exponential trend in global atmospheric CO2 over the past several decades is a pronounced annual cycle. The predominance of land in the Northern Hemisphere means that CO2 is withdrawn from the atmosphere as a whole throughout boreal summer (while the majority of Earth's terrestrial plants are photosynthesizing) and accumulates in the atmosphere throughout boreal winter (while the Northern Hemisphere plants are dormant or decomposing). The entire annual cycle of atmospheric CO2 concentration has an amplitude of approximately 6 ppm between May and September. The increase in atmospheric CO2 concentration over the past century due to fossil fuel emissions, by both amount and pace, is undeniably significant when compared to all of the known natural rhythms of the planet, including those that modern humans have been exposed to.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Carbon dioxide past, present, and future. Atmospheric concentration (ppm) of CO2 derived from Antarctic ice cores (Lüthi et al., 2008), measured directly at Mauna Loa Observatory, and future concentrations associated with Representative Concentration Pathway (RCP) 4.5 and 8.5 (van Vuuren et al., 2011).

What are the direct effects on humans of elevated ambient CO2 concentrations? Almost entirely decoupled from the climate research enterprise is a growing literature on the effects of CO2 exposure on cognitive function. Practical interest in the matter is as old as the infamous “Keeling Curve” of CO2 from Mauna Loa itself, but for rather different reasons. Early experimental studies testing for the influence of relatively high concentrations of CO2 (5–8%) that might be present in confined and enclosed spaces like submarines found significant impacts on ability to respond to a stimulus (Harter, 1967), reasoning (Sayers et al., 1987), and threat processing (Garner et al., 2011). More moderately elevated concentrations (2.5%), such as those that may be present in passenger automobiles and aircraft, have been shown to impair visual perception (Yang et al., 1997) and ability to maneuver an aircraft (Allen et al., 2018).

The last decade saw the CO2‐cognition literature turn an eye toward densely populated indoor spaces with varying levels of ventilation, such as schools and office buildings. Studies focusing on school environments have found impacts of CO2 on standardized test scores (Haverinen‐Shaughnessy & Shaughnessy, 2015; Wargocki et al., 2020) and attendance (Shendell et al., 2004), and significant deterioration of attention, vigilance, memory, and concentration when CO2 levels are elevated (Bakó‐Biró et al., 2012). In simulating office‐like environments under different environmental conditions, several studies have found significant reductions of cognitive performance even under commonly observed indoor CO2 levels relative to typical ambient outdoor levels (Allen et al., 2016; Hong et al., 2018; Satish et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2015).

One recent study was especially useful for understanding the effects of CO2 in work and school settings as it exposed participants to controlled levels of CO2 over a time period corresponding roughly to a day of work or school (6 hr) and used a powerful within‐subjects design to assess how increasing CO2 concentrations affected cognition in each individual (Allen et al., 2016). The study evaluated a range of high‐level cognitive domains, including decision making and planning. Three exposure conditions were applied: CO2 concentrations of 550, 945, and 1,400 ppm. For modern context, 550 ppm is only ~34% higher than the average global atmospheric (outdoor) CO2 concentration in 2019 (411 ppm), 945 ppm is consistent with American Society of Heating, Refrigerating and Air‐Conditioning Engineers (2016) ventilation guidelines for acceptable indoor air quality, and 1,400 ppm is consistent with an average concentration measured in U.S. public and commercial office buildings in the mid‐1990s according to the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency but is much lower than concentrations that have been measured in poorly ventilated school buildings (Bakó‐Biró et al., 2012; Wargocki et al., 2020). Systematic relationships were found between most of the cognitive function scores and CO2 concentration, including from 550–945 ppm and from 945–1,400 ppm. Across the full domain of CO2 concentrations, the apparent statistical relationships were linear for some declines in cognitive function scores with increasing CO2 concentration (e.g., overall ability to make decisions), and nonlinear for others, wherein the decline in cognitive score is more pronounced between 945 and 1,400 ppm (e.g., complex strategizing). Not only were such reductions in cognitive function score statistically significant, they were typically rather large—on the order of tens of percent decrease in performance per ~400‐ppm CO2 increase (equivalent to a doubling of present‐day outdoor CO2 concentration). Many areas of cognition have not been found to be so severely affected, or not affected at all, by increased CO2 (Jacobson et al., 2019; Stankovic et al., 2016). It may be that higher‐level, more cognitively demanding tasks are more likely to be sensitive to the effects of moderate levels of ambient CO2. However, null or nonmonotonic effects have been found even in these demanding tasks for two special populations—submariners (Rodeheffer et al., 2018) and astronaut‐like subjects (Scully et al., 2019). These results suggest that factors like increased experience with demanding cognitive tasks or physiological adaptation to increased ambient CO2 could potentially mitigate the harmful effects of CO2 on cognition. Much more work will be needed to determine which cognitive processes are susceptible to the effects of increased CO2 and under what conditions.

Studies like Satish et al. (2012) and Allen et al. (2016) are part of a growing body of scientific evidence pointing to CO2 as a pollutant—not just a proxy for ventilation rate—with direct detrimental impacts on the cognitive function of humans in schools and offices. How might CO2 lead to these cognitive deficits? High levels of CO2 in the air result in reduced gas transfer and increased CO2 in the alveoli of the lungs, which diffuses into the blood, crossing the blood‐brain barrier (Azuma et al., 2018; Shriram et al., 2019). Increased CO2 in the blood (hypercapnia) within the brain is associated with reduced oxygen (hypoxemia), which is critical for brain function, and brain activity indicating decreased arousal and excitability (Woodbury et al., 1958; Xu et al., 2011). CO2 is known to increase sleepiness (Snow et al., 2019; Vehviläinen et al., 2016) and anxiety (Bailey et al., 2005), both of which in turn harm cognitive function (Dinges & Kribbs, 1991; Vytal et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2015). A study in juvenile rodents found that increased CO2 in the air reduced levels of a neuroprotective growth factor, severely harming brain development, increasing anxiety, and impairing learning and memory (Kiray et al., 2014). Robertson (2001) argued that even modestly elevated concentrations of atmospheric CO2 (720 ppm) are sufficient to induce acidosis (lowered blood pH) in humans, leading to symptoms like restlessness, confusion, and sleepiness. Based on the current literature, the proximal causes of impaired high‐level cognition at the relevant concentrations of CO2 thus seem likely to be increased sleepiness and perhaps increased anxiety. Though these studies provide some insight into the effects of CO2 on the brain, much work is still needed to understand the full mechanistic chain from increased CO2 in the air to specific impaired cognitive processes.

A possible explanation for the apparent decoupling of the scientific literature concerning CO2 impacts on human cognitive function and that of anthropogenic climate change is that the vast majority of the former research focuses on indoor air quality and health (see reviews by Azuma et al., 2018 and Jacobson et al., 2019). Note that CO2 concentrations in buildings are a result of the combination of CO2 infiltrating from outdoors inside, or brought in with the ventilation system outside air, and the CO2 generated by the building occupants. Typical indoor concentrations are similar to outdoor levels if the occupancy is sparse and could be much higher if the building has high occupancy and poor outdoor air supply. However, several studies have also measured a so‐called urban CO2 dome that forms over cities due to proximity to emission sources, primarily fossil fuel burning (Day et al., 2002; George et al., 2007; Idso et al., 2001; Jacobson, 2010; McRae & Graedel, 1979; Mitchell et al., 2018; Moore & Jacobson, 2015). For example, the 5‐year study over a rural‐urban transect through Baltimore, MD, by George et al. (2007) revealed a robust urban CO2 dome with an average urban enhancement of 66 ppm; this is in the middle of the range of enhancements reported by the studies cited above. How does the scale of the modern‐day rise in global atmospheric CO2 concentration compare to the experimental conditions in the aforementioned cognitive studies? It is unclear whether the rise from ~280 to 411 ppm since 1813 due to anthropogenic emissions would have caused a detectable decline in human cognitive function, since most studies used today's ambient outdoor air as the control case (i.e., “ventilated” or “low‐CO2” condition). But society's uncertain energy future provides a compelling set of grand experiments—some version of which will definitely be conducted.

A set of four representative concentration pathways (RCPs) were conceived under the auspices of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC), primarily to be used as prescribed inputs to comprehensive, fully coupled climate and Earth system models (i.e., simulating the global atmosphere, ocean, etc.) to answer questions about how much will the world will warm, what the impacts will be, and how their severity depends on future CO2 emissions. In IPCC parlance, the acronym RCP is followed by a number that refers to the amount of additional energy (or “radiative forcing”) in the Earth system by the year 2100 (e.g., 4.5 W/m2 with RCP4.5), but these are also associated with future trajectories of global carbon dioxide (and other greenhouse gas) emissions and resultant concentrations. The endpoint CO2 concentrations in 2100 for RCP2.6, RCP4.5, RCP6, and RCP8.5 are 420, 540, 625, and 930 ppm, respectively (van Vuuren et al., 2011). While it may be too early to tell which RCP will become closest to reality, RCP8.5 is considered to be the unmitigated emissions scenario, and global emission estimates to date do not point to a detectible divergence from that pathway (Le Quéré et al., 2018). Interestingly, the middle CO2 condition used in the Allen et al. (2016) study of indoor CO2 effects on cognitive function, which was aimed at industry guidelines for indoor air quality, is almost exactly the predicted outdoor concentration in 2100 under RCP8.5. Did Allen et al. (2016) accidentally generate a prediction of the impact of unmitigated CO2 emissions on outdoor human cognitive function at the end of this century?

Predicting future societal behavior and quantifying the impact of air chemistry on the brain are obviously complex and uncertain endeavors. The third and equally complex link is that between outdoor and indoor air, which is a concern of building and air quality engineers. This relationship can be modeled using a mass balance model—a differential equation of the form

VdC/dt=QCoutC+G, (1)

where V is the volume of the indoor space, C is the concentration of CO2 in the indoor space, Q is the outdoor air ventilation rate, C out is the outdoor CO2 concentration, and G is the rate of generation of CO2 occurring in the indoor space—respiration by human occupants of the indoor space (Miller, 2018;Persily, 2018; Persily & de Jonge, 2017). This model assumes that the indoor space is well mixed, a reasonable assumption under many circumstances. The steady‐state solution to (1) is obtained by setting the time derivative to zero, and rearranging for C yields

C=G/Q+Cout. (2)

Therefore, in steady state, indoor CO2 concentration is always at least as high as the outdoor concentration (as neither generation nor ventilation rate can be negative) and simply scales with the ratio of generation to ventilation. For reasonable values of G and Q for elementary school students (0.004 L/s per student) and classrooms (10 L/s per student), respectively, a ratio G/Q equates to 400 ppm (Persily, 2018; Persily & de Jonge, 2017). Under such assumptions, then, an outdoor CO2 concentration of 477 ppm (411 ppm as in 2019, plus a 66‐ppm urban enhancement) would equate to 877 ppm inside the classroom upon reaching equilibrium. Note that in many indoor environments, the generation rate can exceed the design ventilation rate, since many spaces can become overcrowded and not ventilated appropriately, leading to even higher indoor CO2 concentrations (Miller et al., 2009).

With predictions of future global (outdoor) CO2 concentrations informed by IPCC‐related efforts, observed estimates of urban enhancement, an idealized yet physically based model of the indoor‐outdoor concentration relationship, and estimates of various CO2‐cognition relationships derived from recent quantitative experiments on humans, we can roughly estimate the impact of future fossil fuel emissions on human cognitive function, including how it unfolds throughout the century and how it depends on mitigation strategies (Figure 2). Here, we offer a straightforward demonstration, applied to elementary school classrooms in a city similar to Baltimore, MD, achieved by solving (2), assigning reasonable parameters of generation rate G and ventilation rate Q, prescribing transient predictions of outdoor CO2 concentration C out associated with RCPs, and fitting simple functions to robust human subject research results (Allen et al., 2016). We chose the “Basic Activity Level” and “Strategy” (Basic and Complex, respectively, in Figure 3) measures from the Allen et al. (2016) study to include in the model, as they exhibit highly consistent trends across subjects and because they provide examples of how linear (in the case of Basic Activity) and nonlinear (in the case of Strategy) effects may play out. These measures are part of the Strategic Management Simulation battery, which is a commercial product developed to test the decision‐making effectiveness of employees when presented with various real‐world scenarios. Lack of free access to this battery and lack of established correlations with standard cognitive measures are weaknesses of using these measures, but they nevertheless provide clear cases of impacted cognition in the context of a well‐controlled experimental manipulation of CO2 levels. The Basic measure corresponds to the number of actions taken in responding to scenarios; it is simply a measure of task engagement. The Complex measure tracks ability to strategize—to take actions in a scenario that provide the foundation for future useful actions. We believe these measures provide useful examples for proof‐of‐concept simulations.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Flowchart describing the information and modeling required for estimating the impacts of future CO2 emissions on human cognition. Blue elements represent quantities that result from calculations, and red elements represent intermediate steps such as observation and modeling. Smaller text within red elements gives some specifics about the simulations conducted in this study.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Modeling the effect of anthropogenic CO2 emissions on cognitive function. Future global outdoor CO2 concentrations (ppm) associated with RCP4.5 and RCP8.5 (thick lines), equivalent urban outdoor CO2 concentration (thin lines), along with steady‐state indoor CO2 concentrations (dashed lines) assuming reasonable values of generation and ventilation rates (a). Empirical models of normalized cognitive function scores (where normalized refers to the normalization of observations in Allen et al., 2016, such that scores are adjusted to 1.0 at ~500 ppm and bounded [0, 1]) for basic engagement and ability to make decisions in a task (dashed line) and complex strategy (solid line) as a function of indoor CO2 concentration, derived from the Basic Activity Level and Strategy measures in Allen et al. (2016) (b). The dots in panel (b) denote the anchor points extracted from Allen et al. (2016) used for fitting the two models (open for Basic Activity Level and filled for Strategy). Projected normalized cognitive function scores for basic cognitive measure (dashed lines) and complex strategy (solid lines) assuming RCP4.5 (blue lines) and RCP8.5 (red lines) (c). In panel (c), all curves are adjusted to begin at 1.0 in year 2019, thus facilitating a comparison of the changes over time relative to present.

The full end‐to‐end model thus predicts indoor cognitive performance (for the particular studied cognitive processes) as a function of outdoor CO2 concentration. Under these assumptions, the model predictions are quite arresting (Figure 3). On the unmitigated CO2 emission pathway (RCP8.5), we may be in for a ~25% reduction in our indoor basic decision‐making ability and a ~50% reduction in more complex strategic thinking, by the year 2100 relative to today. These results are almost entirely avoidable by reducing global CO2 emissions according to RCP4.5, which is tantamount to achieving the broad goals set forth under the Paris Agreement of the United Nations Framework Convention on Climate Change (UNFCCC).

While the above model calculations are forward‐looking based on projections of CO2 concentration, it is interesting to consider the impact that CO2 emissions to date may have had on human cognition. Interpreting the increase in global atmospheric CO2 since the Industrial Revolution (280–411 ppm from 1813–2019) using the models shown in Figures 3a and 3b, we estimate that our basic engagement and decision‐making ability should have been reduced by about 8% (or 13%, if the urban CO2 dome effect is both entirely anthropogenic and developed over the same time period). It is important to note that this is an estimate of the influence specifically of the rise in CO2 concentration, all other factors being equal. In reality, IQ scores have been increasing since at least the beginning of the twentieth century due to changes in other factors such as education, technology, and nutrition (Pietschnig & Voracek, 2015).

Of note is that the U.S. building sector is a large contributor to CO2 emissions. In 2015, CO2 emissions from fossil fuel combustion in buildings generated 8.6% of total U.S. greenhouse gas emissions; buildings were the fourth highest emitting sector after electric power, transportation, and industry (Center for Climate and Energy Solutions, 2017). Factoring in the indirect emissions from the use of electricity generated off‐site residential and commercial buildings account for 29% of total U.S. emissions (U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, 2017). Within the building sector itself, space heating, ventilating, and cooling account for 30–38% of the CO2 emissions (U.S. Energy Information Administration, 2018). An inventory of greenhouse gas emissions from 2015 found that New York City buildings accounted for 67% of the city's emissions (The City of New York, 2017). It is unfortunately cyclic that much of the CO2 emissions come from the use of energy in buildings and yet the developed world spends 90% of our time in these essential buildings that protect us from the elements, where we are constantly exposed to air pollutant emissions from sources such as cooking, household products, building materials, occupant activities, outdoor air pollution brought indoors by ventilation, and the CO2 that we generate indoors as part of our metabolic processes.

Fossil fuel emissions will continue to have unforeseen consequences for Earth and its inhabitants. As we move closer and closer to experiencing the full scale of climate change, we must consider all impacts including those where CO2 exerts its effects directly and without regard to Earth's climate sensitivity, as with human cognition. Although the above model is relatively straightforward and makes some simplifying assumptions, and the calculations should be considered back‐of‐the‐envelope, they illuminate the principle dynamics and uncertainties involved in understanding and predicting the impact of fossil fuel combustion on human cognition (i.e., all of the red elements in the flowchart, Figure 2). Broad, interdisciplinary teams representing economics and energy policy continue to refine our projections of CO2 emissions through integrated assessment models. Long‐term regional observations and modeling are needed to further constrain the urban and suburban enhancement of global, background CO2 concentrations. Building, air quality, and energy engineering are key to understand the exchange of air between the outdoors and the built environment and to reduce energy consumption in buildings due to heating and cooling; moreover, physiology determines the rate of CO2 generation by its occupants. Finally, there is a clear need for additional experimental studies quantifying the human cognitive response across a broad spectrum of cognitive domains, especially to CO2 concentrations between 500 and 2,000 ppm, and over longer periods of time, to assess potential for physiological adaptation. Just like ocean acidification, reduced cognitive function is one of the “hidden” climate change impacts where warming needn't play middleman, and it will manifest especially in classrooms, offices, hospitals, the transportation sector, and many other populated indoor spaces. Though improved ventilation could be an adaptation measure to mitigate these consequences in some situations, ventilation is not helpful when outdoor air is highly polluted (due to climate change or other factors), and ventilation already comes at the cost of a substantial fraction of building energy consumption. The best way to prevent indoor CO2 levels from reaching levels harmful to cognition is through reduced fossil fuel emissions.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest relevant to this study.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the editor and two anonymous reviewers for their insightful suggestions.

Karnauskas, K. B. , Miller, S. L. , & Schapiro, A. C. (2020). Fossil fuel combustion is driving indoor CO2 toward levels harmful to human cognition. GeoHealth, 4, e2019GH000237 10.1029/2019GH000237

Data Availability Statement

All data are publicly available and/or drawn from primary sources cited in the main text. Historical measurements of atmospheric carbon dioxide concentration (used in Figure 1) are available at the websites (http://ncdc.noaa.gov/paleo/study/17975 and https://www.esrl.noaa.gov/gmd/ccgg/trends/data.html). Future estimates of atmospheric carbon dioxide concentrations associated with RCP4.5 and RCP8.5 (also used in Figure 1) are available at the website (http://www.iiasa.ac.at/web-apps/tnt/RcpDb/).

References

  1. Allen, J. G. , MacNaughton, P. , Satish, U. , Santanam, S. , Vallarino, J. , & Spengler, J. D. (2016). Associations of cognitive function scores with carbon dioxide, ventilation, and volatile organic compound exposures in office workers: A controlled exposure study of green and conventional office environments. Environmental Health Perspectives, 124(6), 805–812. 10.1289/ehp.1510037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen, J. G. , MacNaughton, P. , Cedeno‐Laurent, J. G. , Cao, X. , Flanigan, S. , Vallarino, J. , Rueda, F. , Donnelly‐McLay, D. , & Spengler, J. D. (2018). Airplane pilot flight performance on 21 maneuvers in a flight simulator under varying carbon dioxide concentrations. Journal of Exposure Science & Environmental Epidemiology, 29, 457–468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Society of Heating, Refrigerating and Air‐Conditioning Engineers (2016). Standard 62.2. Ventilation and acceptable indoor air quality in low‐rise residential buildings.
  4. Azuma, K. , Kagi, N. , Yanagi, U. , & Osawa, H. (2018). Effects of low‐level inhalation exposure to carbon dioxide in indoor environments: A short review on human health and psychomotor performance. Environment International, 121(Pt 1), 51–56. 10.1016/j.envint.2018.08.059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bailey, J. E. , Argyropoulos, S. V. , Kendrick, A. H. , & Nutt, D. J. (2005). Behavioral and cardiovascular effects of 7.5% CO2 in human volunteers. Depression and Anxiety, 21(1), 18–25. 10.1002/da.20048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bakó‐Biró, Z. , Clements‐Croome, D. J. , Kochhar, N. , Awbi, H. B. , & Williams, M. J. (2012). Ventilation rates in schools and pupils' performance. Building and Environment, 48, 215–223. [Google Scholar]
  7. Center for Climate and Energy Solutions 2017. Calculations based on U.S. Energy Information Administration, annual energy outlook: Appendix A: Reference cases 2007–2017; Tables A4, A5 (Washington, DC: U.S. Energy Information Administration, 2007–2017). https://www.c2es.org/site/assets/uploads/2018/06/innovation-buildings-background-brief-07-18.pdf. Accessed April 2019. [Google Scholar]
  8. Church, J. A. , Clark, P. U. , Cazenave, A. , Gregory, J. M. , Jevrejeva, S. , Levermann, A. , Merrifield, M. A. , Milne, G. A. , Nerem, R. S. , Nunn, P. D. , Payne, A. J. , Pfeffer, W. T. , Stammer, D. , & Unnikrishnan, A. S. (2013). Sea level change In Stocker T. F., et al. (Eds.), Climate change 2013: The physical science basis (pp. 1137–1216). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  9. The City of New York (2017). Inventory of New York City's greenhouse gas emissions in 2015, Cventure LLC, Cathy Pasion, Christianah Oyenuga, and Kate Gouin, Mayor's Office of Sustainability, New York. http://www.dec.ny.gov/docs/administration_pdf/nycghg.pdf
  10. Day, T. A. , Gober, P. , Xiong, F. S. , & Wentz, E. A. (2002). Temporal patterns in near‐surface CO2 concentrations over contrasting vegetation types in the Phoenix metropolitan area. Agricultural and Forest Meteorology, 110, 229–245. [Google Scholar]
  11. Dinges, D. F. , & Kribbs, N. B. (1991). Performing while sleepy: Effects of experimentally‐induced sleepiness In Monk T. H. (Ed.), Human performance and cognition. Sleep, sleepiness and performance (pp. 97–128). Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
  12. Garner, M. , Attwood, A. , Baldwin, D. S. , James, A. , & Munafò, M. R. (2011). Inhalation of 7.5% carbon dioxide increases threat processing in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(8), 1557–1562. 10.1038/npp.2011.15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gasparrini, A. , Guo, Y. , Sera, F. , Vicedo‐Cabrera, A. M. , Huber, V. , Tong, S. , de Sousa Zanotti Stagliorio Coelho, M. , Nascimento Saldiva, P. H. , Lavigne, E. , Matus Correa, P. , Valdes Ortega, N. , Kan, H. , Osorio, S. , Kyselý, J. , Urban, A. , Jaakkola, J. J. K. , Ryti, N. R. I. , Pascal, M. , Goodman, P. G. , Zeka, A. , Michelozzi, P. , Scortichini, M. , Hashizume, M. , Honda, Y. , Hurtado‐Diaz, M. , Cesar Cruz, J. , Seposo, X. , Kim, H. , Tobias, A. , Iñiguez, C. , Forsberg, B. , Åström, D. O. , Ragettli, M. S. , Guo, Y. L. , Wu, C. F. , Zanobetti, A. , Schwartz, J. , Bell, M. L. , Dang, T. N. , Van, D. D. , Heaviside, C. , Vardoulakis, S. , Hajat, S. , Haines, A. , & Armstrong, B. (2017). Projections of temperature‐related excess mortality under climate change scenarios. The Lancet Planetary Health, 1(9), e360–e367. 10.1016/S2542-5196(17)30156-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. George, K. , Ziska, L. H. , Bunce, J. A. , & Quebedeaux, B. (2007). Elevated atmospheric CO2 concentration and temperature across an urban–rural transect. Atmospheric Environment, 41, 7654–7665. [Google Scholar]
  15. Harter, M. (1967). Effects of carbon dioxide on the alpha frequency and reaction time in humans. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 23(6), 561–563. 10.1016/0013-4694(67)90023-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Haverinen‐Shaughnessy, U. , & Shaughnessy, R. J. (2015). Effects of classroom ventilation rate and temperature on students' test scores. PLoS ONE, 10, e0136165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hong, T. , Kim, J. , & Lee, M. (2018). Integrated task performance score for the building occupants based on the CO2 concentration and indoor climate factors changes. Applied Energy, 228, 1707–1713. [Google Scholar]
  18. Idso, C. D. , Idso, S. B. , & Balling, R. C. (2001). An intensive two‐week study of an urban CO2 dome in Phoenix, Arizona, USA. Atmospheric Environment, 35, 995–1000. [Google Scholar]
  19. Jacobson, M. Z. (2010). Enhancement of local air pollution by urban CO2 domes. Environmental Science & Technology, 44(7), 2497–2502. 10.1021/es903018m [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Jacobson, T. A. , Kler, J. S. , Hernke, M. T. , Braun, R. K. , Meyer, K. C. , & Funk, W. E. (2019). Direct human health risks of increased atmospheric carbon dioxide. Nature Sustainability, 2(8), 691–701. 10.1038/s41893-019-0323-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kiray, M. , Sisman, A. R. , Camsari, U. M. , Evren, M. , Dayi, A. , Baykara, B. , Aksu, I. , Ates, M. , & Uysal, N. (2014). Effects of carbon dioxide exposure on early brain development in rats. Biotechnic & Histochemistry, 89(5), 371–383. 10.3109/10520295.2013.872298 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Le Quéré, C. , Andrew, R. M. , Friedlingstein, P. , Sitch, S. , Hauck, J. , Pongratz, J. , Pickers, P. A. , Korsbakken, J. I. , Peters, G. P. , Canadell, J. G. , Arneth, A. , Arora, V. K. , Barbero, L. , Bastos, A. , Bopp, L. , Chevallier, F. , Chini, L. P. , Ciais, P. , Doney, S. C. , Gkritzalis, T. , Goll, D. S. , Harris, I. , Haverd, V. , Hoffman, F. M. , Hoppema, M. , Houghton, R. A. , Hurtt, G. , Ilyina, T. , Jain, A. K. , Johannessen, T. , Jones, C. D. , Kato, E. , Keeling, R. F. , Goldewijk, K. K. , Landschützer, P. , Lefèvre, N. , Lienert, S. , Liu, Z. , Lombardozzi, D. , Metzl, N. , Munro, D. R. , Nabel, J. E. M. S. , Nakaoka, S. , Neill, C. , Olsen, A. , Ono, T. , Patra, P. , Peregon, A. , Peters, W. , Peylin, P. , Pfeil, B. , Pierrot, D. , Poulter, B. , Rehder, G. , Resplandy, L. , Robertson, E. , Rocher, M. , Rödenbeck, C. , Schuster, U. , Schwinger, J. , Séférian, R. , Skjelvan, I. , Steinhoff, T. , Sutton, A. , Tans, P. P. , Tian, H. , Tilbrook, B. , Tubiello, F. N. , van der Laan‐Luijkx, I. T. , van der Werf, G. R. , Viovy, N. , Walker, A. P. , Wiltshire, A. J. , Wright, R. , Zaehle, S. , & Zheng, B. (2018). Global carbon budget 2018. Earth System Science Data, 10(4), 2141–2194. 10.5194/essd-10-2141-2018 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lüthi, D. , le Floch, M. , Bereiter, B. , Blunier, T. , Barnola, J. M. , Siegenthaler, U. , Raynaud, D. , Jouzel, J. , Fischer, H. , Kawamura, K. , & Stocker, T. F. (2008). High‐resolution carbon dioxide concentration record 650,000–800,000 years before present. Nature, 453(7193), 379–382. 10.1038/nature06949 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. McRae, J. E. , & Graedel, T. E. (1979). Carbon dioxide in the urban atmosphere: Dependencies and trends. Journal of Geophysical Research, 84, 5011. [Google Scholar]
  25. Miller, S. L. (2018). In Kutz M. (Ed.), “Indoor air pollution.” Handbook of environmental engineering (pp. 519–563). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  26. Miller, S. L. , Scaramella, P. , Campe, J. , Goss, C. W. , Diaz‐Castillo, S. , Hendrikson, E. , DiGuiseppi, C. , & Litt, J. (2009). An assessment of indoor air quality in recent Mexican immigrant housing in Commerce City, Colorado. Atmospheric Environment, 43(35), 5661–5667. 10.1016/j.atmosenv.2009.07.037 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Mitchell, L. E. , Lin, J. C. , Bowling, D. R. , Pataki, D. E. , Strong, C. , Schauer, A. J. , Bares, R. , Bush, S. E. , Stephens, B. B. , Mendoza, D. , Mallia, D. , Holland, L. , Gurney, K. R. , & Ehleringer, J. R. (2018). Long‐term urban carbon dioxide observations reveal spatial and temporal dynamics related to urban characteristics and growth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(12), 2912–2917. 10.1073/pnas.1702393115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Moore, J. , & Jacobson, A. D. (2015). Seasonally varying contributions to urban CO2 in the Chicago, Illinois, USA region: Insights from a high‐resolution CO2 concentration and δ13C record. Elementa: Science of the Anthropocene, 3, 000052. [Google Scholar]
  29. National Research Council (2010). Ocean acidification: A national strategy to meet the challenges of a changing ocean. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 10.17226/12904 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Persily, A. (2018). Development of an indoor carbon dioxide metric. Proceedings of the 39th Air Infiltration and Ventilation Centre (AIVC) Conference “Smart Ventilation for Buildings,” Antibes Juan‐Les‐Pins, France, 18–19 September 2018. [Google Scholar]
  31. Persily, A. , & de Jonge, L. (2017). Carbon dioxide generation rates for building occupants. Indoor Air, 27(5), 868–879. 10.1111/ina.12383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Pietschnig, J. , & Voracek, M. (2015). One century of global IQ gains: A formal meta‐analysis of the Flynn effect (1909–2013). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 282–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Robertson, D. S. (2001). The rise in the atmospheric concentration of carbon dioxide and the effects on human health. Medical Hypotheses, 56(4), 513–518. 10.1054/mehy.2000.1256 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Rodeheffer, C. D. , Chabal, S. , Clarke, J. M. , & Fothergill, D. M. (2018). Acute exposure to low‐to‐moderate carbon dioxide levels and submariner decision making. Aerospace Medicine and Human Performance, 89(6), 520–525. 10.3357/AMHP.5010.2018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Rubino, M. , Etheridge, D. M. , Trudinger, C. M. , Allison, C. E. , Battle, M. O. , Langenfelds, R. L. , Steele, L. P. , Curran, M. , Bender, M. , White, J. W. C. , Jenk, T. M. , Blunier, T. , & Francey, R. J. (2013). A revised 1000 year atmospheric δ 13C‐CO2 record from Law Dome and South Pole, Antarctica. Journal of Geophysical Research‐Atmospheres, 118, 8482–8499. 10.1002/jgrd.50668 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Satish, U. , Mendell, M. J. , Shekhar, K. , Hotchi, T. , Sullivan, D. , Streufert, S. , & Fisk, W. J. (2012). Is CO2 an indoor pollutant? Direct effects of low‐to‐moderate CO2 concentrations on human decision‐making performance. Environmental Health Perspectives, 120(12), 1671–1677. 10.1289/ehp.1104789 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Sayers, J. A. , Smith, R. E. , Holland, R. L. , & Keatinge, W. R. (1987). Effects of carbon dioxide on mental performance. Journal of Applied Physiology, 63(1), 25–30. 10.1152/jappl.1987.63.1.25 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Scully, R. R. , Basner, M. , Nasrini, J. , Lam, C.‐w. , Hermosillo, E. , Gur, R. C. , Moore, T. , Alexander, D. J. , Satish, U. , & Ryder, V. E. (2019). Effects of acute exposures to carbon dioxide on decision making and cognition in astronaut‐like subjects. npj Microgravity, 5.1, 1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Shendell, D. G. , Prill, R. , Fisk, W. J. , Apte, M. G. , Blake, D. , & Faulkner, D. (2004). Associations between classroom CO2 concentrations and student attendance in Washington and Idaho. Indoor Air, 14(5), 333–341. 10.1111/j.1600-0668.2004.00251.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Shriram, S. , Ramamurthy, K. , & Ramakrishnan, S. (2019). Effect of occupant‐induced indoor CO2 concentration and bioeffluents on human physiology using a spirometric test. Building and Environment, 149, 58–67. [Google Scholar]
  41. Snow, S. , Boyson, A. S. , Paas, K. H. W. , Gough, H. , King, M.‐F. , Barlow, J. , Noakes, C. J. , & schraefel, m.c. (2019). Exploring the physiological, neurophysiological and cognitive performance effects of elevated carbon dioxide concentrations indoors. Building and Environment, 156, 243–252. 10.1016/j.buildenv.2019.04.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Stankovic, A. , Alexander, D. , Oman, C. M. , & Schneiderman, J. (2016). A review of cognitive and behavioral effects of increased carbon dioxide exposure in humans. National Aeronautics and Space Administration, NASA/TM‐2016‐219277. Available. http://ston.jsc.nasa.gov/collections/TRS [Google Scholar]
  43. Stott, P. A. , Stone, D. A. , & Allen, M. R. (2004). Human contribution to the European heatwave of 2003. Nature, 432(7017), 610–614. 10.1038/nature03089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. U.S. Energy Information Administration (2018). Annual energy outlook 2018. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Energy; https://www.eia.gov/outlooks/aeo, accessed April 2019. [Google Scholar]
  45. U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (2017), Table ES‐3). Inventory of U.S. greenhouse gas emissions and sinks: 1990–2015. Washington, DC: U.S. Environmental Protection Agency; https://www.epa.gov/ghgemissions/inventory-us-greenhouse-gas-emissions-and-sinks-1990-2015 [Google Scholar]
  46. van Vuuren, D. P. , Edmonds, J. , Kainuma, M. , Riahi, K. , Thomson, A. , Hibbard, K. , Hurtt, G. C. , Kram, T. , Krey, V. , Lamarque, J. F. , Masui, T. , Meinshausen, M. , Nakicenovic, N. , Smith, S. J. , & Rose, S. K. (2011). The representative concentration pathways: An overview. Climatic Change, 109(1‐2), 5–31. 10.1007/s10584-011-0148-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Vehviläinen, T. , Lindholm, H. , Rintamäki, H. , Pääkkönen, R. , Hirvonen, A. , Niemi, O. , & Vinha, J. (2016). High indoor CO2 concentrations in an office environment increases the transcutaneous CO2 level and sleepiness during cognitive work. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Hygiene, 13(1), 19–29. 10.1080/15459624.2015.1076160 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Vytal, K. , Cornwell, B. , Arkin, N. , & Grillon, C. (2012). Describing the interplay between anxiety and cognition: From impaired performance under low cognitive load to reduced anxiety under high load: Anxiety and cognition. Psychophysiology, 49(6), 842–852. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01358.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Wargocki, P. , Porras‐Salazar, J. A. , Contreras‐Espinoza, S. , & Bahnfleth, W. (2020). The relationships between classroom air quality and children's performance in school. Building and Environment, 173, 106,749. [Google Scholar]
  50. Woodbury, D. M. , Rollins, L. T. , Gardner, M. D. , Hirschi, W. L. , Hogan, J. R. , Rallison, M. L. , Tanner, G. S. , & Brodie, D. A. (1958). Effects of carbon dioxide on brain excitability and electrolytes. The American Journal of Physiology, 192(1), 79–90. 10.1152/ajplegacy.1957.192.1.79 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Xu, F. , Uh, J. , Brier, M. R. , Hart J Jr, Yezhuvath, U. S. , Gu, H. , Yang, Y. , & Lu, H. (2011). The influence of carbon dioxide on brain activity and metabolism in conscious humans. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 31(1), 58–67. 10.1038/jcbfm.2010.153 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Yang, Y. , Sun, C. , & Sun, M. (1997). The effect of moderately increased CO2 concentration on perception of coherent motion. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 68, 187–191. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Zhang, X. , Wargocki, P. , & Lian, Z. (2015). Effects of exposure to carbon dioxide and human bioeffluents on cognitive performance. Procedia Engineering, 121, 138–142. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

All data are publicly available and/or drawn from primary sources cited in the main text. Historical measurements of atmospheric carbon dioxide concentration (used in Figure 1) are available at the websites (http://ncdc.noaa.gov/paleo/study/17975 and https://www.esrl.noaa.gov/gmd/ccgg/trends/data.html). Future estimates of atmospheric carbon dioxide concentrations associated with RCP4.5 and RCP8.5 (also used in Figure 1) are available at the website (http://www.iiasa.ac.at/web-apps/tnt/RcpDb/).


Articles from GeoHealth are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES